Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 03 April 2023
Sec. Cognitive Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Crossmodal Correspondence View all 14 articles

Self-construal priming modulates sonic seasoning

\nJingxian Xu&#x;Jingxian Xu1Xiyu Guo&#x;Xiyu Guo1Mengying LiuMengying Liu1Hui Xu
Hui Xu2*Jianping Huang
Jianping Huang1*
  • 1Department of Psychology, Soochow University, Suzhou, China
  • 2School of Public Affairs, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China

Introduction: “Sonic seasoning” is when music influences the real taste experiences of consumers. “Self-construal” is how individuals perceive, understand, and interpret themselves. Numerous studies have shown that independent and interdependent self-construal priming can affect a person's cognition and behavior; however, their moderating effect on the sonic seasoning effect remains unclear.

Methods: This experiment was a 2 (self-construal priming: independent self-construal or interdependent self-construal) × 2 (chocolate: milk chocolate or dark chocolate) × 2 (emotional music: positive emotional music or negative emotional music) mixed design, and explored the moderating role of self-construal priming and the effect of emotional music on taste by comparing participants' evaluations of chocolates while listening to positive or negative music after different levels of self-construal priming.

Results: After initiating independent self-construal, participants increased their ratings of milk chocolate sweetness when listening to music that elicited positive emotions, t(32) = 3.11, p = 0.004, Cohen's d = 0.54, 95% CI = [0.33, 1.61]. In contrast, interdependent self-construal priming led participants to perceive dark chocolate as sweeter when they heard positive music, t(29) = 3.63, p = 0.001, Cohen's d = 0.66, 95%CI = [0.44, 1.56].

Discussion: This study provides evidence for improving people's individual eating experience and enjoyment of food.

Introduction

Researchers have been increasingly interested in sonic seasoning in recent decades (Knöferle and Spence, 2012; Spence, 2017; Spence et al., 2019; Spence and Di Stefano, 2022). This term refers to the deliberate matching of music with flavor to enhance the multisensory tasting experience (Sedacca, 2016). For instance, music has been composed for specific flavors such as “sweet,” “bitter,” or “salty” based on the flavors of the food (Crisinel and Spence, 2010). Wang et al. (2017) indicated that spiciness was associated with auditory attributes of high pitch, fast tempo, and high levels of distortion. Wang et al. (2021) further explored the acoustical/musical attributes that best match saltiness and found that auditory attributes based on emotional associations (negative valence, minor mode, and high arousal), long decay durations, regular rhythm, and a high degree of auditory roughness were associated most strongly with saltiness. These findings on sonic seasoning have been used in marketing-led activations (Spence et al., 2021) and further understanding of how music influences consumers' perceptions of food. Crisinel et al. (2012) demonstrated for the first time that individuals evaluate food differently based on the music they hear while tasting it. Participants were asked to rate the taste of several bittersweet toffees while listening to sweet or bitter music. It was found that when they listened to the sweet soundtrack, the sweetness of the toffee was evaluated higher than when listening to the bitter soundtrack. Wang et al. (2020) added that chocolate ratings could be affected only when participants listened to sweet or bitter music before or during a chocolate tasting.

Other previous literature also affirms that when music is playing in the background while people eat, the emotions that individuals experience from or associated with the music are transferred to the tasting experience itself, where these emotions are considered to act as an important mediator during sonic seasoning demonstrations (Kantono et al., 2016, 2019; Reinoso-Carvalho et al., 2019, 2020). For instance, North (2012) showed that emotional connotations associated with background music influenced consumers' taste perception of wine. In addition, Kantono et al. (2016) showed that music evoked positive emotions (satisfaction, happiness, and amusement) that influenced the perceived sweetness of gelati, while music evoked negative emotions (contempt, disappointment, and disgust) that influenced the perceived bitterness of gelati. Reinoso-Carvalho et al. (2019) discovered that compared to music associated with positive emotion when participants listened to music associated with negative emotion, they rated the same beer as more bitter, with higher alcohol content, and were willing to pay more for the beer. The music that evokes positive emotions is often performed with high intensity, a brighter timbre, staccato articulation, and a rapid tempo. Conversely, music that evokes negative emotions is often performed with low intensity, a duller timbre, legato articulation, and a slow tempo (Quinto and Thompson, 2013).

