Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 21 July 2021
Sec. Organizational Psychology

Understanding the Path Toward Financial Well-Being: Evidence From India

\nKanchan SehrawatKanchan Sehrawat1Madhu VijMadhu Vij1Gaurav Talan
Gaurav Talan2*
  • 1Faculty of Management Studies, University of Delhi, New Delhi, India
  • 2University School of Management Studies (USMS), Guru Gobind Singh Indraprastha University, New Delhi, India

Many countries are taking steps to establish national strategies to improve the financial well-being (FWB) of their people. However, FWB as a term is still in the infancy stage with a handful of models developed in the context of developed countries. Thus, there is a need to understand FWB from a multi-disciplinary and multi-dimensional approach to draft and implement efficient strategies, especially in the context of developing countries like India. In this study, we have performed path analysis to identify the specific constituents of financial literacy, financial behavior (FinB), and personality traits that affect the FWB (perceived and objective) of an individual in Indian context. Survey responses of 349 respondents are analyzed to empirically validate the proposed relationships using the partial least squares structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM) approach. The analysis mostly provides support to existing literature and challenges some. The findings provide support to 12 out of 17 tested effects with eight hypotheses. The understanding of pathways that lead to increased FWB of individuals has the potential to facilitate effective policy-making and designing of curriculum to support efforts of individuals toward higher FWB and responsible FinBs.

Introduction

Globally, individuals strive to improve their financial lives. They make financial decisions (spend, save, borrow, etc.) to grow their assets and protect their resources in pursuit of improving their financial status/well-being/health. However, financial decisions can prove to be particularly challenging. Individuals in today's world are witnessing a rapid change in the financial system because of a growing global economy, technological advancement, and proliferation in financial products and services (for instance easy availability of loans). Individuals can easily find themselves caught up in an unpropitious economic situation if it is not handled with a responsible financial behavior (FinB). Financial problems have the potential to negatively impact not only an individual but the economy at large. The World Bank (2013) notes that globally policymakers are concerned with how the financial well-being (FWB) of households can be improved to enhance the financial sector and increase its stability. It is essential to identify specific personality traits, knowledge, or behaviors that help some individuals to endure difficult times and flourish in good times as compared with others in similar situations. Such knowledge can be useful for various stakeholders to facilitate and coordinate their efforts to improve the FWB of individuals (Netemeyer et al., 2018; Riitsalu and Murakas, 2019). Despite the growing need to understand the antecedents of FWB, limited research has been conducted so far in this area (Collins and Urban, 2020). Holistic research on factors elucidating diversities in FWB is still in a nascent stage (Brüggen et al., 2017). Brüggen et al. (2017); Netemeyer et al. (2018); Collins and Urban (2020); and Riitsalu and Murakas (2019), among others, called for a more comprehensive and integrated approach to understand how FWB operates vis-à-vis various FinBs, knowledge, and personality traits.

The purpose of this research is thus 3-fold.

First, existing research in the field of personal finance is restricted to financial inclusion, financial literacy, capability, or specific FinBs. However, FWB is still a novel term in the financial inclusion community (Brüggen et al., 2017; Collins and Urban, 2020). Zyphur et al. (2015) pointed out that research in the area of FWB is still sparser than that of overall well-being. Existing studies either resort to objective measures, such as income, savings, debt-to-income ratio, or a single statement satisfaction question to gauge FWB. However, people in a similar objective financial condition may perceive their FWB differently (i.e., positively or negatively).

Consequently, individuals in an identical objectively measured financial situation may consider their personal FWB more or less positively (Grable et al., 2012). Therefore, the use of any single approach, i.e., subjective or objective, may not be suitable for evaluating a multifaceted and personal phenomenon such as FWB. To the best knowledge of the authors, no study has investigated the interplay between responsible FinBs, financial knowledge, psychological factors, and FWB (subjective and objective). This study aims to empirically test the said interplay in the Indian context.

Second, prior research mainly relies on objective financial knowledge and measurable behaviors while ignoring the role of confidence and motivation (Klapper et al., 2014; OECD, 2016). In contrast, according to their meta-analysis, Fernandes et al. (2014) asserted that financial literacy can predict a mere 0.1% of differences in FinBs. On the other hand, Vlaev and Elliott (2017) and Xiao and Porto (2017), among others, vouched for the higher role of confidence and motivation in explaining key FinBs. Knowledge of the interaction of FinBs with constituents of financial literacy can be instrumental in designing financial education programs to assist individuals in achieving their desired financial goals. This research considers the various constituents of financial literacy, i.e., objective knowledge, awareness, experience, and confidence levels, to throw light on the interplay of these components and their ability to predict FinBs and, ultimately, FWB.

Third, extant literature (Xiao and Dew, 2011; Kempson et al., 2013; Loke, 2017) tends to examine the effects of individual characteristics on one or more selected key behaviors separately. Incorporating various domains of personal financial management is essential since each of these domains has a different but significant impact on the well-being of individuals (Xiao and Dew, 2011). This study incorporates an array of FinBs to factor in various dimensions of personal financial management.

In this research, we aim to comprehensively understand the pathways to FWB and identify key predictors in the context of a developing economy, India. The results of this study can help understand the distribution of FWB across various sections of society, observe trends, and assess the effectiveness of prevalent policies, products, capabilities, and behavioral interventions.

Conceptual Model

The conceptual model of this study finds its roots in the theory of family management system model as propounded by Deacon and Firebaugh (1988). In this study, a family resource management model is applied to individuals to analyze the components of their FWB, as depicted in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The conceptual framework adapted from Deacon and Firebaugh (1988). Source: Self depiction.

We conceptualize input or resources for throughput (i.e., responsible financial behavior, FinB) as components of financial literacy (i.e., awareness, objective financial knowledge, experience, and confidence) and psychological factors (i.e., time orientation, impulsivity, social status, self-control, and locus of control, LOC). The opinion of including psychological factors as input is supported by Mokhtar and Husniyah (2017). The throughput process has been measured using FinBs, namely, credit aversion, daily ease of meeting financial commitments, informed decision making, monitoring, informed product choices, spending attitudes, planning, and savings. The output component is characterized as FWB (objective and subjective).

The elaborated conceptual model for this study, with additional interlinkage among the constructs, is illustrated in Figure 2. The model proposes that given a socio-economic environment, individuals with a high level of financial literacy are expected to have responsible FinBs, which are also influenced by the presence of positive psychological factors. Further, responsible FinBs are expected to affect FWB positively. This relationship can be either: (a) direct relationship, where particular FinB will affect perceptions of an individual of their FWB irrespective of how they are actually/objectively doing financially, or (b) indirect relationship, where particular FinBs will affect the objective financial situation, which would then possibly influence perceived FWB.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Conceptual Model. Source: Self depiction, L&M, Objective financial knowledge.

Socio-demographic characteristics are believed to shape the choices available to a person, how psychological characteristics predict behaviors, and how individuals perceive their well-being. Their effect is examined by correlational investigations unlike the relationships of other independent variables, which are explained as direct or indirect causal relationships. These relationships are analyzed by multigroup analysis (MGA) involving 440 comparisons of the proposed model.

The conceptual model shares many elements with the framework of the Consumer Financial Protection Bureau (CFPB) (2015) and Kempson et al. (2017). Both models conceptualized FWB to be primarily driven by FinBs, which, in turn, are ascertained by the resources of an individual. Further, both models consider knowledge, skills and attitudes, and psychological traits as indirect influencers of FWB through FinBs. In the view of the authors, psychological traits can also affect the perception of being financially well-directly. Further, the socio-economic environment is considered as a direct influencer for all the factors considered except behavior in Kempson et al. (2017), and behavior and well-being in CFPB (2015). In the view of the authors, the effect of socio-demographic variables should be considered in each relationship, i.e., they should be considered as moderators that can influence the strength and even the directions of the proposed relationships among all the constructs of the model. Lastly, unlike the two models, we also explicitly differentiate between the objective situation and the perception of well-being. This is an exploratory relationship proposed by the model to provide further insights into the existing knowledge of the pathways to FWB. The details of the constructs and the operational definitions used in the study are presented in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Operational definitions.

Literature Review and Hypothesis Development

Managing personal finances and coping with financial exigency are essential for the well-being of an individual and their household, and the economy at large. As a response to this need, various stakeholders focus on financial inclusion, i.e., access to various financial products and services (Alliance for Financial Inclusion, 2016, 2017); financial literacy, i.e., building capacity (Lusardi and Mitchell, 2014); and financial capability, i.e., enhancement of knowledge and change in behavior (Stumm et al., 2013; CFPB, 2018). It can be gathered that the common aim of all the above mentioned is toward enhancing the overall FWB of an individual (Kempson and Poppe, 2018). However, FWB is still a novel concept that lacks a conceptual definition and standardized measurement scales.

A handful of studies have attempted to explore comprehensive FWB models. Further, existing proposed models of FWB are predominantly established and examined in developed countries, such as the United States (CFPB, 2015), Norway (Kempson et al., 2017), the United Kingdom (Hayes et al., 2016), Canada (Financial Consumer Agency of Canada, 2018), Australia, and New Zealand (Prendergast et al., 2018a,b). It is important to understand that the theories developed and evidence obtained from data of developed economies may not apply in developing economies (Burgess and Steenkamp, 2006). Xiao et al. (2014) asserted that it is necessary to carry out studies in other developed and developing economies to enhance the understanding of FWB in various socio-economic contexts. Indian studies have largely focused on financial inclusion and financial literacy. However, only few research studies have ventured into the novel concept of FWB.

