ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 17 March 2025

Sec. Health Psychology

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1525216

The effect of expectancy on conditioned pain modulation: evidence from functional near-infrared spectroscopy

Xueshan Li&#x;Xueshan Li1Min Liu&#x;Min Liu1Bo LiuBo Liu1Heng YueHeng Yue2Xiangjuan ChengXiangjuan Cheng3Hugejiletu Bao
Hugejiletu Bao4*
  • 1School of Psychology, Inner Mongolia Normal University, Hohhot, China
  • 2School of Journalism and Communication, Xiamen University, Xiamen, China
  • 3The Psychological Health Education Centre, Anhui Polytechnic University, Wuhu, China
  • 4College of Physical Education, Inner Mongolia Normal University, Hohhot, China

Background and objective: The psychological mechanisms that make Conditioned Pain Modulation (CPM) an effective non-pharmacological intervention are still not fully understood. Expectancy is believed to be a critical psychological factor affecting CPM effects, but its specific role has yet to be fully clarified. This study aims to explore the relationship between expectancy and CPM while providing physiological evidence using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS).

Method: A standardized CPM induction paradigm was implemented, with verbal guidance used to induce expectancy. The Numeric Rating Scale (NRS) assessed the intensity of the test stimulus (TS), while an 11-point scale evaluated participants’ attentional focus on the TS and the effect of expectancy. fNIRS was employed to monitor changes in prefrontal cortex (PFC) activity.

Results: Expectancy significantly amplified the CPM effect (p = 0.036) while markedly reducing attention to the experimental stimulus (p = 0.004). fNIRS findings indicated significant reductions in activity within the left frontal eye field, left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and left frontal pole regions. In the post-test, the control group demonstrated significantly higher cortical activity in the right frontal pole region compared to the expectancy group (p < 0.05). Within the expectancy group, bilateral frontal pole cortical activity was significantly lower in the post-test compared to the pre-test (p < 0.05).

Conclusion: Expectancy represents a key psychological mechanism underlying the CPM effect, potentially modulating its magnitude through attention regulation and accompanied by a reduction in oxygenated hemoglobin activity in the frontal pole region and introduced the Expectancy-Attention-CPM Modulation Model (ECAM).

1 Introduction

Pain is characterized as an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience tied to actual or potential tissue damage or analogous experiences (Song et al., 2020). As a multifactorial phenomenon shaped by physiological, psychological, and social influences, pain functions, from an evolutionary standpoint, as a crucial warning mechanism safeguarding the body from harm. However, maladaptive responses to pain can yield severe repercussions, manifesting as a wide range of emotional and cognitive disturbances (Moriarty et al., 2011). In addition to eliciting significant anxiety, depression, and sleep disturbances in affected individuals, pain imposes a profound economic burden on both families and society (Zhang et al., 2020).

Currently, pain management strategies are broadly categorized into pharmacological and non-pharmacological approaches. Pharmacological treatments pose significant challenges; opioids, for example, can effectively mitigate pain perception but are associated with a risk of dependency (Rieder, 2019) and may induce structural and functional changes in the brains of chronic pain sufferers (Borsook et al., 2018). Consequently, numerous non-pharmacological interventions have risen in prominence and are now widely employed in clinical settings.

Conditioned pain modulation (CPM) is identified as a mechanism underlying endogenous pain inhibition. Specifically, it describes how a pain stimulus delivered to one region of the body (conditioning stimulus, CS) can suppress the perception of pain arising from a separate stimulus applied to another region (test stimulus, TS), thereby achieving what is often referred to as an “analgesia by pain” effect. The degree of TS suppression by CS directly reflects the efficiency of an individual’s CPM mechanism; in essence, the stronger the suppression of TS by CS, the more robust the endogenous pain inhibitory capability (Tang et al., 2016).

CPM’s “top-down” approach to pain inhibition exemplifies the activation of endogenous pain regulatory mechanisms (Pud et al., 2009). The prevailing view holds that the mechanism underlying CPM involves the activation of the spinal-medullary-spinal neural circuit (Zhang et al., 2020). However, some researchers argue that the CPM effect cannot be solely attributed to the activation of this neural circuit by exogenous pain signals; rather, psychological functions also play a crucial role (Nir et al., 2012; Plinsinga et al., 2023). Among these psychological factors, expectancy is considered a potential mechanism contributing to the analgesic effect of CPM. Expectancy is defined as the anticipation of a potential outcome or the occurrence of a desired effect based on prior experiences (Deng et al., 2015). It is widely recognized as a fundamental psychological mechanism driving the placebo effect (Wei et al., 2018) and is often elicited and modulated through verbal suggestions (Cormier et al., 2013). Research indicates that positive expectancy can significantly alleviate pain, while negative expectancy (such as anticipating intensified pain) can heighten pain perception (Taylor et al., 2017). In studies exploring expectancy’s role in CPM, participants with analgesic expectancy exhibited a more pronounced CPM effect compared to those expecting heightened pain sensitivity (Cormier et al., 2013; France et al., 2016). Compared to healthy individuals, chronic pain patients generally demonstrate reduced CPM efficiency (Pickering et al., 2014). Chronic pain persistence in these patients is frequently linked to inadequate self-management, which elevates the risk of pain-related disability (Caes et al., 2021). Effective self-management strategies, however, can markedly reduce chronic pain’s impact and enhance psychological health (Nicholas et al., 2012). Positive analgesic expectancy, as an effective self-management tool, has proven to significantly mitigate pain (Shih et al., 2019).

Extensive research has delved into the neural mechanisms underpinning CPM, revealing that CPM not only reduces pain but also significantly activates the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and the periaqueductal gray (PAG; Mohr et al., 2008). Moreover, the extent of pain relief is strongly correlated with the connectivity between the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and the white matter tracts of the PAG (Bunk et al., 2020). Individual differences in PFC cortical function further influence the expression of conditioned pain modulation effects (Youssef et al., 2016), suggesting that the DLPFC may represent a pivotal brain region in the functioning of CPM.

Expectancy modulates pain through a “top-down” regulatory process, influencing both the objective processing of nociceptive signals and subjective pain perception (Ploghaus et al., 2003). Studies indicate that stimulating the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) via transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) can enhance placebo responses, leading to a reduction in social pain (Wang et al., 2023). Notably, the right DLPFC exhibits higher levels of activation than the left (Kong et al., 2007), highlighting the DLPFC’s critical role in processing pain-related expectancy (Amanzio et al., 2013) and positioning it as a core brain region in expectancy-driven modulation.

Furthermore, the analgesic effects of endogenous opioids suggest a link between expectancy and CPM. It is hypothesized that expectancy may reduce pain perception by modulating endogenous opioid systems. Studies have demonstrated that opioid activity in regions such as the periaqueductal gray (PAG), amygdala, orbital frontal cortex (OFC), insula, left prefrontal cortex (LPFC), and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is closely tied to placebo effects (Wager et al., 2011), while negative expectancy or nocebo effects manifest as deactivation within these regions (Scott et al., 2008). Additionally, placebo-induced effects driven by expectancy can be blocked using the opioid antagonist naloxone (Levine et al., 1978); likewise, when patients are administered opioid antagonists, their CPM efficacy declines (King et al., 2013), and its correlation with the DLPFC weakens (Sprenger et al., 2011). However, some researchers have expressed opposing views, suggesting that the role of expectancy in CPM may not be mediated by opioids (France et al., 2016). Despite observable expectancy effects, CPM responses often remain unchanged (Nir et al., 2012), and some propose that pain relief via CPM and expectancy arises from entirely independent mechanisms (Skyt et al., 2018). In conclusion, the psychological mechanisms by which expectancy influences CPM are still debated and require deeper exploration. Variability in the methods used to induce CPM effects may account for the inconsistencies in research findings and an incomplete understanding of its underlying psychological processes.