Research showed that auditory information like music could impact the subjective evaluation of the taste of food (Spence and Di Stefano, 2022). For example, Reinoso-Carvalho et al. (2020) discovered that emotional music (i.e., music that evokes positive or negative emotions), as opposed to crossmodal music (i.e., soft or hard music; the former means imaging the music is consistent with the smooth food texture, the latter refers to imaging the music is consistent with the rough food texture), had a more prominent effect on food flavor and purchase intention. Specifically, participants rated the chocolate as sweeter, and the purchase intention was higher when music judged to elicit positive valence was played compared to music that was judged to elicit negative valence. However, the same chocolate was judged to be more bitter when exposed to music that conveyed negative rather than positive emotions. The researchers proposed that the sweeter (bitter) taste of chocolate induced by positive (negative) music could be explained by a theory similar to the “attention-shifting or redirection effect” (Johnson and Proctor, 2004). That is music that transmits positive or negative emotions can allow participants to transfer their feelings to the flavor perception of chocolate, which in turn affects their purchase intention. An analogous conclusion was proposed in the study by Ziv (2018) that participants generally considered cookies to taste better when listening to pleasant music.

As described above, people's perception of food is affected by contextual factors. Moreover, a large number of studies have suggested that individuals from different cultural backgrounds are influenced by context to different degrees (Wan et al., 2014; Jeong and Lee, 2021), which may be attributed to differences in cognitive style (Henrich, 2014). As one of the important cultural characteristics, self-construal is often used to explain cultural differences in human behavior, cognition, and emotion. It is primarily about how individuals perceive, understand, and interpret themselves. Markus and Kitayama (1991) suggested that independent self-construal is more predominant in Western culture, which conceptualizes the self as an autonomous and bonded entity, emphasizing self-independence and uniqueness. By contrast, interdependent self-construal is more predominant in East Asian culture, which conceptualizes the self as interconnecting and overlapping with others, emphasizing the importance of living in harmony with other groups and individuals. In general, interdependent self-construal facilitates the spontaneous association of a focal object with context compared to independent self-construal (Goh et al., 2007). Moreover, interdependent self-construal consumers believe that a higher price means a higher quality than independent self-construal consumers because the former are more susceptible to perceiving the association between product elements (Lalwani and Shavitt, 2013).

While self-construal can be judged by scales (Singelis, 1994) or inferred based on an individual's ethnicity (Van Baaren et al., 2003), researchers prefer to manipulate self-construal through priming methods when studying it in a laboratory (Kühnen and Oyserman, 2002; Reinoso-Carvalho et al., 2020). In particular, participants were asked to search first-person pronouns such as “I” or “mine” in a story to represent independent self-construal, while “we” or “our” imply interdependent self-construal. Considering the container that holds the food as a background, Huang et al. (2021) suggested that self-construal priming could modulate the influence of receptacles on food perception. Compared to independent self-construal priming, interdependent self-construal priming elicited a greater influence of the size of the plate on participants' willingness to pay (WTP) for noodles and the pleasantness ratings of the noodles served on the red plate. Therefore, considering emotional music as a contextual factor, it is reasonable to expect that self-construal priming could moderate the effect of emotional music on food perception.

This study investigates whether self-construal can modulate the effects of emotional music on food perception. Owing to how interdependent self-construal can improve the connection between the target and the contextual background (Masuda and Nisbett, 2001; Goh et al., 2007) and influence object processing that is dependent on context (Kühnen and Oyserman, 2002), we hypothesized that the participants' perception of food flavor would not be influenced by the background music under independent self-construal priming. Conversely, different emotional music would affect participants' evaluation of food after initiating the interdependent self-construal.

Methods

Participants

To estimate the sample size, we used the Easypower package (McGarvey, 2015) in R.4.2.0 (R Core Team, 2022) to conduct an a priori power analysis. According to the mixed design of 2 (self-construal priming: independent self-construal or interdependent self-construal) × 2 (chocolate: milk chocolate or dark chocolate) × 2 (emotional music: positive emotional music or negative emotional music). The sample size should not be < 48 with an effect size of the quadruple interaction of 0.25, a statistical power of 0.95, and an alpha of 0.05. Considering the balance between different experimental conditions, a total of 141 college students from Soochow University took part in the experiment. However, we had to exclude the data of 13 participants from analyses because their answers to all the questions were the same. Therefore, 128 valid data were received (Mage = 20.12 years, SDage = 0.16 years, ranging from 18 to 26 years; 35 males). All participants were right-handed and had normal or corrected eyesight, without color blindness or color weakness. All participants were paid 10 Chinese yuan after completing the experiment.

Apparatus and materials

The experiment used a 27-inch monitor with a resolution of 2,560 × 1,440 pixels and a refresh rate of 60 Hz. The online questionnaire was based on the Qualtrics platform (https://www.qualtrics.com), including basic information (e.g., sex, age, and degree of hunger) and the priming of self-construal, which refers to the study by Sui and Han (2007). We measured the degree of hunger based on previous literature that focused on food and/or beverages (Biswas et al., 2021; Moss and McSweeney, 2021) because hunger could potentially influence taste perceptions (Hanci and Altun, 2016). Participants were asked to carefully read a travel story that was presented randomly and then click on all the personal pronouns in the story (Grossmann and Jowhari, 2018).

