Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 25 August 2022
Sec. Cognition
This article is part of the Research Topic Cognitive Reserve and Resilience in Aging View all 10 articles

Musical Activity During Life Is Associated With Multi-Domain Cognitive and Brain Benefits in Older Adults

\r\nAdriana Bttcher,Adriana Böttcher1,2Alexis ZaruchaAlexis Zarucha1Theresa KbeTheresa Köbe1Malo GaubertMalo Gaubert1Angela HppnerAngela Höppner1Slawek Altenstein,Slawek Altenstein3,4Claudia BartelsClaudia Bartels5Katharina Buerger,Katharina Buerger6,7Peter DechentPeter Dechent8Laura Dobisch,Laura Dobisch9,10Michael EwersMichael Ewers7Klaus Fliessbach,Klaus Fliessbach11,12Silka Dawn FreieslebenSilka Dawn Freiesleben4Ingo Frommann,Ingo Frommann11,12John Dylan HaynesJohn Dylan Haynes13Daniel JanowitzDaniel Janowitz6Ingo Kilimann,Ingo Kilimann14,15Luca KleineidamLuca Kleineidam11Christoph Laske,Christoph Laske16,17Franziska MaierFranziska Maier18Coraline Metzger,,Coraline Metzger9,10,19Matthias H. J. Munk,Matthias H. J. Munk16,20Robert Perneczky,,,Robert Perneczky7,21,22,23Oliver Peters,Oliver Peters3,4Josef Priller,,Josef Priller3,4,24Boris-Stephan RauchmannBoris-Stephan Rauchmann23Nina RoyNina Roy11Klaus SchefflerKlaus Scheffler25Anja Schneider,Anja Schneider11,12Annika Spottke,Annika Spottke11,26Stefan J. Teipel,Stefan J. Teipel14,15Jens Wiltfang,,Jens Wiltfang5,27,28Steffen Wolfsgruber,Steffen Wolfsgruber11,12Renat Yakupov,Renat Yakupov9,10Emrah Düzel,Emrah Düzel9,10Frank Jessen,,Frank Jessen11,18,29Sandra RskeSandra Röske11Michael Wagner,Michael Wagner11,12Gerd Kempermann,Gerd Kempermann1,30Miranka Wirth*Miranka Wirth1* on behalf of the DELCODE Study Group
  • 1German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases, Dresden, Germany
  • 2Section of Cognitive Neurophysiology, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany
  • 3German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases, Berlin, Germany
  • 4Department of Psychiatry, Charité – Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Berlin, Germany
  • 5Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medical Center, University of Göttingen, Göttingen, Germany
  • 6Institute for Stroke and Dementia Research, University Hospital, LMU Munich, Munich, Germany
  • 7German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases, Munich, Germany
  • 8MR-Research in Neurology and Psychiatry, Georg-August-University Göttingen, Göttingen, Germany
  • 9German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases, Magdeburg, Germany
  • 10Institute of Cognitive Neurology and Dementia Research, Otto-von-Guericke University, Magdeburg, Germany
  • 11German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases, Bonn, Germany
  • 12Department for Neurodegenerative Diseases and Geriatric Psychiatry, University Hospital Bonn, Bonn, Germany
  • 13Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience, Charité – Universitätsmedizin, Berlin, Germany
  • 14German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases, Rostock, Germany
  • 15Department of Psychosomatic Medicine, Rostock University Medical Center, Rostock, Germany
  • 16German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases, Tübingen, Germany
  • 17Section for Dementia Research, Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research and Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
  • 18Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, University of Cologne, Cologne, Germany
  • 19Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Otto-von-Guericke University, Magdeburg, Germany
  • 20Systems Neurophysiology, Department of Biology, Darmstadt University of Technology, Darmstadt, Germany
  • 21Munich Cluster for Systems Neurology, Munich, Germany
  • 22Ageing Epidemiology Research Unit, School of Public Health, Imperial College London, London, United Kingdom
  • 23Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Hospital, LMU Munich, Munich, Germany
  • 24Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Klinikum Rechts der Isar, Technical University Munich, Munich, Germany
  • 25Department for Biomedical Magnetic Resonance, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
  • 26Department of Neurology, University Hospital Bonn, Bonn, Germany
  • 27German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases, Göttingen, Germany
  • 28Neurosciences and Signaling Group, Institute of Biomedicine, Department of Medical Sciences, University of Aveiro, Aveiro, Portugal
  • 29Excellence Cluster on Cellular Stress Responses in Aging-Associated Diseases, University of Cologne, Cologne, Germany
  • 30Center for Regenerative Therapies Dresden, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany

Regular musical activity as a complex multimodal lifestyle activity is proposed to be protective against age-related cognitive decline and Alzheimer’s disease. This cross-sectional study investigated the association and interplay between musical instrument playing during life, multi-domain cognitive abilities and brain morphology in older adults (OA) from the DZNE-Longitudinal Cognitive Impairment and Dementia Study (DELCODE) study. Participants reporting having played a musical instrument across three life periods (n = 70) were compared to controls without a history of musical instrument playing (n = 70), well-matched for reserve proxies of education, intelligence, socioeconomic status and physical activity. Participants with musical activity outperformed controls in global cognition, working memory, executive functions, language, and visuospatial abilities, with no effects seen for learning and memory. The musically active group had greater gray matter volume in the somatosensory area, but did not differ from controls in higher-order frontal, temporal, or hippocampal volumes. However, the association between gray matter volume in distributed frontal-to-temporal regions and cognitive abilities was enhanced in participants with musical activity compared to controls. We show that playing a musical instrument during life relates to better late-life cognitive abilities and greater brain capacities in OA. Musical activity may serve as a multimodal enrichment strategy that could help preserve cognitive and brain health in late life. Longitudinal and interventional studies are needed to support this notion.

Introduction

Given that world populations are aging, age-related diseases such as Alzheimer’s disease (AD) are on the rise and pose public health challenges of utmost importance (Alzheimer’s Association, 2021). Healthy lifestyle activities are proposed to enhance brain and cognitive resources in older adults (OA) (Valenzuela et al., 2012; Wirth et al., 2014; Arenaza-Urquijo et al., 2015; Livingston et al., 2020), which may strengthen resilience against cognitive decline in aging and AD. Among others, regular musical activity, such as playing a musical instrument, is associated with reduced risk of developing dementia (Verghese et al., 2003; Balbag et al., 2014). To advance targeted intervention strategies, it is important to delineate presumed cognitive benefits and underlying brain correlates associated with musical activity in older age.

It has been proposed that musical activity shares communalities with the concept of environmental enrichment (Fauvel et al., 2014b; Sihvonen et al., 2017). In animal models, far-reaching neurobiological and behavioral benefits of environmental enrichment have been shown (Kempermann, 2019a). Playing a musical instrument is a highly stimulating activity that inherently combines complex stimulations involving the simultaneous perception and integration of multimodal motor, sensory, cognitive, emotional, and social stimulations (Lappe et al., 2008). Active engagement in this multimodal leisure activity is thus proposed to facilitate beneficial brain and cognitive plasticity throughout life until old age (Wan and Schlaug, 2010).

Indeed, there is evidence to suggest that musical activity may preserve or even enhance cognitive abilities that decline in older age (Román-Caballero et al., 2018; Sutcliffe et al., 2020). For example, it has been shown that OA with musical activity (both professionals and/or amateurs) outperform controls in multiple (far-transfer) cognitive domains including executive functions, attention, language, processing speed, visuospatial and/or memory abilities (Hanna-Pladdy and MacKay, 2011; Parbery-Clark et al., 2011; Hanna-Pladdy and Gajewski, 2012; Gooding et al., 2014; Mansens et al., 2018; Strong and Mast, 2019; Gray and Gow, 2020). Moreover, some of these cognitive benefits, in particular processing speed, appear to be particularly pronounced in older participants playing a musical instrument as compared to singing (Mansens et al., 2018). While most of the existing studies are cross-sectional in nature, presumed cognitive benefits of musical activity in older age are supported by musical intervention/training studies. According to such studies in musically non-experienced OA, learning how to play a musical instrument has a robust positive impact on higher-order cognitive abilities including executive functions, working memory, speech perception and visual memory compared to control conditions (Bugos et al., 2007; Seinfeld et al., 2013; Degé and Kerkovius, 2018; Worschech et al., 2021).

Comparatively little is known about the neural underpinnings of musical activity in OA (Schneider et al., 2019). Both, cross-sectional and longitudinal studies have mainly investigated respective brain correlates in young and middle-aged participants with musical activity (both professionals and amateurs) compared to controls. In this population, musical activity seems to be associated with brain plasticity, as e.g., reflected by greater gray matter volume (GMV) as well as white matter (WM) integrity in dedicated brain regions (Wan and Schlaug, 2010; Herholz and Zatorre, 2012). Among others, these areas comprise frontal and temporal GM and WM structures (Sluming et al., 2002; Gaser and Schlaug, 2003; Halwani et al., 2011; Gärtner et al., 2013; Fauvel et al., 2014a; Groussard et al., 2014; James et al., 2014), which are also strongly affected by healthy and pathological aging (Jack et al., 1997; Habes et al., 2016; Wirth et al., 2018). Musical activity is further positively associated with hippocampal structure and function in younger musicians (professionals and/or amateurs) based on cross-sectional (Groussard et al., 2010, 2014; Oechslin et al., 2013) and intervention/training (Herdener et al., 2010) studies.