The gap relating to personal finance and FWB literature could be summarized in the necessity of proposing a holistic model, which can accurately identify the pathways to the FWB of Indians. Hence, in order to fill this gap, this study aims to empirically examine the relationship of responsible FinBs with FWB and how these relationships are determined by various psychological factors, financial literacy components, and sociodemographic factors.

Relationship Between Financial Literacy and Responsible Financial Behavior

Literature highlights a positive relationship between financial literacy and FinBs (Allgood and Walstad, 2016; Bannier and Neubert, 2016). The financial outcome that is part of the study is FWB, which is mainly reported to have a positive correlation with financial literacy (Addin et al., 2013; Sabri and Zakaria, 2015). However, no substantial relationship between financial literacy and FWB is documented by (Shim et al., 2009). Further, Courchane and Zorn (2005) reported a sequential effect of financial knowledge, FinB, and financial performance.

Lee and Hanna (2014) reported that financial knowledge affects the attitude an individual has toward awareness about money management. This attitude affects the actual money management behavior, which, in turn, affects the outcomes of such money management. Financial knowledge, along with financial decision-making experience, has a potential to reduce the likelihood of individuals to get trapped in unscrupulous sales practices of financial product and service providers (Atia, 2012). Lyons (2008) and Rao and Barber (2005) reported that financial education influences FWB through FinBs.

Thus, it can be inferred that an individual who is aware of available financial products and services is expected to have the higher objective financial knowledge and higher financial confidence. This further leads to higher experience in financial decision-making, which ultimately influences FinBs. We, therefore, hypothesized that components of financial literacy have a positive relationship with responsible FinBs.

H1: There is a positive and significant association between financial awareness and FinB via objective financial knowledge (L and M) and financial experience (FinExp).

H2a: There is a positive and significant association between financial awareness and FinB via financial confidence.

H2b: There is a positive and significant association between financial awareness and FinB via financial confidence and financial experience.

H3: There is a positive and significant association between objective financial knowledge (L and M) and FinB via FinExp.

Relationship Between Psychological Factors, Responsible Financial Behaviors, and Financial Well-Being

Literature suggests several potential psychological traits that influence financial decision making and FWB. The psychological factors that are incorporated into the conceptual model of this study are described briefly in subsequent subsections.

Time Orientation

Time orientation can be comprehended as the preference of immediate or current gratification over possible future gains. Short-term time orientation (TO), i.e., urge of immediate gratification, is identified as a key inhibitor of financial effectiveness (Vyvyan et al., 2014). In fact, Shepard and Turner (2019) provided evidence that the positive relationship of future orientation with well-being (measured in terms of health, happiness, life satisfaction, and FWB) is robust across cultures and countries in their sample of 64 countries. Kooij et al. (2018), in their meta-analysis, concluded that having future time perspectives are subjective expectations and beliefs of an individual about their future, which include the ability to set and pursue long-term goals as well as to delay gratification. Further, they concluded that it is a crucial factor that fosters health and well-being.

Literature provides evidence that TO influences several financial contexts, such as retirement savings (Hastings et al., 2011), credit (Benton et al., 2007), risk tolerance (Jacobs-Lawson and Hershey, 2005), savings (Kempson and Poppe, 2018), financial planning (Hershey et al., 2010), compulsive buying or spending restraint (Kempson and Poppe, 2018) and financial or economic well-being (Kooij et al., 2018; Shepard and Turner, 2019).

It can, thus, be anticipated that short-term TO is negatively related to responsible FinB and FWB.

H4a: There is a negative and significant association between short-term TO and responsible FinBs.

H5a: There is a negative and significant association between short-term TO and perceived FWB.

Impulsivity (Impulse)

Impulsivity encompasses a trade-off between long term benefits and immediate satisfaction (Vohs et al., 2012; Bernheim et al., 2015). In terms of FinB, impulsive people seek immediate benefits and make short term financial decisions instead of making decisions that are consistent with their long-term financial goals. Fujita et al. (2006) provide evidence that individuals with high impulsivity tend to act in a non-optimal way. Impulsivity is negatively related to FWB (Kempson and Poppe, 2018). Impulsivity is linked to several negative FinBs like over-indebtedness (Abrantes-Braga and Veludo-de-Oliveira, 2020), financial instability (Lusardi et al., 2010), holding low-interest saving products (Gathergood and Weber, 2014), and low retirement savings (DeHart et al., 2016). It can thus be anticipated that high impulsivity is negatively related to responsible FinB and FWB.

H4b: There is a negative and significant association between high impulsivity (impulse) and responsible FinB.

H5b: There is a negative and significant association between high impulsivity and perceived FWB.

Self-Control

Self-control denotes the capability of controlling the temptation or controlling one's impulses, emotions, actions, and desires to protect a valued goal (Gerhard et al., 2018). Self-control (SC) has been reported as an important predictor of success in several domains of life, such as better educational attainment (Duckworth and Seligman, 2005), and better FinB and FWB (Strömbäck et al., 2017). However, based on their experimental study, Ballinger et al. (2011) argued that the relationship is yet to be established between SC and responsible FinB, such as saving.

Self-control is linked with specific positive financial behaviors, such as spending and borrowing restraint (Achtziger et al., 2015; Kempson and Poppe, 2018), retirement planning, and saving (Strömbäck et al., 2017), asset diversification and wealth (Biljanovska and Palligkinis, 2018), credit score (Arya et al., 2013), and positive FinB in general (Miotto and Parente, 2015).

Though the two constructs, self-control and impulsivity, are inter-related, they are believed to stem from different neurological bases (Lieberman, 2007; Steinberg, 2008) and, thus, it is crucial to consider both, especially in the case of evaluating a decision-making process (Chen and Vazsonyi, 2011).

From the above discussions, it can be anticipated that high SC can lead to responsible FinB and FWB.

H4c: There is a positive and significant association between high SC and responsible FinBs.

H5c: There is a positive and significant association between high SC and perceived FWB.

Locus of Control

Locus of control is the extent to which one feels in control of events that affect them (Hellrigel et al., 2010). Individual's perceived control over outcomes have a significant impact on their financial prosperity (Perry and Morris, 2005). LOC is an important intra-personal component of empowerment which is a strong influencer (both direct and indirect) on financial habits (Angulo-Ruiz and Pergelova, 2015).

Individuals with an internal LOC will demonstrate responsible financial management behavior, as evident in the studies of Angulo-Ruiz and Pergelova (2015) and Mien and Thao (2015), and higher FWB (Prawitz and Cohart, 2016; Kempson and Poppe, 2018; Mahdzan et al., 2019). They also have higher savings and consumption rates (Cobb-Clark et al., 2016; Kempson and Poppe, 2018), better investment returns (Salamanca et al., 2020), low dependence on welfare receipt (Chan, 2017), higher wealth accumulation (Cobb-Clark et al., 2016); and they make informed product choice (Hoffman et al., 2003). It can, thus, be anticipated that higher LOC is positively related to responsible FinB and FWB.

H4d: There is a positive and significant relationship between internal LOC and responsible FinB.

H5d: There is a positive and significant association between internal LOC and perceived FWB.

Social Status

Social status can be understood as the inclination of individuals to follow social norms, also referred to as social validation, herding, or social proof. The process of social comparisons plays an important role in the perception of own financial situation (Clark and Senik, 2010). The perception of financial status in comparison to one's peer group is often a stronger predictor for different behaviors than objective measures. This “keeping up with the Joneses” effect is likely to influence monetary behavior (Masche, 2010), participation in employer-sponsored retirement plans (Duflo and Saez, 2002), stock market participation (Sivaramakrishnan et al., 2017), purchase decisions (Attri, 2013), higher savings (Raue et al., 2020), and prevent incapable borrowing (Kempson and Poppe, 2018).

Thus, it can be inferred that comparing oneself with others tends to promote goal attainment and provides motivation to engage in positive FinBs (Frederiks et al., 2015). Similarly, Kempson et al. (2013) provided evidence of a positive relationship of social status with financial capabilities. Sundarasen et al. (2016) reported a positive influence of social comparison on financial planning and practices. In contrast, Rahman and Gan (2020) revealed no relationship between herding behavior and investment decisions. Furthermore, Money Advice Service (2015) argued that social norms strongly but negatively influence FinBs. Thus, social status, where an individual tries to fit into the social group, can act as a facilitator or barrier to responsible FinB. Norvilitis and Mendes-Da-Silva (2013) and Norvilitis and Mao (2013) revealed a negative relationship between social comparisons and FWB among college students. A positive but low relation is reported by Prendergast et al. (2018a), whereas a non-significant relation is observed by Prendergast et al. (2018b). A negative relationship of social status with subjective FWB is evident in the study of Kempson and Poppe (2018).

It can, thus, be anticipated that high concern for social status is positively related to responsible FinB. However, it holds a negative relation with subjective FWB.

H4e: There is a positive association between high concern for social status (SS) and responsible FinBs.

H5e: There is a negative and significant association between high concern for SS and perceived FWB.

Relationship Between Responsible Financial Behaviors and Financial Well-Being

This study conceptualizes responsible FinB as having eight components, namely, credit aversion, daily ease of meeting financial commitments, informed decision making, monitoring financial activities, product choices, spending attitudes, planning, saving, and investments.

Credit usage has been linked to several problems, such as psychological distress, lower self-esteem, depression, humiliation, and anxiety, which have an adversarial effect on physical and mental health (Hojman et al., 2016). It is also associated with reduced perceived FWB (Norvilitis et al., 2003). The problem in meeting daily financial commitments with ease may result in the use of credit for meeting these “day-to-day” commitments, which can adversely affect the FWB of an individual (Delafrooz and Paim, 2013; Finney, 2016). In the model, we have, thus, included credit aversion and ease of meeting daily commitments in the basket of responsible FinBs.

