
94% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.
Find out more
ORIGINAL RESEARCH article
Front. Psychol. , 28 February 2025
Sec. Organizational Psychology
Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1538914
This study examines the daily relations among workplace anger, coping strategies, work outcomes, and employee dispositions using a conceptual framework based on affective events theory and cognitive perspectives on emotions. A sample of 214 full-time employees took part in a two-week study, contributing 1,611 daily observations through an experience sampling approach. Contrary to the assumption that workplace anger always detrimentally relates to work outcomes, the results showed a nonsignificant relation between workplace anger and workplace resource depletion, as well as a positive link between workplace anger and goal achievement. These relations were dependent on the coping strategies used by employees in response to anger-inducing situations, as well as their attitudes toward workplace affiliation. These findings suggest the need to expand affective events theory to include coping strategies as a mediator between affective responses and work outcomes. They also highlight the importance of integrating employee-level factors into organizational research models.
Facilitators of workplace resource depletion, defined as a psychosomatic state characterized by diminished working memory and self-regulation abilities at the workplace (Baumeister et al., 1998), as well as threats to workplace goal attainment, defined as the daily progress toward or achievement of employees’ work-related goals (Sheldon and Elliot, 1999), pose risks to the sustainable functioning of organizations and the societal system (Mohr, 1973; Sekaran and Snodgrass, 1989). Therefore, it is essential for organizations to address factors that could either contribute to or impede these work outcomes (Godkin and Allcorn, 2008; Morrison and Milliken, 2000). One such factor could be workplace anger. Workplace anger refers to an arousing negative emotion experienced when someone has wronged either oneself or those close to oneself (Kant et al., 2013; Lazarus, 1991; Schwarzmüller et al., 2016). It has often been perceived as the opposite of rationality in organizations (Ashkanasy and Dorris, 2017; see also Pham, 2007) and is adversely associated with various work outcomes (Booth et al., 2017; Callister et al., 2017; Gibson et al., 2009). However, it is worth questioning whether the assumption that workplace anger and work outcomes are always adversely related.
Historically, workplace anger has been adversely associated with work outcomes (see Jäger et al., 2017; Wong et al., 2017; but see also Schmitt et al., 2019), a concept that is supported by organizational behavior theories. Affective events theory (Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996), for instance, suggests that negative emotions, such as anger, can facilitate disadvantageous and impede advantageous work outcomes. This theoretical perspective is further supported by empirical research, which consistently shows an adverse relation between negative emotions and work outcomes (Wong et al., 2017). Nevertheless, it is important to consider potential limitations in theoretical frameworks and empirical studies, which may warrant further exploration.
In exploring the limitations of current conceptual models in understanding the relation between workplace anger and work outcomes, it is evident that a significant gap exists in the lack of consideration of anger’s coping strategies, such as ruminative (excessive internal pondering about an anger-inducing situation; Li et al., 2019) and confrontative coping (openly and antagonistically addressing an anger-inducing situation; Folkman et al., 1986). While affective events theory and empirical research have laid valuable foundations for the organizational sciences, they seem to often miss to address the role of these and other coping strategies in managing emotions in the workplace (e.g., Carlson et al., 2011; Glasø et al., 2010; Wegge et al., 2006). This oversight becomes particularly striking when compared to alternative frameworks, such as cognitivist accounts of emotion (Lazarus, 1991; Frijda, 1987; Moors et al., 2013), which highlight the importance of coping mechanisms in daily working life. It would thus seem reasonable to assume that how individuals cope with anger at work could intersect the relation between their anger and their work outcomes. However, further research is needed to validate this assumption and determine if coping strategies indeed play a substantial role in the connection between workplace anger and work outcomes. Furthermore, while existing research has focused on the influence of organization-and supervisor-level factors on the relation between emotions and work outcomes, there is a noticeable gap in the empirical literature when it comes to examining employee-level constructs, such as dispositions (cf. Glasø et al., 2010; Wegge et al., 2006), within the framework of affective events theory (Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996). This lack of research is significant because alternative theoretical models rooted in cognitivist accounts of emotions also highlight the importance of individual differences in understanding emotions in the workplace. As such, a key question arises regarding whether the relation between workplace anger and work outcomes is consistent for all employees or indeed varies based on their unique dispositions. For instance, an individual’s workplace affiliation disposition, which pertains to their inherent desire to be part of a team or group at work (Yagil and Medler-Liraz, 2017), could play a critical role in how they cope with workplace anger and, subsequently, relate to their work outcomes. In light of this, it is crucial to delve deeper into such employee-level factors within appraisal theory to gain a comprehensive understanding of how it impacts the relations between workplace anger and work outcomes.
As such, our primary objective in this manuscript is to examine the intricate relations between workplace anger and work outcomes, taking into account the potential role of coping strategies and individual differences. To achieve this objective, we have devised a conceptual model based on affective events theory (Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996) and enhancements from cognitivist accounts of emotion (Lazarus, 1991; Frijda, 1987; Moors et al., 2013). Our conceptual model assumes that workplace anger can be disadvantageously associated with work outcomes, but also considers the possibility that this relation may be advantageous under certain circumstances. These circumstances include the coping strategies employed to address workplace anger and the individual differences that may exist among employees. To test our conceptual model, we will gather data from a sample of full-time employees across various industries over a two-week period. Data will be collected each workday using a time-lagged experience-sampling methodology.
Through our current work, we aim to make a significant contribution to the existing literature in three key areas. Firstly, we seek to investigate the assumption of affective events theory (Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996) and cognitivist accounts of emotion (Lazarus, 1991; Frijda, 1987; Moors et al., 2013) regarding the relation between workplace anger and work outcomes, such as work-related resource depletion. While both theories suggest a negative link between workplace anger and resource depletion, empirical evidence supporting this claim is still lacking. By delving into affective events theory and cognitivist accounts of emotions, we aim to clarify the true nature of this relation in work environments. Therefore, our objective is to test the validity of these theories in explaining the connections between workplace anger and resource depletion.
Moreover, we aim to make a theoretical contribution by examining the differing assumptions about the relation between workplace anger and goal attainment in affective events theory (Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996) and cognitivist accounts of emotions (Lazarus, 1991; Frijda, 1987; Moors et al., 2013). We suggest that the discrepancy in these assumptions may stem from the lack of emphasis on coping strategies in affective events theory compared to cognitivist accounts of emotions, which prioritize coping strategies. Our hypothesis is that coping strategies associated with workplace anger can significantly influence the relation between workplace anger and goal attainment. By incorporating coping strategies such as ruminative coping (excessive internal pondering about an anger-inducing situation; Li et al., 2019) and confrontative coping (openly and antagonistically addressing an anger-inducing situation; Folkman et al., 1986) into our conceptual model, we aim to explore how these strategies may impact the relation between workplace anger and goal attainment.
Finally, our study aims to examine the impact of individual differences on the relation between workplace anger and work outcomes. Previous research has overlooked employee-level factors within the context of affective events theory. To address this gap, we include the core human disposition of workplace affiliation in our conceptual model (Ryan and Vansteenkiste, 2023). This disposition is in line with the interaction-oriented theoretical frameworks of affective events theory and cognitivist accounts, suggesting that it may influence the interconnectedness of workplace anger, coping strategies, and work outcomes. By investigating employee-level factors through the lens of affective events theory, we strive to gain a better understanding of the importance of individual differences within this theoretical framework.
