Corrigendum: Differential effects of attachment security on visual fixation to facial expressions of emotion in 14-month-old infants: an eye-tracking study
- 1Center for Research in Psychology for Positive Development, Lusíada University, Porto, Portugal
- 2Center for Psychology at University of Porto, Faculty of Psychology and Education Sciences, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal
- 3Escola Superior de Educação, Instituto Politécnico de Lisboa, Lisboa, Portugal
- 4Neurocognition and Language Research Group, Center for Psychology at University of Porto, Faculty of Psychology and Education Sciences, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal
- 5Faculty of Psychology and Education Science, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal
- 6Laboratory of Neuropsychophysiology, Faculty of Psychology and Education Science, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal
Introduction: Models of attachment and information processing suggest that the attention infants allocate to social information might occur in a schema-driven processing manner according to their attachment pattern. A major source of social information for infants consists of facial expressions of emotion. We tested for differences in attention to facial expressions and emotional discrimination between infants classified as securely attached (B), insecure-avoidant (A), and insecure-resistant (C).
Methods: Sixty-one 14-month-old infants participated in the Strange Situation Procedure and an experimental task of Visual Habituation and Visual Paired-Comparison Task (VPC). In the Habituation phase, a Low-Arousal Happy face (habituation face) was presented followed by a VPC task of 6 trials composed of two contrasting emotional faces always involving the same actress: the one used in habituation (trial old face) and a new one (trial new face) portraying changes in valence (Low-Arousal Angry face), arousal (High-Arousal Happy face), or valence + arousal (High-Arousal Angry face). Measures of fixation time (FT) and number of fixations (FC) were obtained for the habituation face, the trial old face, the trial new face, and the difference between the trial old face and the trial new face using an eye-tracking system.
Results: We found a higher FT and FC for the trial new face when compared with the trial old face, regardless of the emotional condition (valence, arousal, valence + arousal contrasts), suggesting that 14-month-old infants were able to discriminate different emotional faces. However, this effect differed according to attachment pattern: resistant-attached infants (C) had significantly higher FT and FC for the new face than patterns B and A, indicating they may remain hypervigilant toward emotional change. On the contrary, avoidant infants (A) revealed significantly longer looking times to the trial old face, suggesting overall avoidance of novel expressions and thus less sensitivity to emotional change.
Discussion: Overall, these findings corroborate that attachment is associated with infants’ social information processing.
1 Introduction
Different authors, such as Cassidy (1994) and Bridges and Grolnick (1995), argue that infants learn to express and regulate their emotions, particularly negative ones, in a manner that allows their attachment needs to be met by their caregivers. For the most part, infants with a secure attachment experience parental sensitivity in response to a broad range of emotional signals, which promotes open and flexible communication of both positive and negative affect and flexible regulation of emotion based on the demands of the situation (de Wolff and van Ijzendoorn, 1997; Fuertes et al., 2006, 2009, 2020; Lucassen et al., 2011; Barbosa et al., 2019, 2021). Many insecure-avoidant infants may experience parental intrusiveness, which promotes the defensive minimization of affect, and fewer caregiver-oriented (e.g., proximity seeking, looking toward the caregiver, asking the caregiver for assistance) and more self-oriented (e.g., self-soothing, self-distraction) regulation behaviors to prevent additional controlling behavior (Isabella and Belsky, 1991; Swanson et al., 2000; Cantero and Cerezo, 2001; Fuertes et al., 2006, 2009, 2020; Barbosa et al., 2019, 2021). Insecure-resistant infants, in contrast, may experience maternal passivity or unresponsivity, which promotes the maximization or heightening of affect and frequent use of caregiver-oriented regulation behaviors to gain the caregiver’s attention (Cantero and Cerezo, 2001; Fuertes et al., 2006, 2009, 2020; Barbosa et al., 2019, 2021). These patterns, which serve different functions in the context of the parent-infant relationship, are thought to become internalized and then generalized to other contexts in which they may be less adaptive (Bowlby, 1969/1982; Ainsworth et al., 1978; Goldsmith and Alansky, 1987; Bretherton, 1990; Sroufe, 1996; de Wolff and van Ijzendoorn, 1997; Cassidy and Shaver, 1999; Crittenden, 2000; Bakermans-Kranenburg et al., 2003; Bretherton and Munholland, 2008; Beebe et al., 2010; Verhage et al., 2016).
Models of attachment and social information processing suggest that the attention allocated to salient social information, such as facial expressions, might occur in schema-driven processing, according to infants’ attachment patterns (Field and Lester, 2010; Dykas and Cassidy, 2011; Johnson and Chen, 2011; Morales et al., 2016; Gonçalves et al., 2023). Previous studies have shown that recurrent attachment-related experiences shape representational, physiological, and behavioral responses to emotional information (Pollak et al., 2000, 2001; Pollak and Kistler, 2002; Pollak and Sinha, 2002; Dykas and Cassidy, 2011; Vrtička et al., 2012; Vrtička and Vuilleumier, 2012; Cooke et al., 2016, 2018). To our knowledge, Peltola et al. (2015) were the first authors to investigate infants’ attention to facial expressions in association with attachment security, while others have addressed the relationship between caregiver-infant attachment security and infants’ visual preferences using other social–emotional stimuli (see Johnson et al., 2007, 2010; Biro et al., 2015, 2017). They used a longitudinal design and studied infants’ normative developmental bias to fearful faces at 7 months (Peltola et al., 2008, 2009a,b, 2011, 2013; Forssman et al., 2014a) as an antecedent of later attachment quality at 14 months (Peltola et al., 2015). Consistent with the models reviewed above, the main finding was a smaller attentional bias to fearful faces in infants with an insecure attachment pattern vs. secure infants, and this effect was most clearly associated with attachment disorganization, suggesting altered sensitivity to threat-related cues in infancy as a testable trait linking attachment disorganization to later behavioral outcomes (for a review about the outcomes of threat-related attention bias in socio-emotional development please see Pérez-Edgar et al., 2017; Burris et al., 2019a,b; Fu and Pérez-Edgar, 2019). In the same vein, but using neurophysiological measures, Peltola et al. (2018a,b), measured event-related potentials (ERPs) to investigate whether cortical responses to facial expressions of fear were associated with the development of secure and insecure patterns of infant-caregiver attachment during the first year. Based on previous findings showing reduced attentional biases to fearful faces in infants with insecure and disorganized attachment, the authors hypothesized that insecure and disorganized attachment would be associated with reduced ERP differentiation of fearful from non-fearful faces. ERPs to facial expressions were measured at 7 months of age and attachment was assessed at 14 months of age with the Strange Situation Procedure. The results revealed that occipitotemporal face-sensitive ERP responses particularly in the time range of the N290 component were related to attachment security at 14 months. Furthermore, only securely attached infants showed age-typical cortical discrimination of fearful from non-fearful faces at 7 months, whereas a similar pattern of ERP responses was not observed in infants with insecure and disorganized attachment. These results add to previous findings by suggesting that patterns of secure and insecure infant attachment are related to early-emerging differences in the perceptual processing of facial emotions, which could have implications for the development of social competence. More recently, Forslund et al. (2019) found that disorganized children (6 to 7-year-old children) showed lowered attention to facial expressions, a diminished ability to discriminate facial expressions, and elevated emotional reactivity.
However, the specific developmental pathways that link attachment and socio-emotional development are still a topic of research (Belsky, 1997, 2005; Belsky and Pluess, 2009; Ellis et al., 2011; Cassidy et al., 2013; Paschall and Mastergeorge, 2016; Groh et al., 2017; Slagt et al., 2018; Behrendt et al., 2019). In the present work, we expected to contribute to the line of research investigating the relation between the quality of early social–emotional experiences and social information processing in infancy (e.g., Striano et al., 2002; de Haan et al., 2004; Taylor-Colls and Pasco Fearon, 2015; Kungl et al., 2017; Morales et al., 2017; Kataja et al., 2018; Juvrud et al., 2019; Kataja et al., 2019), specifically how infants visually explore facial expressions of emotion. Our primary goal was to test the hypothesis of potential differences in attention to facial expressions and emotional discrimination (sensitivity to emotional change) between infants classified as securely attached (B), insecure-avoidant (A), and insecure-resistant (C), and between infants grouped as securely (B) or insecurely attached (non-B).