Making budgets and regular comparisons of actual and planned expenditures can help individuals with their routine money management, which is positively associated with their well-being (Sabri and Zakaria, 2015). Individuals involved in making informed decisions by exploring product options and regularly monitoring their expenses against their incomes are expected to have higher FWB (Kempson et al., 2013). High propensity to spend money on non-essential items has an inverse relation with FWB (Delafrooz and Paim, 2013). Stress due to unhealthy spending can also reduce physical health and results in lower job performance (Dunn and Mirzaie, 2012). In the model, we measured spending restraint, which is the ability not to indulge in overspending and, thus, is proposed to have a positive impact on FWB.

A healthy balance between spending and savings is imperative for sustaining FWB in the long run (Van Praag et al., 2003). Higher propensity to save is related to improved barraging power or decision-making authority (Schaner, 2017), lower possibilities of selling assets to meet financial emergencies (Jack and Suri, 2014), increased productivity (Knowles, 2013), and ultimately better FWB (Kempson et al., 2013).

Knowledge of financial behaviors in which individuals are involved allows them to deliberate more judiciously about what is necessary to enhance their future financial prospects. Thus, we can propose that an array of responsible FinBs, future-oriented behaviors one was previously engaged in, will be positively related to perceived FWB. As also asserted by Huston (2010), Netemeyer et al. (2018), and Perry and Morris (2005) in their respective studies, FWB is the outcome of FinBs.

Further, after reviewing the published literature and drawing from their conceptual frameworks. It was revealed that FWB consists of two primary components.

1. Financial consequences that individuals encounter and testify through their personal subjective lens, and

2. Financial consequences that are observable from financial records, accounts, and transactions of an individual.

This study relies on the self-reported subjective or perceived FWB and derived a score of objective financial situation. The exploratory links of FinB with objective financial situation and that with perceived FWB are hypothesized to have positive relations. This implies that individuals who engage in responsible FinB are expected to experience higher FWB (both perceived and actual). It is further proposed that achieving higher objectively measured FWB by engaging in responsible FinB further enhances the perceptions of being financially well.

H6: There is a positive and significant association between responsible FinB and objective financial situation (ObjFin).

H7: There is a positive and significant association between responsible FinB and perceived FWB.

H8: There is a positive and significant association between ObjFin and perceived FWB.

Materials and Methods

Sample Size and Data Collection

This study utilized a structured questionnaire to gather data for hypothesis testing and to address the research objectives. The overall population of the study includes any individual who is either aged (i) 24 years or above or (ii) 18 years and above with a work experience of more than 2 years. In general, there is no consensus on how to calculate the sample size for PLS-SEM. Various rules of thumb and software are at disposal of the researchers. Hair et al. (2006) recommended a minimum of 200 respondents as sample size. Further, Brysbaert (2019) argued that running more participants than strictly needed involves a minor financial cost, whereas running fewer participants entails an increased risk of drawing incorrect conclusions. Thus, we aimed to get a sample size higher than 200 to draw meaningful conclusions in light of published literature. A useable sample of 349 responses was achieved with time and financial constraints.

Data for this study was collected by offline (pen and paper) as well as an online survey. The invitation link to the survey site (surveymonkey.com) was sent through emails, and social networking platforms, such as WhatsApp, LinkedIn, and Facebook, between July 2019 and October 2019. A total of 269 responses were gathered utilizing the survey platform, and the in-person survey received 225 questionnaires. Out of the total 494 responses, 394 were found to be complete and met the criteria of the study.

Table 2 explains the profile of the 394 respondents, and among them, 58% were male and 42% were female. An approximately equal number of respondents reported their marital status to be either married or single, and a low number of respondents with divorce or separated status could be reached. Only 4% respondents have educational qualifications less than a gradation or diploma. Around 60% of the respondents do not have any dependent adult or child in the sample. A diverse occupation respondent base is achieved in the sample, with most of the respondents working in the private sector.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Demographic profile of respondents.

Instrument Development

Literature review helped in generating the items on a provisional basis for inclusion in the questionnaire. The construct items were refined with the help of expert interviews. The purification included rewording of the items, editing, adding, and deleting and revising the item statements. This step was followed by face validity and content validity. Subsequently, all the scales were put together in the form of a questionnaire and underwent a pilot test on a convenience sample of 50 respondents.

The final questionnaire is divided into five sections. The first section aims to collect general information and includes the filter questions. The second section deals with day-to-day or month-to-month money management. The third section seeks to collect information regarding savings and investment patterns, followed by the fourth section that records the responses on various risk and risk mitigation strategies followed by the respondent. The last section assesses financial capabilities in terms of decision-making and financial knowledge.

Perceived financial well-being is measured using the original five-item scale developed by CFPB (2017). Objective FWB is evaluated based on seven items, namely, difficulty in making ends meet (ObjFin_1), savings levels (ObjFin_2), ability to absorb negative shock (ObjFin_3), diversification of investment portfolio (ObjFin_4), unpaid loans (ObjFin_5), retirement planning (ObjFin_6), and insurance plan (ObjFin_7).

Psychological factors are measured using a three-item scale on a five-point Likert scale for each of the construct, namely, TO, impulsivity (impulse), SS, SC, and, LOC. The scales are adapted from Antonides et al. (2011), Kempson et al. (2013), Kempson et al. (2017), Perry and Morris (2005), and Prendergast et al. (2018a,b). Responsible FinB in terms of spending restraint is measured using three items adapted from Kempson et al. (2017) and Prendergast et al. (2018a,b). Behavior pertaining to monitoring personal finance, informed decision-making, and product choice is measured using items adapted from Prendergast et al. (2018a,b). Personal financial planning behavior is measured using two items similar to the study of Hayes et al. (2016). Credit aversion behavior is measured on three statements adapted from the FinScope Survey (2017). Active saving behavior is measured using three statements as in Kempson et al. (2013) and Prendergast et al. (2018a,b).

Financial confidence questions are adapted from the study of Farrell et al. (2016), and financial experience (FinEx) is measured with adapted items from the studies of Comerton-Forde et al. (2018) and OECD (2016). Financial awareness is measured by preparing a 20-item list relevant in Indian context grounded on the description of financial literacy (OECD, 2005). Objective financial literacy is evaluated using the five questions developed by Lusardi and Mitchell (2011), also known as the “Big5” questions.

Measures

We have measured psychological factors and subjective FWB as first-order reflective constructs, whereas financial literacy and objective FWB are a first-order formative construct. The formative construct assumes that its indicators cause the construct i.e., the selected indicators encompass all the vital aspects of the particular domain. On the other hand, reflective indicators are produced by the construct i.e., the indicators are highly correlated, and any deletion of an indicator does not change the meaning of the latent variable. Responsible FinB is theorized as a higher-order reflective-formative construct. The multiple reflective indicators are adapted from the extant literature. The formative indicators are adapted from literature and purified with the help of expert interviews. These indicators are then combined and modified to fit the context of this research.

Based on Hair et al. (2017) recommendations, we performed partial least squares modeling (PLS-SEM) (SmartPLS 3.2.6) to access the inter-relationships of various constructs, as proposed in the conceptual model. PLS-SEM is a second-generation advanced statistical technique. It incorporates the characteristics of factor analysis and multiple regression, which facilitate the simultaneous examination of direct and indirect effects of exogeneous and endogenous variables. Thus, PLS-SEM enables working on complex models. To test for statistical significance, we resorted to bootstrapping with 5,000 re-samplings.

Data Analysis and Results

On preliminary analysis of the total 494 responses, missing or invalid responses were observed in 100 survey responses, thus leaving 394 responses for the final empirical analysis. The normality is gauged by comparing the skewness and kurtosis of all the interval scale data. The indicators are under the acceptable value of 2 (skewness) with two exceptions, i.e., credit aversion 3 (−2.164) and ObjFin_1 (−2.649). Further, none of the kurtosis values of an indicator exceeds 7, indicating no significant issues with univariate normality. Following the preliminary analysis, we moved toward the evaluation of measurement and structural models.

It is also important to note here that in the proposed model construct “FinB,” responsible financial behavior, is a reflective-formative type hierarchical component model. To obtain the true relationships of the latent variables on higher order constructs (HOC) in the structural path model, the two-stage approach is employed as suggested by Henseler and Chin (2010). In stage one, a repeated indicator approach is used to obtain the latent variable scores for lower-order constructs of “FinB;” and in stage two, these latent variable scores are used for computing the full model. Further, the assessment of the HOC is in line with the procedure followed for other constructs.

Evaluation of Measurement Model

Reflective Measurement Model

The summary of reflective measurement model evaluation is presented in Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Summary of test results for reflective measurement model.

The internal consistency is gauged using Cronbach's alpha and composite reliability (CR). The Cronbach's alpha of all the constructs, except monitor, informD, and LOC, meet the generally accepted limit of. 6–0.7 (Hair et al., 2006). Further, each of the latent variables has acceptable CR and ranges from 0.778 (Monitor) to 0.92 (Plan) for our reflective scales (Hair et al., 2017). As Cronbach's alpha is sensitive to the number of items in the construct, and it works on a very strong assumption of equal reliability of all the indicators. Moreover, PLS-SEM prioritizes the indicators based on their individual reliability. We, thus, retain all the constructs, as their reliability is established by a CR test. Thus, we can conclude that all the indicators show high internal consistency.