This study therefore recruited healthy participants who were screened based on specified exclusion criteria and employed the CPM induction paradigm as recommended by the European Journal of Pain (Yarnitsky, 2010). Capsaicin-induced pain served as test stimulus, while fixed-duration cold-water immersion was used as the conditioning stimulus. Verbal suggestions were employed to induce analgesic expectancy, allowing for observation of the effects of expectancy on CPM and the activation of the PFC in both the expectancy and control groups.

2 Methods

2.1 Participants

The sample size for this study was calculated using G*Power 3.1.9.2, with parameters set at an effect size f of 0.25 and power (1–β) of 0.8, resulting in a target sample of 34 participants. Accordingly, Study 1 recruited 44 university students. After excluding 5 participants who did not achieve successful activation of positive expectancy, the final sample included 39 participants (18 women), divided into a control group of 19 and an expectancy group of 20. Participants’ ages ranged from 18 to 27 years (M ± SD = 21.95 ± 2.27).

Exclusion criteria were as follows: (1) corrected vision abnormalities; (2) ongoing pain lasting more than 3 months; (3) female participants currently in their menstrual cycle; (4) trauma or tattoos on the inner left hand or right ankle; (5) left-handedness; (6) current alcohol consumption or use of analgesics or neuroactive drugs within the past week. All participants provided informed consent and had the right to withdraw from the study at any point. Upon completion of the study, participants received psychological counseling and a compensation of 25 RMB. This study was approved by the Ethics Review Committee of Inner Mongolia Normal University.

2.2 Induction and assessment of conditioned pain modulation

Since the CPM effect is not contingent upon the characteristics of test stimulus, participant tolerance serves as the key criterion for evaluating the stimulus. Consequently, a range of commonly utilized pain induction methods, including thermal, mechanical, electrical, and chemical stimuli, has been incorporated (Kennedy et al., 2016). Among these methods, capsaicin-induced pain is considered a safe and non-invasive approach that reliably produces stable and lasting pain effects (Modir and Wallace, 2010). The cold pressor test (CPT) is regarded as the most potent method for inducing a conditioning stimulus (Nilsen et al., 2014) and is noted for its strong reliability (Lewis et al., 2012; Nuwailati et al., 2022). Immersion of the wrist in 12°C cold water for 1 min is a frequently recommended and widely utilized protocol (Lewis et al., 2012). Furthermore, except when applied to the same specific location on the same limb, CPM effects are generally unaffected by the stimulus site, with testing often performed on the ipsilateral or contralateral lower limbs (Damien et al., 2018).

In this study, capsaicin application and the cold pressor test (CPT) were employed to induce test stimulus (TS) and conditioning stimulus (CS), respectively, as illustrated in Figure 1. The capsaicin utilized was a 0.1% cream, commonly used in clinical settings to relieve joint pain and sprains, as depicted in Figure 1a. The CPT was controlled using an SME–CTB high-precision low-temperature constant temperature water bath, with a temperature accuracy of ±0.01°C. The container had a diameter of 180 mm and a height of 120 mm, with a target temperature set at 12°C, maintained through continuous water circulation to ensure consistent temperature, as shown in Figure 1b. The room temperature was consistently maintained at 26°C.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Pain induction materials, (a) the left image shows capsaicin ointment, (b) the right image shows cold water apparatus.

The evaluation of conditioned pain effects was performed using the Numeric Rating Scale (NRS), which directly assessed the pain intensity experienced by participants on their right lower leg (experimental stimulus pain intensity). The NRS is widely recognized as an effective clinical tool for accurately and intuitively measuring pain levels (Hong et al., 2003) and has demonstrated the lowest error response rate and greatest utility in non-cross-cultural research (Atisook et al., 2021). This scale utilizes an 11-point system ranging from 0 to 10, with 0 representing no pain and 10 signifying the most severe pain imaginable or intolerable. Measurements showed that the initial pain intensity induced by capsaicin among participants was 7.58 ± 1.03, indicating successful activation of test stimulus.

2.3 fNIRS measurement

Functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) is a relatively non-invasive, safe, portable, and cost-effective neuroimaging technology. By measuring light absorption, it non-invasively calculates changes in the concentrations of oxygenated hemoglobin (HbO2) and deoxygenated hemoglobin (HbR), thereby providing an indirect assessment of brain activity (Irani et al., 2007). This study utilized a LABNIRS SHIMADZU–3000 near-infrared imaging device, manufactured by Shimadzu Corporation, Japan, to record changes in participants’ cortical blood oxygen concentration. The device emits continuous waves at wavelengths of 780 nm, 805 nm, and 830 nm, with a sampling rate of 12.82 Hz. The optode array was configured in an 8 × 3 layout, comprising 12 light source emitters and 12 detectors, forming an optical array with 37 near-infrared measurement channels embedded in the optode cap (Figure 2). The spacing between adjacent emitters and detectors was 3 cm, allowing full coverage of the prefrontal cortex (Figure 2). After the experiment, a 3D digital locator by Polhemus, with a measurement accuracy of 0.01–2.94 mm, was used to capture the 3D spatial positions of the optodes on participants. Reference points were marked in the order of NZ, CZ, AL, and AR, followed by sequential collection of each emitter and detector’s coordinates to determine the position of each channel. The NIRS_SPM tool in MATLAB was then employed to convert these coordinates into MNI standard coordinates and to determine the coverage of the 37 channels across Brodmann areas (Table 1).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. fINRS channel layout information (from left to right: anterior, left, and right views). (a) Position of the optical plate covering the frontal lobe. (b) Layout of the optical pole plate, consisting of 37 measurement channels composed of emission and reception optical poles. The red square represents the emission optical pole, the green square represents the reception optical pole, and the blue square represents the observation channel.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. MNI spatial location information for each fNIRS channel.

2.4 Other measurements

An 11-point scale was employed to assess the level of attention focus and the anticipated analgesic effect (scores above 6).

2.5 Experimental design and procedure

This study adopted a 2 (experimental condition: expectancy group/control group) × 2 (time: pre-test/post-test) mixed experimental design, with experimental condition as a between-subjects variable and time (pre-test/post-test of pain intensity for test stimulus and pre-test/post-test of attention focus on test stimulus) as a within-subjects variable. The dependent variables were the pain score of test stimulus and the attention focus score on test stimulus.