Following the literature reviewed, we identified four major limitations/literature gaps which were addressed in the present study. First, the existing developmental literature has focused on normative attentional patterns according to age, namely, age-expected attentional bias (e.g., to fearful faces at 7-months of age) (e.g., Peltola et al., 2015). In this study, we go beyond the developmental normative attention bias according to age, to consider general visual preferences and their emotional correlates. Second, previous studies have failed to study attachment in a discriminating way, only considering the secure vs. insecure attachment pattern contrast (e.g., Peltola et al., 2018a,b). Here, we consider not only secure vs. insecure but consider A-B-C attachment patterns. We compare all three primary attachment groups other than combining the resistant and avoidant infants into one insecure group, which is critically important as there are different theoretically-derived predictions about the pattern of attention to emotional stimuli that insecure-avoidant and insecure-resistant infants may utilize. Third, several studies have used behavioral measures such as looking time, typically measured by recording cumulative gaze to a whole visual scene, which has been one of the most used behavioral indices of infants’ cognitive and perceptual capabilities (e.g., Johnson, 2004). Here, we used an eye-tracking system that allows for much more accurate measurement of how attention is allocated than general measures of looking time, through greater spatial and temporal precision in quantifying eye movements than video encoding by human scorers (see Papageorgiou et al., 2014; Leppänen et al., 2015; Crawford et al., 2016) and including the analysis of two different measures of infants visual preferences—mean FT and mean FC—, following the suggestion of LoBue et al. (2020) of using multiple outcome measures in infant research. Fourth, most developmental studies of emotion perception are not designed to distinguish whether infants discriminate visual affective features, such as valence (pleasant vs. unpleasant) and arousal (high vs. low) (Barrett et al., 2019). They tend to mix arousal and valence contrasts, or to present valence contrasts ignoring their arousal levels, into impure comparisons that need to be disentangled. For instance, previous studies have compared a pleasant instance of emotion (smiling in happiness) with one that is intended to depict an unpleasant instance of emotion (e.g., scowling in anger, or gasping in fear), ignoring their levels of arousal (e.g., Montague and Walker-Andrews, 2001; Leppänen et al., 2007, 2009). In this sense, we will try to disentangle this by selecting facial stimuli that differ in emotional valence (happy, angry) and the level of arousal (low, high). Therefore, we aimed to analyze if the impact of attachment security on infants’ visual preferences was independent of the emotional contrast presented, by including in our experimental paradigm three different emotional contrasts, namely, valence, arousal, and valence + arousal (following recent debates in the field of Facial Expressions of Emotion; see Almeida et al., 2016; Barrett et al., 2019; Pereira et al., 2019).
Based on the literature reviewed above (Isabella and Belsky, 1991; Swanson et al., 2000; Cantero and Cerezo, 2001; Fuertes et al., 2006, 2009, 2020; Barbosa et al., 2019, 2021), we hypothesized that: (a) since insecure-avoidant infants may experience consistent maternal intrusiveness, they may learn to attend away from feelings of distress and/or social-emotional stimuli to inhibit or prevent expression of distress when frightened, developing an avoidance attentional biases to social stimuli—irrespective of emotional valence—as a function of early interactions with the mother, and (b) contrarily, insecure-resistant infants are likely to learn to attend excessively to social and/or emotional stimuli, developing a hypervigilance attentional bias to social stimuli, due to the higher prevalence of passive or unresponsive maternal behaviors, feeling a higher need to monitor the environment. However, except for the studies developed by Peltola et al. (2015, 2018a,b), no previous study has examined the association between the processing of facial expressions and attachment security in infants. Therefore, our specific aims were to evaluate (1) 14-month-old infants general emotion discrimination abilities (using eye-tracking visual parameters of Fixation Time and Fixation Count, FT and FC, hereafter); (2) their emotion discrimination (FT and FC) as a function of attachment pattern (A, B, C); and (3) their emotion discrimination (FT and FC) as a function of attachment security (secure vs. insecure, B vs. non-B).
2 Method
2.1 Participants
Sixty-five infants and their mothers were recruited from an ongoing longitudinal cohort study. Four infants were excluded due to unsuccessful eye-tracking calibration (n = 2), or due to fussiness, movement, or inattentiveness during the experiment (n = 2). The final sample included 61 infants (35 girls, mean age = 13.93 months, SD = 1.21, range [12.00–16.50], birth weight ≥ 2,400 g; mean birth weight = 3,035 g, SD = 458, range [2,400–3,945]) and their mothers (M = 31.91, SD = 3.54, range [23.00–37.00]). First-minute Apgar scores ranged from 7 to 9 (M = 8.86, SD = 0.41). All infants were born full-term (Gestational Age (GA) ≥ 37 weeks; mean GA = 38.55 weeks, SD = 1.18, range [37.00–41.30]), healthy, and clinically normal at delivery. Parental self-report and medical records indicated no signs of sensory or neurological abnormalities, nor other illnesses or congenital anomalies in the neonatal period. The infants were all from middle-class Caucasian families. At the time of testing, they were free of visual or neurological abnormalities. Mother-infant dyads were recruited after delivery at the Unidade Local de Saúde de Matosinhos (Local Health Unit of Matosinhos) and at the Centro Hospitalar de São João (Hospital Center of São João). Recruitment was authorized by both Hospital administration boards, based on favorable reports of their respective ethical committees. The present study was conducted according to the ethical guidelines presented in the Declaration of Helsinki. Written informed consent was obtained from both parents, before conducting any assessment or data collection.
All infants lived with both parents in the same household. Families were from urban, middle-class socioeconomic backgrounds. No parent had any known serious physical illness, psychiatric condition, or drug/alcohol addiction problems. Most mothers (n = 32, 53%) completed a College/University degree, while 24 (39%) completed the 12th year (upper secondary school level) and five (8%) attained the 9th year (third level of basic education). All mothers were Portuguese, married or living with their spouse, had a healthy gestation, and had no problems at delivery. For 34 mothers (56%), this was their first-born child. At the time of testing, all mothers were employed, healthy, and free of visual or neurological impairments.
2.2 Materials
Stimulus materials for the eye-tracking experiment consisted of color images of female faces from two actresses exhibiting happy or angry expressions, taken from the NimStim Face Stimulus Set (Tottenham et al., 2009). Faces belonged to four conditions: High-Arousal Happy (HiHA), Low-Arousal Happy (LoHA), High-Arousal Angry (HiAN), and Low-Arousal Angry (LoAN). In our experiment, LoHA was used as the habituation face (and as the trial old face in the VPC experiment), with the other three representing changes in valence (LoAN), arousal (HiHA), or valence and arousal simultaneously (HiAN, see Figure 1). We only used the LoHa as the habituation face because it represents a valence (HA) and arousal (Lo) less likely to attract biased/reactive processing (baseline measure). This is because, in low-risk samples, infants’ early perceptual experiences with human faces are very likely dominated by low-arousal pleasant (happy) expressions from their caregivers, leading to perceptual expectations for these facial displays (for more detail, see Pereira et al., 2019). Stimulus selection was based on the following criteria: (1) the actress should be a European-American female; (2) happy and angry expressions with different levels of arousal should be available for the actor chosen; (3) finally, actresses should have at least four stimuli with recognition rate ≥ 0.80 (recognition rates in adults from Tottenham et al., 2009). Importantly, arousal ratings were examined to select an actress that had similar ratings for Low Happy (LoHA) and Low Angry (LoAN) stimuli, and also for High Happy (HiHA) and High Angry (HiAN) stimuli (arousal ratings in adults from Ferreira-Santos, 2013). Faces were presented with a uniform background (all white). Stimulus size and brightness were kept uniform using GIMP (Gimp, 2008).