Indicator reliability is established using outer loadings. The factor loadings should be higher than 0.7. However, indicators with outer loadings between 0.4 and 0.7 can be taken into account if the other criteria are fulfilled (Hair et al., 2017). The factor loadings in the model range from 0.514 (FWB5) to 0.924 (FinB_Plan2), satisfying the psychometric reliability test requirements (Henseler et al., 2009).

Convergent validity is established using average variance explained (AVE) criteria, and except for latent variables FWB and LOC, the AVE for each of the indicator is above 0.5, which signifies that the constructs can capture more than 50% of the variation in relation to the variance due to measurement error. However, FWB is short of only 0.033 and LOC of 0.006 from the required minimum value. Since these differences are very low, we retain the two constructs in their original form.

Discriminant validity is established using Fornell–Larcker, cross loadings, and HTMT (Hair et al., 2017), confirming that there are no discriminant validity issues in the model both at construct and item levels analyzed using the three criteria (refer to Appendix A for result tables).

Formative Measurement Model

Construct validity is established using discriminant validity. Discriminant validity among all the constructs is established using the Fornell–Larcker criterion (Fornell and Larcker, 1981) and the HTMT criterion (Henseler et al., 2015), indicating no discriminant validity issues (refer to Appendix B).

Indicator validity is gauged using variance inflation factor (VIF) and indicator weights. Each formative indicator is unique, and any change in these indicators may change the meaning of the entire construct. Hence, the correlation among the formative indicators is not desirable and expected. To gauge the collinearity among the indicators, VIF is analyzed. A lower value is acceptable, as it highlights lower levels of inflated variance. Table 4 presents the VIF values of all the formative constructs of the model, which are below three, indicating no collinearity issue in the data (Becker et al., 2015).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Collinearity assessment.

Table 5 shows that the weights of all the indicators of the formative construct financial behavior (FinB) are higher than 0.1 and significant on applying bootstrapping procedure indicating indicator validity (Hair et al., 2017).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Significance and relevance of formative construct FinB.

At the first iteration, the two indicators, ObjFin_5 (t = 1.309; p = 0.191) and ObjFin_7 (t = 1.076 and p = 0.282), of the formative construct objective financial situation (ObjFin) are not found to be significant. Adhering to the recommendation of Hair et al. (2017), the outer loading of formative indicators with non-significant weight is checked to gauge their absolute contribution to the indicator ObjFin. The outer-loading of ObjFin_5 is 0.035 and that of ObJFin_7 is 0.182; and as both the outer loadings are below the recommended level of 0.5, the indicators fail to be relatively and absolutely important when tested empirically. Thus, indicator Objfin_7 that represents the insurance ownership is dropped from the construct. However, during the content validation stage, experts have marked the status of unpaid loans measured by ObjFin5 as very important to determine the objective financial situation of the individual. Further, established literature supports a significant negative relation of unpaid loans with the overall FWB of the individual (Hojman et al., 2016; Blomgren et al., 2017). Hence the indicator is retained in the final construct (Table 6).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Significance and relevance of formative construct ObjFin iteration 2.

Evaluation of Structural Model

Collinearity Assessment

Table 7 reports that all the VIF values of the construct indicators are below 2, which specifies that variances of regression coefficient estimator, i.e., Var(bi) are not inflated beyond the recommended limits. Thus, uniqueness of each construct indicator is established, indicating that there is no multi-collinearity issue in the structural model (Lowry and Gaskin, 2014).

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Collinearity assessment.

Significance and Relevance of the Structural Model

Illustrative representation of the significance of the paths of the model based on significant beta values along with the R2 values of each endogenous construct is presented in Figure 3, and the relevance of the path models as established by the bootstrapping method is presented in Figure 4. It can be established that 17 out of the 21 direct path relations are significant, with only four insignificant direct relationships.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Significance of the structural model paths. Source: SmartPLS output.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Relevance of the structural model paths. Source: SmartPLS output; In parenthesis is the t-values at 5,000 bootstraps.

In complex models like ours, various endogenous constructs are being impacted in the model not only directly but also indirectly. Thus, for the complete evaluation and assessment of the structural model, the total effect (TE) of a specific exogenous construct on an endogenous construct needs to be considered. For instance, responsible FinB directly and significantly impacts FWB [FinB –> FWB] with β = 0.122, t = 2.034, p = 0.042 and also indirectly via objective financial situation [FinB –> ObjFin –> FWB] with β = 0.088, t = 3.301, p = 0.001 taking the total effect of FinB on FWB to 0.21, t = 3.718, p < 0. Table 8 displays the details of the total effects of each exogenous construct on each endogenous construct.

TABLE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Significance of total effects.

Out of 35 total effects, 23 are found to be significant with an adjusted R-squared value of 0.247 for FWB, 0.211 for FinB, and 0.181 for Objfin. These values of R2 are considered moderate and satisfactory (Cohen, 1988; Raithel et al., 2012; Ramalho and Forte, 2019). Further effect size (f 2) of awareness on objective financial literacy (L and M) is 0.249, and that of responsible FinB on ObjFin is 0.244 which is considered medium (Hair et al., 2017). Furthermore, all the constructs have a Q2 value of more than zero (i.e., Q2 > 0). The Q2 values FWB = 0.1, FinB = 0.18, FinExp = 0.08, Finconfidence = 0.04, L and M = 0.19, and ObjFin = 0.04 indicate that each endogenous construct of the model has predictive relevance (Fornell and Cha, 1994; Chin, 1998), and are calculated using Stone–Geisser criterion (Geisser, 1974; Stone, 1974) with an omission distance of 7.

The analysis in the preceding sections helps us conclude that out of 17 tested effects, 12 are observed to be statistically significant. Table 9 presents the summary of the hypothesized relations while giving the details of the estimate along with the t and the p-value.

TABLE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Table 9. Hypothesis testing results.

Results and Discussions

It can be concluded that 12 out of the 17 tested effects in the form of eight hypotheses, as proposed in the research model, are empirically supported. Specifically, we provide empirical evidence that responsible financial behaviors (FinBs), psychological factors, components of financial literacy, and objective financial situation have a significant effect on the financial well-being (FWB) of an individual. In addition to establishing these results, the reliability and validity of the constructs were within the threshold limits. Value of the coefficient of determination (R2) extracted for perceived FWB is 26%, and responsible FinB is 22.7%. These are considered as moderate values (Ramalho and Forte, 2019; Castro-González et al., 2020), especially in consumer behavior regarding finances. This value of variance is at par with or above those accounted for by other recent studies formulating complex FinB models by PLS-SEM in developing countries (Ali et al., 2015; Ramalho and Forte, 2019; Zulaihati et al., 2020).

Thus, we confirm the satisfactory level of the model by not just the value of R2 but also the effect size and predictive relevance. The empirical evidence confirms substantial to moderate effect size (f 2) of constructs of interest, namely, objective financial situation, responsible FinB, financial awareness, and psychological factors (time orientation and locus of control) (Cohen, 1988). Further, the Q2-values of all the endogenous constructs with an omission distance of seven have a non-negative and above zero value, establishing the predictive relevance of the model.

Among all the factors directly associated with responsible financial behavior (FinB) in the complete model, financial confidence (β = 0.294, t = 5.787, p < 0.001) has the highest effect followed by psychological factors time orientation with a high negative impact (β = −0.166, t = 3.496, p < 0.001) and social status with a positive impact (β = 0.14, t = 1.92, p = 0.05). The strength of these relationships does not observe much difference when indirect effects are accounted for, and the only increase in the β values that is observed is that for financial confidence (β = 0.3, t = 6.021, p < 0.001). The results imply that individuals with higher confidence in their financial skills have higher responsible FinBs. This observation is consistent with the results of Bannier and Neubert (2016), Farrell et al. (2016), and Fernandes et al. (2014), those asserted that greater confidence is linked with responsible FinBs, such as higher investments, savings, the likelihood of retirement planning, and better credit score.

Allgood and Walstad (2016) found the confidence level to have a more substantial influence on the financial behaviors (FinBs) as compared with financial knowledge. Further, based on the meta-analysis of 201 prior studies, a weak relationship between financial literacy and FinBs, with financial literacy merely explaining 0.1% of the variation in FinBs, is observed by Fernandes et al. (2014). A similar result is obtained for the conceptual model in which confidence has emerged as a superior predictor of responsible FinB in contrast to other components of financial literacy, such as objective financial knowledge. The result that factual or objective knowledge is not a sufficient driver of FinBs finds support in the studies of Kiviat and Morduch (2012) and Serido et al. (2013).

While analyzing the significant effects of various psychological factors impacting responsible financial behavior, it is observed that high social status has (β = 0.14, t = 1.92, p = 0.05) the highest positive effect followed by an internal locus of control (β = 0.118, t = 2.358, p = 0.018). The results imply that individuals with high concerns for society, i.e., high social status score and with an internal locus of control, i.e., feeling in control of their destiny, are involved in responsible FinBs. The positive relationship of social status with FinBs is also established by Frederiks et al. (2015), Kempson et al. (2013), and Sundarasen et al. (2016). These findings are also consistent with previous studies that have reported that internal locus of control is linked with positive FinBs, such as high savings rates (Cobb-Clark et al., 2016), better personal financial management (Perry and Morris, 2005; Angulo-Ruiz and Pergelova, 2015; Mien and Thao, 2015), and informed product choice (Hoffman et al., 2003).

Inverse relationship is observed for time orientation (β = −0.166, t = 3.496, p < 0.001) and impulsivity (β = −0.109, t = 2.26, p = 0.024) with responsible FinB. These results imply that individuals with short-term time orientation and high impulsivity show poor FinB. These results are consistent with observations where long-term time orientation is associated with retirement savings (Jacobs-Lawson and Hershey, 2005), less credit (Benton et al., 2007), and higher savings (Howlett et al., 2008). Further, impulsivity is associated with lower retirement savings (DeHart et al., 2016), over-indebtedness, and lower savings (Gathergood and Weber, 2014).