The experimental procedure is as follows: after entering the laboratory, participants sign an informed consent form and then proceed with the formal experiment. An electrode cap is first placed on each participant to ensure proper signal quality across all channels. Capsaicin is applied to a 6 × 6 cm area on the inner side of the right ankle, which is then covered with plastic wrap for 20 min to facilitate heat generation and maintain a stable, continuous pain sensation (Li et al., 2020). After the 20-min period, participants rate the pain intensity on the inner side of the right ankle. To confirm effective induction of test stimulus, participants must report a moderate pain intensity to proceed; otherwise, they are excluded from the study. Eligible participants immerse their left hand in 12°C cold water for 1 min, after which they rate the pain intensity and attention focus level on the inner side of the right ankle within 3 min. The expectancy group receives verbal instructions (‘The water temperature will be automatically adjusted to achieve a good analgesic effect’), while the control group is informed that the water temperature remains unchanged. Both groups then undergo a second one-minute cold water immersion, after which participants rate the pain intensity, attention focus level, and anticipated analgesic effect on the inner side of the right ankle within 4 min. Finally, a 3D locator captures the channel position information. At the conclusion of the experiment, participants are compensated with 25 RMB, the expectancy group is debriefed on the true nature of the experiment, and psychological counseling is provided to all participants.

2.6 Statistical analyses

Data preprocessing, individual-level analysis, and 2D and 3D result visualization for fNIRS were performed using the NIRS_KIT data processing toolkit in MATLAB R2013b (Hou et al., 2021). Based on the simplest and most direct Bonferroni correction method within the FWE correction criteria (Oshina and Spigulis, 2021), the raw optical density data was converted into blood oxygen concentration changes. The accuracy of fNIRS signal data is often affected by interference noise and slow head movements of participants. To address this, a first-order polynomial regression model for detrending was applied in task-based analysis preprocessing to estimate linear and nonlinear trends, which were then subtracted from the original hemoglobin concentration signals. Temporal Derivative Distribution Repair was utilized to correct motion artifacts (Fishburn et al., 2019). For this study, an infinite impulse response (IIR) bandpass filter range of 0.01–0.018 Hz was applied to eliminate unrelated high- and low-frequency components. Individual-level analysis was performed using a general linear model (GLM), with the hemodynamic response function convolved to calculate β values as activation indices of oxygenated hemoglobin (HbO2) in the prefrontal cortex under different task conditions (Hall et al., 2021; Yeung et al., 2020).

Finally, 2 (experimental condition: expectancy group/neutral group) × 2 (time: pre-test/post-test) ANOVA was conducted on the β values for each channel before and after testing, using the statistical software SPSS 25.0.

3 Results

3.1 Behavioral results

ANOVA for experimental condition groups and time (pre-test/post-test) revealed a significant interaction between experimental condition and pre/post-test scores of the test stimulus pain intensity, F(1, 37) = 4.725, p = 0.036, η2p = 0.113. Simple effects analysis indicated no significant difference between the control group’s pre-test score (4.926 ± 0.359) and post-test score (4.729 ± 0.421) for the test stimulus pain intensity. In contrast, the expectancy group’s post-test score (3.458 ± 0.410) was significantly lower than its pre-test score (4.283 ± 0.350), F(1, 37) = 16.758, p = 0.000, η2p = 0.312. The pre-test scores between the expectancy group (4.283 ± 0.350) and control group (4.926 ± 0.359) showed no significant difference; however, the control group’s post-test score (4.729 ± 0.421) was significantly higher than the expectancy group’s post-test score (3.458 ± 0.410), F(1, 37) = 4.685, p = 0.037, η2p = 0.112, as shown in Figure 3. ANOVA on experimental conditions and pre/post-test attention scores to test stimulus revealed a significant interaction between pre/post-test attention scores and experimental conditions, F(1, 37) = 9.383, p = 0.004, η2p = 0.202. Simple effects analysis showed no significant difference between the pre-test attention scores of the control group (5.061 ± 0.430) and the expectancy group (4.660 ± 0.419). However, the post-test attention score of the control group (4.595 ± 0.366) was significantly higher than that of the expectancy group (3.080 ± 0.357), F(1, 37) = 8.776, p = 0.005, η2p = 0.192. There was no significant difference between the control group’s pre-test (5.061 ± 0.430) and post-test attention scores (4.595 ± 0.366), whereas the expectancy group’s post-test attention score (3.080 ± 0.357) was significantly lower than its pre-test score (4.660 ± 0.419), F(1, 37) = 38.730, p = 0.000, η2p = 0.511, as shown in Figure 3.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Analysis of variance for experimental conditions and time (pre- and post-assessment scores of experimental stimulus pain intensity and attention on the calf). (a) The difference in the pre- and post-assessment scores of experimental stimulus pain intensity perceived by the two groups of participants under different experimental conditions. The pain intensity of the experimental stimulus was measured using an 11-point scale ranging from 0 to 10, where 0 indicates no pain and 10 represents the most unbearable or imaginable pain. (b) The difference in the pre- and post-assessment scores of attention on the right ankle, as measured under different experimental conditions. The level of attention concentration on the right ankle was rated using the 11-point scale. *indicates p < 0.05, **indicates p < 0.01, and the error bars in the figure represent the standard error.

Pearson correlation analysis revealed a significant positive correlation was observed between participants’ post-test pain intensity scores for the experimental stimulus and their attention post-test scores (p < 0.01, r = 0.884).

3.2 fNIRS results

ANOVA on the β values for experimental conditions and pre/post-test times across 37 channels revealed significant main effects in the left frontal eye field (Channel 6), left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Channel 13), and left frontal pole (Channel 20), F(1, 37) = 4.389, p = 0.043, η2p = 0.106; F(1, 37) = 4.621, p = 0.038, η2p = 0.111; F(1, 37) = 5.454, p < 0.025, η2p = 0.128 (Figure 4). As presented in Table 2, post-hoc comparisons indicated that the activation levels in Channels 6, 13, and 20 were significantly lower in the post-test than in the pre-test.

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Brain region activation difference map. (a) The main effect map representing brain region activation (from left to right: right, anterior, left views), with involved brain regions being the Left Frontal eye fields (channel 6), the left DLPFC (channel 13), and the left frontopolar area (channel 20). (b) The interaction effect map representing brain region activation (from left to right: right, anterior, left views), with involved brain regions being the right frontopolar area (channel 18), the frontopolar area (channel 19), and the left frontopolar area (channel 21). The red areas within the borders indicate significant activation of these brain regions.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Main effect results for channels 6, 13, and 20.

The interaction between experimental conditions and the pre/post-test β values for Channels 18, 19, and 21 was significant, F(1, 37) = 4.136, p = 0.05, η2p = 0.106; F(1, 37) = 4.258, p = 0.046, η2p = 0.106; F(1, 37) = 5.222, p = 0.028, η2p = 0.124 (Figure 4).

Simple effects analysis indicated that in the control group, cortical activity intensity in the right frontopolar area (Channel 18) during the post-test (−0.002 ± 0.002) was significantly higher than that observed in the expectancy group (−0.007 ± 0.002). For the expectancy group, cortical activity intensity in the right frontopolar area (Channel 18) during the post-test (−0.007 ± 0.002) was significantly lower compared to the pre-test (−0.002 ± 0.002). Similarly, post-test activation levels in frontopolar area (Channel 19) for the expectancy group (−0.008 ± 0.002) were significantly lower than the pre-test levels (−0.001 ± 0.002). Additionally, in the expectancy group, cortical activation in the left Frontopolar area (Channel 21) during the post-test (−0.006 ± 0.002) was significantly lower than during the pre-test (0.001 ± 0.003).

