![Man ultramarathon runner in the mountains he trains at sunset](https://d2csxpduxe849s.cloudfront.net/media/E32629C6-9347-4F84-81FEAEF7BFA342B3/0B4B1380-42EB-4FD5-9D7E2DBC603E79F8/webimage-C4875379-1478-416F-B03DF68FE3D8DBB5.png)
94% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.
Find out more
DATA REPORT article
Front. Psychol.
Sec. Environmental Psychology
Volume 16 - 2025 | doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1517430
This article is part of the Research Topic The Role of Empathy in Behavioral Change Toward Ethical Consumption and Environmental Sustainability View all 4 articles
The final, formatted version of the article will be published soon.
You have multiple emails registered with Frontiers:
Please enter your email address:
If you already have an account, please login
You don't have a Frontiers account ? You can register here
As humans become more sensitive to global warming, due to the visible effects on the planet and our daily lives, particularly our health (for example the increase of zoonotic diseases exacerbated by deforestation), we have an increasing need to reconnect with the natural world (Greenwell et al., 2023;Johansson et al., 2024). In cities, far from forests and savannas, zoos become the bridge to wild habitats, aiming to encourage proximity and connection between people and animals. Zoos allow their visitors to be close to wild animals, in many cases for the first time, while providing them with basic information related to the ecology, behavior, and conservation of these species. In zoos, friendly human-animal associations may further develop into bonds that positively influence both parties (Thomas et al., 2023). Moreover, visitors with a better learning experience and deeper connections with the animals have a better understanding of biodiversity conservation (Moss et al., 2017), are more critical of bad captive-wildlife tourist attractions (Sampaio et al., 2021), are more willing to donate to conservation actions (Ballantyne et al., 2007;Howell et al., 2019), and have increased desire to visit the animals in the wild (76% of visitors expressing a desire to see animals in their habitat [Adetola, & Akinboboye, 2020]). Most of these studies have been focused on a few African countries, some European countries, the USA, and Australia, largely overlooking non-western countries (MacNally et al., 2024). While there are exceptions, such as the studies by Wu et al. (2017) in China and Musa et al. (2015) in Malaysia, these are relatively few in Asia. People's connection to wildlife, and their beliefs and attitudes towards conservation needs, depend on cultural and socio-demographic traits (Ballentyne et al., 2021;Breuer et al., 2018;Thomas-Walters et al., 2023), we currently lack, the understanding of how zoos in some parts of the world can effectively transmit conservation messages tailored to their specific audiences. In Japan, the number of zoo visitors has increased in the last few decades (Davey, 2007) and the physiological effects of zoos were studied. The mandatory quarantine was not imposed on the Japanese population during the pandemic, and many indoor entertainment activities were suspended. Nonetheless, most Japanese zoos did not close their doors during the pandemic and were one of the few tourist attractions available to the public, providing close contact with wildlife. Zoos are considered to positively impact people's mental health through "green prescribing" interventions (Rose and Riley, 2022). Using both psychological (mood states) and physical indicators (i.e., blood pressure, and salivary cortisol and oxytocin levels), Akiyama and colleagues (2021) found a positive health effect associated with a zoo visit among a sampled population of elderly Japanese. Similarly, Sakagami and Ohta (2010) also found a decrease in blood pressure, increased physical exercise, and improved life sub-scale scores (i.e., generic health-related evaluation questionnaire) in participants of all ages visiting two different Japanese zoos. However, little research has been done on the actual interactions between visitors and zoo animals and their emotional responses and motivation for preserving species. In other words, how connected people are with animals is still poorly understood. Connection sense is defined here as an innate, empathetic bond that visitors may experience toward the animals they observe, ranging from absent to neutral, to a high level of connection. In this Data report, we survey visitants' motivation for conservation support, their feeling of connectedness with the captive animals in the zoo, and to what level they can relate zoonotic transmission between humans and animals. By focusing on a specific species, we provide participants with a precise "target", which can help them to reflect better and more accurately on the answers. This approach is expected to yield more sincere and insightful responses, as participants are more likely to form genuine connections with particular species. Moreover, targeting specific species allows us to understand which species elicit higher levels of awareness related to the viewing times.Permission to conduct this study was obtained from the Japan Monkey Centre (No. 2022-05). Consent forms were obtained from all visitors inquired following a short explanation of the study, attesting that responses were anonymous and that the data collected would be used for the study analysis only. No personal data was collected and taking part in the surveys did not interfere with the participant's visit to the zoo. Questionnaires to Chubu Gakuin University students were provided before and after a class on animal welfare and a visit to the Japan Monkey Centre for a behavioral observation in a lecture of Comparative Cognitive Development.Two zoological parks were included in this study: the Japan Monkey Centre (JMC) and Higashiyama Zoo and Botanical Gardens (HZ), Japan. JMC is home to 60 primate species focusing on this Genus only, while HZ hosts a wide range of animals and plants. Both zoos work closely with researchers and public education. Data were collected from August to November 2022 in JMC and from May to September 2023 in HZ. Participants (aged above 16 years old) were approached from 10:00 to 16:00 during zoo opening hours. Questionnaires were handed out to visitors on paper within the zoos' premises, while non-visitors completed an online version of the questionnaire. University students were provided with a link to the online questionnaire.Participants completed the questionnaire that included demographic information (sex, age, zoo membership), favorite animal, 21 questions related to general aspects of the zoo visit and awareness of wildlife conservation (adapted from Howell et