In line with Reinoso-Carvalho et al. (2020), our study used milk chocolate (Callebaut N. 823, containing milk and at least 33.6% cocoa solids) and dark chocolate (Callebaut N. 811, containing no milk and at least 54.5% cocoa solids), as well as positive and negative emotional music from https://tinyurl.com/music-emotions-xcultural. Specifically, the negative emotional music sample could evoke more negative valence, and the positive emotional music sample could evoke more positive valence. The chocolate rating task consisted of the following four questions (7-point Likert scale): “What do you think of the sweetness of the chocolate?” “What do you think of the bitterness of the chocolate?” “How much do you like the chocolate in the experiment?” and “How likely are you to purchase the chocolate?” In addition, they were required to answer how much they would be willing to pay for a bar of chocolate in RMB (Renminbi, Chinese Yuan). The Chinese revision of the Positive and Negative Affect Scale (PANAS; Watson et al., 1988) developed by Qiu et al. (2008) was applied to determine participants' emotions. Specifically, participants had to rate on a 5-point scale positive (e.g., gratitude, energetic, active, cheerful, excited, enthusiastic, proud, happy, and joyful) or negative emotional words (e.g., scared, afraid, guilty, ashamed, nervous, irritable, angry, jittery, and sad) that appeared randomly. Scores from 1 to 5 represent very slightly or not at all, a little, moderately, quite a bit, and extremely, respectively.

An online pretest was conducted to ensure that the emotional music samples were effective (the negative emotional music sample evoked more negative valence, and the positive emotional music sample evoked more positive valence). A total of 34 participants (eight men and 26 women) between the ages of 19 and 25 years (M = 21.26 years, SD = 1.58) were recruited. Participants were required to listen to the emotional music sample and complete the Chinese PANAS mentioned earlier. The number of participants who listened to the positive music emotional sample is equal to the number of participants who listened to the negative emotional music sample. The results showed that participants rated significantly higher on the Positive (M = 2.30, SD = 0.87) than Negative Affect Scale (M = 1.29, SD = 0.35) after listening to the positive emotional music sample, t(16) = 4.34, p = 0.001, Cohen's d = 1.05, 95% CI = [0.51, 1.50]. As expected, participants rated significantly higher on the Negative (M = 2.66, SD = 0.47) than Positive Affect Scale (M = 1.63, SD = 0.47) after listening to the negative emotional music sample, t(16) = 6.52, p < 0.001, Cohen's d = 1.58, 95% CI = [0.69, 1.36]. These results indicated that the positive emotional music sample we used in this experiment could evoke more positive valence while the negative emotional music sample could evoke significantly more negative valence.

Design

This experiment was a 2 (self-construal priming: independent self-construal or interdependent self-construal) × 2 (chocolate: milk chocolate or dark chocolate) × 2 (emotional music: positive emotional music or negative emotional music) mixed design. Self-construal priming and chocolate were between-subject variables, and emotional music was a within-subject variable. The type of chocolate was randomly matched with the two pieces of music. The dependent variables were the rating of the sweetness, bitterness, liking, WTP, and purchase intention of the chocolate that they ate. Each participant ate no more than two pieces of chocolate twice.

Procedure

Participants were first asked to fill in personal information and complete the self-construal priming task. Then they had to complete the PANAS as baseline emotion. After that, participants rinsed their mouths with water and put on headphones. It should be noted that although the duration of positive and negative music was inconsistent (69 vs. 60 s), the volume of the two kinds of music was controlled at 70 ± 6 dB. The experiment requested participants to taste a bar of chocolate on a plate we had prepared while listening to a random type of music played through headphones. Meanwhile, they were allowed to continue savoring a second of the same chocolate if they had finished tasting the first piece before the end of the music. Next, participants were required to evaluate the chocolate they ate in the five dimensions mentioned above and accomplish the PANAS again. After rinsing their mouths again with water, participants completed a similar task of matching another type of music with another type of chocolate. All the analysis data are publicly available from the Open Science Framework repository (OSF) at https://osf.io/qhdgz/.