Pearson correlation analysis revealed a significant positive correlation between Channel 7(Left pre–SMA) and participants’ post-test attention scores (p < 0.05, r = 0.347).

4 Discussion

4.1 The relationship between expectancy and CPM

By manipulating the experimental treatments for the expectancy and control groups, this study compared changes in pre- and post-test experimental stimulus pain intensity under different conditions, where lower pain intensity indicates a stronger CPM effect. Results showed that when positive expectancy was effectively induced, the expectancy group experienced more pronounced pain relief, resulting in a significantly greater CPM effect. This finding confirms that expectancy, as an intrinsic psychological mechanism, indeed plays a pivotal role in modulating CPM effects. The DLPFC, recognized as a key brain region in CPM modulation, is often associated with activation during expectancy-driven pain alleviation (Wager et al., 2011; Watson et al., 2009). However, this study revealed that under conditions of certain positive expectancy, the DLPFC exhibited notable deactivation. In fact, DLPFC activation depends on the certainty of the expectancy; uncertain expectancy is more likely to induce significant DLPFC activation than certain expectancy (Carlsson et al., 2006). Increased DLPFC signaling is, conversely, linked to reduced CPM capacity (Youssef et al., 2016). Moreover, a study on neuropathic pain treatment found that the analgesic effects of neuropharmacological agents were more closely related to deactivation, rather than activation, of the occipital and frontal lobes (Iannetti et al., 2005; Kong et al., 2007). Therefore, DLPFC deactivation supports the occurrence of CPM-induced analgesic effects.

4.2 The relationship between attention and CPM

While prior research has suggested that attention and CPM regulate pain through independent physiological mechanisms (Moont et al., 2012) and do not rely on the role of attention (Ladouceur et al., 2012), this study observed significant differences in post-test attention scores for test stimulus between the control and experimental groups, as well as significant pre- and post-test differences in attention scores within the expectancy group. These results indicate that attention may indeed play a meaningful role in CPM.

The perception of pain is regulated by top-down attentional mechanisms (Desimone and Duncan, 1995; Kastner and Ungerleider, 2000; Mouraux and Iannetti, 2009; Petersen and Posner, 2012). The prefrontal cortex, a critical part of the cerebral cortex, comprises regions such as the frontal eye fields, Frontopolar area, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Fuster, 2001). It is implicated in diverse complex cognitive activities, including decision-making, working memory, and attentional regulation (Fuster, 2001; Miller and Cohen, 2001), with the left prefrontal region particularly essential for higher-order cognitive tasks. Studies employing the N-back paradigm to explore prefrontal structure and function have underscored the significance of the left prefrontal cortex in higher-level cognitive control (Volle et al., 2008). Additionally, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies on executive functions have confirmed that the left prefrontal cortex plays a role in attentional shifting (Sylvester et al., 2003). The frontal eye fields (FEF), part of the prefrontal cortex, are directly involved in the control of stimulus-driven attention (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002). While traditionally associated with visual attention and ocular movement control (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002), the FEF’s involvement in this study’s analysis of the relationship between CPM effects and attention demonstrates its role in modulating pain information, Undoubtedly, the changes in attention are intricately linked to the regulation by the SMA brain region (Borsook et al., 2013), The significant positive correlation between Channel 7 (Left pre–SMA) and the post-test attention scores further reinforces this connection, supporting the possibility that attention contributes to the regulation of pain within CPM-induced analgesia. The significant positive correlation between post-test attention and post-test TS pain intensity offers compelling evidence that reinforces the potential role of attention in modulating the CPM analgesic effect.

4.3 The relationship between expectancy and attention

Contemporary models of attention comprise both ‘top-down’ and ‘bottom-up’ selection mechanisms (Legrain et al., 2009). ‘Top-down’ selection represents a goal-directed, conscious cognitive process in which attention is unconsciously captured by stimuli (Failing and Theeuwes, 2018). This process enhances neural activity in response to relevant stimuli while suppressing activity related to irrelevant stimuli, thereby modulating the sensitivity of stimulus-specific neural responses (Desimone and Duncan, 1995).

Pain is recognized as a ‘bottom-up’ control mechanism that automatically draws attention through warning signals, disrupting ongoing activities (Eccleston and Crombez, 1999; Van Damme et al., 2004) and prioritizing protective behaviors to avert bodily harm (Legrain et al., 2011). This indicates that ‘bottom-up’ attention to pain can be modulated through ‘top-down’ mechanisms. In pain management, ‘top-down’ regulation of attention using distraction tasks has demonstrated efficacy in reducing pain perception (Johnson, 2005; Kohl et al., 2013). For example, engaging participants in highly demanding visual tasks reduces the amplitude of the P2 component of nociceptive-evoked potentials (Legrain et al., 2005). Expectancy, as a proactive coping strategy initiated by the brain before the onset of a stimulus, influences the allocation of attentional resources (Atlas and Wager, 2012; Desimone and Duncan, 1995; Summerfield and Egner, 2009). The frontal pole is not only involved in complex task-related decision-making and expectancy (Koechlin and Hyafil, 2007) but also plays a role in attentional regulation (Irani et al., 2007). This study revealed significant behavioral differences in pre- and post-test attention to experimental stimuli between the expectancy and control groups, as well as variations in blood oxygen changes in the left and right frontal poles detected via fNIRS. The marked changes in blood oxygen concentration in the frontal pole may suggest that expectancy processing of pain information within the CPM effect facilitated shifts in attention away from test stimulus pain. This implies that the psychological mechanism of the CPM effect is driven by expectancy through a ‘top-down’ process, influencing analgesic outcomes by modulating the allocation of attention.

4.4 Changes in the PFC within CPM

The PFC serves as a central component of the brain’s cognitive control network (Badre, 2008), with distinct functional differences between the left and right PFC. The left PFC predominantly processes the meaning and features of individual events, facilitates rapid event activation, and strongly inhibits adjacent events. In contrast, the right PFC emphasizes cross-temporal and spatial integration of event meanings and characteristics, engages in slower event activation, and exhibits weaker facilitation of neighboring events (Wood and Grafman, 2003). Research using alpha brainwave activity to examine lateralization effects in the PFC has shown that the left PFC supports the behavioral activation system (BAS), while the right PFC underpins the behavioral inhibition system (BIS; Coan and Allen, 2003). BAS is linked to reward pursuit and positive behavior, functioning as a motivational system that drives individuals toward desired outcomes or potentially rewarding stimuli (Kennis et al., 2013). In contrast, BIS is described as a system that interrupts ongoing behavior, heightens arousal, and increases attentional focus in response to potential punishment, novel stimuli, or uncertain outcomes (Gray, 1994; Corr, 2004). Furthermore, the lateralization of the PFC is evident in emotion processing, with the left PFC primarily regulating positive emotions, while the right PFC specializes in processing negative emotions (Demaree et al., 2005). Activation of the BAS system promotes approach behaviors and elicits positive emotional experiences, whereas deficits in the BAS system increase the risk of depression (McFarland et al., 2006). Conversely, activation of the BIS system is associated with negative emotions, such as anxiety (Li et al., 2008).