In the same line, sparse investigations are suggestive of beneficial brain–behavior relationships relating greater GM and WM integrity to musical activity in OA. Higher levels of musical activity are correlated with larger GMV in frontal (inferior) and temporal (parahippocampal) brain regions in OA with various musical experiences (Chaddock-Heyman et al., 2021). Moreover, larger GMV in frontal (inferior) and temporal (hippocampal and superior temporal) regions has been found in a lifespan sample of professional piano tuners compared to controls (Teki et al., 2012). Another study showed greater WM microstructural integrity in the superior longitudinal fasciculus and uncinate fasciculus in aging musicians (Andrews et al., 2021). Finally, recent intervention/training studies in musically non-experienced OA have reported enhanced neural efficiency in a distributed frontal, temporal, and parietal network (Guo et al., 2021), stabilized WM microstructural integrity in the fornix (Jünemann et al., 2022) and increased cortical thickness in auditory brain regions (Worschech et al., 2022) in response to musical instrument playing compared to control conditions.

The main goal of the present cross-sectional study was to shed light on the association between participating in musical activity and indicators of brain and cognitive health in OA. Existing findings propose that regular musical activity, in particular playing a musical instrument (Mansens et al., 2018), may have benefits for late-life cognitive abilities. There is further indication that musical activity is associated with neurophysiological correlates of cognitive reserve and/or brain reserve in older age (Sutcliffe et al., 2020). Together, these mechanisms could contribute to better cognitive abilities (Chaddock-Heyman et al., 2021) and help counteract brain pathology (Stern et al., 2020) in late life, warranting further investigations. Here, we combined measures of long-term musical activity (as assessed by the self-reported frequency of playing a musical instrument across three life periods), multi-domain cognitive abilities and regional brain morphology to investigate the association and interplay among these variables in cognitively unimpaired OA. We hypothesized that musical activity is associated with better late-life abilities in multiple cognitive domains as well as larger GMV in pre-selected frontal, temporal and hippocampal regions and at the voxel level. We further anticipated that musical activity could be associated with a more efficient use of structural brain capacities, which may convey resilience in late life.

Materials and Methods

Overall Study Approach

To address our research questions, the present study assembled data from the ongoing, multi-center, observational DZNE-Longitudinal Cognitive Impairment and Dementia (DELCODE) study (Jessen et al., 2018) using a unique methodological approach: (1) Musical activity was assessed in a large sample of cognitively unimpaired OA (aged ≥ 60 years) as a binary group variable (i.e., participants with musical activity and well-matched controls) based on the self-reported frequency of playing a musical instrument across three life periods (i.e., young adulthood, mid-life, and late-life). (2) Cognitive abilities were assessed using sensitive latent factor composite scores across five cognitive domains and a global cognitive score (Wolfsgruber et al., 2020). (3) Brain morphology was assessed within pre-selected regions-of-interest (ROI) and at the voxel level to identify the spatial pattern of GMV associated with musical activity. (4) We accounted for reserve proxies (Stern, 2009) of high educational attainment, crystallized intelligence, socioeconomic status (SES) and participation in physical activity, most of which were considered in earlier studies on musical activity and cognitive abilities (Hanna-Pladdy and MacKay, 2011; Gooding et al., 2014; Mansens et al., 2018; Gray and Gow, 2020). In addition, we controlled for these potential confounders in respective associations with brain morphology.

Participants

The sample used in the present study was obtained from the baseline DELCODE study. The overall design of the DELCODE study is explained in the supplement. In the present study, cognitively unimpaired participants, i.e., OA, first-degree relatives of AD patients (family history, FH), and participants with subjective cognitive decline (SCD), were included and merged across the three groups to increase the final sample size and statistical power. Recruitment procedures and inclusion criteria are described in detail elsewhere (Jessen et al., 2018). In brief, all participants were aged ≥ 60 years, German speaking, and provided informed consent. Normal cognitive function was defined as a test performance within –1.5 standard deviations of age-, sex- and education-adjusted norms on all subtests of the Consortium to Establish a Registry of Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD) test battery (Morris et al., 1989). Exclusion criteria for OA, FH, and SCD were comprised of medical conditions including current or past major medical, neurological, or psychiatric disorders.

The DELCODE baseline dataset (total: n = 1079, data release for this study: 01.2021) was used to select a subset of participants into the present study. The selection procedure is detailed in the supplement (Supplementary Figure 1). We were able to include n = 70 older participants that reported musical activity across the life course (group of interest) and a well-matched control group (n = 70, for methodological details see below).

Measurements

Measurement of Musical Activity

We operationalized musical activity by the self-reported frequency of playing a musical instrument across three life periods ranging from young adulthood (13 30 years), mid-life (30–65 years) to late-life (65 years onwards). The approach was chosen, because participation in musical activity across the life course might be particularly beneficial with brain plasticity thought to continue throughout life (Wan and Schlaug, 2010). In agreement with a previous large-scale population study (Mansens et al., 2018), we used the available self-reported information to assess participation in musical activity. In the DELCODE cohort, musical activity was measured with the Lifetime of Experiences Questionnaire (LEQ, Valenzuela and Sachdev, 2007) adapted for the German population (Roeske et al., 2018). Details on the quantification and analysis of musical activity are provided in the supplement.

In brief, for each life period, the frequency of musical activity was measured using the respective LEQ item (“How often did you play a musical instrument?”) with responses provided on a 6-point Likert scale (0/‘never,’ 1/‘less than 1 time per month,’ 2/‘1 time per month,’ 3/‘2 times per month,’ 4/‘weekly,’ 5/‘daily’). We constructed a coding scheme to assess musical activity during life as a binary variable that was comprised of two groups, similar to the previous study (Mansens et al., 2018): (1) The musical activity group (group of interest) included participants that reported having played a musical instrument during life. All participants started in early adulthood and continued playing until the current life period with higher frequency (‘2 times per month’ or more) in at least one life period, given their respective age. Notably, a total of n = 67 started playing a musical instrument with higher frequency in young adulthood, n = 47 played a musical instrument with higher frequency in at least two life periods and n = 23 played a musical instrument with higher frequency in one life period. A detailed graphical description showing the individual trajectories of musical activity across the given life periods is provided in the supplement (Supplementary Figure 2). (2) The control group included participants that reported to never have played a musical instrument in any of the given life periods.

Measurement of Cognitive Abilities

Cognitive abilities were assessed using latent composite scores of five cognitive domains, namely (1) learning and memory, (2) working memory, (3) executive functions and mental processing speed, (4) language, and (5) visuospatial abilities and a global cognitive score (i.e., the mean across the five cognitive domains). The composite scores were created by using a confirmatory factor analysis on neurocognitive tests from the extensive neuropsychological test battery in the DELCODE cohort, as described (Wolfsgruber et al., 2020) and applied (e.g., Amaefule et al., 2021; Ballarini et al., 2021; Wesselman et al., 2021) in previous reports. Information on the methodological procedure are provided in the supplement. Notably, the construction of latent factors scores across multiple neuropsychological tests is an established procedure (Hedden et al., 2012; Benson et al., 2018) with several advantages (Gross et al., 2015): Each latent factor represents a construct measured by the shared variance across multiple indicator variables. Thus, the latent variables are adjusted for measurement error and specificities of the individual tests. The method allows for an objective evaluation of cognitive performance across multiple tests and results can be generalized above the specific measurement methods.

The following neuropsychological tests contributed to each cognitive domain: (1) Learning and memory: Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale-Cognitive Subscale (ADAS-cog) word list: trial 1, 2, 3, delayed recall and recognition, Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test (FCSRT) free recall and cue efficiency, Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS) logical memory 1 and 2, Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD) figure savings, Symbol-Digit-Modalities Test (SDMT) incidental learning, Face Name Test. (2) Working memory: Digit Span Forward and Backward, FCSRT interference task (Serial 3 s). (3) Executive functions: Trail Making Test (TMT) A and B, Number Cancelation, SDMT, Flanker Task. (4) Language: verbal fluency groceries and animals, Boston Naming Test (20 items), FCSRT naming. (5) Visuospatial abilities: Clock copying and drawing, CERAD Figure copying. Each cognitive composite score was z-transformed using the here-selected DELCODE baseline sample.

MRI Acquisition and Processing

The MRI data were acquired using 3-Tesla MRI scanners (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany), including three TIM Trio systems, four Verio systems, one Skyra system, and one Prisma system. The extensive MRI protocol of the DELCODE study is described elsewhere (Jessen et al., 2018). For the present analysis, we used T1-weighted images (i.e., 3D GRAPPA PAT 2, 1 mm3 isotropic, 256 × 256 px, 192 slices, sagittal, ∼ 5 min, TR 2500 ms, TE 4.33 ms, TI 110 ms, FA 7°) and T2-weighted images (i.e., 0.5 × 0.5 × 1.5 mm3, 384 × 384 px, 64 slices, orthogonal to hippocampal long axis, ∼12 min, TR 3500 ms, TE 353 ms, optimized for volumetric assessment of the medial temporal lobe). All scans underwent quality assessment provided by the DZNE imaging network (iNET, Magdeburg).

Regional GMV analysis was conducted in pre-selected ROI robustly affected by healthy and pathological aging due to AD. Based on prior findings (Jack et al., 1997; Habes et al., 2016; Wirth et al., 2018), we chose three ROIs comprising the frontal lobe, the temporal lobe and the hippocampus. Cortical GMV was evaluated as a global measure of brain integrity. For each of the ROIs, we used regional GMV measures provided in the DELCODE database, as described previously (Düzel et al., 2018). In brief, structural MRI images were segmented in native space using an automated cortical parcellation pipeline (Fischl et al., 2004) implemented in FreeSurfer1 (version 6.0) and an advanced segmentation tool (Iglesias et al., 2015) to derive ROI-based GMV measures. Frontal and temporal GMV were calculated as the sum over selected ROIs of the left and right hemisphere, as proposed by Desikan et al. (2006). Left and right hippocampal volume were summed as a measure of the overall hippocampal volume. Details on the ROI computation are provided in the supplement. Regional GMV measures were adjusted for total intracranial volume (TIV), as estimated using FreeSurfer (Buckner et al., 2004).