Figure 1. Stimulus materials used in the eye-tracking Visual Paired-Comparison (VPC) experiment. Due to image reproduction authorization restrictions, the images/actresses here presented are merely illustrative, although they are part of the NimStim Face Stimulus Set. Face stimuli here presented are merely illustrative, sourced from the NimStim set of Facial Expressions (Tottenham et al., 2009) and available to the scientific community at https://macbrain.org/resources/. All faces are from the Nim Stim data set publicly available for scientific research. The codes of the images for the two actresses used in our study from the NimStim set were: LoHA: 08_F_HA_O and 09_F_HA_O; HiHA: 08_F_HA_X and 09_F_HA_X; LoAN: 08_F_ AN_C and 09_F_AN_C; HiAN: 08_F_AN_O and 09_F_AN_O.
In the test phase, the four faces were paired such that we had three types of contrasts between old face and new face for each actress: Arousal contrast: old face [LoHA] paired with new arousal face [HiHA], Valence contrast: old face [LoHA] paired with new valence face [LoAN], and Valence+Arousal contrast: old face [LoHA] paired with the new valence+arousal face [HiAN]. The “old face vs. new face” refers to the change of the facial expression or lack thereof, and not to the face’s identity; whereby the old/new contrast always involved the same actress. For each contrast (experimental condition), there were two versions: one with the new face on the left side, and the other with the new face on the right. Thus, each actress generated a set of six face pairs, each set defining a six-trial experimental protocol with three conditions. By switching the presentation order of 3 contrasts/conditions × 2 versions, we created 12 experimental protocols per actress (see Supplementary Table A1), and thus 12 × 2 = 24 protocols in total. Each infant was assigned to one of these (meaning that each infant only saw one actress throughout the experimental task).
2.3 Procedure
Potential participant mothers were contacted in the neonatal obstetric units within the first 72 h after the infants’ birth. The purpose and procedures of the study were explained, and mothers were invited to participate. If that was the case, informed consent was signed by the infants’ parents. Participant mothers were first administered a brief questionnaire to collect demographic information and data concerning infants’ perinatal health status was abstracted from medical records, to determine eligibility. When infants completed 14 months of age, a laboratory session was scheduled with the parents at the Faculty of Psychology of the University of Porto. The session began with the eye-tracking experiment, followed by the Strange Situation Procedure (SSP) (Ainsworth et al., 1978).
In the eye-tracking experiment, infants were accompanied by one of the parents, who was asked to help keep the infant’s attention focused on the stimuli during the session. Parents were told they could point to the computer screen or could say “look at the face,” but that they should not mention the emotion being displayed. Infants sat on a baby chair approximately 60 cm in front of a screen (47.5 × 29.5 cm), where they viewed the pictures of facial expressions. At this distance, each face corresponded to a horizontal visual angle of 9.52° and a vertical angle of 14.25° (both in habituation and in the VPC paradigm). Faces size was 10 cm × 15 cm and paired faces (in the VPC trials) were separated by a 12 cm interval, ear to ear. Infants’ mothers or fathers remained behind them with a hand on their shoulder, so that they would feel secure. Parents were asked to look above the screen to avoid any interference with infants’ gaze tracking. The infant’s body was stabilized with an Infant car seat with an arm support. Eye movements were recorded monocularly with a remote eye-tracking system (SMI RED 2501), at a 120 Hz sampling rate.
Stimuli were presented using the SMI Experiment Center (version 3.0). Each testing session began with a 5-point calibration procedure. The calibration point was an animation that appeared in each of the four corners of the computer monitor and the center of the screen, accompanied by a playful sound to capture the infant’s attention. Calibrations were considered successful when deviations were below 2°. Unsuccessful calibrations were reattempted twice. In case of repeated failure, the experiment would not run, and the participant was excluded from the experiment (two infants were excluded due to unsuccessful calibration as explained in the sample subsection). Each infant was assigned to one of the 24 available protocols (see Supplementary Table A1). Stimulus presentation was automated after a successful calibration. For all protocols (see Figure 2), each trial began with a Low Happy face (LoHA, habituation face) which was displayed in the center of the screen for 20s (see, e.g., Pascalis et al., 2002). The habituation phase was followed by the test phase with the six Visual Paired Comparison (VPC) trials. Each trial consisted of two emotional faces—the old face (similar to the habituation face), accompanied by a new face (HiHA, LoAN, or HiAN). Given the infants’ limited attention span, trials were organized into two blocks, three trials per block. Each trial was displayed for 5,000 ms (5 s). To maintain infants’ attention throughout the experiment and prevent fatigue, an attention-grabber was used. We used four different animated audiovisual stimuli accompanied by a piece of baby-Disney music (random duration between 1,000 ms and 1,200 ms).
Figure 2. Experimental paradigm used in the eye-tracking session. Face stimuli here presented are merely illustrative, sourced from the NimStim set of Facial Expressions (Tottenham et al., 2009) and available to the scientific community at https://macbrain.org/resources/. All faces are from the Nim Stim data set publicly available for scientific research. The codes of the images for the two actresses used in our study from the NimStim set were: LoHA: 08_F_HA_O and 09_F_HA_O; HiHA: 08_F_HA_X and 09_F_HA_X; LoAN: 08_F_ AN_C and 09_F_AN_C; HiAN: 08_F_AN_O and 09_F_AN_O.
After the eye-tracking experiment, mother-infant dyads participated in the Strange Situation Procedure (SSP) (Ainsworth et al., 1978). The SSP is a 21-min laboratory paradigm designed for assessing the infant’s attachment pattern. The paradigm involves a sequence of eight episodes designed to place mild but increasing levels of stress on the infant (i.e., being introduced to an unfamiliar playroom, interacting with an unfamiliar adult stranger, and brief separations from and reunions with the mother). By standard procedures, the SSP consisted of eight 3-min episodes, including two separations from and two reunions with the mother, and interaction with a female stranger.
2.4 Data analysis
2.4.1 Preprocessing of eye-tracking data
The eye-tracking data quality in terms of attrition rate and proportion of valid trials had to be similar to previous infancy eye-tracking studies (i.e., attrition rate around 20–35% or lower, based on Watanabe et al., 2012; Ambrosini et al., 2013; Oakes and Ellis, 2013), and proportion of valid trials in each eye tracking task at greater than 70%, based on Forssman et al. (2014b) and Leppänen et al. (2015). Trials were inspected for artifacts, and those with more than 20% data loss were rejected (i.e., since trials were displayed for 5 s, only valid trials with signal processing for 4 s were retained). This resulted in the loss of two infants (as explained in the sample subsection). Only participants with all six valid trials (i.e., two valid trials for each of the three conditions) were included in the statistical analysis. Rectangular Areas Of Interest (AOIs) were created around each face - the habituation face as well as each face in VPC trials. For each AOI x Trial x Participant, BeGaze software (v. 3.0) provided measures of fixation time (ms) and fixation count (number of fixations). For VPC trials, we computed the difference between the old face and the new face, which was used as a dependent variable in the analysis.
2.4.2 Classification of attachment patterns from SSP
Infants’ attachment behaviors were coded from video recordings, according to the attachment patterns (secure, insecure-avoidant, insecure-resistant) defined by Ainsworth et al. (1978). The scoring was made by two expert and reliable coders. For the four-way ABC classifications and presence vs. absence of a secure attachment (B vs. non-B), intercoder agreement was 92%. Different classifications were resolved in conference and consensus was achieved between the coders.