Affective events theory is an affect-centered theory from the organizational sciences that focuses on the relation between emotions arising from workplace situations and the work outcomes that are expected to result from these emotions (Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996). According to the theory, emotions and work outcomes typically have similar valences. Positive emotions are likely to lead to positive work outcomes, while negative emotions are likely to lead to negative work outcomes. The theory also suggests that employee-level factors, such as dispositions, are important in understanding this relation. However, the specific impact of dispositional factors on the relation between emotions and work outcomes has not been extensively researched and remains a theoretical assumption at this time. It is worth noting that affective events theory does not expressively address the role of coping mechanisms within its theoretical framework.
Cognitivist accounts of emotions challenge the notion of affective events theory that emotions and work outcomes always align in terms of valence. Cognitivist accounts propose that an employee’s coping strategies mediate the valence of an emotion onto the valence of a workplace outcome (Frijda, 1987; Lazarus, 1991; Moors et al., 2013). Two main coping strategies typically available to employees in such situations are emotion-focused coping and problem-focused coping (Lazarus, 1991).
Emotion-focused coping involves managing one’s emotions through internal thought regulation processes, while problem-focused coping entails actively addressing the situation itself (Lazarus, 1991). Cognitivist accounts of emotions suggest that regardless of the initial emotion triggered by the situation, emotion-focused coping tends to have a negative mediating effect on the relation between emotions and work outcomes (Folkman et al., 1986; Lazarus, 1991; Roseman, 2013). In contrast, problem-focused coping is believed to have a positive mediating effect on this relation. Empirical evidence seems to support these assumptions regarding the mediating role of coping strategies in the relation between emotions and work outcomes (Baker and Berenbaum, 2007; Boyd et al., 2009; Carver, 2006).
Cognitivist accounts of emotion align with affective events theory in recognizing the significance of individual differences (Lazarus, 1991; Frijda, 1987; Moors et al., 2013), such as dispositions, in the link between emotions and work outcomes. However, there is a difference in perspective between the two theories. While affective events theory suggests that dispositions directly impact the relation between emotions and work outcomes (Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996), cognitivist accounts propose that dispositions impact this relation indirectly (Lazarus, 1991; Frijda, 1987; Moors et al., 2013).
According to cognitivist accounts of emotion, dispositions affect the mediation path between emotions and coping strategies of employees, as well as the mediation path between coping strategies and work outcomes (Lazarus, 1991; Frijda, 1987; Moors et al., 2013). In contrast, affective events theory suggests that individual differences impact the direct relation between emotions and work outcomes (Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996). This means that in cognitivist accounts, the connections between emotions and coping strategies, as well as coping strategies and work outcomes, are moderated by dispositions. On the other hand, affective events theory maintains that only the direct link between emotions and work outcomes is moderated by dispositions (Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996).
In summary, affective events theory and cognitivist accounts of emotion offer different theoretical assumptions when it comes to the relation between emotions and work outcomes. Affective events theory suggests a same-valenced and direct link between emotions and work outcomes, with employee dispositions playing a significant role in shaping this direct relation (Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996). On the other hand, cognitivist accounts of emotion propose that coping strategies play a mediator role in concluding the relation between emotions and work outcomes, with employee dispositions presumably also impacting this relation (Lazarus, 1991; Frijda, 1987; Moors et al., 2013). We will now delve deeper into these discrepancies and their possible resolutions, following the procedural structure of our conceptual model depicted in Figure 1.
In light of the conflicting perspectives presented by different theoretical frameworks, it is crucial to establish which framework better aligns with evidence regarding their assumptions. One emotion that holds significance in workplace settings and may provide insight in this regard is anger. Anger (an arousing negative emotion that is experienced when someone has wronged either oneself or those close to oneself; Lazarus, 1991) seems to be a particularly fitting emotion for our investigation because it is commonly viewed as a negative emotion (Lazarus, 1991; Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996), suggesting, according to affective events theory (Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996), that it should have a detrimental impact on work outcomes. However, from a cognitivist perspective (Lazarus, 1991; Frijda, 1987; Moors et al., 2013), anger is also seen as an adaptive emotion due to its coping strategies, implying a potentially positive relation with work outcomes. Therefore, in this manuscript, our focus will be on exploring the relation between anger in the workplace (referred to as workplace anger) and work outcomes.
Building on the argument above, it is important to identify specific work outcomes that we can use to test our two theoretical frameworks. Workplace resource depletion, a psychosomatic state characterized by diminished working memory and self-regulation abilities at the workplace (Baumeister et al., 1998), and workplace goal attainment, the progress toward or achievement of work-related goals (Sheldon and Elliot, 1999), seem to stand out as suitable work outcomes for our analysis. Not only are they crucially important for organizations (Kiresuk et al., 2014; Mohr, 1973; Sekaran and Snodgrass, 1989), but they also highlight a significant discrepancy between our two theoretical perspectives in terms of their relation to workplace anger.
According to affective events theory (Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996), workplace resource depletion, as a negative work outcome, should be positively related to anger, which is considered a negative emotion. Cognitivist perspectives of emotion seem to support this view, as all emotions and coping strategies must, by their nature, be related to higher resource depletion (Lazarus, 1991; Vohs and Baumeister, 2011; Vohs and Heatherton, 2000).
However, this intertheoretical alignment disappears when we consider workplace goal attainment. This positive work outcome should be inversely related to anger according to affective events theory (Baumeister et al., 1998; Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996). In contrast, cognitivist perspectives on emotion (Lazarus, 1991; Frijda, 1987; Moors et al., 2013) suggest that the relation between workplace anger and goal attainment could be either negative or positive, depending on the employee’s coping strategies.
Given these intertheoretical alignments and discrepancies, our study will focus on exploring the connections between workplace anger and workplace resource depletion and goal attainment, due to its theoretical and practical relevance. With this focus in mind, we will now delve into the reasons why such intertheoretical discrepancies might exist and propose our hypotheses accordingly.
We have established that the anticipated results may differ when viewed through competing theoretical frameworks. Specifically, we argue that the variation in these results can be attributed to the differing emphasis placed on coping strategies in affective events theory (Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996) compared to cognitivist theories of emotions (Frijda, 1987; Lazarus, 1991; Moors et al., 2013). While cognitivist theories highlight coping strategies as crucial elements, affective events theory tends to downplay their importance (Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996). Therefore, our assumption suggests that coping strategies associated with workplace anger may significantly impact the direction of the relation between workplace anger and workplace resource depletion, as well as goal attainment. In essence, coping strategies could determine whether this connection is positive or negative. To further explore this hypothesis, we will examine the potential role of coping strategies in shaping the link between workplace anger and workplace resource depletion, as well as goal attainment.
According to cognitivist theories, employees can employ either emotion-focused coping or problem-focused coping strategies to handle situations that trigger emotions in the workplace (Lazarus, 1991). Emotion-focused coping involves internally regulating emotions, while problem-focused coping involves taking action to address the situation that caused the emotion. Emotion-focused coping strategies related to anger often involve excessive internal rumination about the anger-inducing situation, known as ruminative coping (Folkman et al., 1986; Folkman and Lazarus, 1985; Lazarus, 1991). On the other hand, problem-focused coping strategies associated with anger typically involve directly confronting the anger-inducing situation, referred to as confrontative coping. Based on this, we propose the following two hypotheses to be tested:
Hypothesis 1: There is a positive relation between workplace anger and (a) ruminative coping as well as (b) confrontative coping.