Thus, from the above discussion, we can infer that individuals with higher confidence in their skills to take financial decisions, long-term time orientation, more concern for social status, low impulsivity, and high locus of control are expected to engage in responsible FinB. At the lower order of the construct, savings (β = 0.244, t = 33.912), credit aversion (β = 0.238, t = 30.448), and spending restraint (β = 0.236, t = 31.33) emerge as the most significant and positive relations for responsible FinB. These results suggest that among all the responsible financial behaviors evaluated, active savings, credit management, and spending restraint are the most critical ones.

For objective financial situation, responsible FinB (β = 0.428, t = 9.816, p < 0.001) has the highest significant positive impact followed by financial confidence (β = 0.128, t = 4.879, p < 0.001). This result implies a better objective financial situation for individuals with high financial confidence and responsible FinB. Among the psychological factors, LOC and SS have a significant positive relationship with the objective financial situation. These relationships imply that individuals with an internal locus of control and a high concern for social status have a better objective financial situation. Extant literature supports a positive association of internal locus of control with objective financial situations, such as higher earnings (Heineck and Anger, 2010), wealth accumulation (Cobb-Clark et al., 2016), and financial status (Morgan and Eckert, 2004). Further, impulsivity and time orientation have a significant negative relationship with the objective financial situation. This relationship implies that high impulsivity and short-term time orientation are associated with the poor objective financial situation.

Among all the factors affecting FWB, the direct effect of the objective financial situation (β = 0.205, t=3.6, p < 0.001) and locus of control (β = 0.171, t = 3.178, p < 0.001) is observed to be highest. Positive and significant association of objective financial situation with financial satisfaction, which can be considered a proxy of FWB, is also reported by Shim et al. (2009) and Xiao et al. (2014). The strength of the relationships of various constructs changes when considering the total-effects they have on the construct of interest, i.e., perceived FWB. The strength of the relationship of responsible FinB with FWB increases from 0.122 to 0.21 when considering the total effect. Responsible FinBs positively impact the objective financial situation, which in turn positively and significantly impacts the perceived FWB of an individual (β= 0.21, t = 3.711, p < 0.001), as also evident in the results of Rowley et al. (2012) that adopting responsible FinBs facilitate individuals to take better financial decisions and also helps them cope with changes. A positive direct link of responsible FinB and FWB is also established in the studies of Kempson et al. (2013), Kempson et al. (2017) and Netemeyer et al. (2018).

Among the psychological factors impacting financial well-being (FWB) perception, internal locus of control (β = 0.196, t = 3.798, p < 0.001) has the highest positive impact, consistent with observations of Kempson et al. (2017), Mahdzan et al. (2019), and Prawitz and Cohart (2016). This is followed by self-control (β = 0.156, t = 3.107, p = 0.002), consistent with Strömbäck et al. (2017). Impulsivity (β = −0.145, t = 2.89, p = 0.004), and social status (β = −0.102, t = 1.751, p= 0.08) have significant negative effect on FWB. These results imply that individuals with a high internal locus of control, i.e., they feel they are in control of their destiny, and self-control have higher perceived FWB. On the other hand, individuals with high impulsivity and high concern for society have a negative perception about their FWB, as is evident in the studies of Kempson and Poppe (2018), Shepard and Turner (2019), and Strömbäck et al. (2017).

Surprisingly, none of the components of financial literacy other than financial confidence (β = 0.063, t = 2.987, p = 0.003) has a significant positive relationship with perceived FWB. The empirical results also provide strong evidence to support the relationship between the constituents of financial literacy (awareness and confidence) with responsible FinB, i.e., Awareness –> Finconfidence –> FinB. This implies higher financial awareness results in higher financial confidence, which, in turn, influences the FinBs positively. These results are consistent with the observation by Allgood and Walstad (2016), which reported that financial confidence is the better predictor of FinB as compared with financial knowledge. It can be argued that as confidence and perception are both self-felt phenomena, they need not be associated with the underlying objective knowledge and are more likely to move in the same direction, whereas the other constituents of financial literacy, i.e., objective financial knowledge and financial experience, were more factual. Thus, there is a possibility that an individual may have lower levels of objective financial knowledge while having a higher confidence level.

Empirical results provide strong evidence of the relationship between responsible FinB, objective financial situation and perceived financial well-being (FWB), i.e., FinB –> ObjFin–> FWB. This result implies that individuals who engage in responsible FinB experience a higher objective financial situation, which ultimately leads to a higher perception of FWB. Responsible FinB encompasses a basket of positive financial management behaviors, such as saving regularly, not overspending, making informed choices, etc. The positive relationship between responsible FinB and objective financial situation indicates that individuals who report engaging in or believing in traits of responsible FinBs can achieve a higher objective financial situation. For example, individuals who believe in saving regularly (responsible FinB) report higher levels of savings in terms of income saved (objective financial situation).

Further, the strong positive relationship between objective financial situation and perceived FWB implies that individuals experience higher FWB when they have a high objective financial situation. This relationship implies that the perception of being financially well is supported by the real objective financial situation of an individual (i.e., savings level, loan due, contingency planning, etc.). Thus, the objective financial situation goes hand in hand with the perception of one's FWB. These results find support in the studies of Shim et al. (2009) and Xiao et al. (2014) that reported good FinB leads to higher subjective FWB, while irresponsible FinB adversely affects the FWB of an individual (Kim and Garman, 2003).

Conclusion

This research study presented results of an empirically tested holistic financial well-being (FWB) model in a developing economy context. The results showed that the objective financial situation of an individual, as measured by self-reported ease of meeting routine financial commitments, level of liquid savings, status of credit due, ability to absorb financial shock, diversification of investment portfolio, and retirement planning, is positively correlated with her/his perceived/subjective FWB (β = 0.205, p < 0.001). However, FWB and objective financial situation are not perfect correlates. This result implies that the subjective FWB of individuals, as captured in the study, not only reflects the well-being of individuals as measured by the traditional objective wellness indicators, but it also reflects other possible contributors of FWB, such as experiences and expectations of individuals.

Financial behavior is associated with financial well-being directly and indirectly via objective financial situation. Although both the relationships are significant, a higher direct positive relationship is observed between FinBs and the objective financial situation (β = 0.428, p < 0.001). This implies that higher responsible FinB (as gauged by credit aversion, informed product choice, active saving, spending restraint, informed decision-making, planning, monitoring, and daily ease) is associated with a better objective financial situation, which, in turn, is related to higher perceived FWB. However, there are still certain aspects of responsible FinB that are not reflected via objective financial situation, but they directly enhance perceived FWB (β = 0.122, t = 2.061, p = 0.039). While this finding is not surprising, it reinforces the existing knowledge of the vital link between responsible FinB and the objective financial situation, i.e., what we do/actions and the results. This result has an important implication for the overall well-being and FWB, specifically.

The empirical evidence provides that psychological factors, namely, high impulsivity and concern for social status have significant negative relation, whereas internal locus of control and self-control have a significant positive relationship with FWB. Further, internal locus of control emerged as the highest impacting factor followed by self-control. High impulsivity and short-term time orientation are inversely related to responsible FinB, whereas internal locus of control and social status are observed to impact responsible FinB positively. Furthermore, short-term time orientation emerges to be the highest impacting factor, followed by social status.

The financial literacy components that significantly influenced responsible behavior are financial confidence and financial awareness. These results support the previous literature that contends limited role of objective financial knowledge in explaining FinBs when compared with factors related to personal characteristics, such as confidence and other psychological traits.

The strength of this study can be concluded in its contribution to the literature by introducing multiple personal, financial literacy, and behavioral components in a single framework and subsequently applying them to a novel context, i.e., South Asian developing country (India). Furthermore, FWB is measured using both subjective and objective measures. Although a sincere attempt is made to analyze and report FWB in a holistic manner, the respondents of this study are from urban areas, leaving scope for further research on rural population or comparative research that could test whether geography plays a role in determining FinBs and FWB. Further, the objective financial situation score in this study is derived from the self-reported data of the respondents. Banks or financial institutions, in the capacity of having access to personal financial data, can conduct research that further enhances the understanding of the relationships by comparing the reported and actual data on the financial situation.

This study is an earnest attempt to contribute to the growing body of scientific literature in the field. However, there remains a scope for further improvement. This study is based on respondents from urban areas, and further research can be conducted on rural population. Also, comparative research that could test whether geography plays a role in determining FinBs and FWB can be conducted. Thus, the results of the study should be interpreted with caution before generalizing them, as they are based on a specific sample without conducting a power analysis for the model.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data (without respondents personal details) will be made available for academic use with due credit given to the authors.