2.4.3 Statistical analysis
Preliminary analyses were first carried out to evaluate the distributional properties of the study variables, including identifying potential outliers, kurtosis, and skewness, following the criteria recommended by Kline (1998), and to calculate descriptive statistics for the study variables. Before the aim-oriented analyses, we tested the normality of the variables with Kolmogorov–Smirnov statistics to decide on the tests to perform. For t-tests and ANOVAs, preliminary analyses were first carried out to check their assumptions. We ran Levene’s test for homogeneity of variances. Since the homogeneity of variance assumption was not violated, parametric statistics were used.
To test for successful emotion discrimination, we first conducted a one-sample t-test (t = 0) entering the value of the difference [trial old face-trial new face] for both mean fixation time and the mean fixation count, taking the average values of all three new faces. To test the main hypothesis, we ran a mixed ANOVA, with Emotional Contrast (Valence, Arousal, Valence+Arousal) as a within-subjects factor, and Attachment Pattern (A, B, C) as a between-subjects factor. An additional analysis considering only whether the attachment pattern was secure (B) or not (non-B) was also carried out. Finally, to ensure that infants’ patterns of emotion discrimination were due to attachment pattern per se, and not to potential differences in habituation time (learning) across attachment patterns, we compared the habituation time across the three attachment patterns. To that end, we ran a one-way ANOVA using the mean fixation time toward the habituation face as the dependent variable and infants’ attachment patterns as the independent variable.
All statistical analyses were carried out using SPSS, version 27. Findings were denoted as statistically significant using an alpha of ≤0.05.
3 Results
3.1 Attachment patterns
In the final sample of 61 infants, 40 infants (66%) were classified as securely attached (B), while 21 were classified as insecurely attached, of which 11 (18%) were classified as insecure-avoidant (A), and 10 (16%) insecure-resistant (C). No infants met the criteria to be classified as disorganized (i.e., had scores higher than 5 in disorganization behaviors) (Main and Solomon, 1990). Cohen’s kappa coefficient for ABC classification (0.90) indicated excellent intercoder reliability. The final scores for discrepant cases were discussed and agreed upon by conferencing with an expert Strange Situation coder.
3.1.1 General emotion discrimination abilities
We found significantly higher mean fixation time and fixation count for the trial new face when compared with the trial old face (Table 1) regardless of the emotional condition (valence, arousal, valence + arousal contrasts): t(60) = −2.30, p < 0.05, d = 0.35 and t(60) = −2.03, p < 0.05, d = 0.31, for FT and FC, respectively. This suggests that 14-month-old infants were able to discriminate emotional faces based on novelty preference.
Table 1. Infants’ descriptive statistics for eye-tracking data: habituation, overall difference [trial old face-trial new face], and difference [trial old face-trial new face] according to valence, arousal, and valence+arousal.
3.1.2 Emotion discrimination as a function of attachment pattern (A, B, C)
The mixed-factors ANOVA with mean fixation time as the dependent variable showed that the main effect of the emotional condition (trial old face—trial new face per valence, arousal, and valence+arousal, Table 1) was not significant: F(2, 116) = 1.79, p = 0.174, η2 p = 0.043. Infants showed sensitivity to the new face, independently of the novel emotional condition (valence, arousal, or valence + arousal contrasts). There was, however, a significant large main effect of attachment pattern on the mean fixation time: F(2, 58) = 20.72, p < 0.001, η2 p = 0.509 (see Figure 3 for descriptives). Tukey HSD post hoc tests revealed that there were statistically significant differences between insecure-avoidant infants (M = 489.63, SD = 167.51) and insecure-resistant infants (M = −1026.06, SD = 167.51, p < 0.001); between secure infants (M = −409.89, SD = 161.83) and insecure-resistant (p = 0.012); and between infants insecure-avoidant and secure infants (p < 0.001). Overall, mean FT differences [trial old face-trial new face] were larger for the insecure-resistant pattern (C) compared to both insecure-avoidant (A) and secure (B) patterns, indicating that children classified with an insecure-resistant attachment pattern remain hypervigilant toward emotional change (see Figure 4). The interaction between emotional contrast and attachment pattern for mean FT was not significant: F(4, 116) = 2.31, p = 0.065, η2 p = 0.103.
Figure 3. Mean FT (ms) for valence, arousal, and valence+arousal according to infants’ attachment pattern classification (A, B, C).
Figure 4. The main effect of attachment classification (A, B, C) on infants mean Fixation Time [trial old face – trial new face], *p < 0.05; **p < 0.001.
Concerning fixation count as a dependent variable (Table 1), we found no significant main effect of emotional contrast [F(2, 116) = 1.77, p = 0.18, η2 p = 0.042]. There was, however, a significant large main effect of attachment pattern on the mean fixation count: F(2, 58) = 19.43, p < 0.001, η2 p = 0.493 (see Figure 5 for descriptives). Tukey HSD post hoc tests revealed that there were statistically significant differences between infants A (M = 1.31, SD = 0.52) and C (M = −3.23, SD = 0.52, p < 0.001); between infants B (M = −1.41, SD = 0.50) and C (p < 0.05); and between infants A and B (p < 0.01). Overall, mean FC differences [trial old face-trial new face] were larger for pattern C compared to both A and B, indicating that C children remain hypervigilant toward emotional change (see Figure 6). The interaction between emotional contrast and attachment pattern for mean FC was not significant: [F(4, 116) = 2.30, p = 0.066, η2 p = 0.103].
Figure 5. Mean FC for valence, arousal, and valence+arousal according to infants’ attachment pattern classification (A, B, C).
Figure 6. The main effect of attachment classification (A, B, C) on infants mean Fixation Count [trial old face – trial new face], *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.
3.1.3 Emotion discrimination as a function of attachment security (B vs. non-B)
Using two levels for attachment pattern (secure vs. insecure, B vs. non-B), we found no significant main effects of emotional contrast [F(2, 118) = 1.61, p = 0.207, η2 p = 0.038], attachment pattern [F(1, 59) = 0.253, p = 0.618, η2 p = 0.006], nor any interaction effect between the two [F(2, 118) = 1.68, p = 0.192, η2 p = 0.039] on mean fixation time.
The same tendency of no effect was found for fixation count as the dependent variable [emotional contrast: F(2, 118) = 0.975, p = 0.382, η2 p = 0.023, attachment pattern: F(1, 59) = 0.279, p = 0.600, η2 p = 0.006; interaction: F(2, 118) = 0.504, p = 0.606, η2 p = 0.012].
3.1.4 Fixation time in habituation across attachment patterns (control analysis)
A One-Way ANOVA revealed that the effect of attachment pattern on the mean Fixation Time for the habituation face was not significant at the p < 0.05 level [F(2, 60) = 1.25, p = 0.514, η2 p = 0.012]. Therefore, the increased sensitivity of infants classified with an insecure-resistant attachment pattern to new faces does not seem to result from increased habituation time.
4 Discussion
In the present study, we found significantly higher mean FT and FC for the new face when compared with the old face, evaluated in the 6 experimental VPC trials, regardless of emotional condition (valence, arousal, valence + arousal contrasts), indicating that 14-month-old infants can discriminate novel emotional expressions. This finding was expected according to the developmental literature regarding the ontogeny of facial emotion processing, where behavioral studies have shown that infants’ ability to visually discriminate emotional expressions emerges around 5–7 months of age (e.g., Nelson and Dolgin, 1985; Nelson, 1987; de Haan and Nelson, 1998; de Haan, 2001; Kotsoni et al., 2001; Frank et al., 2009; Leppänen and Nelson, 2009; Frank et al., 2014).