We further assessed GMV at the voxel level using the following procedure. Structural MRI images were segmented to extract gray matter (GM), white matter (WM), and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) tissues using the unified segmentation algorithm in CAT122 (version 12.6) with default parameters. Warping to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template space was performed using Diffeomorphic Anatomical Registration Through Exponentiated Lie Algebra (DARTEL) with default parameters and registration to existing templates (Ashburner, 2007). TIV was computed as the sum of volumes of GM, WM, and CSF using the SPM “Estimate TIV and global tissue volumes” routine. Voxel-based statistical analyses were performed on the warped and modulated GMV maps, which were smoothed by a three-dimensional Gaussian kernel with full width at half maximum of 8 mm3.

Additional Measures

Age, sex, education, diagnostic category and known reserve proxies of crystallized intelligence, SES, and self-reported participation in physical activity were considered as potential confounders. Educational attainment was measured in years of education. Crystallized intelligence was estimated using the Multiple-Choice Vocabulary Intelligence Test (MWT, min. score: 0, max. score: 37), with higher scores being proportionally related to a higher level of verbal intelligence (Lehrl, 2005). The MWT is considered an established tool for the assessment of crystallized intelligence. The SES was estimated for each participant using the international socio-economic index of occupational information score (ISEI, min. score: 16, max. score: 90) (Ganzeboom et al., 1992), based on the occupational history assessed by the LEQ. Details on the SES computation are provided in the supplement.

Participation in physical activity (long-term and current) was assessed using the respective items of the LEQ and the Physical Activity Scale for the Elderly (PASE, min. score: 0) (Washburn et al., 1999), respectively. Self-reported participation in long-term physical activity was measured based on LEQ responses on the frequency of physical activity recorded for each life period using the 6-point Likert scale (see above). A mean score was calculated over the available participant’s responses including at least two life periods. In addition, current physical activity was assessed through the Physical Activity Scale for the Elderly (PASE, min. score: 0) (Washburn et al., 1999). The PASE includes leisure, household and occupational activities measured over the previous week. Based on frequency, duration, and intensity of these activities, a total score is calculated with higher scores indicating greater levels of physical activity. Long-term physical activity was significantly correlated with current physical activity in the matched sample (n = 140, r = 0.35, p < 0.001), supporting the validity of the measure. Long-term physical activity was used as a covariate in statistical analyses, since the measure was available from all participants.

Statistical Analyses

Statistical analyses were conducted using R (version 3.5.1.) and Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM, version 12, Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London, United Kingdom). Figures were generated using the package ggplot2 (Wickham, 2016). Before conducting statistical models, statistical assumptions were assessed visually using diagnostic plots.

Sample Characteristics

Participants reporting musical activity during life and controls with no musical activity were matched using a one-to-one matching procedure taking into account age, sex, diagnostic category, education, SES, crystallized intelligence, and long-term physical activity. Further details are provided in the supplement including sample descriptive of the pre-matching sample (Supplementary Table 1). The one-to-one matching procedure was carried out using propensity score matching with the R package MatchIt (version 4.1.0.) (Ho et al., 2011). This statistical matching technique aims to estimate treatment or intervention effects by accounting for several covariates. Observations were matched based on the nearest-neighbor method, as a simple and effective procedure for selecting well-matched groups (Stuart, 2010). After matching, musical activity groups were compared in baseline demographic, behavioral, neuropsychological, and neuroimaging variables. Independent Student’s t-tests were used for all continuous variable and chi-squared (χ2) tests were applied for all categorical variables.

Regions-of-Interest-Based Analyses

Multiple linear regression models were used to assess our main hypotheses. In these statistical analyses, an alpha value of 0.05 was considered statistically significant. In addition, correction for multiple comparisons was performed using a false discovery rate (FDR)-adjusted p-value threshold (alpha) of 0.05 (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995). Uncorrected p-values were reported, when results survived FDR correction, this is highlighted in respective result tables.

Firstly, the association between the musical activity groups (modeled as a main effect) and cognitive abilities were assessed using the cognitive composite scores. Multiple linear regression models were computed including musical activity (binary group) as an independent variable and each cognitive composite score (z-transformed) as a dependent variable, respectively. Next, the association between musical activity and brain morphology was examined using similar multiple linear regressions. The models included musical activity (binary group) as independent variable and each ROI-based GMV (frontal, temporal and hippocampus, all TIV adjusted) as dependent variable along with scanner site as covariate (dummy coded). Selected associations were visualized to facilitate the interpretation of findings using box plots of unadjusted data.

Next, we assessed the moderation of musical activity on the association between ROI-based GMV and cognitive abilities. To do this, musical activity (binary group), ROI-based GMV (frontal, temporal, and hippocampal, all mean-centered), and the interaction (musical activity × ROI-based GMV) along with scanner site as covariate (dummy coded) were entered into multiple regression models with each cognitive composite score (z-transformed) as dependent variable. To specify the directionality of significant interactions, simple slope analyses were carried out (Aiken and West, 1991; Cohen et al., 2003). Moderation effects were visualized using unadjusted data as follows: cognitive composite scores (z-transformed) were graphed as a function of musical activity and ROI-based GMV, respectively.

Voxel-Based Analysis

To elucidate the spatial distribution of the associations between musical activity and brain morphology at the voxel level, multiple linear regressions were computed in SPM12. For the present purpose, voxel-wise results were presented at p < 0.001 uncorrected and, if applicable, p < 0.05 with family-wise error (FWE) correction at peak level, in combination with the estimated expected voxels per cluster (k), as automatically calculated by SPM.

First, a multiple regression model was computed with musical activity (binary group) as independent variable and the modulated, warped, and smoothed GMV maps as dependent variable. Next, a moderation of musical activity was evaluated at the voxel level. This multiple regression model included musical activity (binary group), the respective cognitive composite score (z-transformed), and the interaction term (musical activity × cognitive composite score) as independent variables with GMV maps as dependent variable. The later analysis was carried out for global cognition and all cognitive domains. For reasons of simplicity, results of this analysis were displayed for one cognitive domain (i.e., with the largest effect size), as selected by the strongest moderation effect in the ROI-based analysis. Additional findings were provided in the extended data documentation.

All voxel-based analyses were adjusted for TIV as well as scanner site (dummy coded) and restricted to cerebral GM using an explicit binary GM mask derived from the present sample (i.e., average GM map thresholded at a level of > 0.3, excluding cerebellum and brain stem). Cluster peaks are specified by anatomical site, as labeled using the Hammersmith atlas (Hammers et al., 2003) provided by the CAT12 toolbox. Additionally, Brodmann areas (BA) were identified for cluster peaks using the BioImage Suite Web 1.2.03 (GitHub, Retrieved December 2, 2021). Finally, mean values were extracted for each participant within combined clusters using the warped, modulated, and non-smoothed GMV images using the MarsBaR toolbox (release: 0.444) (Brett et al., 2002), to provide complementary visualizations of the associations.

Results

Sample Characteristics

This study included a total sample of 140 older participants (aged ≥ 60 years) selected from the DELCODE cohort. The present sample comprised 70 participants reporting participation in musical activity during life and 70 well-matched controls without musical activity. The two groups (musical activity, no musical activity) were comparable in age, sex, distribution of diagnostic categories as well as reserve proxies including higher educational attainment, crystallized intelligence, SES, and participation in both long-term and current physical activity (all p’s > 0.05, Table 1). Slight group differences in frontal and total GMV (unadjusted raw values) were observed, with larger volumes in the older participants with musical activity compared to controls. No significant differences were observed in the temporal and hippocampal GMV (unadjusted raw values).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive characteristics of the matched DELCODE sample (n = 140).

Musical Activity and Cognitive Abilities

First, we assessed the association between musical activity and cognitive abilities using the latent composite scores. We found significant group differences for global cognition, working memory, executive function, language and visuospatial abilities (all p’s < 0.05, Table 2 and Figure 1). Performance in these cognitive abilities was significantly better in participants with musical activity compared to controls. In contrast, the two groups did not differ significantly the domain of learning and memory (p = 0.209).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results of linear regression analyses between musical activity and cognition.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Main effect of musical activity on multi-domain cognitive abilities. Significant group differences were found for global cognition (A, GLOBAL), working memory (C, WM), executive function (D, EXEC), language (E, LAN), and visuospatial abilities (F, VIS). These multi-domain cognitive abilities were enhanced for participants with musical activity (MA+, blue) compared to controls (no musical activity across lifespan, MA-, gray). The association was not significant for the learning and memory composite (B, MEM). Boxplots display unadjusted data with individual data points. The “notch” shows the median with 95% confidence intervals and interquartile range with lower (25th) and upper percentiles (75th). Significance levels (uncorrected): ***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05. MA+, musical activity; MA–, no musical activity.

Musical Activity and Brain Morphology in Regions-of-Interest

Second, we assessed the association between musical activity and regional GMV in pre-selected frontal, temporal and hippocampal ROIs (TIV-adjusted values). There were no significant group differences between participants with musical activity compared to controls in frontal (p = 0.822), temporal (p = 0.711), and hippocampal (p = 0.551) GMVs. Furthermore, the two groups did not differ significantly in total cortical GMV (p = 0.722). Results are shown in Table 3. In a post hoc analysis, the association between musical activity and frontal, temporal and hippocampal ROIs was analyzed separately for left and right hemispheres. There were no significant differences in the ROI-based GMVs between participants with musical activity and controls (all p’s > 0.1, Supplementary Table 2).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Results of linear regression analyses between musical activity and GMV in regions-of-interest.