Results

Before formal analysis, we first conducted a manipulation check on the emotional scores of the participants after listening to positive and negative emotional music. As expected, music that conveyed positive emotions induced significantly higher positive (M = 2.25, SD = 0.83) than negative feelings (M = 1.15, SD = 0.33), t(127) = 14.46, p < 0.001, Cohen's d = 1.28, 95% CI = [0.95, 1.25]. Surprisingly, the participants also scored significantly higher on the Positive (M = 1.86, SD = 0.77) than Negative Affect Scale (M = 1.44, SD = 0.51) after listening to negative emotional music, t(127) = 4.74, p < 0.001, Cohen's d = 0.42, 95% CI = [0.24, 0.59]. We further analyzed the emotional scores of the participants after listening to positive and negative emotional music. The results showed that positive emotional music (M = 2.25, SD = 0.83) evoked significantly more positive valence than negative emotional music (M = 1.86, SD = 0.77), t(127) = 6.02, p < 0.001, Cohen's d = 0.53, 95% CI = [0.27, 0.53], while negative emotional music (M = 1.44, SD = 0.51) scored significantly higher negative valence than positive emotional music (M = 1.15, SD = 0.33), t(127) = 6.50, p < 0.001, Cohen's d = 0.57, 95% CI = [0.20, 0.38].

We performed a 2 (self-construal priming: independent self-construal or interdependent self-construal) × 2 (chocolate: milk chocolate or dark chocolate) × 2 (emotional music: positive emotional music or negative emotional music) repeated-measures ANOVA on valid data. The results found that the main effect of emotional music was significant in all five aspects of chocolate evaluation [Fsweetness(1, 124) = 21.92, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.15; Fbitterness(1, 124) = 6.91, p = 0.010, ηp2 = 0.05; Fliking(1, 124) = 21.62, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.15; Fpurchaseintentions(1, 124) = 24.97, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.17; FWTP(1, 124) = 10.43, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.08]. As shown in Figure 1, compared to negative music, the participants rated the chocolate as having higher sweetness (M1 = 3.48, SD1 = 1.68; M2 = 4.16, SD2 = 1.54), liking (M1 = 4.19, SD1 = 1.72; M2 = 4.97, SD2 = 1.43), and lower bitterness (M1 = 2.91, SD1 = 1.80; M2 = 2.47, SD2 = 1.56) when the emotional music was positive. In addition, the purchase intentions (M1 = 4.19, SD1 = 1.67; M2 = 3.34, SD2 = 1.63) and WTP (M1 = 1.84, SD1 = 2.97; M2 = 1.20, SD2 = 1.93) for chocolate were further intensive when participants were exposed to positive rather than negative music. The experimental results also revealed that the type of chocolate had a significant main effect on the sweetness [F(1, 124) = 33.18, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.21], bitterness [F(1, 124) = 36.46, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.23], liking [F(1, 124) = 17.45, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.12], and purchase intention [F(1, 124) = 13.16, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.10] of chocolate (as shown in Figure 2). Specifically, participants considered milk chocolate to be sweeter (Mdark = 3.16, SDdark = 1.13; Mmilk = 4.43, SDmilk = 1.32) and less bitter (Mdark = 3.37, SDdark = 1.19; Mmilk = 2.07, SDmilk = 1.24) than dark chocolate. Then, they preferred milk chocolate (Mdark = 4.11, SDdark = 1.09; Mmilk = 5.00, SDmilk = 1.27) and more purchase intentions for it (Mdark = 3.33, SDdark = 1.15; Mmilk = 4.16, SDmilk = 1.39) compared to dark chocolate.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The main effect of emotional music on the five dimensions. Note that error bars show the standard errors of the means, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01. and ***p < 0.001.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The main effect of chocolate is on the five dimensions. Note that error bars show the standard errors of the means, ***p < 0.001.

The results demonstrated that there was a significant triple interaction among emotional music, self-construal priming, and chocolate on the sweetness of chocolate, F(1, 124) = 4.15, p = 0.04, ηp2 = 0.03. Based on this, we further analyzed the effects of self-construal priming and chocolate type on the evaluation of chocolate sweetness under two music conditions. As shown in Figure 3, positive music improved the sweetness of milk chocolate (M = 4.97, SD = 1.53) more than negative music (M = 4.00, SD = 1.75) when independent self-construal was primed, t(32) = 3.11, p = 0.004, Cohen's d = 0.54, 95% CI = [0.33, 1.61]. However, music with positive emotions increased the sweetness of dark chocolate (M = 3.53, SD = 1.57) more than music with negative emotions (M = 2.53, SD = 1.25) under the priming of interdependent self-construal, t(29) = 3.63, p = 0.001, Cohen's d = 0.66, 95% CI = [0.44, 1.56].

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Triple interaction effect among emotional music, self-construal priming, and chocolate on the sweetness of chocolate. Note that error bars show the standard errors of the means, **p < 0.01.