Models of attachment and socio-emotional information processing have indicated that infants’ security in the primary caregiver availability and responsivity might influence the attention allocated to socio-emotional stimuli, namely, facial expressions of emotion (Field and Lester, 2010; Dykas and Cassidy, 2011; Johnson and Chen, 2011; Morales et al., 2016). Indeed, we found a significant main effect of attachment pattern on infants’ mean fixation time toward facial expressions emotional change, independent of the nature of that facial expression change (in each emotional contrast), suggesting that attachment is related to social information processing. The present results converge to suggest that patterns of secure and insecure infant attachment are related to differences in perceiving and attending to facial emotions, as has also been suggested in studies with children (e.g., Laible and Thompson, 1998; Pollak and Kistler, 2002; Pollak and Sinha, 2002; Bosmans et al., 2007; Steele et al., 2008; Vandevivere et al., 2014; Peltola et al., 2015; Forslund et al., 2017; Meinz et al., 2017; Peltola et al., 2018a,b), adolescents (e.g., Cassidy et al., 2003), and adults (e.g., Mikulincer, 1998; Mikulincer et al., 2000, 2002; Dewitte et al., 2007; Dewitte and De Houwer, 2008; Edelstein and Gillath, 2008).
Furthermore, post hoc tests revealed that there were significant differences between infants insecure-avoidant (A) and insecure-resistant (C), and between secure infants (B) and insecure-resistant (C). Overall, differences [old face-new face] were larger for attachment pattern C compared to both A and B, indicating that insecure-resistant children remain hypervigilant toward emotional change. Insecure-resistant infants show a preference for the new face, through a more negative value for the mean difference in the contrast [old face-new face] for both FT and FC, revealing a higher sensitivity to the novelty effect (an increased sensitivity to faces and facial expressions of emotion). Insecure-resistant infants’ attention deployment can be characterized by an overall hypervigilance visual tendency, as they seem to be more aware or hypervigilant to the new stimulus, suggesting a higher facial emotional discrimination (sensitivity to emotional change). The group of resistant infants is the only group that, compared with their counterparts, always prefers to look at the new face compared with the old face (across the three emotional contrasts), showing heightened attention toward the new stimuli.
Contrary to the hypervigilant pattern of emotion processing that characterized the resistant attachment pattern, insecure-avoidant infants revealed a positive value for the mean difference [old face-new face] FT, meaning that they tend to look less to the new face in comparison with the habituation face, and thus revealing less sensitivity to emotional change. Possibly using disengaging-deactivating strategies, infants classified as insecure-avoidant did not show the same sensitivity to emotional change, in contrast with infants classified as insecure-resistant (C), as they gave priority to familiar stimuli, which is by both theoretical and empirical work with adults (Niedenthal et al., 2002; Fraley et al., 2006).
Moreover, our results concerning infants’ visual preferences across the specific attachment sub-groups (secure, resistant, and avoidant), are similar to the tendency found in the adult literature (e.g., Mikulincer, 1998; Mikulincer et al., 2000, 2002; Niedenthal et al., 2002; Fraley et al., 2006; Dewitte et al., 2007; Dewitte and De Houwer, 2008; Edelstein and Gillath, 2008). According to adult attachment theory, individual differences in attachment-related anxiety reflect variations in individuals’ vigilance to cues relevant to appraising and monitoring the availability and responsiveness of significant others.
However, despite this study’s contributions, the sample size limits the generalization of our findings. Furthermore, since the assessment of both variables (infants’ attachment and the attention deployed to facial expressions of emotions) happened concurrently, we were not able to make inferences about the direction of the association found, which would be interesting to address prospectively. We must also note that in our design, pleasant and unpleasant expressions were, respectively, collinear with the emotional categories of happiness and anger, which precludes a conclusive interpretation of this factor. However, we hope that our findings come to be complemented by future work on the effects of affective properties across a broader range of emotional categories.
In conclusion, our results are consistent with the notion that variation in perceptual vigilance, and particularly the heightened vigilance to changes in emotional expression, may underlie attachment-related anxiety. It is noteworthy that our findings were generally consistent regardless of the valence and/or arousal (or emotional change resulting from the combination of both) of the emotions being perceived. In other words, highly resistant infants were just as sensitive to changes in positive emotions, such as happiness, as they were to negative emotions, such as anger, as well as to more or less intense emotions (e.g., High Angry Face vs. Low Angry Face). The heightened sensitivity to emotional change in insecure-resistant infants independent of emotional contrast (positive vs. negative, more vs. less intense) is an important finding because it suggests that in appraising the availability and responsiveness of the attachment figure the attachment system does not differentially weigh signals that are indicative of happiness (acceptance, sensitivity) vs. angriness (rejection, control) (see also Zhang and Hazan, 2002).
In this work, we have attempted to understand how different attachment patterns are linked to different types of emotional information processing. Future studies should proceed in advancing this line of research, expanding our findings across other age groups, and integrating different stimuli (e.g., both attachment and non-attachment related) and/or using different experimental protocols or paradigms (the importance of using multiple outcome measures in infant research was recently addressed by LoBue et al., 2020). Multimodal assessment of attention in infants using different tasks or paradigms may facilitate the identification of early behavioral and neurocognitive markers of risk factors for adverse developmental outcomes such as socioemotional difficulties.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, upon request, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
The studies involving humans were approved by Comissão de Ética para a Saúde do Centro Hospitalar de São João e Comissão de Ética da Unidade Local de Saúde de Matosinhos. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.
Author contributions
JG: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. MF: Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. SS: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Writing – review & editing. PL-d-S: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. FF-S: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by FCT - Fundação para a Ciência e Tecnologia, I.P. by project reference UIDB/04375/2020 and DOI identifier: 10.54499/UIDB/04375/2020 (https://doi.org/10.54499/UIDB/04375/2020).
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1302657/full#supplementary-material.
Footnotes
References
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M., Waters, E., and Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: New Jersey.
Almeida, P. R., Ferreira-Santos, F., Chaves, P. L., Paiva, T. O., Barbosa, F., and Marques-Teixeira, J. (2016). Perceived arousal of facial expressions of emotion modulates the N170, regardless of emotional category: time domain and time–frequency dynamics. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 99, 48–56. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.11.017
Ambrosini, E., Reddy, V., De Looper, A., Costantini, M., Lopez, B., and Sinigaglia, C. (2013). Looking ahead: anticipatory gaze and motor ability in infancy. PLoS ONE 8:e67916. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0067916
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., and Juffer, F. (2003). Less is more: Meta-analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood. Psychol. Bull. 129, 195–215. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.2.195
Barbosa, M., Beeghly, M., Gonçalves, J. L., Moreira, J., Tronick, E., and Fuertes, M. (2019). Predicting patterns of regulatory behavior in the still-face paradigm at 3 months. Infancy 24, 501–525. doi: 10.1111/infa.12293
Barbosa, M., Beeghly, M., Moreira, J., Tronick, E., and Fuertes, M. (2021). Emerging patterns of infant regulatory behavior in the still-face paradigm at 3 and 9 months predict mother-infant attachment at 12 months. Attach Hum. Dev. 23, 814–830. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2020.1757730
Barrett, L. F., Adolphs, R., Marsella, S., Martinez, A. M., and Pollak, S. D. (2019). Emotional expressions reconsidered: challenges to inferring emotion from human facial movements. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 20, 1–68. doi: 10.1177/1529100619832930
Beebe, B., Jaffe, J., Markese, S., Buck, K., Chen, H., Cohen, P., et al. (2010). The origins of 12-month attachment: a microanalysis of 4-month mother-infant interaction. Attach Hum. Dev. 12, 3–141. doi: 10.1080/14616730903338985
Behrendt, H. F., Scharke, W., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., Konrad, K., and Firk, C. (2019). Like mother, like child? Maternal determinants of children's early social-emotional development. Infant Ment. Health J. 40, 234–247. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21765
Belsky, J. (1997). Theory testing, effect-size evaluation, and differential susceptibility to rearing influence: the case of mothering and attachment. Child Dev. 68, 598–600. doi: 10.2307/1132110
Belsky, J. (2005). “Differential susceptibility to rearing influences: an evolutionary hypothesis and some evidence” in Origins of the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and child development. eds. B. Ellis and D. Bjorklund (New York: Guilford Press), 139–163.