Moderations of Musical Activity in Regions-of-Interest

Third, a moderation of musical activity was assessed by the interaction between musical activity and GMV in the frontal, temporal and hippocampal ROIs, respectively. We found overall positive associations of frontal GMV with global and domain-specific cognitive abilities (all p’s ≤ 0.01, data not shown). Importantly, musical activity interacted with frontal GMV for global cognition, working memory, and language abilities (all p’s < 0.05; Table 4). Visualization of this moderation effect (Figure 2) showed that the association between frontal GMV and those cognitive abilities was enhanced in participants with musical activity compared to controls. That is, larger frontal GMV was significantly associated with better cognitive abilities in the participants with musical activity (all p’s < 0.05, Figure 2). The moderation effect was not detectable for the domain of learning and memory (p = 0.441).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Results of the moderation analyses between musical activity and frontal GMV.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Moderation of musical activity in the frontal region. A significant moderation of musical activity was observed for global cognition (A, GLOBAL), working memory (C, WM), and language abilities (E, LAN), such that larger frontal GMV was associated with better global in participants with musical activity (MA+, blue) compared to controls (MA–, gray). This interaction was not significant for learning and memory (B, MEM), executive functions (D, EXEC), and visuospatial abilities (F, VIS). Individual data points (dots and triangles), linear trends (solid lines), 95% confidence intervals (shaded areas), and standardized regression coefficients (β) within each group are provided. Gray vertical lines display the 90th percentile of the prefrontal GMV distribution in AD patients of the DELCODE study. Significance levels (uncorrected): ***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01. GMV, gray matter volume; MA+, musical activity; MA–, no musical activity; TIV, total intracranial volume.

For the temporal and hippocampal ROIs, we found overall positive associations of temporal GMV with global and domain-specific cognitive abilities (all p’s < 0.01, data not shown). For the temporal ROI, we detected a moderation of musical activity on the association of temporal GMV and global cognition, working memory and language abilities (all p’s < 0.05 uncorrected, Supplementary Table 3). There was no significant moderation of musical activity on the association between hippocampal GMV and cognitive abilities (all p’s > 0.1, Supplementary Table 4).

Voxel-Based Analysis

Our analysis at the voxel level largely confirmed the ROI-based findings. Regarding the main effect, there was a subtle positive association between musical activity and local GMV within a small cluster in the left postcentral gyrus (p < 0.001 uncorrected; Table 5 and Figures 3A,B). No other significant clusters were found. When investigating the moderation of musical activity at the voxel level, significant clusters of regional GMV were detected in prefrontal (lateral and medial), inferior temporal, and precentral regions (p < 0.001 uncorrected; Table 5). Visualization of this moderation effect for working memory (i.e., domain with the largest effect size) indicated that larger GMV was associated with better working memory ability only in participants with musical activity (Figures 3C,D). Overall, the results of the voxel-wise moderation analysis across all cognitive composites were essentially similar, with some variations in the number and location of significant clusters (Supplementary Table 5 and Supplementary Figure 3).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Results of analyses between musical activity and GMV at the voxel level.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Association between musical activity and regional volume distribution. (A,B) Results of the main effect analysis. Statistical map (A) shows significant clusters (p < 0.001 uncorrected, color-coded in magenta) with larger GMV in participants with musical activity compared to controls. The corresponding graph (B) displays the association using mean GMV values extracted from the corresponding cluster in the postcentral gyrus. The box plot displays the median with 95% confidence intervals, interquartile range with lower (25th) and upper percentiles (75th), and individual data points. (C,D) Results of the moderation analysis. The statistical map (C) displays clusters (p < 0.001 uncorrected, color-coded in magenta) with a significant moderation effect of musical activity. The corresponding scatter plot (D) shows the association using mean values extracted from the GMV maps in the combined cluster. Larger GMV in the combined cluster was associated with better working memory ability selectively in participants with musical activity (MA+, blue) compared to controls (MA–, gray). Individual data points, linear trends (solid lines), 95% confidence intervals (shaded areas), and standardized regression coefficients (β) within each musical activity group are provided. The statistical maps are depicted on a glass brain. Significance levels (uncorrected): ***p < 0.001. MA+, musical activity; MA–, no musical activity; GMV, gray matter volume; TIV, total intracranial volume.

Discussion

Summary of Findings

The current study examined cognitive abilities, brain morphology, and their interplay in OA that reported having played a musical instrument across three life periods. Participants with a history of long-term musical activity were compared to controls (i.e., without a history of musical instrument playing) that were closely matched for known reserve proxies. We document three main findings: (1) Participants with musical activity outperformed controls in global and multi-domain cognitive abilities, but not in learning and memory. (2) Participants with musical activity did not significantly differ from controls in GMV within the higher-order frontal, temporal, and hippocampal regions. (3) The association between GMV in distributed frontal-to-temporal regions and multi-domain cognitive abilities was enhanced in participants with musical activity compared to controls. Together, our correlational findings suggest that participation in musical activity during life is associated with brain and cognitive benefits in late life and could strengthen cognitive resilience. Longitudinal studies are needed to support this interpretation.

Musical Activity and Cognitive Abilities

We show that musical activity during life is associated with better late-life cognitive abilities in OA. More precisely, participants with musical activity outperformed the matched controls in global cognition and multiple cognitive domains including working memory, executive functions, language and visuospatial abilities. These findings agree with a body of studies, suggesting that active participation in musical activity is associated with higher-order cognitive abilities in OA, based on correlational (Hanna-Pladdy and MacKay, 2011; Hanna-Pladdy and Gajewski, 2012; Mansens et al., 2018; Groussard et al., 2020) and intervention/training (Bugos et al., 2007; Seinfeld et al., 2013; Worschech et al., 2021) studies. A recent meta-analysis of active musical training further demonstrates a small but measurable benefit of this leisure-time activity on cognitive functioning in OA with mild cognitive impairment and dementia (Dorris et al., 2021).

In our study, the association between musical activity and late-life cognitive abilities was greatest for working memory (β = 0.304), a fundamental cognitive domain that is central to overall cognitive functioning (Baddeley, 2003). The result mirrors existing findings in younger (Talamini et al., 2017) and in older (Hanna-Pladdy and Gajewski, 2012; Grassi et al., 2017; Mansens et al., 2018) musically active adults and corroborates a particular involvement of frontal-lobe functions in playing music (Jäncke, 2013; Sutcliffe et al., 2020). In contrast, participants with musical activity did not differ from controls in the domain of learning and memory, although this must be perceived as an essential cognitive skill involved in playing a musical instrument (Talamini et al., 2017). Some correlational studies have reported that musical activity is indeed associated with better episodic memory in OA (Hanna-Pladdy and MacKay, 2011; Gooding et al., 2014; Mansens et al., 2018; Romeiser et al., 2021), while others do not find such effects (Hanna-Pladdy and Gajewski, 2012; Fauvel et al., 2014b; Strong and Mast, 2019). Taken together, our findings in OA substantiate that musical activity may predominantly favor cognitive abilities involving the frontal lobe.

One might argue that the present null result in learning and memory could be explained by a lack in sensitivity of our composite measure. However, this latent factor score was previously shown to capture even subtle inter-individual differences in memory performance of OA (Wolfsgruber et al., 2020). Alternatively, it is plausible that specific memory sub-processes are enhanced by musical/auditory expertise involving the tonal stimulus modality (Talamini et al., 2017), such as long-term musical memory (Groussard et al., 2010) or auditory navigation (Teki et al., 2012), which are not mapped by our memory composite score. Experimental, neuroimaging, and neuropsychological markers tapping into more specific hippocampal processes (e.g., Stark et al., 2019) will be needed to gain further insight into presumed memory benefits associated with musical activity in older age.

Moderation of Musical Activity

As a novel finding, the present study documents a moderation of musical activity in OA. More specifically, we found that larger GMV was significantly associated with better multi-domain cognitive abilities in participants with musical activity compared to controls. This specific moderation was observed for global cognition, working memory, as well as language abilities mainly in the pre-selected frontal ROI and extended to a network of frontal, temporal and motor-sensory regions at the voxel level. A similar observation has been reported by Oechslin et al. (2013), where larger hippocampal volume was associated with better general cognitive abilities in younger musicians (professionals and amateurs), but not in non-musicians. The current study highlights that a similar association is detectable in OA with musical experience, where it is linked to distributed brain areas supporting sensory, motor and cognitive functions.

In general, the present result may reflect a more efficient use of an overall younger brain age in musically active people, as shown previously (Rogenmoser et al., 2018). Interestingly, the observed frontal-to-temporal regions partially overlap with brain networks that show enhanced functional and/or structural connectivity in young to middle-aged musicians compared to non-musicians (Halwani et al., 2011; Andrews et al., 2021; Leipold et al., 2021) and support cognitive reserve processes in older age (Colangeli et al., 2016; Marques et al., 2016; Franzmeier et al., 2017; Benson et al., 2018). In this light, our findings may imply that the long-term playing of a musical instrument could be associated with a more efficient recruitment of dedicated brain networks, as a potential benefit that might help to preserve cognitive health in late life.