Discussion

This study aimed to explore the moderating role of different types of self-construal in the process of emotional music affecting food evaluation. The experimental results mainly suggested the following three points. First, positive music can improve participants' ratings of chocolate and purchase intention. Consistent with previous studies (Ziv, 2018; Kantono et al., 2019; Reinoso-Carvalho et al., 2019, 2020), our study elucidated that participants find chocolate to be sweeter after listening to positive music, which in turn increases their liking and purchase intention for it. Skaczkowski et al. (2016) argued that positive (negative) emotions evoked by positive (negative) music could be transferred to individuals' authentic tasting experience based on sensation transference effects.

Second, our results demonstrated that self-construal priming could modulate sonic seasoning. In particular, music with a positive emotion was more likely to enhance the participants' evaluation of the sweetness of milk chocolate under the priming of independent self-construal and dark chocolate under the priming of interdependent self-construal. However, independent self-construal priming did not influence participants' evaluation of dark chocolate sweetness. As researchers have suggested before, individuals with independent self-construal tend to adopt an analytic thinking style that emphasizes the independence of individual objects, whereas individuals with interdependent self-construal tend to adopt a holistic style of thinking emphasizing that the world is composed of interrelated elements (Nisbett et al., 2001; Monga and John, 2007, 2008). This may explain why different self-construal priming types have different influences on sonic seasoning. The self-construal of interdependence can guide the participants to adopt a holistic way of thinking to perceive the relationship between the background music and dark chocolate, which leads to the influence of positive music on the evaluation of dark chocolate's sweetness. However, an analysis of milk chocolate showed that the evaluation of the sweetness of milk chocolate was not affected by emotional music under the initiation of the self-construal of mutual dependence. The possible reason is that milk chocolate itself has been perceived as sweet, so there is no significant difference in the evaluation of sweetness between positive and negative music, even though the participants were under the priming of interdependent self-construal.

Previous studies have focused on the sonic seasoning of food by individuals from different countries (Reinoso-Carvalho et al., 2019, 2020) or on explaining the influence of self-construal priming on food perception (Hansen, 2019; Huang et al., 2021). The innovation of this study is to make a thorough inquiry into how self-construal priming impacts the relationship between music-evoked emotion and the perception of taste. Theoretically, by comparing independent and interdependent self-construal priming on food evaluations, the present study reveals the moderating role of self-construal priming types on the effect of emotional music on real food tasting, which could enrich the findings of sonic seasoning. For the marketing field, our study provides a certain theoretical reference for improving individual diet experience and boosting happiness in eating. In particular, marketers can control consumers' perception of flavors in food or beverage products by playing different styles of music according to their cultural background. In addition, it is a bright prospect for marketers to adopt words that represent different self-construals on the packaging of food to change consumers' cognition.

Like other studies, this study has unavoidable limitations. First, after listening to the negative music, participants did not report a higher negative valence than positive valence. There were several possible explanations. First, a previous study found that compared to an audio-only presentation, an audio-visual congruent presentation, which represented congruent audio and visual emotions (e.g., happy face and happy music), could lead to a more intense emotional response. This effect occurred in both positive and negative music, and the effect was larger for positive music (Pan et al., 2019). The baseline emotional score in our study showed that participants were significantly more positive before the experiment. Thus, it was possible that the negative emotional music was not capable of evoking a strong negative emotional response to make negative emotion dominant for the participants. Moreover, people listened to music while eating chocolate and then completed the PANAS, which might result in emotional scores that not only represent the valence evoked by the music but also the taste of chocolate. As a result, future experiments should measure participants' positive or negative valence to the music itself using the GEMS (Zentner et al., 2008) or GEMIAC questionnaire (Coutinho and Scherer, 2017) and evaluate the music with respect to different taste categories. In the current study, we focused on the effects of music that can induce positive or negative emotions on food perception. Although we can see the effects of the music on the five dimensions and between independent and interdependent self-construal priming we cannot gauge the extent to which these effects are significant because there was no control (i.e., a non-music condition) for comparison. As a result, future experiments could add a non-music condition.

Second, the participants in this study were university students aged ~20 and were Chinese. We are not sure whether the findings of the study apply to people of other ages and cultures. Reinoso-Carvalho et al. (2020) found that the sonic seasoning effects were different on people from LATAM and Asia. Moreover, previous studies have found that sex differences in self-construal vary across different nations (Costa et al., 2001; Schmitt et al., 2008). Future experiments could expand the sample to different cultures and ages.