Belsky, J., and Pluess, M. (2009). Beyond diathesis–stress: differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychol. Bull. 135, 885–908. doi: 10.1037/a0017376
Biro, S., Alink, L. R. A., Huffmeijer, R., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., and van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2015). Attachment and maternal sensitivity are related to infants’ monitoring of animated social interactions. Brain Behav. 5:e00410. doi: 10.1002/brb3.410
Biro, S., Alink, L. R. A., Huffmeijer, R., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., and van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2017). Attachment quality is related to the synchrony of mother and infant monitoring patterns. Attach Hum. Dev. 19, 243–258. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2017.1302487
Bosmans, G., De Raedt, R., and Braet, C. (2007). The invisible bonds: does the secure base script of attachment influence children's attention toward their mother? J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 36, 557–567. doi: 10.1080/15374410701662717
Bowlby, J. (1969/1982). Attachment and Loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. 2, New York, NY: Basic Books. (Original work published 1969).
Bretherton, I. (1990). Communication patterns, internal working models, and the intergenerational transmission of attachment relationships. Infant Ment. Health J. 11, 237–252. doi: 10.1002/1097-0355(199023)11:3<237::aid-imhj2280110306>3.0.co;2-x
Bretherton, I., and Munholland, K. A. (2008). “Internal working models in attachment relationships: elaborating a central construct in attachment theory” in Handbook of attachment: Theory, research and clinical applications. eds. J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver. 2nd ed (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 102–129.
Bridges, L. J., and Grolnick, W. S. (1995). “The development of emotional self-regulation in infancy and early childhood” in Review of personality and social psychology. Vol. 15. Social development. ed. N. Eisenberg (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage), 185–211.
Burris, J. L., Buss, K., LoBue, V., Pérez-Edgar, K., and Field, A. P. (2019a). Biased attention to threat and anxiety: on taking a developmental approach. J. Exp. Psychopathol. 10, 204380871986071–204380871986021. doi: 10.1177/2043808719860717
Burris, J. L., Oleas, D., Reider, L., Buss, K. A., Pérez-Edgar, K., and LoBue, V. (2019b). Biased attention to threat: answering old questions with young infants. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 28, 534–539. doi: 10.1177/0963721419861415
Cantero, M. J., and Cerezo, M. Á. (2001). Interacción madre-hijo como predictora de conductas de apego: evaluación de dos modelos causales. Infancia y aprendizaje 24, 113–132. doi: 10.1174/021037001316899956
Cassidy, J. (1994). Emotion regulation: influences of attachment relationships. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 59, 228–249. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.1994.tb01287.x
Cassidy, J., Jones, J. D., and Shaver, P. R. (2013). Contributions of attachment theory and research: a framework for future research, translation, and policy. Dev. Psychopathol. 25, 1415–1434. doi: 10.1017/s0954579413000692
Cassidy, J., and Shaver, P. R. (Eds.) (1999). Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications. New York, NY: Guilford.
Cassidy, J., Ziv, Y., Mehta, T. G., and Feeney, B. C. (2003). Feedback seeking in children and adolescents: associations with self-perceptions, attachment representations, and depression. Child Dev. 74, 612–628. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.7402019
Cooke, J. E., Kochendorfer, L. B., Stuart-Parrigon, K. L., Koehn, A. J., and Kerns, K. A. (2018). Parent–child attachment and children’s experience and regulation of emotion: a meta-analytic review. Emotion 19, 1103–1126. doi: 10.1037/emo0000504
Cooke, J. E., Stuart-Parrigon, K. L., Movahed-Abtahi, M., Koehn, A. J., and Kerns, K. A. (2016). Children’s emotion understanding and mother–child attachment: a meta-analysis. Emotion 16, 1102–1106. doi: 10.1037/emo0000221
Crawford, H., Moss, J., Oliver, C., Elliott, N., Anderson, G. M., and McCleery, J. P. (2016). Visual preference for social stimuli in individuals with autism or neurodevelopmental disorders: an eye-tracking study. Mol. Autism. 7, 1–12. doi: 10.1186/s13229-016-0084-x
Crittenden, P. M. (2000). “A dynamic-maturational exploration of the meaning of security and adaptation: empirical, cultural, and theoretical considerations” in The organization of attachment relationships: Maturation, culture, and context. ed. P. M. C. A. Claussen (New York: Cambridge University Press), 358–384.
de Haan, M. (2001). “The neuropsychology of face processing during infancy and childhood” in Handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience. eds. C. A. Nelson and M. Luciana (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 381–398.
de Haan, M., Belsky, J., Reid, V., Volein, A., and Johnson, M. H. (2004). Maternal personality and infants’ neural and visual responsivity to facial expressions of emotion. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 45, 1209–1218. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00320.x
de Haan, M., and Nelson, C. A. (1998). “Discrimination and categorization of facial expressions of emotion during infancy” in Perceptual development: Visual, auditory and speech perception in infancy, 287–309.
de Wolff, M. S., and van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (1997). Sensitivity and attachment: a meta-analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Dev. 68, 571–591. doi: 10.2307/1132107
Dewitte, M., and De Houwer, J. (2008). Adult attachment and attention to positive and negative emotional face expressions. J. Res. Pers. 42, 498–505. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2007.07.010
Dewitte, M., De Houwer, J., Koster, E., and Buysse, A. (2007). What’s in a name? Attachment-related attentional bias. Emotion 7, 535–545. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.535
Dykas, M. J., and Cassidy, J. (2011). Attachment and the processing of social information across the life span: theory and evidence. Psychol. Bull. 137, 19–46. doi: 10.1037/a0021367
Edelstein, R. S., and Gillath, O. (2008). Avoiding interference: adult attachment and emotional processing biases. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 34, 171–181. doi: 10.1177/0146167207310024
Ellis, B. J., Boyce, W. T., Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., and van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2011). Differential susceptibility to the environment: an evolutionary–neurodevelopmental theory. Dev. Psychopathol. 23, 7–28. doi: 10.1017/s0954579410000611
Ferreira-Santos, F. (2013). Modulation of event-related potentials by facial expressions of emotion in infants (at 9, 16, and 24 months) and adults: Contributions for the understanding of the ontogenesis of emotional face processing (doctoral thesis). University of Porto, Portugal.
Field, A. P., and Lester, K. J. (2010). Is there room for ‘development’ in developmental models of information processing biases to threat in children and adolescents? Clin. Child. Fam. Psychol. Rev. 13, 315–332. doi: 10.1007/s10567-010-0078-8
Forslund, T., Kenward, B., Granqvist, P., Gredebäck, G., and Brocki, K. C. (2017). Diminished ability to identify facial emotional expressions in children with disorganized attachment representations. Dev. Sci. 20:e12465. doi: 10.1111/desc.12465
Forslund, T., Peltola, M. J., and Brocki, K. C. (2019). Disorganized attachment representations, externalizing behavior problems, and socioemotional competences in early school-age. Attach Hum. Dev. 22, 448–473. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2019.1664603
Forssman, L., Peltola, M. J., Yrttiaho, S., Puura, K., Mononen, N., Lehtimäki, T., et al. (2014a). Regulatory variant of the TPH2 gene and early life stress are associated with heightened attention to social signals of fear in infants. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 55, 793–801. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12181
Forssman, L., Wass, S. V., and Leppänen, J. M. (2014b). Training non-social attention control improves infants’ socio-cognitive abilities. In: Poster session presented at the Biennial International Conference on Infant Studies, Berlin.