Musical Activity and Brain Morphology

In our study, musical activity was not significantly associated with greater brain reserve in higher-order brain regions. More precisely, participants with musical activity did not differ from controls in GMV of the pre-selected frontal, temporal or hippocampal regions. Our voxel-based analysis largely confirmed the ROI-based observations. Merely a smaller cluster with larger GMV was found in the somatosensory area of participants with musical activity compared to controls, presumably reflecting brain plasticity in response to the intense tactile stimulations induced by playing a musical instrument (Gaser and Schlaug, 2003; Gärtner et al., 2013). Earlier studies have shown a positive association between musical activity and GMV within higher-order fontal, temporal, and also parietal regions, albeit mainly in young musician compared to controls (Gaser and Schlaug, 2003; Groussard et al., 2010; Gärtner et al., 2013) with limited evidence in OA (Chaddock-Heyman et al., 2021).

Importantly, we accounted for several reserve proxies that may help to preserve brain morphology in late life (Arenaza-Urquijo et al., 2013; Wirth et al., 2014) and thereby act as potential confounders. Given this effort, it seems reasonable to assume that there is a limited added benefit of musical activity on structural brain resources (as measured using GMV) in OA within regions that are susceptible to aging and AD. Alternatively though, subtle morphological associations of musical activity could be unnoticed in the older population, due to increased morphological variability by brain aging and/or brain pathology as found in a considerable proportion of cognitively unimpaired OA (Hedden and Gabrieli, 2004; Knopman et al., 2012; Wirth et al., 2013). Lastly, it is important to note that previous cross-sectional studies reporting a positive association between musical activity and GMV in hippocampal regions as well as WM microstructural integrity included professional experts with intense musical/auditory experience (Groussard et al., 2010; Teki et al., 2012; Andrews et al., 2021). Therefore, the lack of current results with regard to a presumed modification of GMV through long-term musical activity may also have to do with the different proficiency level of our cohort.

Synopsis and Concluding Remarks

Taken together, the present study adds supportive evidence to the picture that participation in musical activity may constitute a protective factor in OA. Nevertheless, the observed health benefits associated with playing a musical instrument could be encouraged by a general engagement in an advantageous lifestyle. Those participants reporting musical activity were characterized by a high-reserve profile, including higher education, SES, crystallized intelligence, and more frequent participation in physical activity. A similar pattern was observed in previous studies (Mansens et al., 2018), but not in all (Hanna-Pladdy and Gajewski, 2012; Gray and Gow, 2020). Together these variables may resemble a lifestyle that comprises various beneficial body and mind activities that could in synergy be associated with cognitive or brain reserve in late life (Kempermann, 2019b,2022). Notably though, we observed superior cognitive abilities in the musically active group with those reserve proxies accounted for by our one-to-one matching procedure. This may suggest an added benefit of musical activity on late-life brain and cognitive functions. In addition, more emphasis could be placed on the assessment of a more holistic lifestyle (i.e., going beyond individual lifestyle activities) to highlight associations and presumed synergies with brain aging and reserve in future studies.

One might argue that high-functioning individuals are more likely to engage in and continue to play a musical instrument during life. In line with this argumentation, one might further expect that these high performers exhibit higher education, intelligence, and SES compared to controls. Indeed, there is confirmatory evidence to suggest that greater early-adulthood general cognitive abilities predict educational and occupational success in later adulthood (Daly et al., 2015; Kremen et al., 2019). In the present study, however, the high-functioning group was comparable to controls in the above-mentioned measures, which were deliberately accounted for when selecting the well-matched control group. Given this notion, one may reason that reverse causation seems less likely to apply to our findings, with caution that needs to be considered in correlational findings (Schellenberg, 2020).

Overall, our results converge with the view that musical activity may serve as a low-threshold multimodal enrichment strategy throughout life until old age. However, targeted intervention studies are needed to validate the impact of musical activity on late-life cognitive abilities and underlying brain correlates in OA (James et al., 2020). In light of our findings, it may be proposed that musical activity and the associated mulitmodal stimulations could strengthen cognitive resilience through benefits involving neural capacities and connections in dedicated motor-sensory-cognitive brain networks, as suggested by previous studies (Halwani et al., 2011; Andrews et al., 2021; Leipold et al., 2021). Prospective longitudinal and interventional studies must clarify whether or not musically active older people are indeed more protected against cognitive decline, which could inform targeted public health strategies.

Strengths and Limitations

Our study has several strengths. We assembled data from the observational DELCODE cohort to assess a well-characterized sample of cognitively unimpaired OA with and without self-reported participation in musical activity using cognitive, behavioral, and brain volume measures. All measures were acquired in the same participants using standardized operation procedures and high-quality data assessments. The detailed phenotyping, as provided by cohort-based studies, generated new evidence on potential brain and cognitive health benefits associated with musical activity in the older population. Moreover, we examined late-life cognitive abilities using latent factor scores, which can be generalized above the measurement method. Lastly, the availability of a wide range of variables made it possible to account for several reserve proxies, known to be enhanced in musically active older people (e.g., Mansens et al., 2018).

Several limitations need to be considered. (1) Our cohort-based approach included a limited description of musical activity/experience in the present older participants. More detailed information on lifetime musical activity including, e.g., the type of musical instrument, age of acquisition, training intensity and other musical abilities would be desirable (Okely et al., 2021), given that these features may impact brain plasticity and cognitive abilities (Bangert and Schlaug, 2006; Hanna-Pladdy and MacKay, 2011). (2) Due to the cross-sectional design, caution is needed in drawing conclusions on the directionality of the here-observed associations. Although we accounted for a number of confounding variables, it may be possible that other unmeasured variables facilitate playing a musical instrument across the life course, such as genetic predispositions, personality traits or early-life environmental exposures (Corrigall et al., 2013; Zatorre, 2013; Altenmüller and Furuya, 2017). These factors could play a role in the observed relationships, warranting further investigation. (3) The assessment of musical activity was based on self-reports that were partially retrospective. The information was extracted from the LEQ, as a validated questionnaire that has been applied in the assessment of cognitive reserve/resilience (Chan et al., 2018; Collins et al., 2021). Self-reports can be biased by the current cognitive status of a person. However, our participants were cognitively unimpaired and screened for current and passed mental health conditions. Based on our study, it can be recommended that cohort studies include a more detailed and objective evaluation of lifetime musical activity (e.g., Okely et al., 2021) to strengthen validity and accuracy of the measure. (4) Finally, it is important to note that other aspects of musical activity, such as listening to music or choir singing, have beneficial effects in healthy and cognitively impaired OA, e.g., in the rehabilitation or intervention of neurological and neurodegenerative conditions (Särkämö, 2018; Gold et al., 2019).

Conclusion

To conclude, the present findings are promising to suggest that long-term participation in musical activity, as an accessible leisure-time activity, could be associated with greater brain and cognitive health in late life. Well-designed studies in OA are needed to further assess detailed information about the nature of playing a musical instrument and underlying functional and structural brain correlates associated with this complex multimodal activity.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support findings of the present study are available on reasonable request from the DELCODE database. Requests to access these datasets should be directed to the German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Bonn.

Ethics Statement

The general study protocol for the DELCODE study was approved by the ethical committees of the medical faculties of all sites, i.e., the ethical committees of Berlin (Charité – Universitätsmedizin), Bonn (Medical Faculty, University of Bonn), Cologne (Medical Faculty, University of Cologne), Göttingen (Universitätsmedizin Göttingen), Magdeburg (Medical Faculty, Otto-von-Guericke University, Magdeburg), Munich (Medical Faculty, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität), Rostock (Medical Faculty, University of Rostock), and Tübingen (Medical Faculty, University of Tübingen). The process was led and coordinated by the ethical committee of the medical faculty of the University of Bonn under the registration number: 171/13. The patients/participants provided written informed consent to participate in the DELCODE study.

Author Contributions

AB, TK, AH, SR, MWa, GK, and MWi: conceptualization and design of the current study. OP, SF, JP, SA, ASc, IF, ASp, NR, SW, LK, JW, CB, FM, CM, LD, RY, KB, DJ, RP, B-SR, ST, IK, CL, MM, JH, PD, ME, KS, ED, FJ, SR, and MWa: overall design and implementation of the DELCODE study. AB, AZ, MG, and MW: methodology and statistical analysis. AB, AZ, TK, AH, SW, SR, MWa, GK, and MWi: interpretation of data. AB, AZ, TK, MG, AH, OP, SF, JP, SA, ASc, KF, IF, ASp, NR, SW, LK, JW, CB, FM, CM, LD, RY, KB, DJ, RP, B-SR, ST, IK, CL, MM, JH, PD, ME, KS, ED, FJ, SR, MWa, GK, and MWi: drafting and/or revision of manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The DELCODE study was funded by the German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (Deutsches Zentrum für Neurodegenerative Erkrankungen [DZNE]), reference number: BN012.