The fNIRS results from this study indicated that changes in brain activity were predominantly localized to the left PFC regions, specifically the left frontal eye field, left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and Left Frontopolar area. This highlights the prominent role of the left PFC in pain regulation. During experimentally induced pain, increased activity in the left PFC correlates with pain perception, and when pain is alleviated due to motivational drives for pain relief rewards, activity in the left PFC decreases (Silva Passadouro et al., 2022). This suggests that inducing a clear analgesic expectancy effectively activates reward-seeking motivation (Fields, 2007), leading to reduced levels of oxygenated hemoglobin in the left PFC and the suppression of right PFC activity in pain modulation. Supporting evidence includes studies demonstrating that right PFC oxygenated hemoglobin activity diminishes during expectancy-induced pain relief (Silva Passadouro et al., 2022).

4.5 Psychological mechanism of expectancy in modulating CPM

Pain is not merely a direct interpretation of noxious stimuli; rather, it represents a complex interplay of sensory, emotional, motivational, and cognitive experiences. This protective function fosters motivation and learning, encoded by the brain’s reward-motivation cortical circuits that register both the aversive aspects of pain and its relief (Navratilova and Porreca, 2014). These circuits encompass regions such as the prefrontal cortex, ventral tegmental area, amygdala, and hypothalamic (Haber and Knutson, 2010; Navratilova and Porreca, 2014; Schultz, 2000). Positive expectancy activates reward-seeking motivations, while negative expectancy triggers avoidance responses (Fields, 2007). Our findings also confirmed that when analgesic expectancy was clearly established, participants’ BAS system exhibited significant activation. This activation of positive expectancy aligns reward-seeking motivations with the drive to avoid pain, supported by neuroimaging evidence showing activation in the left PFC.

Moreover, the allocation of attention to information is shaped by motivation (Di Nocera et al., 2014) and modulated by emotional states, with their interplay influencing an individual’s subjective pain experience (Villemure and Schweinhardt, 2010). Studies in China have also established a significant correlation between heightened attention to pain and increased negative emotions (Liu, 2024). Positive and negative emotional states drive individuals to engage in escape or approach behaviors, while allowing them to learn and anticipate whether future circumstances will be threatening or beneficial (Wiech and Tracey, 2013).

According to the Cyclical Model of Pain, Executive Function, Emotion Regulation, and Self-Management (the COPES model), there exists a cyclical relationship among immature executive function and emotional regulation, reduced self-management capability, chronic pain experiences, and increased pain-related disability (Caes et al., 2021). Chinese scholars have suggested that individuals can learn to reduce negative experiences, thereby updating the perceived meaning of pain and fostering positive expectancy for future pain episodes (Liu et al., 2022). This theory provides new perspectives on the psychological mechanisms by which CPM alleviates pain. Researchers have proposed the Expectancy-Attention-CPM Modulation Model (EACM Model), depicted in Figure 5.

Figure 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. EACM model, where “–” indicates a decrease and “+” indicates an increase.

According to the EACM model: During the process of the CPM effect, individuals can learn to manage their pain expectancy through self-regulation. When positive expectancies are formed, they activate the individual’s positive motivational system, generating analgesic motivations aligned with pain avoidance. This influences attentional resource allocation, shifting focus away from pain, thereby reducing pain perception and enhancing the CPM analgesic effect. In contrast, negative expectancies elicit conflicting motivations that are inconsistent with pain avoidance, leading to increased attention to pain and heightened pain perception, ultimately weakening the CPM effect. Moreover, observed changes in PFC activity are closely linked to emotional regulation (Etkin et al., 2015). Hence, within this framework, emotions emerge as another psychological factor affecting CPM efficacy. Emotions can interact with attention to directly shape pain experiences or exert direct influence on the CPM effect. For example, negative emotional states such as pain catastrophizing correlate with reduced pain modulation capacity, where higher levels of pain catastrophizing are associated with weaker pain regulation and lower CPM efficiency (King et al., 2013).

4.6 Limitations

This study utilized two distinct pain induction methods. The first method involved applying capsaicin to the body to induce heat pain as an experimental paradigm, offering a highly reproducible form of stimulation. The second method employed was the cold pressor test (CPT), a widely used and effective pain induction paradigm in laboratory settings, in which participants immerse their hands in cold water up to the wrist. Researchers used 12°C water to elicit pain in participants, effectively serving as a conditioned stimulus. However, due to the relatively slow temperature regulation of cold-water apparatuses, precise control over the intensity of the conditioned stimulus remains challenging. Future studies should explore alternative induction methods, such as laser or electrically induced pain stimuli. Moreover, pain and conditioned pain modulation, as experiences and regulatory processes with emotional dimensions, involve not only cortical structures like the DLPFC but also deeper nuclei such as the periaqueductal gray (PAG) and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC; Yarnitsky, 2010). Regrettably, due to the inherent limitations of fNIRS (Scholkmann et al., 2014), this study was confined to observing cortical activity, thereby precluding the investigation of brain activity in deeper structures implicated in conditioned pain modulation. This limitation may have constrained the depth of the findings. Future research could integrate fMRI, TMS, and other technologies to further investigate additional brain regions associated with pain processing, offering a more comprehensive view of the neural mechanisms underlying CPM-induced analgesia.

Laboratory conditions differ substantially from clinical settings. The ecological validity of the conclusions derived from this study in the laboratory—regarding their potential application in clinical practice—remains to be further examined. Furthermore, as the participants in this study were all healthy adults, it is crucial to explore whether these findings can be generalized to other populations, such as chronic pain sufferers or the elderly. Future research should investigate the psychological and neural mechanisms involved in pain processing during conditioned pain modulation across diverse populations, thereby improving the ecological validity of this study.

The expectancy manipulation in this study was induced through verbal guidance, potentially subject to individual differences such as personality traits, which could lead to varying degrees of effectiveness in the expectancy group. Furthermore, the expectancy approach utilized may involve elements of deception. Although participants provided informed consent prior to the experiment and received full debriefing afterward, this process may still raise concerns about infringing upon their right to informed consent. Thus, minimizing or avoiding deception remains a pressing issue. Researchers have proposed the use of an ‘open-label’ approach, which may serve as a means to induce expectancy effects without deception. This involves enlisting participants who have previously undergone expectancy induction to facilitate expectancy effects in current participants (Charlesworth et al., 2017). Nevertheless, whether the ‘open-label’ approach can truly avoid deception warrants further investigation.