The relation between coping strategies, specifically ruminative and confrontative coping, and workplace resource depletion is well-established, as there is consensus across various theories that any type of regulation, whether directed toward others or oneself, depletes internal resources (Baumeister et al., 1998; Lazarus, 1991; Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996). Therefore, we hypothesize that both primary anger coping strategies, ruminative and confrontative coping, are positively related with workplace resource depletion. Though initially grounded in theory, empirical evidence supports the idea that these relations are as expected (see Keltikangas-Järvinen et al., 1996; Vohs and Heatherton, 2000). Therefore, we suggest testing the following hypotheses:
Hypothesis 2: There is a (a) positive relation between ruminative coping and workplace resource depletion as well as a (b) positive relation between confrontative coping and workplace resource depletion.
However, as mentioned previously, this theoretical alignment breaks down when examining the relation between coping strategies and workplace goal achievement. Specifically, cognitivist views suggest that, in general, using emotion-focused coping may impede goal achievement, while utilizing a problem-focused coping strategy can be advantageous to achieve goals (see Folkman et al., 1986; Lazarus, 1991; Roseman, 2013). This notion is supported by empirical evidence (Baker and Berenbaum, 2007; Boyd et al., 2009; Carver, 2006).
In our assertion, we propose that the relation between coping strategies and workplace goal attainment should follow a similar pattern. Therefore, it is crucial to explore the coping strategies associated with anger in the workplace. With this understanding, it is reasonable to hypothesize that ruminative coping may negatively relate to workplace goal attainment, while confrontative coping may have a positive relation.
The dichotomy between emotion-focused coping and confrontative coping lies in their abilities to change the anger-inducing situation (Lazarus, 1991; Lazarus and Folkman, 1984). Emotion-focused coping may change how an employee subjectively interprets a situation, but it does not objectively change the situation itself. In contrast, confrontative coping has the potential to directly change the anger-inducing situation. Unresolved anger-inducing situations can lead to employees expending cognitive resources on managing their emotions rather than focusing on workplace goals (see also Baumeister and Vohs, 2004; Vohs and Heatherton, 2000). Problem-focused coping that addresses or resolves the anger-inducing situation can free up cognitive resources for employees to focus on their goals.
Empirical evidence suggests that ruminative self-focus, which could be regarded as a characteristic of ruminative coping (see Frijda, 1987), is negatively related to goal attainment (Moberly and Watkins, 2009), while approach behavior, a component of confrontative coping (Lazarus, 1991), is positively associated with goal attainment (Brodscholl et al., 2007). As such, along with our theoretical rationale, we propose the following hypotheses:
Hypothesis 3: There is a (a) negative relation between ruminative coping and workplace goal attainment as well as a (b) positive relation between confrontative coping and workplace goal attainment.
So far, we have examined the assumed direct relations between our theoretical constructs. However, it is important to note that both affective events theory and cognitive accounts of emotions suggest a procedural nature of these relations and constructs. As our main focus in this paper is to determine if the relation between workplace anger and work outcomes is consistently positive when taking coping strategies into account, it is important to investigate this in a more detailed manner. Therefore, we plan to explore the indirect effects between workplace anger and work outcomes through the use of coping strategies. Building upon the hypotheses we have developed regarding their direct effects, we propose four mediation hypotheses to be tested in our research:
Hypothesis 4: There is a (a) positive relation between workplace anger and workplace resource depletion via ruminative coping as well as a (b) positive relation between workplace anger and workplace resource depletion via confrontative coping.
Hypothesis 5: There is a (a) negative relation between workplace anger and workplace goal attainment via ruminative coping as well as a (b) positive relation between workplace anger and workplace goal attainment via confrontative coping.
As we compare the core assumptions of different theoretical frameworks and their alignment with empirical evidence, it is important to also consider the validity of their auxiliary assumptions. One key aspect to consider is the moderating impact of employee dispositions on the assumed relations. This is especially crucial as it illuminates another significant theoretical discrepancy between affective events theory and cognitivist accounts of emotion (Lazarus, 1991; Frijda, 1987; Moors et al., 2013). Essentially, in cognitivist accounts, the relations between emotions and coping strategies, and between coping strategies and work outcomes, are moderated by employee dispositions. In contrast, affective events theory argues that only the direct link between emotions and work outcomes is moderated by employee dispositions (Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996).
To assert the incremental validity of the two theoretical frameworks, we may want to consider examining a distinct disposition to test our theoretical assumptions with. Workplace affiliation (an inherent desire to be part of a team or group at work; Yagil and Medler-Liraz, 2017) seems to be a promising choice for this analysis as it seems to be regarded as a disposition in both frameworks (Lazarus, 1991; Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996), has a strong association with anger due to anger’s contrary affiliative nature (Lazarus, 1991; Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996; see also Oatley, 2009), and is a distinct and measurable (Yagil and Medler-Liraz, 2017) construct. Thus, our research will concentrate on investigating the role of workplace affiliation in moderating the connections between workplace anger, coping strategies, and workplace resource depletion as well as goal attainment. This leads us to formulate and subsequently investigate the following research question:
Research question: How does an employee’s workplace affiliation disposition impact the relations between workplace anger, coping strategies, and workplace resource depletion as well as goal attainment?
In order to address the challenges of methodological incongruence in the organizational sciences (Thurston et al., 2008), our research aims to align our methodology closely with the nature of our theoretical constructs to minimize any potential misfit that may arise. One key consideration in this alignment is the temporal volatility of our constructs. Constructs such as workplace anger, workplace resource depletion and goal attainment, and coping strategies like ruminative and confrontative coping are likely to fluctuate daily (Lazarus, 1991; Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996), while a workplace affiliation disposition tends to change less rapidly. Furthermore, considering our theoretical frameworks that suggest a procedural relation between workplace anger, coping strategies, and workplace resource depletion as well as goal attainment (Lazarus, 1991; Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996), it is also vital to select a methodological approach that can capture this sequential nature.
We identified a daily time-lagged experience-sampling approach as a suitable option, as it can accommodate the differing volatilities of our constructs and the procedural relations we assume (Gabriel et al., 2019; Ohly et al., 2010; Trull and Ebner-Priemer, 2009). While this approach offers the advantage of increased external validity (Findley et al., 2021), concerns about internal validity and causality may arise. However, given the partly exploratory nature of our research, we consider that using an experience-sampling approach would provide the most significant theoretical and practical insights.
We preregistered our hypotheses, study, and analysis plan on the Open Science Framework, accessible at https://osf.io/3tdwv/?view_only=30658bc72bc743e28775dd6876cce1d5. The data summary and analysis code will also be available in the same directory upon publication. Informed consent was acquired from every participant prior to data collection.
The research conducted in this study was ethically approved by the committee at authors’ university. Our goal was to obtain a diverse sample of employed adults who worked a minimum of 30 h per week, with the exclusion of employees under 18 or over 67 years old. By recruiting participants through student networks, we utilized a comparable and conventionally robust approach to Burmeister et al. (2020) and Fasbender et al. (2021), thereby gaining a wide variety of participants from different networks. To capture changes in participants’ experiences, we utilized an experience sampling format over 10 consecutive workdays (Gabriel et al., 2019). As an incentive for participation, participants received gift vouchers. Surveys were sent to participants at three specific times daily: 9:30 a.m. (morning survey; at work), 12:30 p.m. (noon survey; at work), and 3:30 p.m. (evening survey; at work). Data collection occurred between May and December 2022. Our study’s attrition rate was consistent with previous research (Ohly et al., 2010; Xia et al., 2021), with a 9% dropout rate and a 68% compliance rate for daily surveys. We observed no significant effects of gender or age on attrition rates.