Fraley, C. R., Niedenthal, P. M., Marks, M., Brumbaugh, C., and Vicary, A. (2006). Adult attachment and the perception of emotional expressions: probing the hyperactivating strategies underlying anxious attachment. J. Pers. 74, 1163–1190. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2006.00406.x
Frank, M. C., Amso, D., and Johnson, S. P. (2014). Visual search and attention to faces in early infancy. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 118, 13–26. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.08.012
Frank, M. C., Vul, E., and Johnson, S. P. (2009). Development of infants’ attention to faces during the first year. Cognition 110, 160–170. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.11.010
Fu, X., and Pérez-Edgar, K. (2019). Threat-related attention bias in socioemotional development: a critical review and methodological considerations. Dev. Rev. 51, 31–57. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2018.11.002
Fuertes, M., Lopes-dos-Santos, P., Beeghly, M., and Tronick, E. (2009). Infant coping and maternal interactive behavior predict attachment in a Portuguese sample of healthy preterm infants. Eur. Psychol. 14, 320–331. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040.14.4.320
Fuertes, M., Ribeiro, C., Gonçalves, J. L., Rodrigues, C., Beeghly, M., Lopes-dos-Santos, P., et al. (2020). Maternal perinatal representations and their associations with mother–infant interaction and attachment: a longitudinal comparison of Portuguese and Brazilian dyads. Int. J. Psychol. 55, 224–233. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12577
Fuertes, M., Santos, P. L. D., Beeghly, M., and Tronick, E. (2006). More than maternal sensitivity shapes attachment: infant coping and temperament. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1094, 292–296. doi: 10.1196/annals.1376.037
Gimp, G. N. U. (2008). Image manipulation program. User Manual, Edge-Detect Filters, Sobel, The GIMP Documentation Team 8.7
Goldsmith, H. H., and Alansky, J. A. (1987). Maternal and infant temperamental predictors of attachment: a meta-analytic review. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 55, 805–816. doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.55.6.805
Gonçalves, J. L., Fuertes, M., Lopes-dos-Santos, P., and Ferreira-Santos, F. (2023). Attachment, facial expressions of emotion, and socio-emotional development: A comprehensive review. [Manuscript submitted for publication].
Groh, A. M., Fearon, R. P., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., and Roisman, G. I. (2017). Attachment in the early life course: Meta-analytic evidence for its role in socioemotional development. Child Dev. Perspect. 11, 70–76. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12213
Isabella, R. A., and Belsky, J. (1991). Interactional synchrony and the origins of infant-mother attachment: a replication study. Child Dev. 62, 373–384. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01538.x
Johnson, S. P. (2004). Development of perceptual completion in infancy. Psychol. Sci. 15, 769–775. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00754.x
Johnson, S. C., and Chen, F. S. (2011). Socioemotional information processing in human infants: from genes to subjective construals. Emot. Rev. 3, 169–178. doi: 10.1177/1754073910387945
Johnson, S. C., Dweck, C. S., and Chen, F. S. (2007). Evidence for infants' internal working models of attachment. Psychol. Sci. 18, 501–502. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01929.x
Johnson, S. C., Dweck, C. S., Chen, F. S., Stern, H. L., Ok, S. J., and Barth, M. (2010). At the intersection of social and cognitive development: internal working models of attachment in infancy. Cogn. Sci. 34, 807–825. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01112.x
Juvrud, J., Rennels, J. L., Kayl, A. J., Gredebäck, G., and Herlitz, A. (2019). Attention during visual preference tasks: relation to caregiving and face recognition. Infancy 24, 356–367. doi: 10.1111/infa.12285
Kataja, E. L., Karlsson, L., Leppänen, J. M., Pelto, J., Häikiö, T., Nolvi, S., et al. (2018). Maternal depressive symptoms during the pre-and postnatal periods and infant attention to emotional faces. Child Dev. 91, e475–e480. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13152
Kataja, E. L., Karlsson, L., Parsons, C. E., Pelto, J., Pesonen, H., Häikiö, T., et al. (2019). Maternal pre-and postnatal anxiety symptoms and infant attention disengagement from emotional faces. J. Affect. Disord. 243, 280–289. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.09.064
Kotsoni, E., de Haan, M., and Johnson, M. H. (2001). Categorical perception of facial expressions by 7-month-old infants. Perception 30, 1115–1125. doi: 10.1068/p3155
Kungl, M. T., Bovenschen, I., and Spangler, G. (2017). Early adverse caregiving experiences and preschoolers' current attachment affect brain responses during facial familiarity processing: an ERP study. Front. Psychol. 8:2047. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02047
Laible, D. J., and Thompson, R. A. (1998). Attachment and emotional understanding in preschool children. Dev. Psychol. 34, 1038–1045. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.34.5.1038
Leppänen, J. M., Forssman, L., Kaatiala, J., Yrttiaho, S., and Wass, S. V. (2015). Widely applicable MATLAB routines for automated analysis of saccadic reaction times. Behav. Res. Methods 47, 538–548. doi: 10.3758/s13428-014-0473-z
Leppänen, J. M., Moulson, M. C., Vogel-Farley, V. K., and Nelson, C. A. (2007). An ERP study of emotional face processing in the adult and infant brain. Child Dev. 78, 232–245. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00994.x
Leppänen, J. M., and Nelson, C. A. (2009). Tuning the developing brain to social signals of emotions. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 37–47. doi: 10.1038/nrn2554
Leppänen, J. M., Richmond, J., Vogel-Farley, V. K., Moulson, M. C., and Nelson, C. A. (2009). Categorical representation of facial expressions in the infant brain. Infancy 14, 346–362. doi: 10.1080/15250000902839393
LoBue, V., Reider, L. B., Kim, E., Burris, J. L., Oleas, D. S., Buss, K. A., et al. (2020). The importance of using multiple outcome measures in infant research. Infancy 25, 420–437. doi: 10.1111/infa.12339
Lucassen, N., Tharner, A., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Volling, B. L., Verhulst, F. C., et al. (2011). The association between paternal sensitivity and infant–father attachment security: a meta-analysis of three decades of research. J. Fam. Psychol. 25, 986–992. doi: 10.1037/a0025855
Main, M., and Solomon, J. (1990). “Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth strange situation” in Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research, and intervention. eds. M. T. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, and E. M. Cummings (Chicago: University of Chicago Press)
Meinz, P., Morton, J. B., Pederson, D. R., and Moran, G. (2017). Biases in attention for social stimuli in children are associated with patterns of infant attachment: a brief report. Soc. Dev. 26, 80–90. doi: 10.1111/sode.12184
Mikulincer, M. (1998). Adult attachment style and individual differences in functional versus dysfunctional experiences of anger. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 513–524. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.2.513
Mikulincer, M., Birnbaum, G., Woddis, D., and Nachmias, O. (2000). Stress and accessibility of proximity-related thoughts: exploring the normative and intra-individual components of attachment theory. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 78, 509–523. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.78.3.509
Mikulincer, M., Gillath, O., and Shaver, P. R. (2002). Activation of the attachment system in adulthood: threat-related primes increase the accessibility of mental representations of attachment figures. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 83, 881–895. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.83.4.881
Montague, D. P., and Walker-Andrews, A. S. (2001). Peekaboo: a new look at infants' perception of emotion expressions. Dev. Psychol. 37, 826–838. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.37.6.826
Morales, S., Brown, K. M., Taber-Thomas, B. C., LoBue, V., Buss, K. A., and Pérez-Edgar, K. E. (2017). Maternal anxiety predicts attentional bias towards threat in infancy. Emotion 17, 874–883. doi: 10.1037/emo0000275