5 Conclusion

This study employed a CPM induction paradigm with capsaicin-induced pain as the test stimulus (TS) and a fixed-duration cold-water immersion as the conditioning stimulus (CS). The findings underscored that expectancy serves as a pivotal psychological mechanism in CPM, with “top-down” expectancy amplifying the analgesic effects of CPM. Moreover, the study identified a potential role for attention in modulating CPM effects, suggesting that expectancy may regulate CPM through attention control. To further elucidate these mechanisms, the researchers introduced the ECMA model, aiming to describe the psychological dynamics of CPM and offer innovative perspectives for its clinical application, ultimately benefiting patients experiencing pain.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

This study had got the approval and the informed consent from the Ethics Committee of Inner Mongolia Normal University. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

XL: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. ML: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. BL: Data curation, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft. HY: Data curation, Software, Writing – original draft. XC: Data curation, Writing – original draft. HB: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. Inner Mongolia Natural Science Foundation Program, grant number: 2021MS03099.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Amanzio, M., Benedetti, F., Porro, C. A., Palermo, S., and Cauda, F. (2013). Activation likelihood estimation meta–analysis of brain correlates of placebo analgesia in human experimental pain. Human brain mapping 34, 738–752. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21471

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Atisook, R., Euasobhon, P., Saengsanon, A., and Jensen, M. P. (2021). Validity and utility of four pain intensity measures for use in international research. Journal ofPain Research 14, 1129–1139. doi: 10.2147/JPR.S303305

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Atlas, L. Y., and Wager, T. D. (2012). How expectations shape pain. Neurosci. Lett. 520, 140–148. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.03.039

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Badre, D. (2008). Cognitive control, hierarchy, and the rostro–caudal organization of the frontal lobes. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 193–200. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Borsook, D., Edwards, R., Elman, I., Becerra, L., and Levine, J. (2013). Pain and analgesia: the value of salience circuits. Prog. Neurobiol. 104, 93–105. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.02.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Borsook, D., Youssef, A. M., Simons, L., Elman, I., and Eccleston, C. (2018). When pain gets stuck, the evolution of pain chronification and treatment resistance. Pain 159, 2421–2436. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001401

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bunk, S., Emch, M., Koch, K., Lautenbacher, S., Zuidema, S., and Kunz, M. (2020). Pain processing in older adults and its association with prefrontal characteristics. Brain Sci. 10:477. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10080477

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Caes, L., Dick, B., Duncan, C., and Allan, J. (2021). The cyclical relation between chronic pain, executive functioning, emotional regulation, and self–management. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 46, 286–292. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsaa114

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlsson, K., Andersson, J., Petrovic, P., Petersson, K. M., Ohman, A., and Ingvar, M. (2006). Predictability modulates the affective and sensory–discriminative neural processing of pain. NeuroImage 32, 1804–1814. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.05.027

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Charlesworth, J., Petkovic, G., Kelley, J. M., Hunter, M., Onakpoya, I., Roberts, N., et al. (2017). Effects of placebos without deception compared with no treatment, a systematic review and meta–analysis. J. Evid. Based Med. 10, 97–107. doi: 10.1111/jebm.12251

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Coan, J. A., and Allen, J. J. (2003). Frontal EEG asymmetry and the behavioral activation and inhibition systems. Psychophysiology 40, 106–114. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.00011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Corbetta, M., and Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal–directed and stimulus–driven attention in the brain. Nature reviews. Neuroscience 3, 201–215. doi: 10.1038/nrn755

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cormier, S., Piché, M., and Rainville, P. (2013). Expectations modulate heterotopic noxious counter–stimulation analgesia. J. Pain 14, 114–125. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2012.10.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Corr, P. J. (2004). Reinforcement sensitivity theory and personality. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 28, 317–332. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.01.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Damien, J., Colloca, L., Bellei–Rodriguez, C. É., and Marchand, S. (2018). Pain Modulation, From Conditioned Pain Modulation to Placebo and Nocebo Effects in Experimental and Clinical Pain. International review of neurobiology, 139, 255–296. doi: 10.1016/bs.irn.2018.07.024

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Demaree, H. A., Everhart, D. E., Youngstrom, E. A., and Harrison, D. W. (2005). Brain lateralization of emotional processing, historical roots and a future incorporating “dominance”. Behav. Cogn. Neurosci. Rev. 4, 3–20. doi: 10.1177/1534582305276837

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Deng, X. F., Luo, F., and Guo, J. Y. (2015). Opposing twins: mechanisms of placebo and nocebo effects based on pain. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 23, 822–835. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2015.00822

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Desimone, R., and Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 193–222. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Nocera, D., Finzi, A., Rossi, S., and Staffa, M. (2014). The role of intrinsic motivations in attention allocation and shifting. Front. Psychol. 5:273. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00273

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eccleston, C., and Crombez, G. (1999). Pain demands attention, a cognitive–affective model of the interruptive function of pain. Psychological bulletin 125, 356–366. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.125.3.356

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Etkin, A., Büchel, C., and Gross, J. J. (2015). The neural bases of emotion regulation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 693–700. doi: 10.1038/nrn4044

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Failing, M., and Theeuwes, J. (2018). Selection history, how reward modulates selectivity of visual attention. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 25, 514–538. doi: 10.3758/s13423-017-1380-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fields, H. L. (2007). Understanding how opioids contribute to reward and analgesia. Reg. Anesth. Pain Med. 32, 242–246. doi: 10.1097/00115550-200705000-00012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fishburn, F. A., Ludlum, R. S., Vaidya, C. J., and Medvedev, A. V. (2019). Temporal derivative distribution repair (TDDR), a motion correction method for fNIRS. NeuroImage 184, 171–179. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.025

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

France, C. R., Burns, J. W., Gupta, R. K., Buvanendran, A., Chont, M., Schuster, E., et al. (2016). Expectancy effects on conditioned pain modulation are not influenced by naloxone or morphine. Annals of behavioral medicine, a publication of the Society of Behavioral Medicine 50, 497–505. doi: 10.1007/s12160-016-9775-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuster, J. M. (2001). The prefrontal cortex––an update, time is of the essence. Neuron 30, 319–333. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00285-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, J. A. (1994). The psychophysiological basis of personality. In: Handbook of personality: Theory and research. Eds. C. R. Martin and D. M. H. Pearson (Cambridge University Press), pp. 83–114.

Google Scholar

Haber, S. N., and Knutson, B. (2010). The reward circuit, linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacol., Official Pub. American College of Neuropsychopharmacol. 35, 4–26. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.129

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, M., Kidgell, D., Perraton, L., Morrissey, J., and Jaberzadeh, S. (2021). Pain induced changes in brain oxyhemoglobin, a systematic review and Meta–analysis of functional NIRS studies. Pain Med. (Malden, Mass.) 22, 1399–1410. doi: 10.1093/pm/pnaa453

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hong, R. Q., Wang, Y. R., Lin, S. E., and Li, D. R. (2003). Application of the numeric pain rating scale in cancer pain treatment. Practical Nurs. J. 7, 48–49. doi: 10.3760/cma.j.issn.1672-7088.2003.13.036

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hou, X., Zhang, Z., Zhao, C., Duan, L., Gong, Y., Li, Z., et al. (2021). NIRS–KIT, a MATLAB toolbox for both resting–state and task fNIRS data analysis. Neurophotonics 8:010802. doi: 10.1117/1.NPh.8.1.010802

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Iannetti, G. D., Zambreanu, L., Wise, R. G., Buchanan, T. J., Huggins, J. P., Smart, T. S., et al. (2005). Pharmacological modulation of pain–related brain activity during normal and central sensitization states in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 102, 18195–18200. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0506624102

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Irani, F., Platek, S. M., Bunce, S., Ruocco, A. C., and Chute, D. (2007). Functional near infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS), an emerging neuroimaging technology with important applications for the study of brain disorders. The Clinical neuropsychologist 21, 9–37. doi: 10.1080/13854040600910018

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, M. H. (2005). How does distraction work in the management of pain. Curr. Pain Headache Rep. 9, 90–95. doi: 10.1007/s11916-005-0044-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kastner, S., and Ungerleider, L. G. (2000). Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 23, 315–341. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.315