A total of 1,611 observations were collected on the employee-day experiences of 214 participants. The average number of observations per employee was 7.53. This sample size exceeds the standard experience sampling standards outlined by Gabriel et al. (2019). Of the participants, 60% were female, with an average age of 34.83 years (SD = 13.08) and a weekly work schedule of 39.26 h (SD = 4.80). Their average general work experience was 15.08 years (SD = 12.95), and they had been with their organization for an average of 9.23 years (SD = 9.87). Of the participants, 26% held leadership positions, while 74% performed office duties. Additionally, 26% performed non-office tasks, and 66% worked in an office environment. Furthermore, 13% worked from home, 11% at customers’, and 10% in public locations. Regarding company size, 58% worked in large companies, 16% in medium-sized firms, 15% in small businesses, and 11% in micro-companies. Lastly, 17% worked in banking, finance, and insurance, 16% in production/manufacturing, 10% in public administration, 9% in healthcare, 7% in IT and communications, 6% in craft trades, 6% in education and training, 5% in retail/wholesale trade, 3% in energy and water supply, 2% in catering/hospitality, 2% in transportation, less than 1% in agriculture and forestry, less than 1% in science, and 16% in other industries.
The translation of the measurement scales from English to German was conducted using the back-translation method described by Brislin (1970). Unless otherwise stated, a 5-point Likert scale was utilized, with response options ranging from 1 (indicating strong disagreement) to 5 (indicating strong agreement). Our selection of items was determined by considering factor loading matrices and theoretical congruence while prioritizing the minimization of participant burden by keeping the number of items low (Gabriel et al., 2019). We adapted the scale to the workplace and the study context by adding “Today at work, …” to each item.
To assess workplace anger, we utilized four items from Spielberger et al.’s (1983) scale, whereby participants were asked to indicate their present experience of anger. An example statement was, “Right now, I feel angry.” The internal consistency of this scale was high, with a McDonald’s Omega (ω; McDonald, 1999; see also Geldhof et al., 2014) value of 0.95.
Ruminative coping was evaluated using the 3-item scale developed by Li et al. (2019). An example of a sample item was, “Throughout my workday today, I could not stop thinking about an event that made me angry.” The scale exhibited a high level of reliability (ω = 0.95).
We evaluated confrontative coping utilizing a scale developed by Folkman et al. (1986). A sample item is, “Today at work, since the beginning of my workday, I stood my ground and fought for what I wanted” (ω = 0.76).
We employed a set of three items derived from Lanaj et al.’s (2014) scale to monitor workplace resource depletion. One of the sample questions included in this set was, “Today at work, since filling out the last questionnaire, my mind has felt unfocused” (ω = 0.87).
To guarantee the precision of measuring workplace goal attainment, we have integrated the two items from Judge et al.’s (2005) goal attainment scale and added one item (“Today at work, since filling out the last questionnaire, I achieved my goals at work”), ensuring a minimum of three items. The scale had an omega value of ω = 0.91.
In the baseline survey, we utilized the 4-item scale developed by Van Yperen et al. (2014) to measure the participant’s workplace affiliation disposition. An example item from the scale reads, “I am a person who feels the need to feel like they are part of a team or group at work.” (ω = 0.86).
Drawing on our theoretical framework, we posit that the experience of negative affect in the morning, such as sadness or anxiety, may impact how employees employ coping strategies later in the day (Lazarus, 1991; Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996). To gauge these morning affects, we utilized a scale deployed by Sonnentag et al. (2008, see also Watson et al., 1988) that assessed negative affect, achieving a solid internal consistency with a McDonald’s omega of 0.92. We determined that our results remained consistent regardless of whether we included or excluded controls, with one exception. The moderation effect of workplace affiliation dispositions on the relation between confrontative coping and goal attainment was found to be borderline non-significant (E(moderation effect) = 0.15, 95% CI [−0.001, 0.330], p = 0.05). In this connection, we will be reporting the results of our controlled effects moving forward.
In our study, we conducted data preparation using R version 4.2.2 (R Core Team, 2022) and data analysis using Mplus version 8.4 (Muthén and Muthén, 2017). The dataset consisted of multiple observations for each employee (Hayes, 2006), prompting us to utilize 2-level multilevel modeling with random intercepts and slopes (Hamaker and Muthén, 2020). Due to the presence of non-normality, model complexity, and outliers, we employed Bayesian inference in our analysis, which has conventionally been robust to these issues (Asparouhov and Muthén, 2021; Depaoli, 2021).
We utilized the Gibbs sampler algorithm with a 1.02 Gelman-Rubin potential scale reduction factor (Gelman et al., 2013) and ran two Markov chains for 50,700 iterations. To ensure convergence, we visually inspected trace and autocorrelation plots. Our model estimates were presented using the median as a point estimate, with diffuse priors1 used for parameter interpretation similar to traditional maximum likelihood estimation (cf. Depaoli, 2021). Missing data were included in our model estimation (Finch and Bolin, 2017).
Group-mean centering of the predictor variable was automatically applied through confirmatory factor analysis (see Muthén and Muthén, 2017). Outliers were retained in our analysis (Grubbs, 1950; see also Asparouhov and Muthén, 2021; Finch and Bolin, 2017). For level-1 observations, only participants who worked during specific sampling time intervals, as measured in each questionnaire, were included. We included the direct effect of workplace anger on workplace resource depletion and goal attainment, which adhered to the guidelines provided by Kline (2015).
Data diagnostics revealed no abnormalities in data quality (Cullen and Frey, 1999; Delignette-Muller and Dutang, 2015; Gabriel et al., 2019). The mean percentage of missing responses across variables was 23.68%, with specific rates of 16.60% for workplace anger, 25.60% for ruminative and confrontative coping, and 26.90% for workplace goal attainment and resource depletion.
The descriptive statistics for the focal variables are presented in Table 1. Results indicate that the participants exhibited low levels of workplace anger, ruminative coping, confrontative coping, and workplace resource depletion while presenting moderate levels of workplace goal attainment. Notably, all variables displayed significant variance on both levels, with a near-equal spread.
Table 1. Means, standard deviations, interclass correlation coefficients, reliabilities, and correlations among the focal factors.
We assessed the factor structure through confirmatory factor analyses (see Table 2). We employed both maximum likelihood and Bayesian estimators to verify the results. The hypothesized factor structure demonstrated a satisfactory level of fit. To confirm the discriminant validity of this structure, we also conducted CFAs for alternative models. The outcomes revealed that the alternative models exhibited a significantly poorer fit to the data than the hypothesized model.
We present the results of our Bayesian structural equation modeling analyses for the direct and indirect effects in Tables 3, 4, respectively. Support was found for Hypothesis 1a, as workplace anger was positively associated with ruminative coping (E(γ) = 0.392, p < 0.001). Similarly, support was found for Hypothesis 1b, with workplace anger positively related to confrontative coping (E(γ) = 0.191, p < 0.001).