Third, this study showed that self-construal could modulate the effects of emotional music on food perception. Previous research discovered that crossmodal congruency between music and food/beverage had effects on the tasting experience (Wang and Spence, 2015; Reinoso-Carvalho et al., 2020; Motoki et al., 2022). For example, Wang and Spence (2015) demonstrated that the music chosen to be congruent with each wine was indeed rated a better match than other pieces of music, and the music significantly influenced the perceived acidity and fruitiness of the wine. Future experiments could investigate whether self-construal could modulate the effects of crossmodally congruent music on food perception.

Finally, Mawad et al. (2015) found that field-independent and field-dependent tendencies affected the visual attention processing of yogurt label information. Individuals with an independent tendency would pay more attention to the health label information of yogurt. It is well-known that individuals with field-independent tendencies are more detached from their surroundings and less susceptible to external cues than field-dependent individuals. Likewise, individuals who have experienced independent self-construal were also more focused on themselves. Therefore, we can further explore the influence of music on the process of an individual's attention to food and whether self-construal still modulates this process in future studies. Moreover, Peng-Li et al. (2020) discovered through eye tracking that sweet music improved gaze time to sweet food, while salty music increased their fixation time on salty food. Notably, Peng-Li et al. (2020) demonstrated the influence of different music on food with different flavors based on the cross-modality association of music and flavor. In light of this, follow-up studies should further examine the moderating role of self-construal in the effect of emotional music on an individual's attention to sweet or bitter foods.

Conclusion

The results of this study suggest that self-construal priming has a moderating role in how emotional music affects the authentic flavor experience. For example, under independent self-construal priming, there was no difference in the evaluation of the sweetness of dark chocolate between positive and negative music. However, positive music significantly improved participants' evaluation of the sweetness of milk chocolate. In contrast, interdependent self-construal priming obtained the converse result. Compared with negative music, positive music can enhance the sweetness of dark chocolate, but neither kind of music influences the sweetness of milk chocolate. This study provides evidence that could improve an individual's eating experience by promoting happiness when eating.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Human Research Ethics Committee of the Department of Psychology of Soochow University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author contributions

JX: methodology, investigation, data curation, and formal analysis. XG: formal analysis, methodology, writing, and original draft preparation. ML: writing, review, and editing. HX: supervision, writing, review, and editing. JH: conceptualization, supervision, writing, review, and editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China, awarded to JH (Grant No. 32100875).

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Mengting Hu for their assistance in data collection. Comments concerning this article should be sent to JH at anBodWFuZyYjeDAwMDQwO3N1ZGEuZWR1LmNu.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1041202/full#supplementary-material

References

Biswas, D., Labrecque, L. I., and Lehmann, D. R. (2021). Effects of sequential sensory cues on food taste perception: Cross-modal interplay between visual and olfactory stimuli. J. Consum. Psychol. 31, 746–764. doi: 10.1002/jcpy.1231

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, Jr. P. T., Terracciano, A., and McCrae, R. R. (2001). Gender differences in personality traits across cultures: Robust and surprising findings. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 81, 322. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coutinho, E., and Scherer, K. R. (2017). Introducing the GEneva Music-Induced Affect Checklist (GEMIAC): A brief instrument for the rapid assessment of musically induced emotions. Music Percept. 34, 371–386. doi: 10.1525/mp.2017.34.4.371

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crisinel, A., and Spence, C. (2010). As bitter as a trombone: Synesthetic correspondences in nonsynesthetes between tastes/flavors and musical notes. Attent. Percept. Psychophys. 72, 1994–2002. doi: 10.3758/APP.72.7.1994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crisinel, A.-S., Cosser, S., King, S., Jones, R., Petrie, J., and Spence, C. (2012). A bittersweet symphony: Systematically modulating the taste of food by changing the sonic properties of the soundtrack playing in the background. Food Qual. Pref. 24, 201–204. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2011.08.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goh, J. O., Chee, M. W., Tan, J. C., Venkatraman, V., Hebrank, A., Leshikar, E. D., et al. (2007). Age and culture modulate object processing and object—Scene binding in the ventral visual area. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 7, 44–52. doi: 10.3758/CABN.7.1.44

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossmann, I., and Jowhari, N. (2018). Cognition and the self: Attempt of an independent close replication of the effects of self-construal priming on spatial memory recall. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 74, 65–73. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2017.08.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanci, D., and Altun, H. (2016). Hunger state affects both olfactory abilities and gustatory sensitivity. Eur. Archiv. Oto-Rhino-Laryngol. 273, 1637–1641. doi: 10.1007/s00405-015-3589-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hansen, J. (2019). Construal level and cross-sensory influences: High-level construal increases the effect of color on drink perception. J. Exp. Psychol. 148, 890. doi: 10.1037/xge0000548

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henrich, J. (2014). Rice, psychology, and innovation. Science 344, 593–594. doi: 10.1126/science.1253815