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kennedy, D. L., Kemp, H. I., Ridout, D., Yarnitsky, D., and Rice, A. (2016). Reliability of conditioned pain modulation, a systematic review. Pain 157, 2410–2419. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000689

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kennis, M., Rademaker, A. R., and Geuze, E. (2013). Neural correlates of personality, an integrative review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 73–95. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.10.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

King, C. D., Goodin, B., Kindler, L. L., Caudle, R. M., Edwards, R. R., Gravenstein, N., et al. (2013). Reduction of conditioned pain modulation in humans by naltrexone, an exploratory study of the effects of pain catastrophizing. Journal of behavioral medicine 36, 315–327. doi: 10.1007/s10865-012-9424-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koechlin, E., and Hyafil, A. (2007). Anterior prefrontal function and the limits of human decision–making. Science 318, 594–598. doi: 10.1126/science.1142995

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kohl, A., Rief, W., and Glombiewski, J. A. (2013). Acceptance, cognitive restructuring, and distraction as coping strategies for acute pain. J. Pain 14, 305–315. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2012.12.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kong, J., Kaptchuk, T. J., Polich, G., Kirsch, I., and Gollub, R. L. (2007). Placebo analgesia, findings from brain imaging studies and emerging hypotheses. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 173–190. doi: 10.1515/revneuro.2007.18.3-4.173

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ladouceur, A., Tessier, J., Provencher, B., Rainville, P., and Piché, M. (2012). Top–down attentional modulation of analgesia induced by heterotopic noxious counterstimulation. Pain 153, 1755–1762. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.05.019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Legrain, V., Bruyer, R., Guérit, J. M., and Plaghki, L. (2005). Involuntary orientation of attention to unattended deviant nociceptive stimuli is modulated by concomitant visual task difficulty. Evidence from laser evoked potentials. Clin. Neurophysiol. Official J. Int. Federation of Clin. Neurophysiol. 116, 2165–2174. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.05.019

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Legrain, V., Damme, S. V., Eccleston, C., Davis, K. D., Seminowicz, D. A., and Crombez, G. (2009). A neurocognitive model of attention to pain, behavioral and neuroimaging evidence. Pain 144, 230–232. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.03.020

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Legrain, V., Iannetti, G. D., Plaghki, L., and Mouraux, A. (2011). The pain matrix reloaded, a salience detection system for the body. Prog. Neurobiol. 93, 111–124. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2010.10.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Levine, J. D., Gordon, N. C., and Fields, H. L. (1978). The mechanism of placebo analgesia. Lancet 312, 654–657. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(78)92762-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, G. N., Heales, L., Rice, D. A., Rome, K., and McNair, P. J. (2012). Reliability of the conditioned pain modulation paradigm to assess endogenous inhibitory pain pathways. Pain Res. Manag. 17, 98–102. doi: 10.1155/2012/610561

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, W., Liu, P., Hu, Y., and Meng, J. (2020). Pain modulates responses to emotional stimuli. Front. Psychol. 11:595987. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.595987

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Y. Z., Zhang, Y., Jiang, Y., Li, H., Mi, S., Yi, G. J., et al. (2008). Reliability and validity analysis of the Chinese version of the behavioral inhibition/activation system scale. Chinese J. Mental Health 8, 613–616. doi: 10.3321/j.issn:1000-6729.2008.08.015

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, B. (2024). Psychological and neural mechanisms of inhibition function in pain relief (Master's thesis). Inner Mongolia Normal University, Hohhot.

Google Scholar

Liu, B., Cheng, X. J., Yue, H., and Bao, H. G. J. L. (2022). The role of inhibition function in pain. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 30, 1253–1261. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.01253

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McFarland, B. R., Shankman, S. A., Tenke, C. E., Bruder, G. E., and Klein, D. N. (2006). Behavioral activation system deficits predict the six–month course of depression. J. Affect. Disord. 91, 229–234. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.01.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, E. K., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167–202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Modir, J. G., and Wallace, M. S. (2010). Human experimental pain models 3, heat/capsaicin sensitization and intradermal capsaicin models. Methods Molecular Biol. (Clifton) 617, 169–174. doi: 10.1007/978-1-60327-323-7_14

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohr, C., Leyendecker, S., Mangels, I., Machner, B., Sander, T., and Helmchen, C. (2008). Central representation of cold–evoked pain relief in capsaicin induced pain, an event–related fMRI study. Pain 139, 416–430. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2008.05.020

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moont, R., Crispel, Y., Lev, R., Pud, D., and Yarnitsky, D. (2012). Temporal changes in cortical activation during distraction from pain, a comparative LORETA study with conditioned pain modulation. Brain Res. 1435, 105–117. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.11.056

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moriarty, O., McGuire, B. E., and Finn, D. P. (2011). The effect of pain on cognitive function, a review of clinical and preclinical research. Prog. Neurobiol. 93, 385–404. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2011.01.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mouraux, A., and Iannetti, G. D. (2009). Nociceptive laser–evoked brain potentials do not reflect nociceptive–specific neural activity. J. Neurophysiol. 101, 3258–3269. doi: 10.1152/jn.91181.2008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Navratilova, E., and Porreca, F. (2014). Reward and motivation in pain and pain relief. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 1304–1312. doi: 10.1038/nn.3811

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicholas, M. K., Asghari, A., Corbett, M., Smeets, R. J., Wood, B. M., Overton, S., et al. (2012). Is adherence to pain self–management strategies associated with improved pain, depression and disability in those with disabling chronic pain. European J. Pain (London, England) 16, 93–104. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2011.06.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nilsen, K. B., Olsen, I. C., Solem, A. N., and Matre, D. (2014). A large conditioned pain modulation response is not related to a large blood pressure response, a study in healthy men. European J. Pain, EJP 18, 1271–1279. doi: 10.1002/j.1532-2149.2014.486.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nir, R. R., Yarnitsky, D., Honigman, L., and Granot, M. (2012). Cognitive manipulation targeted at decreasing the conditioning pain perception reduces the efficacy of conditioned pain modulation. Pain 153, 170–176. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2011.10.010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nuwailati, R., Bobos, P., Drangsholt, M., and Curatolo, M. (2022). Reliability of conditioned pain modulation in healthy individuals and chronic pain patients, a systematic review and meta–analysis. Scand J Pain 22, 262–278. doi: 10.1515/sjpain-2021-0149

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Oshina, I., and Spigulis, J. (2021). Beer-Lambert law for optical tissue diagnostics: current state of the art and the main limitations. J. Biomed. Opt. 26:100901. doi: 10.1117/1.JBO.26.10.100901

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, S. E., and Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system of the human brain, 20 years after. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 35, 73–89. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pickering, G., Pereira, B., Dufour, E., Soule, S., and Dubray, C. (2014). Impaired modulation of pain in patients with postherpetic neuralgia. Pain Res. Manag. 19, e19–e23. doi: 10.1155/2014/507947

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Plinsinga, M. L., Vuvan, V., Maclachlan, L., Klyne, D., Vicenzino, B., Hodges, P., et al. (2023). Pain–related cognitions and emotional distress are not associated with conditioned pain modulation: an explorative analysis of 1142 participants with acute, subacute, and chronic pain. Pain 164, 1593–1599. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000002864