Table 4. Unstandardized coefficient estimates and posterior standard deviations of indirect effects.
In line with Hypothesis 2a, ruminative coping was found to be positively related to workplace resource depletion (E(γ) = 0.108, p = 0.004). However, contrary to Hypothesis 2b, confrontative coping showed no positive association with workplace resource depletion (E(γ) = 0.032, p = 0.674).
Support for Hypothesis 3a was found, indicating a negative relation between ruminative coping and workplace goal attainment (E(γ) = −0.070, p = 0.046). Hypothesis 3b was not supported, as confrontative coping did not show a positive relation with workplace goal attainment (E(γ) = 0.075, p = 0.288).
Support was found for Hypothesis 4a, revealing a positive relation between workplace anger and workplace resource depletion via ruminative coping (E(indirect) = 0.041, p = 0.004). Hypothesis 4b was not supported, as workplace anger was not positively related to workplace resource depletion via confrontative coping (E(indirect) = 0.006, p = 0.674).
Hypothesis 5a was supported, showing a negative relation between workplace anger and workplace goal attainment via ruminative coping (E(indirect) = −0.026, p = 0.046). Hypothesis 5b was not supported, as workplace anger was not positively related to workplace goal attainment via confrontative coping (E(indirect) = 0.014, p = 0.288).
Table 4 presents the results of our analyses on the conditional effects. Our study found that the relation between ruminative coping and workplace resource depletion was moderated by an employee’s workplace affiliation disposition (moderation effect = −0.136, p < 0.001). Our results indicate that the relation between ruminative coping and workplace resource depletion was significant for low (−1 standard deviation; simple slope = 0.245, p < 0.001) and but not high (+1 standard deviation; simple slope = −0.029, p = 0.590) expressions of an employee’s workplace affiliation disposition. The difference between the slopes was significant (difference = −0.273, p < 0.001). Similarly, the relation between workplace anger and workplace resource depletion via ruminative coping was significant for low (−1 standard deviation; conditional effect = 0.093, p < 0.001) but not high (+1 standard deviation; conditional effect = −0.011, p = 0.590) expressions of an employee’s workplace affiliation disposition. The difference between the slopes was significant (difference = −0.104, p < 0.001). The corresponding region of significance plots, presented in Figures 2, 3, show that the relation between ruminative coping and workplace resource depletion as well as the relation between workplace anger and workplace resource depletion via ruminative coping did become non-significant with values of a workplace affiliation disposition of equal to or greater than 0.240 standard deviations above the sample mean, while becoming negative and significant with values of a workplace affiliation disposition of equal to or greater than 2.080 standard deviations above the sample mean (Table 5).
Figure 2. Region of significance for cross-level interaction effect of workplace affiliation disposition on the relation between ruminative coping and workplace resource depletion.
Figure 3. Region of significance for cross-level interaction effect of workplace affiliation disposition on the relation workplace anger and workplace resource depletion via ruminative coping.
Our results also showed that the relation between confrontative coping and workplace goal attainment was moderated by an employee’s workplace affiliation disposition (moderation effect = 0.154, p = 0.042). Our results indicate that the relation between confrontative coping and workplace goal attainment was still insignificant for low (−1 standard deviation; simple slope = −0.077, p = 0.452) but not high (+1 standard deviation; simple slope = 0.229, p = 0.032) expressions of an employee’s workplace affiliation disposition. The difference between the slopes was significant (difference = 0.308, p = 0.042). Similarly, the relation between workplace anger and workplace goal attainment via confrontative coping was also still insignificant for low (−1 standard deviation; conditional effect = −0.014, p = 0.452) but not high (+1 standard deviation; conditional effect = 0.043, p = 0.032) expressions of an employee’s workplace affiliation disposition. The difference between the slopes was significant (difference = 0.058, p = 0.042). The corresponding region of significance plots, presented in Figures 4, 5, show that the relation between confrontative coping and workplace goal attainment as well as the relation between workplace anger and workplace goal attainment via confrontative coping did not become negative and significant, while becoming positive and significant with values of a workplace affiliation disposition of equal to or greater than 0.640 standard deviations above the sample mean.
Figure 4. Region of significance for cross-level interaction effect of workplace affiliation disposition on the relation between confrontative coping and workplace goal attainment.
Figure 5. Region of significance for cross-level interaction effect of workplace affiliation disposition on the relation workplace anger and workplace goal attainment via confrontative coping.
In this manuscript, our primary focus was on examining the intricate relation between workplace anger, workplace resource depletion, and goal attainment while taking into account the impact of coping strategies and individual differences. Our main objective was to challenge the common belief that workplace anger always impedes work outcomes. To achieve this goal, we developed a theoretical model based on affective events theory (Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996) and insights from cognitive theories of emotions (Lazarus, 1991; Frijda, 1987; Moors et al., 2013). Our conceptual model suggests that workplace anger may be positively related to workplace resource depletion and may have negative associations with workplace goal attainment in certain situations, but could also have a positive relation with workplace goal attainment under other circumstances. Factors such as coping strategies for managing anger and individual differences among employees were taken into consideration in this model. In order to test our theoretical framework, we gathered data from a group of full-time employees across various industries using a daily time-lagged experience-sampling method over a two-week period. Contrary to previous research, our results indicated a lack of significant association between workplace anger and workplace resource depletion, as well as a positive connection between workplace anger and goal attainment, depending on the coping mechanisms employed by employees when faced with anger-triggering situations, as well as their workplace affiliations.
Our results have significant theoretical implications. Firstly, our findings suggest that the assumptions of affective events theory (Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996) and cognitivist accounts of emotion, which propose an adverse relation between workplace anger and resource depletion, appear to be supported by empirical evidence, although only to a limited extent. The effect size of the relation between workplace anger and resource depletion is below the threshold for a small effect (E(β) = 0.041, r = 0.091; Cohen, 1988; Gignac and Szodorai, 2016), leading us to question the practical validity of these assumptions. Additionally, as this relation was only significant when mediated by coping strategies, it is important to consider that the interplay between affective responses and work outcomes is contingent upon the intersection of these coping strategies. Therefore, it may be beneficial to integrate coping strategies into affective events theory (Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996) to provide a more comprehensive understanding of the relation between emotions and work outcomes.
The importance of integrating coping strategies into affective events theory (Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996) is further clarified when considering the various intersecting effects of coping strategies in the relation between workplace anger and work outcomes. Our research supports the concept of cognitivist accounts of emotions (Frijda, 1987; Lazarus, 1991; Moors et al., 2013) by demonstrating how various coping strategies intersect and direct the valence of the relation between workplace anger and work outcomes in different ways. Specifically, we found that the connection between workplace anger and goal attainment is significantly adverse when ruminative coping is considered but becomes negligible when confrontative coping is taken into account. This same pattern is also evident in the link between workplace anger and resource depletion. Notably, the indirect effects of workplace anger on work outcomes through confrontative coping, as well as the direct effects between confrontative coping and work outcomes, are close to zero. This suggests that the lack of significant results may not be due to a lack of statistical power, but rather indicate a practical buffering effect of confrontative coping between workplace anger and work outcomes. This highlights the interplay between coping strategies and these constructs, emphasizing the crucial role of coping strategies in the relation between workplace anger and work outcomes. Therefore, incorporating coping strategies into affective events theory (Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996) could improve our understanding of how workplace anger relates to work outcomes.