Author Contributions

KS was involved in all the steps of the process and was the primary writer of the text. GT was involved in the questionnaire development, data collection and analysis. MV supervised in the research design and analysis as well as write up of the text. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.638408/full#supplementary-material

References

Abrantes-Braga, F. D. M., and Veludo-de-Oliveira, T. (2020). Help me, I can't afford it! Antecedents and consequence of risky indebtedness behaviour. Euro. J. Market. 54, 2223–2244. doi: 10.1108/EJM-06-2019-0455

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Achtziger, A., Hubert, M., Kenning, P., Raab, G., and Reisch, L. (2015). Debt out of control: the links between self-control, compulsive buying, and real debts. J. Econ. Psychol. 49, 141–149. doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2015.04.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Addin, M. M., Nayebzadeh, S., Taft, M. K., and Sadrabadi, M. M. M. (2013). Financial strategies and investigating the relationship among financial literacy, financial well-being, and financial worry. Euro. Online J. Nat. Soc. Sci. 2, 1279–1289. Available online at: http://european-science.com

Google Scholar

Ali, A., Abd Rahman, M. S., and Bakar, A. (2015). Financial satisfaction and the influence of financial literacy in Malaysia. Soc. Indic. Res. 120, 137–156. doi: 10.1007/s11205-014-0583-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allgood, S., and Walstad, W. B. (2016). The effects of perceived and actual financial literacy on financial behaviors. Econ. Inquiry 54, 675–697. doi: 10.1111/ecin.12255

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alliance for Financial Inclusion. (2016). Policy Frameworks to Support Women's Financial Inclusion. Retrieved from http://www.afi-global.org

Google Scholar

Alliance for Financial Inclusion. (2017). Integrating Gender And Women's Financial Inclusion Into National Strategies. Guideline Note No.27. Retrieved from www.afiglobal.org

Google Scholar

Angulo-Ruiz, F., and Pergelova, A. (2015). An empowerment model of youth financial behavior. J. Cons. Aff. 49, 550–575. doi: 10.1111/joca.12086

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antonides, G., De Groot, I. M., and Van Raaij, W. F. (2011). Mental budgeting and the management of household finance. J. Econ. Psychol. 32, 546–555. doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2011.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arya, S., Eckel, C., and Wichman, C. (2013). Anatomy of the credit score. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 95, 175–185. doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2011.05.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atia, C. (2012). Building a Financially Literate Society for Development: The CAMFED Campaign for Female Education Model. Research Report. Retrieved from http://www.uds.edu (accessed January 11, 2017).

Google Scholar

Attri, R. (2013). Spending & saving habits of youth in the city of Indore. Bauddhik J. Manage. 3, 8–15. Retrieved From: https://www.academia.edu/18748684/Spending_and_Saving_Habits_of_Yo

Google Scholar

Ballinger, T. P., Hudson, E., Karkoviata, L., and Wilcox, N. T. (2011). Saving behavior and cognitive abilities. Exp. Econ. 14, 349–374. doi: 10.1007/s10683-010-9271-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bannier, C. E., and Neubert, M. (2016). “Actual and perceived financial sophistication and wealth accumulation: the role of education and gender,” in Annual Conference 2016 (Augsburg): Demographic Change (No. 145593). Augsburg: Verein für Socialpolitik/German Economic Association. doi: 10.2139/ssrn.2714823

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F. (2002). Yielding to temptation: self-control failure, impulsive purchasing, and consumer behavior. J. Cons. Res. 28, 670–676. doi: 10.1086/338209

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Becker, J. M., Ringle, C. M., Sarstedt, M., and Völckner, F. (2015). How collinearity affects mixture regression results. Market. Lett. 26, 643–659. doi: 10.1007/s11002-014-9299-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benton, M., Meier, S., and Sprenger, C. (2007). Overborrowing and Undersaving: Lessons and Policy Implications From Behavioral Research. Federal Reserve Bank of Boston Paper, 07–04. Retrieved from https://core.ac.uk

Google Scholar

Bernheim, B. D., Ray, D., and Yeltekin, S. (2015). Poverty and self-control. Econometrica 83, 1877–1911. doi: 10.3982/ECTA11374

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biljanovska, N., and Palligkinis, S. (2018). Control thyself: self-control failure and household wealth. J. Bank. Fin. 92, 280–294. doi: 10.1016/j.jbankfin.2016.10.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blomgren, J., Maunula, N., and Hiilamo, H. (2017). Do debts lead to disability pension? Evidence from a 15-year follow-up of 54,000 Finnish men and women. J. Euro. Social Policy 27, 109–122. doi: 10.1177/0958928716676548

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brüggen, E. C., Hogreve, J., Holmlund, M., Kabadayi, S., and Löfgren, M. (2017). Financial well-being: a conceptualization and research agenda. J. Bus. Res. 79, 228–237. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2017.03.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brysbaert, M. (2019). How many participants do we have to include in properly powered experiments? A tutorial of power analysis with reference tables. J. Cogn. 2:16. doi: 10.5334/joc.72

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgess, S. M., and Steenkamp, J. B. E. (2006). Marketing renaissance: how research in emerging markets advances marketing science and practice. Int. J. Res. Market. 23, 337–356. doi: 10.1016/j.ijresmar.2006.08.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castro-González, S., Fernández-López, S., Rey-Ares, L., and Rodeiro-Pazos, D. (2020). The influence of attitude to money on individuals' financial well-being. Soc. Indic. Res. 148, 747–764. doi: 10.1007/s11205-019-02219-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

CFPB (2015). Financial Well-Being: The Goal of Financial Education. Washington, DC: Consumer Financial Protection Bureau. Retrieved from consumerfinance.gov/data-research/research-reports/financial-well-being/

Google Scholar

CFPB (2017). Financial Well-Being in America. Washington, DC: Consumer Financial Protection Bureau. Retrieved from https://files.consumerfinance.gov

Google Scholar

CFPB (2018). Understanding the Pathways to Financial Well-Being. Washington, DC: Consumer Financial Protection Bureau. Retrieved from https://www.consumerfinance.gov

Google Scholar

Chan, M. K. (2017). Welfare dependence and self-control: an empirical analysis. Rev. Econ. Stud. 84, 1379–1423. doi: 10.1093/restud/rdx011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, P., and Vazsonyi, A. T. (2011). Future orientation, impulsivity, and problem behaviors: a longitudinal moderation model. Dev. Psychol. 47:1633. doi: 10.1037/a0025327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chin, W. W. (1998). Commentary: issues and opinion on structural equation modeling. MIS Q. 19, 7–16

Google Scholar

Clark, A. E., and Senik, C. (2010). Who compares to whom? The anatomy of income comparisons in Europe. Econ. J. 120, 573–594. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0297.2010.02359.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cobb-Clark, D. A., Kassenboehmer, S. C., and Sinning, M. G. (2016). Locus of control and savings. J. Bank. Fin. 73, 113–130. doi: 10.1016/j.jbankfin.2016.06.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd Edn. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.

Google Scholar

Collins, M. J., and Urban, C. (2020). Measuring financial well-being over the lifecourse. Euro. J. Fin. 26, 341–359. doi: 10.1080/1351847X.2019.1682631

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Comerton-Forde, C., Ip, E., Ribar, D. C., Ross, J., Salamanca, N., and Tsiaplias, S. (2018). Using survey and banking data to measure financial wellbeing. Commonwealth Bank of Australia and Melbourne Institute Financial Wellbeing Scales Technical Report. Melbourne Institute: Applied Economic and Social Research, The University of Melbourne, Victoria.

Google Scholar

Courchane, M., and Zorn, P. (2005). “Consumer literacy and credit worthiness,” in Proceedings Federal Reserve Bank of Chicago (Washington, DC).

Google Scholar

Deacon, R. E., and Firebaugh, F. M. (1988). Family Resource Management: Principles and Applications. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Google Scholar

DeHart, W. B., Friedel, J. E., Lown, J. M., and Odum, A. L. (2016). The effects of financial education on impulsive decision making. PLoS ONE, 11:e0159561. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0159561

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delafrooz, N., and Paim, L. H. (2013). Role of financial stress on relationship between financial problem and financial wellness among Malaysia workers. Afr. J. Bus. Manage. 7, 1966–1972. doi: 10.5897/AJBM12.082

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duckworth, A. L., and Seligman, M. E. (2005). Self-discipline outdoes IQ in predicting academic performance of adolescents. Psychol. Sci. 16, 939–944. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01641.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duflo, E., and Saez, E. (2002). Participation and investment decisions in a retirement plan: the influence of colleagues' choices. J. Public Econ. 85, 121–148. doi: 10.1016/S0047-2727(01)00098-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, L. F., and Mirzaie, I. A. (2012). Determinants of Consumer Debt Stress: Differences by Debt Type and Gender. Columbus, OH: Department of Economics; Ohio State University.

Google Scholar

Farrell, L., Fry, T. R., and Risse, L. (2016). The significance of financial self-efficacy in explaining women's personal finance behaviour. J. Econ. Psychol. 54, 85-99. doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2015.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandes, D., Lynch, J. G. Jr., and Netemeyer, R. G. (2014). Financial literacy, financial education, and downstream financial behaviors. Manage. Sci. 60, 1861–1883. doi: 10.1287/mnsc.2013.1849

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Financial Consumer Agency of Canada (2018). Financial wellbeing in Canada ISBN:978-0-660-33029-7. Retrieved from https://www.canada.ca/en/financial-consumer-agency/programs/research/financial-well-being-survey-results.html

Google Scholar

Finney, A. (2016). Components of Financial Capability: Defining, Measuring and Predicting Scores in the UK POPULATION. London: Money Advice Service.