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, J., Wang, C., and Wan, X. (2021). Self-construal priming modulates the influence of receptacles on food perception. J. Sensory Stud. 36, e12642. doi: 10.1111/joss.12642

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeong, S., and Lee, J. (2021). Effects of cultural background on consumer perception and acceptability of foods and drinks: A review of latest cross-cultural studies. Curr. Opin. Food Sci. 42, 248–256. doi: 10.1016/j.cofs.2021.07.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, A., and Proctor, R. W. (2004). Attention: Theory and Practice. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Google Scholar

Kantono, K., Hamid, N., Shepherd, D., Lin, Y. H. T., Skiredj, S., and Carr, B. T. (2019). Emotional and electrophysiological measures correlate to flavour perception in the presence of music. Physiol. Behav. 199, 154–164. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.11.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kantono, K., Hamid, N., Shepherd, D., Yoo, M. J., Carr, B. T., and Grazioli, G. (2016). The effect of background music on food pleasantness ratings. Psychol. Music 44, 1111–1125. doi: 10.1177/0305735615613149

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knöferle, K., and Spence, C. (2012). Crossmodal correspondences between sounds and tastes. Psychon. Bullet. Rev. 2012, 1–15. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0321-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kühnen, U., and Oyserman, D. (2002). Thinking about the self influences thinking in general: Cognitive consequences of salient self-concept. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 38, 492–499. doi: 10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00011-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lalwani, A. K., and Shavitt, S. (2013). You get what you pay for? Self-construal influences price-quality judgments. J. Consumer Res. 40, 255–267. doi: 10.1086/670034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Markus, H. R., and Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychol. Rev. 98, 224–253. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masuda, T., and Nisbett, R. E. (2001). Attending holistically vs. analytically: Comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 81, 922–934. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.922

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mawad, F., Trías, M., Giménez, A., Maiche, A., and Ares, G. (2015). Influence of cognitive style on information processing and selection of yogurt labels: Insights from an eye-tracking study. Food Res. Int. 74, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.foodres.2015.04.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGarvey, A. (2015). easypower: Sample Size Estimation for Experimental Designs. R package version, 1.0.1.

Google Scholar

Monga, A. B., and John, D. R. (2007). Cultural differences in brand extension evaluation: The influence of analytic versus holistic thinking. J. Consumer Res. 33, 529–536. doi: 10.1086/510227

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monga, A. B., and John, D. R. (2008). When does negative brand publicity hurt? The moderating influence of analytic vs. holistic thinking. J. Consumer Psychol. 18, 320–332. doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2008.09.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moss, R., and McSweeney, M. B. (2021). Do consumers want seaweed in their food? A study evaluating emotional responses to foods containing seaweed. Foods 10, 2737. doi: 10.3390/foods10112737

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Motoki, K., Takahashi, N., Velasco, C., and Spence, C. (2022). Is classical music sweeter than jazz? Crossmodal influences of background music and taste/flavour on healthy and indulgent food preferences. Food Qual. Pref. 96, 104380. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2021.104380

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., and Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: Holistic vs. analytic cognition. Psychol. Rev. 108, 291. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.291

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

North, A. C. (2012). The effect of background music on the taste of wine. Br. J. Psychol. 103, 293–301. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02072.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pan, F., Zhang, L., Ou, Y., and Zhang, X. (2019). The audio-visual integration effect on music emotion: Behavioral and physiological evidence. PLoS ONE 14, e0217040. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0217040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peng-Li, D., Byrne, D. V., Chan, R. C., and Wang, Q. J. (2020). The influence of taste-congruent soundtracks on visual attention and food choice: A cross-cultural eye-tracking study in Chinese and Danish consumers. Food Qual. Pref. 85, 103962. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2020.103962

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qiu, L., Zheng, X., and Wang, Y. F. (2008). Revision of the positive affect and negative affect scale. Chin. J. Appl. Psychol. 14, 249–254.

Google Scholar

Quinto, L., and Thompson, W. F. (2013). Composers and performers have different capacities to manipulate arousal and valence. Psychomusicology 23, 137–150. doi: 10.1037/a,0034775

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2022). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available online at: https://www.r-project.org/

Google Scholar

Reinoso-Carvalho, F., Dakduk, S., Wagemans, J., and Spence, C. (2019). Not just another pint! Measuring the influence of the emotion induced by music on the consumer's tasting experience. Multisens. Res. 32, 367–400. doi: 10.1163/22134808-20191374

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reinoso-Carvalho, F., Gunn, L., Molina, G., Narumi, T., Spence, C., Suzuki, Y., et al. (2020). A sprinkle of emotions vs. a pinch of crossmodality: Towards globally meaningful sonic seasoning strategies for enhanced multisensory tasting experiences. J. Bus. Res. 117, 389–399. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2020.04.055

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmitt, D. P., Realo, A., Voracek, M., and Allik, J. (2008). Why can't a man be more like a woman? Sex differences in Big Five personality traits across 55 cultures. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 94, 168. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.94.1.168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sedacca, M. (2016). Sonic Seasoning” is the Growing Scientific Field That Uses Sound To Make Food Taste Better. Quartz. Available online at: https://qz.com/871605/sonic-season-changing-taste-with-sound/ (accessed September 5, 2021).