Morales, S., Fu, X., and Pérez-Edgar, K. E. (2016). A developmental neuroscience perspective on affect-biased attention. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 26–41. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2016.08.001
Nelson, C. A. (1987). The recognition of facial expressions in the first two years of life: mechanisms of development. Child Dev. 58, 889–909. doi: 10.2307/1130530
Nelson, C. A., and Dolgin, K. G. (1985). The generalized discrimination of facial expressions by seven-month-old infants. Child Dev. 56, 58–61. doi: 10.2307/1130173
Niedenthal, P. M., Brauer, M., Robin, L., Innes-Ker, A. H., and Innes-Ker, Å. H. (2002). Adult attachment and the perception of facial expression of emotion. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 82, 419–433. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.82.3.419
Oakes, L. M., and Ellis, A. E. (2013). An eye-tracking investigation of developmental changes in infants’ exploration of upright and inverted human faces. Infancy 18, 134–148. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2011.00107.x
Papageorgiou, K. A., Smith, T. J., Wu, R., Johnson, M. H., Kirkham, N. Z., and Ronald, A. (2014). Individual differences in infant fixation duration relate to attention and behavioral control in childhood. Psychol. Sci. 25, 1371–1379. doi: 10.1177/0956797614531295
Pascalis, O., de Haan, M., and Nelson, C. A. (2002). Is face processing species-specific during the first year of life? Science 296, 1321–1323. doi: 10.1126/science.1070223
Paschall, K. W., and Mastergeorge, A. M. (2016). A review of 25 years of research in bidirectionality in parent–child relationships: an examination of methodological approaches. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 40, 442–451. doi: 10.1177/0165025415607379
Peltola, M. J., Forssman, L., Puura, K., van IJzendoorn, M. H., and Leppänen, J. M. (2015). Attention to faces expressing negative emotion at 7 months predicts attachment security at 14 months. Child Dev. 86, 1321–1332. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12380
Peltola, M. J., Hietanen, J. K., Forssman, L., and Leppänen, J. M. (2013). The emergence and stability of the attentional bias to fearful faces in infancy. Infancy 18, 905–926. doi: 10.1111/infa.12013
Peltola, M. J., Leppänen, J. M., and Hietanen, J. K. (2011). Enhanced cardiac and attentional responding to fearful faces in 7-month-old infants. Psychophysiology 48, 1291–1298. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01188.x
Peltola, M. J., Leppänen, J. M., Mäki, S., and Hietanen, J. K. (2009a). Emergence of enhanced attention to fearful faces between 5 and 7 months of age. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 4, 134–142. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsn046
Peltola, M. J., Leppänen, J. M., Palokangas, T., and Hietanen, J. K. (2008). Fearful faces modulate looking duration and attention disengagement in 7-month-old infants. Dev. Sci. 11, 60–68. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00659.x
Peltola, M. J., Leppänen, J. M., Vogel-Farley, V. K., Hietanen, J. K., and Nelson, C. A. (2009b). Fearful faces but not fearful eyes alone delay attention disengagement in 7-month-old infants. Emotion 9, 560–565. doi: 10.1037/a0015806
Peltola, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., and Yrttiaho, S. (2018a). Attachment security and cortical responses to fearful faces in infants. Attach Hum. Dev. 22, 174–188. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2018.1530684
Peltola, M. J., Yrttiaho, S., and Leppänen, J. M. (2018b). Infants’ attention bias to faces as an early marker of social development. Dev. Sci. 21:e12687. doi: 10.1111/desc.12687
Pereira, M. R., Barbosa, F., de Haan, M., and Ferreira-Santos, F. (2019). Understanding the development of face and emotion processing under a predictive processing framework. Dev. Psychol. 55, 1868–1881. doi: 10.1037/dev0000706
Pérez-Edgar, K., Morales, S., LoBue, V., Taber-Thomas, B. C., Allen, E. K., Brown, K. M., et al. (2017). The impact of negative affect on attention patterns to threat across the first two years of life. Dev. Psychol. 53, 2219–2232. doi: 10.1037/dev0000408
Pollak, S. D., Cicchetti, D., Hornung, K., and Reed, A. (2000). Recognizing emotion in faces: developmental effects of child abuse and neglect. Dev. Psychol. 36, 679–688. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.36.5.679
Pollak, S. D., and Kistler, D. J. (2002). Early experience is associated with the development of categorical representations for facial expressions of emotion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 99, 9072–9076. doi: 10.1073/pnas.142165999
Pollak, S. D., Klorman, R., Thatcher, J. E., and Cicchetti, D. (2001). P3b reflects maltreated children’s reactions to facial displays of emotion. Psychophysiology 38, 267–274. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3820267
Pollak, S. D., and Sinha, P. (2002). Effects of early experience on children's recognition of facial displays of emotion. Dev. Psychol. 38, 784–791. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.38.5.784
Slagt, M., Dubas, J. S., van Aken, M. A. G., Ellis, B. J., and Deković, M. (2018). Sensory processing sensitivity as a marker of differential susceptibility to parenting. Dev. Psychol. 54, 543–558. doi: 10.1037/dev0000431
Sroufe, L. A. (1996). Emotional development: The organization of emotional life in the early years. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Steele, H., Steele, M., and Croft, C. (2008). Early attachment predicts emotion recognition at 6 and 11 years old. Attach Hum. Dev. 10, 379–393. doi: 10.1080/14616730802461409
Striano, T., Brennan, P. A., and Vanman, E. J. (2002). Maternal depressive symptoms and 6-month-old infants’ sensitivity to facial expressions. Infancy 3, 115–126. doi: 10.1207/s15327078in0301_6
Swanson, K., Beckwith, L., and Howard, J. (2000). Intrusive caregiving and quality of attachment in prenatally drug-exposed toddlers and their primary caregivers. Attach Hum. Dev. 2, 130–148. doi: 10.1080/14616730050085527
Taylor-Colls, S., and Pasco Fearon, R. M. (2015). The effects of parental behavior on infants' neural processing of emotion expressions. Child Dev. 86, 877–888. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12348
Tottenham, N., Tanaka, J., Leon, A. C., McCarry, T., Nurse, M., Hare, T. A., et al. (2009). The Nim stim set of facial expressions: judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Res. 168, 242–249. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006
Vandevivere, E., Braet, C., Bosmans, G., Mueller, S. C., and De Raedt, R. (2014). Attachment and children’s biased attentional processing: evidence for the exclusion of attachment-related information. PLoS ONE 9:e103476. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0103476
Verhage, M. L., Schuengel, C., Madigan, S., Fearon, R. M. P., Oosterman, M., Cassibba, R., et al. (2016). Narrowing the transmission gap: a synthesis of three decades of research on intergenerational transmission of attachment. Psychol. Bull. 142, 337–366. doi: 10.1037/bul0000038
Vrtička, P., Sander, D., and Vuilleumier, P. (2012). Influence of adult attachment style on the perception of social and non-social emotional scenes. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 29, 530–544. doi: 10.1177/0265407512443451
Vrtička, P., and Vuilleumier, P. (2012). Neuroscience of human social interactions and adult attachment style. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:212. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00212
Watanabe, H., Forssman, L., Green, D., Bohlin, G., and von Hofsten, C. (2012). Attention demands influence 10-and 12-month-old infants' perseverative behavior. Dev. Psychol. 48, 46–55. doi: 10.1037/a0025412
Keywords: attachment, information processing, eye-tracking, affect-biased attention, facial expression of emotion, infancy
Citation: Gonçalves JL, Fuertes M, Silva S, Lopes-dos-Santos P and Ferreira-Santos F (2024) Differential effects of attachment security on visual fixation to facial expressions of emotion in 14-month-old infants: an eye-tracking study. Front. Psychol. 15:1302657. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1302657
Edited by:
Serena Grumi, Neurological Institute Foundation Casimiro Mondino (IRCCS), ItalyReviewed by:
Elisa Roberti, Neurological Institute Foundation Casimiro Mondino (IRCCS), ItalyMichelle Dow Keawphalouk Narangajavana, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, United States
Copyright © 2024 Gonçalves, Fuertes, Silva, Lopes-dos-Santos and Ferreira-Santos. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Joana L. Gonçalves, am9hbmFnbG9wZXNAZ21haWwuY29t