Google Scholar

FinScope Survey (2017) Final Questionnaire. Retrieved from https://www.fsdt.or.tz

Google Scholar

Fornell, C., and Cha, J. (1994). Partial least squares in Advanced Methods of Marketing Research, ed R. P Bagozzi (Cambridge, MA: Blackwell), 52–78

Google Scholar

Fornell, C., and Larcker, D. F. (1981). Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. J. Market. Res. 18, 39–50. doi: 10.1177/002224378101800104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frederiks, E. R., Stenner, K., and Hobman, E. V. (2015). Household energy use: applying behavioural economics to understand consumer decision-making and behaviour. Renew. Sust. Energy Rev. 41, 1385–1394. doi: 10.1016/j.rser.2014.09.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fujita, K., Trope, Y., Liberman, N., and Levin-Sagi, M. (2006). Construal levels and self-control. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 90, 351–367. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.3.351

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gathergood, J., and Weber, J. (2014). Self-control, financial literacy & the co-holding puzzle. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 107, 455–469. doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2014.04.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geisser, S. (1974). A predictive approach to the random effect model. Biometrika 61, 101–107 doi: 10.1093/biomet/61.1.101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerhard, P., Gladstone, J. J., and Hoffmann, A. O. (2018). Psychological characteristics and household savings behavior: the importance of accounting for latent heterogeneity. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 148, 66–82. doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2018.02.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grable, J. E., Cupples, S., Fernatt, F., and Anderson, N. (2012). Evaluating the link between perceived income adequacy and financial satisfaction: a resource deficit hypothesis approach. Soc. Indic. Res. 114, 1109–1124. doi: 10.1007/s11205-012-0192-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hair, J. F., Hult, G. T. M., Ringle, C. M., and Sarstedt, M. (2017). A Primer on Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Google Scholar

Hair, J. F. Jr., Black William, C, Babin Barry, J, Anderson Rolph, E., and Tatham Ronald, L. (2006). Multivariate Data Analysis, 6th Edn. New Jersey: Pearson Prentice Hall, Pearson Education, Inc.

Google Scholar

Hastings, J., Mitchell, O. S., and Chyn, E. (2011). “Fees, framing, and financial literacy in the choice of pension manager,” in Financial Literacy: Implications for Retirement Security and the Financial Marketplace, (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 101–115. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199696819.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, D., Jamie, E., and Andrea, F. (2016). Momentum Household Financial Wellness Index: Wave One. Retrieved from http://www.bristol.ac.uk

Google Scholar

Heineck, G., and Anger, S. (2010). The returns to cognitive abilities and personality traits in Germany. Lab. Econ. 17, 535–546. doi: 10.1016/j.labeco.2009.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hellrigel, D., Slocum, J. W., and Woodman, R. W. (2010). Organizational Behaviour. Mason: South Western Cengage Learning.

Google Scholar

Henseler, J., and Chin, W. W. (2010). A comparison of approaches for the analysis of interaction effects between latent variables using partial least squares path modeling. Struct. Equat. Model. 17, 82–109. doi: 10.1080/10705510903439003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henseler, J., Ringle, C. M., and Sarstedt, M. (2015). A new criterion for assessing discriminant validity in variance-based structural equation modeling. J. Acad. Market. Sci. 43, 115–135. doi: 10.1007/s11747-014-0403-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henseler, J., Ringle, C. M., and Sinkovics, R. R. (2009). “The use of partial least squares path modeling in international marketing,” in New Challenges to International Marketing (Advances in International Marketing, Vol. 20), eds R. R. Sinkovics, and P. N. Ghauri (Bingley: Emerald Group Publishing Limited), 277–319. doi: 10.1108/S1474-7979(2009)0000020014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hershey, D. A., Henkens, K., and Van Dalen, H. P. (2010). Aging and financial planning for retirement: interdisciplinary influences viewed through a cross-cultural lens. Int. J. Aging Hum. Dev. 70, 1–38. doi: 10.2190/AG.70.1.a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffman, D. L., Novak, T. P., and Schlosser, A. E. (2003). Locus of control, web use, and consumer attitudes toward internet regulation. J. Public Policy Market. 22, 41–57. doi: 10.1509/jppm.22.1.41.17628

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hojman, D. A., Miranda, Á., and Ruiz-Tagle, J. (2016). Debt trajectories and mental health. Soc. Sci. Med. 167, 54–62. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.08.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howlett, E., Kees, J., and Kemp, E. (2008). The role of self-regulation, future orientation, and financial knowledge in long-term financial decisions. J. Consum. Aff. 42, 223–242. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6606.2008.00106.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huston, S. J. (2010). Measuring financial literacy. J. Cons. Aff. 44, 296–316. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6606.2010.01170.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jack, W., and Suri, T. (2014). Risk sharing and transactions costs: evidence from Kenya's mobile money revolution. Am. Econ. Rev. 104, 183–223. doi: 10.1257/aer.104.1.183

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobs-Lawson, J. M., and Hershey, D. A. (2005). Influence of future time perspective, financial knowledge, and financial risk tolerance on retirement saving behaviors. Fin. Serv. Rev. Greenwich 14, 331–344. Retrieved From: https://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.392.5839&rep=rep1&type=pdf

Google Scholar

Kempson, E., and Finney A Poppe, C. (2017). Financial Well-Being: A Conceptual Model and Preliminary Analysis. SIFO Project Note no. 3-2017. Oslo: Oslo and Akershus University College of Applied Sciences. Available online at: www.bristol.ac.uk

Google Scholar

Kempson, E., Perotti, V., and Scott, K. (2013) Measuring Financial Capability: A New Instrument Results From Low- Middle-Income Countries. Washington DC: World Bank.

Google Scholar

Kempson, E., and Poppe, C. (2018). Understanding Financial Well-Being and Capability. A Revised Model and Comprehensive Analysis. Oslo: Consumption Research Norway (SIFO), Oslo Metropolitan University.

Google Scholar

Kim, J., and Garman, E. T. (2003). Financial stress and absenteeism: an empirically derived model. Fin. Couns. Plan. 14(1), 31–42.

Google Scholar

Kiviat, B., and Morduch, J. (2012). From Financial Literacy to Financial Action. Financial Access Initiative, Robert F. Wagner Graduate School of Public Service. New York University and McGraw-Hill Research Foundation. Available online at: http://www.mhfigi.com/wp-content/~uploads/2013/08/Financial_Literacy_WP.pdf

Google Scholar

Klapper, L., Lusardi, A., and Van Oudheusden, P. (2014). Financial Literacy Around the World. Standard & Poor's Ratings Services Global Financial Literacy Survey. Washington, DC: Standard & Poor's. Retrieved from www.openfininc.org

Google Scholar

Knowles, J. (2013). Increasing the Productivity and Earnings of Rural Women: What Works? A Roadmap for Promoting Women's Economic Empowerment. Exxon Mobil, UN Foundation. Retrieved from www.womeneconroadmap.org

Google Scholar

Kooij, D. T. A. M., Kanfer, R., Betts, M., and Rudolph, C. W. (2018). Future time perspective: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Appl. Psychol. 103, 867–893. doi: 10.1037/apl0000306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, E., and Hanna, S. D. (2014). Gender differences of Asian college students' financial knowledge pathways. Asian Women 30, 27–55. doi: 10.14431/aw.2014.03.30.2.27

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Social cognitive neuroscience: a review of core processes. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58, 259–289. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085654

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loke, Y. J. (2017). The influence of socio-demographic and financial knowledge factors on financial management practices of Malaysians. Int. J. Bus. Soc. 18, 33–50. doi: 10.33736/ijbs.488.2017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lowry, P. B., and Gaskin, J. (2014). Partial least squares (PLS) structural equation modeling (SEM) for building and testing behavioral causal theory: when to choose it and how to use it. IEEE Trans. Prof. Commun. 57, 123–146. doi: 10.1109/TPC.2014.2312452

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lusardi, A., and Mitchell, O. S. (2011a). Financial literacy around the world: an overview. J. Pension Econ. Finan. 10, 497–508. doi: 10.1017/S1474747211000448

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lusardi, A., and Mitchell, O. S. (2014). The economic importance of financial literacy: theory and evidence. J. Econ. Lit. 52, 5–44. doi: 10.1257/jel.52.1.5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lusardi, A., Mitchell, O. S., and Curto, V. (2010). Financial literacy among the young. J. Cons. Aff. 44, 358–380. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6606.2010.01173.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyons, A. C. (2008). “Risky credit card behavior of college students,” in Handbook of Consumer Finance Research, editor J. J. Xiao (New York, NY: Springer), 185–208. doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-75734-6_11

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahdzan, N. S., Zainudin, R., Sukor, M. E. A., Zainir, F., and Ahmad, W. M. W. (2019). Determinants of subjective financial well-being across three different household income groups in Malaysia. Soc. Indic. Res. 146, 699–726. doi: 10.1007/s11205-019-02138-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masche, J. G. (2010). Explanation of normative declines in parents' knowledge about their adolescent children. J. Adolesc. 33, 271–284. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2009.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mien, N. T. N., and Thao, T. P. (2015). “Factors affecting personal financial management behaviors: evidence from vietnam,” in Proceedings of the Second Asia-Pacific Conference on Global Business, Economics, Finance and Social Sciences (AP15Vietnam Conference), 10-12/07/2015 (Danang).

Google Scholar

Miotto, A. P. S., and Parente, J. (2015). Antecedents and consequences of household financial management in Brazilian lower-middle-class. Rev. Admin. Empres. 55, 50–64. doi: 10.1590/S0034-759020150106

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mokhtar, N., and Husniyah, A. R. (2017). Determinants of financial well-being among public employees in Putrajaya, Malaysia. Pertanika J. Soc. Sci. Hum. 25, 1241–1260.