Google Scholar

Singelis, T. M. (1994). The measurement of independent and interdependent self-construals. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bullet. 20, 580–591. doi: 10.1177/0146167294205014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skaczkowski, G., Durkin, S., Kashima, Y., and Wakefield, M. (2016). The effect of packaging, branding and labeling on the experience of unhealthy food and drink: A review. Appetite 99, 219–234. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.01.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spence, C. (2017). “Sonic seasoning,” in Audio Branding: Using Sound to Build Your Brand, eds L. Minsky and C. Fahey (London: Kogan Page), 52–58.

Google Scholar

Spence, C., and Di Stefano, N. (2022). Crossmodal harmony: Looking for the meaning of harmony beyond hearing. i-Perception 13, 20416695211073817. doi: 10.1177/20416695211073817

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spence, C., Reinoso-Carvalho, F., Velasco, C., and Wang, Q. J. (2019). Extrinsic auditory contributions to food perception and consumer behaviour: An interdisciplinary review. Multisens. Res. 32, 275–318. doi: 10.1163/22134808-20191403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spence, C., Wang, Q. J., Reinoso-Carvalho, F., and Keller, S. (2021). Commercializing sonic seasoning in multisensory offline experiential events and online tasting experiences. Front. Psychol. 12, 740354. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.740354

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sui, J., and Han, S. (2007). Self-construal priming modulates neural substrates of self-awareness. Psychol. Sci. 18, 861–866. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01992.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Baaren, R. B., Maddux, W. W., Chartrand, T. L., De Bouter, C., and Van Knippenberg, A. (2003). It takes two to mimic: Behavioral consequences of self-construals. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 84, 1093. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.5.1093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wan, X., Velasco, C., Michel, C., Mu, B., Woods, A. T., and Spence, C. (2014). Does the type of receptacle influence the crossmodal association between colour and flavour? A cross-cultural comparison. Flavour 3, 1–7. doi: 10.1186/2044-7248-3-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Q., and Spence, C. (2015). Assessing the effect of musical congruency on wine tasting in a live performance setting. i-Perception 6, 2041669515593027. doi: 10.1177/2041669515593027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Q. J., Keller, S., and Spence, C. (2017). Sounds spicy: Enhancing the evaluation of piquancy by means of a customised crossmodally congruent soundtrack. Food Qual. Pref. 58, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2016.12.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Q. J., Keller, S., and Spence, C. (2021). Metacognition and crossmodal correspondences between auditory attributes and saltiness in a large sample study. Multisens. Res. 34, 785–805. doi: 10.1163/22134808-bja10055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Q. J., Spence, C., and Knoeferle, K. (2020). Timing is everything: Onset timing moderates the crossmodal influence of background sound on taste perception. J. Exp. Psychol. 46, 1118–1126. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000820

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zentner, M., Grandjean, D., and Scherer, K. R. (2008). Emotions evoked by the sound of music: Characterization, classification, and measurement. Emotion 8, 494–521. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.8.4.494

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ziv, N. (2018). Musical flavor: The effect of background music and presentation order on taste. Eur. J. Market. 52, 1485–1504. doi: 10.1108/EJM-07-2017-0427

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: self-construal priming, emotional music, sensory marketing, sonic seasoning, tasting experience

Citation: Xu J, Guo X, Liu M, Xu H and Huang J (2023) Self-construal priming modulates sonic seasoning. Front. Psychol. 14:1041202. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1041202

Received: 10 September 2022; Accepted: 02 March 2023;
Published: 03 April 2023.

Edited by:

Na Chen, Bar-Ilan University, Israel

Reviewed by:

Steve Keller, Studio Resonate-SXM Media, United States
Nicola Di Stefano, National Research Council (CNR), Italy
Bruno Mesz, National University of Tres de Febrero, Argentina

Copyright © 2023 Xu, Guo, Liu, Xu and Huang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jianping Huang, anBodWFuZyYjeDAwMDQwO3N1ZGEuZWR1LmNu; Hui Xu, eHVodWk5MDA4MzAmI3gwMDA0MDsxNjMuY29t

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.