However, we also noted that the relations between workplace anger and work outcomes depended on how an employee expressed their disposition toward workplace affiliation. Prior research has not thoroughly explored individual employee-level factors within the context of affective events theory, making these findings especially significant. This disposition appears to play a crucial role in shaping the relations between workplace anger, coping strategies, and work outcomes. It is important to note that the relation between workplace anger and resource depletion through ruminative coping became insignificant when considering the interaction between a high disposition toward workplace affiliation and these coping strategies. Conversely, the relation between workplace anger and goal attainment through confrontative coping became positive and significant in the same context, aligning with principles of cognitivist theories of emotions (Lazarus, 1991). This highlights the potential value of integrating this interaction into affective events theory (Weiss and Cropanzano, 1996), in conjunction with other theoretical propositions of constructivist accounts of emotions (Lazarus, 1991; Frijda, 1987; Moors et al., 2013), to enhance our comprehension of how affective responses are linked to work outcomes.
Our study suggests that leaders may benefit from adopting a dual approach when dealing with elevated levels of anger among their employees. The first aspect of this strategy involves encouraging confrontational behavior among employees without imposing penalties. This can be achieved through providing assertiveness training to help employees confront others at work (see Abdelaziz et al., 2020; Omura et al., 2017; Speed et al., 2018).
Simultaneously, it is crucial to implement interventions that enhance workplace affiliation. Improving the quality of relations among employees by fostering workplace friendships (Methot et al., 2016; van Dick et al., 2004) or creating mutually dependent work teams (Dietz and Fasbender, 2021) should help increase the desire of employees to affiliate with one another, at least in the short term.
Our research findings suggest that the positive relation between anger and goal attainment is dependent on the interplay of assertiveness and affiliation factors. Thus, solely focusing on one type of intervention may not be as effective. Instead, implementing both assertiveness and affiliation strategies concurrently can lead to higher employee goal attainment rates, ultimately improving organizational performance on a daily basis.
In interpreting the results of our study, it is imperative to consider its limitations. First, self-report measures may have introduced common method bias (Doty and Glick, 1998), which could compromise the validity of our findings. Therefore, it is advisable for forthcoming research efforts to integrate other-report study design components, such as those that evaluate employee dyads. This approach can help mitigate the potential bias through inter-method reliability.
Second, our time-based sampling strategy may have impacted our estimates by missing event-based variance due to fixed sampling intervals (Trull and Ebner-Priemer, 2009). We attempted to address this limitation by setting narrow sampling intervals. Nonetheless, future research could consider incorporating random survey prompts to reduce this potential bias further.
Our study did not examine the precise sources and targets of workplace anger (organizational, non-organizational, metaphysical) or the employee’s situational control related to such workplace anger (Lazarus, 1991; Lazarus and Folkman, 1984; Potegal et al., 2010). This complexity was beyond the scope of our study design. Although our theoretical framework and results should still hold under these distinctions, we recommend that future researchers incorporate the sources and targets of workplace anger as well as situational control into their study designs to enhance understanding further about the mechanisms underlying our conceptual model (see also Schwarzmüller et al., 2018, for a good starting point for this direction).
Overall, it is important to note that these limitations may somewhat restrict the generalizability and viability of our interpretations and results. Therefore, future research is encouraged to address these challenges through replication and conceptual expansion.
One potential direction for future research is to experimentally replicate the relations that were proposed and tested in the current paper. Our approach lacks internal validity, indicating that conducting experimental studies could help address this limitation. Vignette studies could be used by future researchers to test our assumptions and bring both internal and external validity to the assumed relations (Aguinis and Bradley, 2014).
Additionally, it may be beneficial for future research to investigate the reasons behind the impact of a workplace affiliation disposition on the relation between ruminative coping and workplace resource depletion as well as the relation between confrontative coping and workplace goal attainment. Qualitative research, such as in-depth interviews, could be utilized to uncover the underlying reasons for these associations (see Fitness, 2000). Following this, conceptual research could identify commonalities from the interviews (Locke, 2007), which could then be tested in experience-sampling studies and replicated across various business settings (Gabriel et al., 2019).
Future research should further explore the lack of a significant effect of confrontative coping on resource depletion. It is worth investigating whether organizational and leadership practices that incorporate confrontative approaches toward anger-inducing employees could provide a viable framework for sustaining productivity while ensuring both short-and long-term employee well-being. Additionally, future studies could examine whether these effects—particularly those related to different expressions of workplace affiliation—remain consistent across various workplace cultures and organizational roles.
The primary objective of organizations is to be resourceful and achieve their goals (Mohr, 1973; Sekaran and Snodgrass, 1989). While anger has traditionally been viewed as a barrier to these objectives (see Jäger et al., 2017; Wong et al., 2017; but see also Schmitt et al., 2019), our study challenges this notion. We have found evidence suggesting that anger is not linked to resource depletion, and in fact, it can be advantageous for employees in reaching their work-related goals, especially when the individual experiencing anger is integrated within the organizational framework and well-established within it.
Data and materials are shared openly as part of the publication of this article at https://osf.io/3tdwv/?view_only=30658bc72bc743e28775dd6876cce1d5.
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors without undue reservation.
The studies involving humans were approved by Ethics Committee of the University of Hohenheim. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
RU: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. UF: Supervision, Writing – review & editing.
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The authors declare that Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript. ChatGPT was used for copyediting.
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
1. ^ν, λ, β, α ~ N(0, ∞); θ ~ IG(−1, 0); ψ ~ IW(0, −p-1) (see Muthén and Muthén, 2017, p. 775, for notation).
Abdelaziz, E. M., Diab, I. A., Ouda, M. M. A., Elsharkawy, N. B., and Abdelkader, F. A. (2020). The effectiveness of assertiveness training program on psychological wellbeing and work engagement among novice psychiatric nurses. Nurs. Forum 55, 309–319. doi: 10.1111/NUF.12430
Aguinis, H., and Bradley, K. J. (2014). Best practice recommendations for designing and implementing experimental vignette methodology studies. Organ. Res. Methods 17, 351–371. doi: 10.1177/1094428114547952
Ashkanasy, N. M., and Dorris, A. D. (2017). Emotions in the workplace. Annu. Rev. Organ. Psych. Organ. Behav. 4, 67–90. doi: 10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-032516-113231
Asparouhov, T., and Muthén, B. (2021). Bayesian estimation of single and multilevel models with latent variable interactions. Struct. Equ. Model. 28, 314–328. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2020.1761808
Baker, J. P., and Berenbaum, H. (2007). Emotional approach and problem-focused coping: a comparison of potentially adaptive strategies. Cognit. Emot. 21, 95–118. doi: 10.1080/02699930600562276
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., and Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: is the active self a limited resource? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 1252–1265. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1252
Baumeister, R., and Vohs, K. (2004). “Handbook of self-regulation: research, theory, and applications” in Handbook of self-regulation: research, theory, and applications. eds. R. F. Baumeister and K. D. Vohs (New York: The Guilford Press).