Conflict of Interest

OP received fees for consultation from Abbvie, Biogen, Eisai, Griffols, MSD Roche, and Schwabe. JP received fees for consultation, lectures, and patents from Neurimmune, Axon, Desitin, and Epomedics. JW was an advisory board member of Abbott, Biogen, Boehringer Ingelheim, Immunogenetics, Lilly, MSD Sharp & Dohme, and Roche Pharma and received honoraria for lectures from Actelion, Amgen, Beeijing Yibai Science and Technology Ltd., Janssen Cilag, Med Update GmbH, Pfizer, Roche Pharma and holds the following patents: PCT/EP 2011 001724 and PCT/EP 2015 052945. JW was supported by an Ilidio Pinho professorship, iBiMED (UIDB/04501/2020) at the University of Aveiro, Portugal. ED received fees for consultation from Roche, Biogen, RoxHealth and holds shares in neotiv. FJ received fees for consultation from Eli Lilly, Novartis, Roche, BioGene, MSD, Piramal, Janssen, and Lundbeck.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful for the tremendous efforts of all DELCODE study teams across participating DZNE sites. Further, we express our sincere gratitude to all volunteers and their family members that participate in the DELCODE study. We gratefully acknowledge all administrative and scientists staff members involved in data acquisition, data management, as well as quality control. We sincerely thank Klaus Fabel for the helpful comments on data analysis, interpretation and manuscript writing. We also thank Andrea Dell’Orco for the support in data visualization. DELCODE study group: H. Amthauer, A. Cetindag, N. Cosma, D. Diesing, M. Ehrlich, F. Fenski, S. Freiesleben, M. Fuentes, D. Hauser, N. Hujer, E. Incesoy, C. Kainz, C. Lange, K. Lindner, H. Megges, O. Peters, L. Preis, S. Altenstein, A. Lohse, C. Franke, J. Priller, E. Spruth, I. Villar Munoz, M. Barkhoff, H. Boecker, F. Brosseron, M. Daamen, T. Engels, J. Faber, K. Fließbach, I. Frommann, M. Grobe-Einsler, G. Hennes, G. Herrmann, L. Jost, P. Kalbhen, O. Kimmich, X. Kobeleva, B. Kofler, C. McCormick, L. Miebach, C. Miklitz, A. Müller, D. Oender, A. Polcher, V. Purrer, S. Röske, C. Schneider, A. Schneider, A. Spottke, I. Vogt, M. Wagner, S. Wolfsgruber, S. Yilmaz, C. Bartels, P. Dechent, N. Hansen, L. Hassoun, S. Hirschel, S. Nuhn, I. Pfahlert, L. Rausch, B. Schott, C. Timäus, C. Werner, J. Wiltfang, L. Zabel, H. Zech, A. Bader, J. C. Baldermann, B. Dölle, A. Drzezga, C. Escher, N. Ghiasi, K. Hardenacke, F. Jessen, H. Lützerath, F. Maier, B. Marquardt, A. Martikke, D. Meiberth, S. Petzler, A. Rostamzadeh, L. Sannemann, A. Schild, S. Sorgalla, S. Stockter, M. Thelen, M. Tscheuschler, F. Uhle, P. Zeyen, D. Bittner, A. Cardenas-Blanco, L. Dobisch, E. Düzel, D. Grieger-Klose, D. Hartmann, C. Metzger, P. Nestor, C. Ruß, F. Schulze, O. Speck, G. Wenzel, R. Yakupov, G. Ziegler, C. Brauneis, K. Bürger, C. Catak, L. Coloma Andrews, M. Dichgans, A. Dörr, B. Ertl-Wagner, D. Frimmer, B. Huber, D. Janowitz, M. Kreuzer, E. Markov, C. Müller, A. Rominger, J. Schmid, A. Seegerer, J. Stephan, A. Zollver, L. Burow, S. de Jonge, P. Falkai, N. Garcia Angarita, T. Görlitz, S. Gürsel, I. Horvath, C. Kurz, E. Meisenzahl-Lechner, R. Perneczky, J. Utecht, M. Dyrba, H. Janecek-Meyer, I. Kilimann, C. Lappe, E. Lau, H. Pfaff, H. Raum, P. Sabik, M. Schmidt, H. Schulz, S. Schwarzenboeck, S. Teipel, M. Weber, M. Buchmann, T. Heger, P. Hinderer, E. Kuder-Buletta, C. Laske, M. Munk, C. Mychajliw, S. Soekadar, P. Sulzer, and T. Trunk.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.945709/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/
  2. ^ http://dbm.neuro.uni-jena.de/vbm
  3. ^ https://github.com/bioimagesuiteweb/bisweb
  4. ^ http://marsbar.sourceforge.net/

References

Aiken, L. S., and West, S. G. (1991). Multiple Regression: Testing And Interpreting Interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.

Google Scholar

Altenmüller, E., and Furuya, S. (2017). Apollos gift and curse: making music as a model for adaptive and maladaptive plasticity. e-Neuroforum 23, 57–75. doi: 10.1515/nf-2016-A054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alzheimer’s Association (2021). 2021 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimers Dement. 17, 327–406. doi: 10.1002/alz.12328

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amaefule, C. O., Dyrba, M., Wolfsgruber, S., Polcher, A., Schneider, A., Fliessbach, K., et al. (2021). Association between composite scores of domain-specific cognitive functions and regional patterns of atrophy and functional connectivity in the Alzheimer’s disease spectrum. Neuroimage Clin. 29:102533. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102533

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andrews, E., Eierud, C., Banks, D., Harshbarger, T., Michael, A., and Rammell, C. (2021). Effects of lifelong musicianship on white matter integrity and cognitive brain reserve. Brain Sci. 11:67. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11010067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arenaza-Urquijo, E. M., Landeau, B., La Joie, R., Mevel, K., Mezenge, F., Perrotin, A., et al. (2013). Relationships between years of education and gray matter volume, metabolism and functional connectivity in healthy elders. Neuroimage 83C, 450–457. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arenaza-Urquijo, E. M., Wirth, M., and Chetelat, G. (2015). Cognitive reserve and lifestyle: moving towards preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7:134. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00134

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashburner, J. (2007). A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage 38, 95–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 829–839. doi: 10.1038/nrn1201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balbag, M. A., Pedersen, N. L., and Gatz, M. (2014). Playing a musical instrument as a protective factor against dementia and cognitive impairment: a population-based twin study. Int. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2014:836748. doi: 10.1155/2014/836748

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballarini, T., Melo Van Lent, D., Brunner, J., Schröder, A., Wolfsgruber, S., Altenstein, S., et al. (2021). Mediterranean diet, alzheimer disease biomarkers, and brain atrophy in old age. Neurology 96, e2920–e2932. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000012067

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bangert, M., and Schlaug, G. (2006). Specialization of the specialized in features of external human brain morphology. Eur. J. Neurosci. 24, 1832–1834. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05031.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benjamini, Y., and Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B (Methodological) 57, 289–300. doi: 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benson, G., Hildebrandt, A., Lange, C., Schwarz, C., Kobe, T., Sommer, W., et al. (2018). Functional connectivity in cognitive control networks mitigates the impact of white matter lesions in the elderly. Alzheimers Res. Ther. 10:109. doi: 10.1186/s13195-018-0434-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brett, M., Anton, J. L., Valabrgue, R., and Poline, J.-B. (2002). Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox. Presented at the 8th international conference on functional mapping of the human brain, june 2-6, 2002, sendai, japan. Neuroimage 13, 210–217.

Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L., Head, D., Parker, J., Fotenos, A. F., Marcus, D., Morris, J. C., et al. (2004). A unified approach for morphometric and functional data analysis in young, old, and demented adults using automated atlas-based head size normalization: reliability and validation against manual measurement of total intracranial volume. Neuroimage 23, 724–738. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bugos, J. A., Perlstein, W. M., Mccrae, C. S., Brophy, T. S., and Bedenbaugh, P. H. (2007). Individualized piano instruction enhances executive functioning and working memory in older adults. Aging Ment. Health 11, 464–471. doi: 10.1080/13607860601086504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaddock-Heyman, L., Loui, P., Weng, T. B., Weisshappel, R., Mcauley, E., and Kramer, A. F. (2021). Musical training and brain volume in older adults. Brain Sci. 11:50. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11010050

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, D., Shafto, M., Kievit, R., Matthews, F., Spink, M., Valenzuela, M., et al. (2018). Lifestyle activities in mid-life contribute to cognitive reserve in late-life, independent of education, occupation, and late-life activities. Neurobiol. Aging 70, 180–183. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., and Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis For The Behavioral Sciences. Mahwah, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates.

Google Scholar

Colangeli, S., Boccia, M., Verde, P., Guariglia, P., Bianchini, F., and Piccardi, L. (2016). Cognitive reserve in healthy aging and alzheimer’s disease: a meta-analysis of FMRI studies. Am. J. Alzheimers Dis. Other Dement. 31, 443–449. doi: 10.1177/1533317516653826

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, J. M., Hill, E., Bindoff, A., King, A. E., Alty, J., Summers, M. J., et al. (2021). Association between components of cognitive reserve and serum bdnf in healthy older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 13:725914. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2021.725914

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corrigall, K., Schellenberg, E. G., and Misura, N. (2013). Music training, cognition, and personality. Front. Psychol. 4:222. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00222

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daly, M., Egan, M., and O’reilly, F. (2015). Childhood general cognitive ability predicts leadership role occupancy across life: evidence from 17,000 cohort study participants. Leadersh. Q. 26, 323–341. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2015.03.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Degé, F., and Kerkovius, K. (2018). The effects of drumming on working memory in older adults. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1423, 242–250. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13685

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desikan, R. S., Segonne, F., Fischl, B., Quinn, B. T., Dickerson, B. C., Blacker, D., et al. (2006). An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage 31, 968–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dorris, J. L., Neely, S., Terhorst, L., Vonville, H. M., and Rodakowski, J. (2021). Effects of music participation for mild cognitive impairment and dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 69, 2659–2667. doi: 10.1111/jgs.17208

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Düzel, E., Berron, D., Schütze, H., Cardenas-Blanco, A., Metzger, C., Betts, M., et al. (2018). CSF total tau levels are associated with hippocampal novelty irrespective of hippocampal volume. Alzheimers Dement. (Amst) 10, 782–790. doi: 10.1016/j.dadm.2018.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fauvel, B., Groussard, M., Chételat, G., Fouquet, M., Landeau, B., Eustache, F., et al. (2014a). Morphological brain plasticity induced by musical expertise is accompanied by modulation of functional connectivity at rest. Neuroimage 90, 179–188. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fauvel, B., Groussard, M., Mutlu, J., Arenaza-Urquijo, E. M., Eustache, F., Desgranges, B., et al. (2014b). Musical practice and cognitive aging: two cross-sectional studies point to phonemic fluency as a potential candidate for a use-dependent adaptation. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:227. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00227