Google Scholar

Money Advice Service (2015). Financial Capability and Wellbeing. Available online at: https://masjumpprdstorage.blob.core.windows.net/cms-production/financial-capability-and-wellbeing.pdf

Google Scholar

Morgan, L. A., and Eckert, J. K. (2004). Retirement financial preparation: implications for policy. J. Aging Soc. Policy 16, 19–34. doi: 10.1300/J031v16n02_02

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Netemeyer, R. G., Warmath, D., Fernandes, D., and Lynch, J. G. Jr. (2018). How am I doing? Perceived financial well-being, its potential antecedents, and its relation to overall well-being. J. Cons. Res. 45, 68–89. doi: 10.1093/jcr/ucx109

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norvilitis, J., and Mendes-Da-Silva, W. (2013). Attitudes toward credit and finances among college students in Brazil and the United States. J. Bus. Theory Pract. 1, 132–151. doi: 10.22158/jbtp.v1n1p132

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norvilitis, J. M., and Mao, Y. (2013). Attitudes towards credit and finances among college students in China and the United States. Int. J. Psychol. 48, 389–398. doi: 10.1080/00207594.2011.645486

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norvilitis, J. M., Szablicki, P. B., and Wilson, S. D. (2003). Factors influencing levels of credit-card debt in college students 1. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 33, 935–947. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2003.tb01932.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

OECD (2005). Improving Financial Literacy: Analysis of Issues and Policies. Paris: OECD. doi: 10.1787/fmt-v2005-art11-en

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

OECD (2013). OECD/INFE Set of Criteria, Principles, Guidelines and Policy Guidance to Improve Financial Education: Part 3 Measurement and Evaluation Tools. Paris: OECD.

Google Scholar

OECD (2016). OECD/INFE International Survey of Adult Financial Literacy Competencies. Paris: OECD. Retrieved from www.oecd.org

Google Scholar

Perry, V. G., and Morris, M. D. (2005). Who is in control? The role of self-perception, knowledge, and income in explaining consumer financial behavior. J. Cons. Aff. 39, 299–313. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6606.2005.00016.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prawitz, A. D., and Cohart, J. (2016). Financial management competency, financial resources, locus of control, and financial wellness. J. Fin. Couns. Plan. 27, 142–157. doi: 10.1891/1052-3073.27.2.142

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prendergast, S., Blackmore, D., Kempson, E., Russell, R., and Kutin, J. (2018a). Financial Well-Being: A Survey of Adults in Australia. Retrieved from: https://www.anz.com/resources/2/f/2f348500-38a2-4cfe-8411-060cb753573d/financial-wellbeing-aus18.pdf?~MOD=AJPERES

Google Scholar

Prendergast, S., Blackmore, D., Kempson, E., Russell, R., and Kutin, J. (2018b). Financial Well-Being: A Survey of Adults in New Zealand. Retrieved from https://bluenotes.anz. com/content/dam/bluenotes/images/financial-wellbeing/ANZ%20Financial%20Wellbeing %20Summary%20Report%20-%20New%20Zealand.pdf

Google Scholar

Rahman, M., and Gan, S. S. (2020). Generation Y investment decision: an analysis using behavioural factors. Manage. Fin. 46, 1023–1041. doi: 10.1108/MF-10-2018-0534

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raithel, S., Sarstedt, M., Scharf, S., and Schwaiger, M. (2012). On the value relevance of customer satisfaction. Multiple drivers and multiple markets. J. Acad. Market. Sci. 40, 509–525. doi: 10.1007/s11747-011-0247-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramalho, T. B., and Forte, D. (2019). Financial literacy in Brazil–do knowledge and self-confidence relate with behavior? RAUSP Manage. J. 54, 77–95. doi: 10.1108/RAUSP-04-2018-0008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rao, A., and Barber, B. L. (2005). Financial well-being: descriptors and pathways. Take Charge America Institute (TCAI) Working Paper-5-2. Take Charge America Institute for Consumer Financial Education and Research, Tucson, AZ.

Google Scholar

Raue, M., D'Ambrosio, L. A., and Coughlin, J. F. (2020). The power of peers: Prompting savings behavior through social comparison. J. Behav. Fin. 21, 1–13. doi: 10.1080/15427560.2019.1587762

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riitsalu, L., and Murakas, R. (2019). Subjective financial knowledge, prudent behaviour and income. Int. J. Bank Market. 37, 934–950. doi: 10.1108/IJBM-03-2018-0071

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychol. Monogra. Gen. Appl. 80, 1–28. doi: 10.1037/h0092976

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rowley, M., Lown, J. M., and Piercy, K. (2012). Motivating women to adopt positive financial behaviors. J. Fin. Couns. Plan. 23, 48. Retrieved From: https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2220862

Google Scholar

Rutledge, D., and Deshpande, S. (2015). “The influence of time orientation on personal finance behaviours,” in Ideas in Marketing: Finding the New and Polishing the Old (Cham: Springer), 67–76. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-10951-0_32

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sabri, M. F., and Zakaria, N. F. (2015). “Financial well-being among young employees in Malaysia,” in Handbook of Research on Behavioral Finance and Investment Strategies: Decision Making in the Financial Industry (IGI Global), 221–235. doi: 10.4018/978-1-4666-7484-4.ch013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salamanca, N., de Grip, A., Fouarge, D., and Montizaan, R. (2020). Locus of control and investment in risky assets. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 177, 548–568. doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2020.06.032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaner, S. (2017). The cost of convenience? Transaction costs, bargaining power, and savings account use in Kenya. J. Hum. Res. 52, 919–945. doi: 10.3368/jhr.52.4.0815-7350R1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serido, J., Shim, S., and Tang, C. (2013). A developmental model of financial capability: a framework for promoting a successful transition to adulthood. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 37, 287–297. doi: 10.1177/0165025413479476

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shepard, J., and Turner, T. (2019). Future orientation as a universal feature of wellbeing: leveraging big data for cross-cultural research. Sci. Reli. Cult. 6, 138–145. doi: 10.17582/journal.src/2019.6.1.138.145

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shim, S., Xiao, J. J., Barber, B. L., and Lyons, A. C. (2009). Pathways to life success: a conceptual model of financial well-being for young adults. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 30, 708–723. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2009.02.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sivaramakrishnan, S., Srivastava, M., and Rastogi, A. (2017). Attitudinal factors, financial literacy, and stock market participation. Int. J. Bank Market. 35, 818–841. doi: 10.1108/IJBM-01-2016-0012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Dev. Rev. 28, 78–106. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stone, M. (1974). Cross-validatory choice and assessment of statistical predictions. J. R. Stat. Soc. 36, 111–147. doi: 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1974.tb00994.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strömbäck, C., Lind, T., Skagerlund, K., Västfjäll, D., and Tinghög, G. (2017). Does self-control predict financial behavior and financial well-being? J. Behav. Exp. Fin. 14, 30–38. doi: 10.1016/j.jbef.2017.04.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stumm, S., O'Creevy, M. F., and Furnham, A. (2013). Financial capability, money attitudes and socioeconomic status: risks for experiencing adverse financial events. Pers. Indivi. Diff. 54, 344–349. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.09.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sundarasen, S. D. D., Rahman, M. S., Othman, N. S., and Danaraj, J. (2016). Impact of financial literacy, financial socialization agents, and parental norms on money management. J. Bus. Stud. Q. 8, 137.

Google Scholar

Van Praag, B. M., Frijters, P., and Ferrer-i-Carbonell, A. (2003). The anatomy of subjective well-being. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 51, 29–49. doi: 10.1016/S0167-2681(02)00140-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vlaev, I., and Elliott, A. (2017). “Defining and influencing financial capability,” in Economic Psychology, ed R. Ranyard (John Wiley and Sons), 187–205. doi: 10.1002/9781118926352.ch12

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vohs, K. D., Baumeister, R. F., and Schmeichel, B. J. (2012). Motivation, personal beliefs, and limited resources all contribute to self-control. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 48, 943–947. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.03.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vyvyan, V., Blue, L., and Brimble, M. (2014). Factors that influence financial capability and effectiveness: exploring financial counsellors' perspectives. Austra. Account. Bus. Fin. J. 8, 3–22. doi: 10.14453/aabfj.v8i4.2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Bank (2013). Financial Capability Surveys Around the World: Why Financial Capability is Important and How Surveys Can Help (No. 80767). The World Bank.

Google Scholar

Xiao, J. J., Chen, C., and Chen, F. (2014). Consumer financial capability and financial satisfaction. Soc. Indicators Res. 118, 415–432. doi: 10.1007/s11205-013-0414-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiao, J. J., and Dew, J. (2011). The financial management behavior scale: development and validation. J.Fin. Couns. Plan. 22, 43–59.

Google Scholar

Xiao, J. J., and Porto, N. (2017). Financial education and financial satisfaction. Int. J. Bank Market. 35, 805–817. doi: 10.1108/IJBM-01-2016-0009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zulaihati, S., Susanti, S., and Widyastuti, U. (2020). Teachers' financial literacy: does it impact on financial behaviour? Manage. Sci. Lett. 10, 653–658. doi: 10.5267/j.msl.2019.9.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zyphur, M. J., Li, W. D., Zhang, Z., Arvey, R. D., and Barsky, A. P. (2015). Income, personality, and subjective financial well-being: the role of gender in their genetic and environmental relationships. Front. Psychol. 6:1493. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01493

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: financial well-being, responsible financial behaviors, financial literacy, psychological factors, India

Citation: Sehrawat K, Vij M and Talan G (2021) Understanding the Path Toward Financial Well-Being: Evidence From India. Front. Psychol. 12:638408. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.638408

Received: 06 December 2020; Accepted: 09 June 2021;
Published: 21 July 2021.

Edited by:

Kamlesh Singh, Indian Institute of Technology, India

Reviewed by:

Pasquale Dolce, University of Naples Federico II, Italy
Andrea P. Malizia, IMT School for Advanced Studies Lucca, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Sehrawat, Vij and Talan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Gaurav Talan, gauravtalan@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.