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ploghaus, A., Becerra, L., Borras, C., and Borsook, D. (2003). Neural circuitry underlying pain modulation, expectation, hypnosis, placebo. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 197–200. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00061-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pud, D., Granovsky, Y., and Yarnitsky, D. (2009). The methodology of experimentally induced diffuse noxious inhibitory control (DNIC)–like effect in humans. Pain 144, 16–19. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.02.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rieder, T. N. (2019). The painful truth about pain. Nature 573:S16. doi: 10.1038/d41586-019-02688-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Scholkmann, F., Kleiser, S., Metz, A. J., Zimmermann, R., Mata Pavia, J., Wolf, U., et al. (2014). A review on continuous wave functional near–infrared spectroscopy and imaging instrumentation and methodology. NeuroImage 85, 6–27. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, W. (2000). Multiple reward signals in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 1, 199–207. doi: 10.1038/35044563

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, D. J., Stohler, C. S., Egnatuk, C. M., Wang, H., Koeppe, R. A., and Zubieta, J. K. (2008). Placebo and nocebo effects are defined by opposite opioid and dopaminergic responses. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 65, 220–231. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.34

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shih, Y. W., Tsai, H. Y., Lin, F. S., Lin, Y. H., Chiang, C. Y., Lu, Z. L., et al. (2019). Effects of positive and negative expectations on human pain perception engage separate but interrelated and dependently regulated cerebral mechanisms. J. Neurosci. 39, 1261–1274. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2154-18.2018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Silva Passadouro, B., Delgado Sanchez, A., Henshaw, J., Lopez Diaz, K., Trujillo Barreto, N. J., Jones, A., et al. (2022). Frontal alpha asymmetry, a potential biomarker of approach–withdrawal motivation towards pain. Front. Pain Res. (Lausanne, Switzerland) 3:962722. doi: 10.3389/fpain.2022.962722

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Skyt, I., Moslemi, K., Baastrup, C., Grosen, K., Svensson, P., Jensen, T. S., et al. (2018). Does conditioned pain modulation predict the magnitude of placebo effects in patients with neuropathic pain. European J. Pain, EJP 22, 784–792. doi: 10.1002/ejp.1164

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, X. J., Fan, B. F., Wan, Y., Zhang, D. Y., Lü, Y., and Han, J. S. (2020). A brief analysis of the revised pain definition by the International Association for the Study of Pain. Chinese J. Pain Med. 26, 641–644. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1006-9852.2020.09.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sprenger, C., Bingel, U., and Büchel, C. (2011). Treating pain with pain, supraspinal mechanisms of endogenous analgesia elicited by heterotopic noxious conditioning stimulation. Pain 152, 428–439. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.11.018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Summerfield, C., and Egner, T. (2009). Expectation (and attention) in visual cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 403–409. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sylvester, C. Y., Wager, T. D., Lacey, S. C., Hernandez, L., Nichols, T. E., Smith, E. E., et al. (2003). Switching attention and resolving interference, fMRI measures of executive functions. Neuropsychologia 41, 357–370. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00167-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, Y., Tang, D. D., Peng, W. W., and Hu, L. (2016). 'Pain relieving pain': conditioned pain modulation. Sci. Bull. 61, 642–653. doi: 10.1360/N972015-00872

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, V. A., Chang, L., Rainville, P., and Roy, M. (2017). Learned expectations and uncertainty facilitate pain during classical conditioning. Pain 158, 1528–1537. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000948

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Damme, S., Crombez, G., and Eccleston, C. (2004). Disengagement from pain, the role of catastrophic thinking about pain. Pain 107, 70–76. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2003.09.023

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Villemure, C., and Schweinhardt, P. (2010). Supraspinal pain processing, distinct roles of emotion and attention. Neuroscientist, Rev. J. Bringing Neurobiol., Neurol. Psychiatry 16, 276–284. doi: 10.1177/1073858409359200

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Volle, E., Kinkingnéhun, S., Pochon, J. B., Mondon, K., Thiebaut De Schotten, M., Seassau, M., et al. (2008). The functional architecture of the left posterior and lateral prefrontal cortex in humans. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y., 1991) 18, 2460–2469. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wager, T. D., Atlas, L. Y., Leotti, L. A., and Rilling, J. K. (2011). Predicting individual differences in placebo analgesia, contributions of brain activity during expectancy and pain experience. J. Neurosci. 31, 439–452. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3420-10.2011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, M., Cheng, S., Li, Y. W., Li, H., and Zhang, D. D. (2023). The role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the placebo effect: a study on social emotion regulation. Acta Psychol. Sin. 55, 1063–1073. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.01063

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, A., El-Deredy, W., Iannetti, G. D., Lloyd, D., Tracey, I., Vogt, B. A., et al. (2009). Placebo conditioning and placebo analgesia modulate a common brain network during pain Expectancy and perception. Pain 145, 24–30. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.04.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wei, H., Zhou, L., Zhang, H., Chen, J., Lu, X., and Hu, L. (2018). The influence of expectation on nondeceptive placebo and nocebo effects. Pain Res. Manag. 2018, 8459429–8459428. doi: 10.1155/2018/8459429

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiech, K., and Tracey, I. (2013). Pain, decisions, and actions, a motivational perspective. Front. Neurosci. 7:46. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00046

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, J. N., and Grafman, J. (2003). Human prefrontal cortex, processing and representational perspectives. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 139–147. doi: 10.1038/nrn1033

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yarnitsky, D. (2010). Conditioned pain modulation (the diffuse noxious inhibitory control–like effect): its relevance for acute and chronic pain states. Curr. Opin. Anaesthesiol. 23, 611–615. doi: 10.1097/ACO.0b013e32833c348b

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeung, M. K., Lee, T. L., and Chan, A. S. (2020). Neurocognitive development of flanker and Stroop interference control, a near–infrared spectroscopy study. Brain Cogn. 143:105585. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2020.105585

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Youssef, A. M., Macefield, V. G., and Henderson, L. A. (2016). Cortical influences on brainstem circuitry responsible for conditioned pain modulation in humans. Human brain mapping 37, 2630–2644. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23199

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Z. P., Zhang, X. A., Feng, B. B., Guo, S. M., Zhao, Y. Q., and Wang, Y. L. (2020). The impact of exercise intervention on the human conditioned pain modulation system. China Rehab. 35, 609–612. doi: 10.3870/zgkf.2020.11.010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: pain, conditioned pain modulation, expectancy, attention, prefrontal cortex, functional near-infrared spectroscopy

Citation: Li X, Liu M, Liu B, Yue H, Cheng X and Bao H (2025) The effect of expectancy on conditioned pain modulation: evidence from functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Front. Psychol. 16:1525216. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1525216

Received: 09 November 2024; Accepted: 26 February 2025;
Published: 17 March 2025.

Edited by:

Wenxin Niu, Tongji University, China

Reviewed by:

Tengyu Zhang, National Research Center for Rehabilitation Technical Aids, China
Feng Li, Shanghai University of Sport, China

Copyright © 2025 Li, Liu, Liu, Yue, Cheng and Bao. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Hugejiletu Bao, YmFvaHVnZUBpbW51LmVkdS5jbg==

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.