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B., Van Der Kouwe, A., Destrieux, C., Halgren, E., Ségonne, F., Salat, D. H., et al. (2004). Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex. Cereb. Cortex 14, 11–22. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg087

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franzmeier, N., Duering, M., Weiner, M., Dichgans, M., and Ewers, M. (2017). Left frontal cortex connectivity underlies cognitive reserve in prodromal Alzheimer disease. Neurology 88, 1054–1061. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000003711

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ganzeboom, H. B. G., De Graaf, P. M., and Treiman, D. J. (1992). A standard international socio-economic index of occupational status. Soc. Sci. Res. 21, 1–56. doi: 10.1016/0049-089X(92)90017-B

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gärtner, H., Minnerop, M., Pieperhoff, P., Schleicher, A., Zilles, K., Altenmüller, E., et al. (2013). Brain morphometry shows effects of long-term musical practice in middle-aged keyboard players. Front. Psychol. 4:636. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00636

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaser, C., and Schlaug, G. (2003). Brain structures differ between musicians and non-musicians. J. Neurosci. 23, 9240–9245. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-27-09240.2003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gold, C., Eickholt, J., Assmus, J., Stige, B., Wake, J. D., Baker, F. A., et al. (2019). Music interventions for dementia and depression in elderly care (MIDDEL): protocol and statistical analysis plan for a multinational cluster-randomised trial. BMJ Open 9:e023436. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-023436

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gooding, L. F., Abner, E. L., Jicha, G. A., Kryscio, R. J., and Schmitt, F. A. (2014). Musical training and late-life cognition. Am. J. Alzheimers Dis. Other Dement. 29, 333–343. doi: 10.1177/1533317513517048

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grassi, M., Meneghetti, C., Toffalini, E., and Borella, E. (2017). Auditory and cognitive performance in elderly musicians and nonmusicians. PLoS One 12:e0187881. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0187881

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, R., and Gow, A. J. (2020). How is musical activity associated with cognitive ability in later life? Neuropsychol. Dev. Cogn. B Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 27, 617–635. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2019.1660300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, A. L., Power, M. C., Albert, M. S., Deal, J. A., Gottesman, R. F., Griswold, M., et al. (2015). Application of latent variable methods to the study of cognitive decline when tests change over time. Epidemiology (Cambridge, Mass.) 26, 878–887. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0000000000000379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Groussard, M., Coppalle, R., Hinault, T., and Platel, H. (2020). Do musicians have better mnemonic and executive performance than actors? Influence of regular musical or theater practice in adults and in the elderly. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:557642. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.557642

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Groussard, M., La Joie, R., Rauchs, G., Landeau, B., Chételat, G., Viader, F., et al. (2010). When music and long-term memory interact: effects of musical expertise on functional and structural plasticity in the hippocampus. PLoS One 5:e13225. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013225

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Groussard, M., Viader, F., Landeau, B., Desgranges, B., Eustache, F., and Platel, H. (2014). The effects of musical practice on structural plasticity: the dynamics of grey matter changes. Brain Cogn. 90, 174–180. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2014.06.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guo, X., Yamashita, M., Suzuki, M., Ohsawa, C., Asano, K., Abe, N., et al. (2021). Musical instrument training program improves verbal memory and neural efficiency in novice older adults. Hum. Brain Mapp. 42, 1359–1375. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Habes, M., Erus, G., Toledo, J. B., Zhang, T., Bryan, N., Launer, L. J., et al. (2016). White matter hyperintensities and imaging patterns of brain ageing in the general population. Brain 139, 1164–1179. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halwani, G. F., Loui, P., Rüber, T., and Schlaug, G. (2011). Effects of practice and experience on the arcuate fasciculus: comparing singers, instrumentalists, and non-musicians. Front. Psychol. 2:156. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hammers, A., Allom, R., Koepp, M. J., Free, S. L., Myers, R., Lemieux, L., et al. (2003). Three-dimensional maximum probability atlas of the human brain, with particular reference to the temporal lobe. Hum. Brain Mapp. 19, 224–247. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanna-Pladdy, B., and Gajewski, B. (2012). Recent and past musical activity predicts cognitive aging variability: direct comparison with general lifestyle activities. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:198. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanna-Pladdy, B., and MacKay, A. (2011). The relation between instrumental musical activity and cognitive aging. Neuropsychology 25, 378–386. doi: 10.1037/a0021895

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hedden, T., and Gabrieli, J. D. (2004). Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 87–96. doi: 10.1038/nrn1323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hedden, T., Mormino, E. C., Amariglio, R. E., Younger, A. P., Schultz, A. P., Becker, J. A., et al. (2012). Cognitive profile of amyloid burden and white matter hyperintensities in cognitively normal older adults. J. Neurosci. 32, 16233–16242. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2462-12.2012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herdener, M., Esposito, F., Di Salle, F., Boller, C., Hilti, C. C., Habermeyer, B., et al. (2010). Musical training induces functional plasticity in human hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 30, 1377–1384. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4513-09.2010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herholz, S. C., and Zatorre, R. J. (2012). Musical training as a framework for brain plasticity: behavior, function, and structure. Neuron 76, 486–502. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, D., Imai, K., King, G., and Stuart, E. A. (2011). MatchIt: nonparametric preprocessing for parametric causal inference. J. Stat. Softw. 42, 1–28.

Google Scholar

Iglesias, J. E., Augustinack, J. C., Nguyen, K., Player, C. M., Player, A., Wright, M., et al. (2015). A computational atlas of the hippocampal formation using ex vivo, ultra-high resolution MRI: application to adaptive segmentation of in vivo MRI. Neuroimage 115, 117–137. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.04.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jack, C. R. Jr., Petersen, R. C., Xu, Y. C., Waring, S. C., O’brien, P. C., Tangalos, E. G., et al. (1997). Medial temporal atrophy on MRI in normal aging and very mild Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 49, 786–794. doi: 10.1212/WNL.49.3.786

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

James, C. E., Altenmüller, E., Kliegel, M., Krüger, T. H. C., Van De Ville, D., Worschech, F., et al. (2020). Train the brain with music (TBM): brain plasticity and cognitive benefits induced by musical training in elderly people in Germany and Switzerland, a study protocol for an RCT comparing musical instrumental practice to sensitization to music. BMC Geriatr. 20:418. doi: 10.1186/s12877-020-01761-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

James, C. E., Oechslin, M. S., Van De Ville, D., Hauert, C. A., Descloux, C., and Lazeyras, F. (2014). Musical training intensity yields opposite effects on grey matter density in cognitive versus sensorimotor networks. Brain Struct. Funct. 219, 353–366. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0504-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jäncke, L. (2013). Music making and the aging brain. Z. Neuropsychol. 24, 113–121. doi: 10.1024/1016-264X/a000095

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jessen, F., Spottke, A., Boecker, H., Brosseron, F., Buerger, K., Catak, C., et al. (2018). Design and first baseline data of the DZNE multicenter observational study on predementia Alzheimer’s disease (DELCODE). Alzheimers Res. Ther. 10:15. doi: 10.1186/s13195-017-0314-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jünemann, K., Marie, D., Worschech, F., Scholz, D. S., Grouiller, F., Kliegel, M., et al. (2022). Six months of piano training in healthy elderly stabilizes white matter microstructure in the fornix, compared to an active control group. Front. Aging. Neurosci. 14:817889. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.817889

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kempermann, G. (2019a). Environmental enrichment, new neurons and the neurobiology of individuality. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 20, 235–245. doi: 10.1038/s41583-019-0120-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kempermann, G. (2019b). Making DEEP sense of lifestyle risk and resilience. Front. Aging Neurosci. 11:171. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00171

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kempermann, G. (2022). Embodied prevention. Front. Psychol. 13:841393. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.841393

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knopman, D. S., Jack, C. R. Jr., Wiste, H. J., Weigand, S. D., Vemuri, P., Lowe, V. J., et al. (2012). Brain injury biomarkers are not dependent on beta-amyloid in normal elderly. Ann. Neurol. 73, 472–480. doi: 10.1002/ana.23816

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kremen, W. S., Beck, A., Elman, J. A., Gustavson, D. E., Reynolds, C. A., Tu, X. M., et al. (2019). Influence of young adult cognitive ability and additional education on later-life cognition. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.A.S. 116, 2021–2026. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1811537116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lappe, C., Herholz, S. C., Trainor, L. J., and Pantev, C. (2008). Cortical plasticity induced by short-term unimodal and multimodal musical training. J. Neurosci. 28, 9632–9639. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2254-08.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehrl, S. (2005). Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenztest : MWT-B. Balingen: Spitta.