Booth, J., Ireland, J. L., Mann, S., Eslea, M., and Holyoak, L. (2017). Anger expression and suppression at work: causes, characteristics and predictors. Int. J. Confl. Manag. 28, 368–382. doi: 10.1108/IJCMA-06-2016-0044
Boyd, N. G., Lewin, J. E., and Sager, J. K. (2009). A model of stress and coping and their influence on individual and organizational outcomes. J. Vocat. Behav. 75, 197–211. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2009.03.010
Brislin, R. W. (1970). Back-translation for cross-cultural research. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 1, 185–216. doi: 10.1177/135910457000100301
Brodscholl, J. C., Kober, H., and Higgins, E. T. (2007). Strategies of self-regulation in goal attainment versus goal maintenance. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 37, 628–648. doi: 10.1002/EJSP.380
Burmeister, A., Wang, M., and Hirschi, A. (2020). Understanding the motivational benefits of knowledge transfer for older and younger workers in age-diverse coworker dyads: an actor-partner interdependence model. J. Appl. Psychol. 105, 748–759. doi: 10.1037/APL0000466
Callister, R. R., Geddes, D., and Gibson, D. F. (2017). When is anger helpful or hurtful? Status and role impact on anger expression and outcomes. Negot. Confl. Manag. Res. 10, 69–87. doi: 10.1111/ncmr.12090
Carlson, D., Kacmar, K. M., Zivnuska, S., Ferguson, M., and Whitten, D. (2011). Work-family enrichment and job performance: a constructive replication of affective events theory. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 16, 297–312. doi: 10.1037/a0022880
Carver, C. S. (2006). Approach, avoidance, and the self-regulation of affect and action. Motiv. Emot. 30, 105–110. doi: 10.1007/s11031-006-9044-7
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.). Routledge. doi: 10.4324/9780203771587
Cullen, A., and Frey, H. (1999). Probabilistic techniques in exposure assessment. 1st Edn. New York: Plenum Publishing Co.
Delignette-Muller, M. L., and Dutang, C. (2015). Fitdistrplus: an R package for fitting distributions. J. Stat. Softw. 64, 1–34. doi: 10.18637/jss.v064.i04
Dietz, L., and Fasbender, U. (2021). Age-diverse workplace friendship: a systematic literature review and recommendations for future research. Work Aging Retire. 8, 163–197. doi: 10.1093/WORKAR/WAAB028
Doty, D. H., and Glick, W. H. (1998). Common methods bias: does common methods variance really bias results? Organ. Res. Methods 1, 374–406. doi: 10.1177/109442819814002
Fasbender, U., Gerpott, F. H., and Unger, D. (2021). Give and take? Knowledge exchange between older and younger employees as a function of generativity and development striving. J. Knowl. Manag. 25, 2420–2443. doi: 10.1108/JKM-11-2020-0856
Findley, M. G., Kikuta, K., and Denly, M. (2021). External validity. Annu. Rev. Polit. Sci. 24, 365–393. doi: 10.1146/ANNUREV-POLISCI-041719-102556
Fitness, J. (2000). Anger in the workplace: an emotion script approach to anger episodes between workers and their superiors, co-workers and subordinates. J. Organ. Behav. 21, 147–162. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-1379(200003)21:2<147::AID-JOB35>3.0.CO;2-T
Folkman, S., and Lazarus, R. S. (1985). If it changes it must be a process: study of emotion and coping during three stages of a college examination. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 48, 150–170. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.48.1.150
Folkman, S., Lazarus, R. S., Dunkel-Schetter, C., DeLongis, A., and Gruen, R. J. (1986). Dynamics of a stressful encounter. Cognitive appraisal, coping, and encounter outcomes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 50, 992–1003. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.50.5.992
Gabriel, A. S., Podsakoff, N. P., Beal, D. J., Scott, B. A., Sonnentag, S., Trougakos, J. P., et al. (2019). Experience sampling methods: a discussion of critical trends and considerations for scholarly advancement. Organ. Res. Methods 22, 969–1006. doi: 10.1177/1094428118802626
Geldhof, G. J., Preacher, K. J., and Zyphur, M. J. (2014). Reliability estimation in a multilevel confirmatory factor analysis framework. Psychol. Methods 19, 72–91. doi: 10.1037/A0032138
Gelman, A., Carlin, J. B., Stern, H. S., Dunson, D. B., Vehtari, A., and Rubin, D. B. (2013). Bayesian data analysis. 3rd Edn. Oxfordshire: Taylor & Francs.
Gibson, D. E., Schweitzer, M. E., Callister, R. R., and Gray, B. (2009). The influence of anger expressions on outcomes in organizations. Negot. Confl. Manag. Res. 2, 236–262. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-4716.2009.00039.x
Gignac, G. E., and Szodorai, E. T. (2016). Effect size guidelines for individual differences researchers. Pers. Individ. Dif. 102, 74–78. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.06.069
Glasø, L., Vie, T. L., Holmdal, G. R., and Einarsen, S. (2010). An application of affective events theory to workplace bullying. Eur. Psychol. 16, 198–208. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000026
Godkin, L., and Allcorn, S. (2008). Overcoming organizational inertia: a tripartite model for achieving strategic organizational change. J. Appl. Bus. Econ. 8:82.
Grubbs, F. E. (1950). Sample criteria for testing outlying observations. Ann. Math. Stat. 21, 27–58. doi: 10.1214/AOMS/1177729885
Hamaker, E. L., and Muthén, B. (2020). The fixed versus random effects debate and how it relates to centering in multilevel modeling. Psychol. Methods 25, 365–379. doi: 10.1037/met0000239
Hayes, A. F. (2006). A primer on multilevel modeling. Hum. Commun. Res. 32, 385–410. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2958.2006.00281.x
Jäger, A., Loschelder, D. D., and Friese, M. (2017). Using self-regulation to overcome the detrimental effects of anger in negotiations. J. Econ. Psychol. 58, 31–43. doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2016.09.003
Judge, T. A., Erez, A., Bono, J. E., and Locke, E. A. (2005). Core self-evaluations and job and life satisfaction: the role of self-concordance and goal attainment. J. Appl. Psychol. 90, 257–268. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.90.2.257
Kant, L., Skogstad, A., Torsheim, T., and Einarsen, S. (2013). Beware the angry leader: trait anger and trait anxiety as predictors of petty tyranny. Leadersh. Quart. 24, 106–124. doi: 10.1016/J.LEAQUA.2012.08.005
Keltikangas-Järvinen, L., Räikkönen, K., Hautanen, A., and Adlercreutz, H. (1996). Vital exhaustion, anger expression, and pituitary and adrenocortical hormones: implications for the insulin resistance syndrome. Arterioscler. Thromb. Vasc. Biol. 16, 275–280. doi: 10.1161/01.ATV.16.2.275
Kiresuk, T. J., Smith, A., and Cardillo, J. E. (2014). Goal attainment scaling: applications, theory, and measurement. New York: Psychology Press.
Kline, R. B. (2015). The mediation myth. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 37, 202–213. doi: 10.1080/01973533.2015.1049349
Lanaj, K., Johnson, R. E., and Barnes, C. M. (2014). Beginning the workday yet already depleted? Consequences of late-night smartphone use and sleep. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 124, 11–23. doi: 10.1016/J.OBHDP.2014.01.001
Li, J. B., Dou, K., Situ, Q. M., Salcuni, S., Wang, Y. J., and Friese, M. (2019). Anger rumination partly accounts for the association between trait self-control and aggression. J. Res. Pers. 81, 207–223. doi: 10.1016/J.JRP.2019.06.011
Locke, E. A. (2007). The case for inductive theory building. J. Manag. 33, 867–890. doi: 10.1177/0149206307307636
Methot, J. R., Lepine, J. A., Podsakoff, N. P., and Christian, J. S. (2016). Are workplace friendships a mixed blessing? Exploring tradeoffs of multiplex relations and their associations with job performance. Pers. Psychol. 69, 311–355. doi: 10.1111/peps.12109
Moberly, N. J., and Watkins, E. R. (2009). Negative affect and ruminative self-focus during everyday goal pursuit. Cognit. Emot. 24, 729–739. doi: 10.1080/02699930802696849
Mohr, L. B. (1973). The concept of organizational goal. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 67, 470–481. doi: 10.2307/1958777
Moors, A., Ellsworth, P. C., Scherer, K. R., and Frijda, N. H. (2013). Appraisal theories of emotion: state of the art and future development. Emot. Rev. 5, 119–124. doi: 10.1177/1754073912468165
Morrison, E. W., and Milliken, F. J. (2000). Organizational silence: a barrier to change and development in a pluralistic world. Acad. Manag. Rev. 25, 706–725. doi: 10.5465/amr.2000.3707697
Oatley, K. (2009). Communications to self and others: emotional experience and its skills. Emot. Rev. 1, 206–213. doi: 10.1177/1754073909103588
Ohly, S., Sonnentag, S., Niessen, C., and Zapf, D. (2010). Diary studies in organizational research. J. Pers. Psychol. 9, 79–93. doi: 10.1027/1866-5888/A000009
Omura, M., Maguire, J., Levett-Jones, T., and Stone, T. E. (2017). The effectiveness of assertiveness communication training programs for healthcare professionals and students: a systematic review. Int. J. Nurs. Stud. 76, 120–128. doi: 10.1016/J.IJNURSTU.2017.09.001
Pham, M. T. (2007). Emotion and rationality: a critical review and interpretation of empirical evidence. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 11, 155–178. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.11.2.155
Potegal, M., Stemmler, G., and Spielberger, C. (2010). International handbook of anger: constituent and concomitant biological, psychological, and social processes. New Jersey: Springer.
R Core Team. (2022). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Available online at: https://www.r-project.org/ (Accessed May 27, 2024).
Roseman, I. J. (2013). Appraisal in the emotion system: coherence in strategies for coping. Emot. Rev. 5, 141–149. doi: 10.1177/1754073912469591
Ryan, R. M., and Vansteenkiste, M. (2023). “Self-determination theory: metatheory, methods, and meaning” in The Oxford handbook of self-determination theory. ed. R. M. Ryan. (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 3–30.
Schmitt, A., Gielnik, M. M., and Seibel, S. (2019). When and how does anger during goal pursuit relate to goal achievement? The roles of persistence and action planning. Motiv. Emot. 43, 205–217. doi: 10.1007/s11031-018-9720-4
Schwarzmüller, T., Brosi, P., and Welpe, I. (2016). Sparking anger and anxiety: how leader anger displays enhance (Dys)functional follower behaviors. Acad. Manag. Proc. 2016:10679. doi: 10.5465/AMBPP.2016.10679abstract
Schwarzmüller, T., Brosi, P., and Welpe, I. M. (2018). Sparking anger and anxiety: why intense leader anger displays trigger both more deviance and higher work effort in followers. J. Bus. Psychol. 33, 761–777. doi: 10.1007/s10869-017-9523-8
Sekaran, U., and Snodgrass, C. R. (1989) in Organizational effectiveness and its attainment BT-organizational science abroad: constraints and perspectives. ed. C. A. B. Osigweh. (Boston, MA: Springer US), 269–292.
Sheldon, K. M., and Elliot, A. J. (1999). Goal striving, need satisfaction, and longitudinal well-being: the self-concordance model. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 76, 482–497. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.3.482
Sonnentag, S., Binnewies, C., and Mojza, E. J. (2008). “Did you have a nice evening?” A day-level study on recovery experiences, sleep, and affect. J. Appl. Psychol. 93, 674–684. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.93.3.674
Speed, B. C., Goldstein, B. L., and Goldfried, M. R. (2018). Assertiveness training: a forgotten evidence-based treatment. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 25:e12216. doi: 10.1111/CPSP.12216
Spielberger, C. D., Jacobs, G., Russell, S., and Crane, R. S. (1983). “Assessment of anger: the state-trait anger scale” in Advances in personality assessment. eds. J. N. Butcher and C. D. Spielberger, vol. 2 (New York: Routledge), 161–189.
Thurston, W. E., Cove, L., and Meadows, L. M. (2008). Methodological congruence in complex and collaborative mixed method studies. Int. J. Multiple Res. Approach. 2, 2–14. doi: 10.5172/mra.455.2.1.2
Trull, T. J., and Ebner-Priemer, U. W. (2009). Using experience sampling methods/ecological momentary assessment (ESM/EMA) in clinical assessment and clinical research: introduction to the special section. Psychol. Assess. 21, 457–462. doi: 10.1037/A0017653
van Dick, R., Wagner, U., Pettigrew, T. F., Christ, O., Wolf, C., Petzel, T., et al. (2004). Role of perceived importance in intergroup contact. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87, 211–227. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.2.211
Van Yperen, N. W., Rietzschel, E. F., and De Jonge, K. M. M. (2014). Blended working: for whom it may (not) work. PLoS One 9:e102921. doi: 10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0102921
Vohs, K. D., and Baumeister, R. F. (2011) in Handbook of self-regulation: research, theory, and applications. eds. K. D. Vohs and R. F. Baumeister. 2nd ed. (New York: The Guilford Press).
Vohs, K. D., and Heatherton, T. F. (2000). Self-regulatory failure: a resource-depletion approach. Psychol. Sci. 11, 249–254. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00250
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
Wegge, J., van Dick, R., Fisher, G. K., West, M. A., and Dawson, J. F. (2006). A test of basic assumptions of affective events theory (AET) in call centre work. Br. J. Manag. 17, 237–254. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8551.2006.00489.x
Wong, E., Tschan, F., and Semmer, N. K. (2017). Effort in emotion work and well-being: the role of goal attainment. Emotion 17, 67–77. doi: 10.1037/emo0000196
Xia, Q., Yan, S., Zhao, H., and Cheng, K. (2021). Request politeness and knowledge hiding: a daily diary study through an affective events perspective. Eur. J. Work Organ. Psy. 31, 496–509. doi: 10.1080/1359432X.2021.2004126
Keywords: workplace anger, ruminative and confrontative coping, resource depletion, goal attainment, workplace affiliation disposition
Citation: Umbra R and Fasbender U (2025) The daily relations between workplace anger, coping strategies, work outcomes, and workplace affiliation. Front. Psychol. 16:1538914. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1538914
Received: 02 January 2025; Accepted: 18 February 2025;
Published: 28 February 2025.
Edited by:
Atsushi Oshio, Waseda University, JapanReviewed by:
Watcharin Joemsittiprasert, New York Institution for Continuing Education, United StatesCopyright © 2025 Umbra and Fasbender. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Robin Umbra, cm9iaW4udW1icmFAdW5pLWhvaGVuaGVpbS5kZQ==; Ulrike Fasbender, dWxyaWtlLmZhc2JlbmRlckB1bmktaG9oZW5oZWltLmRl
Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Research integrity at Frontiers
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.