Google Scholar

Leipold, S., Klein, C., and Jäncke, L. (2021). Musical expertise shapes functional and structural brain networks independent of absolute pitch ability. J. Neurosci. 41, 2496–2511. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1985-20.2020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Livingston, G., Huntley, J., Sommerlad, A., Ames, D., Ballard, C., Banerjee, S., et al. (2020). Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet (London, England) 396, 413–446. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30367-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mansens, D., Deeg, D. J. H., and Comijs, H. C. (2018). The association between singing and/or playing a musical instrument and cognitive functions in older adults. Aging Ment. Health 22, 964–971. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2017.1328481

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marques, P., Moreira, P., Magalhães, R., Costa, P., Santos, N., Zihl, J., et al. (2016). The functional connectome of cognitive reserve. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 3310–3322. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23242

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, J. C., Heyman, A., Mohs, R. C., Hughes, J. P., Van Belle, G., Fillenbaum, G., et al. (1989). The consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer’s disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 39, 1159–1165. doi: 10.1212/WNL.39.9.1159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oechslin, M. S., Descloux, C., Croquelois, A., Chanal, J., Van De Ville, D., Lazeyras, F., et al. (2013). Hippocampal volume predicts fluid intelligence in musically trained people. Hippocampus 23, 552–558. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okely, J. A., Deary, I. J., and Overy, K. (2021). The Edinburgh lifetime musical experience questionnaire (ELMEQ): responses and non-musical correlates in the lothian birth cohort 1936. PLoS One 16:e0254176. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0254176

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parbery-Clark, A., Strait, D. L., Anderson, S., Hittner, E., and Kraus, N. (2011). Musical experience and the aging auditory system: implications for cognitive abilities and hearing speech in noise. PLoS One 6:e18082. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018082

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roeske, S., Wolfsgruber, S., Kleineidam, L., Zulka, L., Buerger, K., Ewers, M., et al. (2018). P3-591: a german version of the lifetime of experiences questionnaire (leq) to measure cognitive reserve: validation results from the DELCODE study. Alzheimers Dement. 14, 1352–1353. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.06.1957

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogenmoser, L., Kernbach, J., Schlaug, G., and Gaser, C. (2018). Keeping brains young with making music. Brain Struct. Funct. 223, 297–305. doi: 10.1007/s00429-017-1491-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Román-Caballero, R., Arnedo, M., Triviño, M., and Lupiáñez, J. (2018). Musical practice as an enhancer of cognitive function in healthy aging – a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS One 13:e0207957. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0207957

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romeiser, J. L., Smith, D. M., and Clouston, S. A. P. (2021). Musical instrument engagement across the life course and episodic memory in late life: an analysis of 60 years of longitudinal data from the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study. PLoS One 16:e0253053. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0253053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Särkämö, T. (2018). Music for the ageing brain: cognitive, emotional, social, and neural benefits of musical leisure activities in stroke and dementia. Dementia (London) 17, 670–685. doi: 10.1177/1471301217729237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schellenberg, E. (2020). Correlation = causation? Music training, psychology, and neuroscience. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 14, 475–480. doi: 10.1037/aca0000263

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, C. E., Hunter, E. G., and Bardach, S. H. (2019). Potential cognitive benefits from playing music among cognitively intact older adults: a scoping review. J. Appl. Gerontol. 38, 1763–1783. doi: 10.1177/0733464817751198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seinfeld, S., Figueroa, H., Ortiz-Gil, J., and Sanchez-Vives, M. (2013). Effects of music learning and piano practice on cognitive function, mood and quality of life in older adults. Front. Psychol. 4:810. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00810

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sihvonen, A. J., Särkämö, T., Leo, V., Tervaniemi, M., Altenmüller, E., and Soinila, S. (2017). Music-based interventions in neurological rehabilitation. Lancet Neurol. 16, 648–660. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(17)30168-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sluming, V., Barrick, T., Howard, M., Cezayirli, E., Mayes, A., and Roberts, N. (2002). Voxel-based morphometry reveals increased gray matter density in Broca’s area in male symphony orchestra musicians. Neuroimage 17, 1613–1622. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1288

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stark, S. M., Kirwan, C. B., and Stark, C. E. L. (2019). Mnemonic similarity task: a tool for assessing hippocampal integrity. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 938–951. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, Y. (2009). Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia 47, 2015–2028. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, Y., Arenaza-Urquijo, E. M., Bartrés-Faz, D., Belleville, S., Cantilon, M., Chetelat, G., et al. (2020). Whitepaper: defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimers Dement. 16, 1305–1311. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.07.219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strong, J. V., and Mast, B. T. (2019). The cognitive functioning of older adult instrumental musicians and non-musicians. Neuropsychol. Dev. Cogn. B Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 26, 367–386. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2018.1448356

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stuart, E. A. (2010). Matching methods for causal inference: a review and a look forward. Stat. Sci. 25, 1–21. doi: 10.1214/09-STS313

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sutcliffe, R., Du, K., and Ruffman, T. (2020). Music making and neuropsychological aging: a review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 113, 479–491. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.03.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Talamini, F., Altoè, G., Carretti, B., and Grassi, M. (2017). Musicians have better memory than nonmusicians: a meta-analysis. PLoS One 12:e0186773. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0186773

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teki, S., Kumar, S., Von Kriegstein, K., Stewart, L., Lyness, C. R., Moore, B. C., et al. (2012). Navigating the auditory scene: an expert role for the hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 32, 12251–12257. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0082-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valenzuela, M. J., and Sachdev, P. (2007). Assessment of complex mental activity across the lifespan: development of the lifetime of experiences questionnaire (LEQ). Psychol. Med. 37, 1015–1025. doi: 10.1017/S003329170600938X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valenzuela, M. J., Matthews, F. E., Brayne, C., Ince, P., Halliday, G., Kril, J. J., et al. (2012). Multiple biological pathways link cognitive lifestyle to protection from dementia. Biol. Psychiatry 71, 783–791. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.07.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verghese, J., Lipton, R. B., Katz, M. J., Hall, C. B., Derby, C. A., Kuslansky, G., et al. (2003). Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. N. Engl. J. Med. 348, 2508–2516. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa022252

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wan, C. Y., and Schlaug, G. (2010). Music making as a tool for promoting brain plasticity across the life span. Neuroscientist 16, 566–577. doi: 10.1177/1073858410377805

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Washburn, R. A., Mcauley, E., Katula, J., Mihalko, S. L., and Boileau, R. A. (1999). The physical activity scale for the elderly (PASE): evidence for validity. J Clin Epidemiol 52, 643–651. doi: 10.1016/S0895-4356(99)00049-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wesselman, L. M. P., Van Lent, D. M., Schröder, A., Van De Rest, O., Peters, O., Menne, F., et al. (2021). Dietary patterns are related to cognitive functioning in elderly enriched with individuals at increased risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Eur. J. Nutr. 60, 849–860. doi: 10.1007/s00394-020-02257-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wickham, H. (2016). ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-24277-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wirth, M., Bejanin, A., La Joie, R., Arenaza-Urquijo, E. M., Gonneaud, J., Landeau, B., et al. (2018). Regional patterns of gray matter volume, hypometabolism, and beta-amyloid in groups at risk of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging 63, 140–151. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.10.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wirth, M., Haase, C. M., Villeneuve, S., Vogel, J., and Jagust, W. J. (2014). Neuroprotective pathways: lifestyle activity, brain pathology, and cognition in cognitively normal older adults. Neurobiol. Aging 35, 1873–1882. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.02.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wirth, M., Villeneuve, S., Haase, C. M., Madison, C. M., Oh, H., Landau, S. M., et al. (2013). Associations between Alzheimer disease biomarkers, neurodegeneration, and cognition in cognitively normal older people. JAMA Neurol. 70, 1512–1519. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2013.4013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolfsgruber, S., Kleineidam, L., Guski, J., Polcher, A., Frommann, I., Roeske, S., et al. (2020). Minor neuropsychological deficits in patients with subjective cognitive decline. Neurology 95, e1134–e1143. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000010142

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Worschech, F., Altenmüller, E., Jünemann, K., Sinke, C., Krüger, T. H. C., Scholz, D. S., et al. (2022). Evidence of cortical thickness increases in bilateral auditory brain structures following piano learning in older adults. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1513, 21–30. doi: 10.1111/nyas.14762

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Worschech, F., Marie, D., Jünemann, K., Sinke, C., Krüger, T. H. C., Großbach, M., et al. (2021). Improved speech in noise perception in the elderly after 6 months of musical instruction. Front. Neurosci. 15:696240. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.696240

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zatorre, R. J. (2013). Predispositions and plasticity in music and speech learning: neural correlates and implications. Science 342, 585–589. doi: 10.1126/science.1238414

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: brain aging, resilience, cognitive reserve, prevention, brain plasticity, instrument playing

Citation: Böttcher A, Zarucha A, Köbe T, Gaubert M, Höppner A, Altenstein S, Bartels C, Buerger K, Dechent P, Dobisch L, Ewers M, Fliessbach K, Freiesleben SD, Frommann I, Haynes JD, Janowitz D, Kilimann I, Kleineidam L, Laske C, Maier F, Metzger C, Munk MHJ, Perneczky R, Peters O, Priller J, Rauchmann B-S, Roy N, Scheffler K, Schneider A, Spottke A, Teipel SJ, Wiltfang J, Wolfsgruber S, Yakupov R, Düzel E, Jessen F, Röske S, Wagner M, Kempermann G and Wirth M (2022) Musical Activity During Life Is Associated With Multi-Domain Cognitive and Brain Benefits in Older Adults. Front. Psychol. 13:945709. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.945709

Received: 16 May 2022; Accepted: 23 June 2022;
Published: 25 August 2022.

Edited by:

Renata Kochhann, Hospital Moinhos de Vento, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Veronika Diaz Abrahan, University of Palermo, Argentina
Edna Andrews, Duke University, United States

Copyright © 2022 Böttcher, Zarucha, Köbe, Gaubert, Höppner, Altenstein, Bartels, Buerger, Dechent, Dobisch, Ewers, Fliessbach, Freiesleben, Frommann, Haynes, Janowitz, Kilimann, Kleineidam, Laske, Maier, Metzger, Munk, Perneczky, Peters, Priller, Rauchmann, Roy, Scheffler, Schneider, Spottke, Teipel, Wiltfang, Wolfsgruber, Yakupov, Düzel, Jessen, Röske, Wagner, Kempermann and Wirth. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Miranka Wirth, miranka.wirth@dzne.de

The members of the DELCODE study group are listed in the acknowledgments

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.