al., 2019 andSkibins andPowell, 2013), and an additional free space for comments. The questions regarding wildlife connection consisted of closed response options and scaling (1 to 7) the level of connection and agreement with items regarding their personal views of wildlife and wildlife conservation (Supplementary material, Table 1). No definition of connection, wildlife, conservation and welfare was provided to the respondents prior to the questionnaire.A total of 630 participants completed the questionnaires. Two groups of undergraduate students were given the questionnaire twice: once before and once after visiting the zoo. A total of 56 students replied on both occasions and 53 students replied to the questionnaire either before or after the trip. The individuals who replied twice to the questionnaires were given ID codes to ensure their anonymity and were included in the analysis (others were excluded). These students were divided into 4 groups: 1) S22B stands for the Students of the 2022 Class Before the trip to the zoo, 2) S22A stands for the Students of the 2022 Class After the trip to the zoo, 3) S23B stands for the Students of the 2023 Class Before the trip to the zoo and 4) S23A stands for the Students of the 2022 Class After the trip to the zoo.In terms of gender distribution, 43 students identified as female in the university student population, 177 visitors identified as female in HZ, and 159 visitors identified as female in JMC. For male respondents, 11 students identified as male in the students, 123 visitors identified as male in HZ, and 101 visitors identified as male in JMC. 16 respondents replied "Other" or did not provide any information regarding their sex.All undergraduate students belonged to the GenZ generation. We classify Gen Z as individuals born between 1995 and2015;Millennials between 1980 and1994;Gen X between 1965 and1979;and Baby Boomers as those born before 1964 (Supplementary material Table 2).The percentages of responses related to the rate of connection with the animals reported by participants and the frequency reported for observation and interaction with the animals are presented in Table 1.The closed responses section of Likert Scales are presented in Figure 1. Overall, the HZ population exhibited a more varied response distribution compared to the JMC population. Responses from HZ often showed peaks at different response levels across multiple questions, indicating a higher degree of variability. In contrast, the JMC population demonstrated a more consistent pattern, with responses frequently clustering around specific levels. The Chubu Gakuin student population exhibited a relatively consistent response distribution across the questions.For each population, the analysis of Cronbach's Alpha showed α≥ 0.9 when combining all 16 questions together. To compare our findings with those of Howell et al. (2019), from which we adapted the questionnaire, we also tested the composite variables using the questions shared between both studies (Supplementary material, Table 3). The species-specific conservation caring composite variable demonstrated a Cronbach's Alpha similar to that found in Howell et al. (2019), suggesting that our measure of conservation caring is reliable and comparable to that of Howell's study. However, for other composite variables, the internal consistency was less robust. In the cases where there were fewer questions available for analysis, Cronbach's Alpha values ranged from 0.6 to 0.7, indicating questionable internal consistency and suggesting that more items may be needed for these variables to provide a reliable measure.In addition, focusing on the zoo sampled populations, we conducted a Principal Component Analysis to compare with Howell's findings, in particular for that same composite variable "species-specific conservation caring". We found a lower correlation (Pearson r-value= 0.3) to that of Howell's study but it still suggests that more individuals care about conservation as stronger their perceived connection to wildlife. Still the difference in the strength of this correlation may be attributed to cultural differences, or sample size.For preliminary statistical analysis focusing on our data and with the added questions, a Bayesian model was fitted to evaluate the effects of the 1) Connection rates in relation to the level of agreement in Q1-Q16 questions, 2) sex and age classes, 3) duration of animal observation, and 4) interaction with animals. For point 1, the response variable was the items of each question (Q1-Q16) and the predictor variable Connection was divided into "Low" (Likert items 1, 2 and 3) and "High" (items 5, 6 and 7), removing the "4" (medium) responses from the data to address the issue of potential imbalance on a 7point Likert scale. For points 2 to 4, the response variables were the Connection rate (continuous from 1-7) and the predictor variables were: Gender (women, men); Generation (Baby Boomer, Gen X, Gen Z, and Millennial); Time spent with the animals was categorized between "Short" (less than 10 minutes), "Medium" (between 11 to 30 minutes) and "High" (more than 30 minutes); and Interaction made with the animals were categorized between "No" (absent), "Short" (less than 5 seconds) or "Long" (more than 5 seconds). We were not able to correlate regular zoo visitors (with more than 10 visits to the zoo) and new visitors as the latter group is much more frequent. The model used a Gaussian family distribution, with 4 chains and 2,000 iterations per chain. All analyses were conducted in R version 4.3.1, with the brms package version 2.21.0. Statistical significance was interpreted through the 95% highest posterior density interval.For zoo visitors, we found that a stronger connection with favorite animals correlates with a stronger agreement with statements concerning interest in, motivation to protect, and emotional connection to animals. Across the questions Q1-Q15, the estimated effect sizes ranged from small to moderate, with corresponding 95% credible intervals ranging not containing zero. For Q16 ("Getting close to wild animals is dangerous"), the credible interval included zero (E = 0.123, 95% CI = [-0.251, 0.498]), meaning no clear evidence of an association, which suggests that risk assessment of proximity to wildlife does not correlate with the level of connection to the animals (Supplementary material, Table 4). In addition, we asked participants to choose one or more options regarding 1) their understanding of the consequences of getting close to wild animals in their habitat, and 2) what actions they would take when visiting tourist sites to observe wild animals (Table 2). Noting that each respondent could choose one or more responses, these results indicate that most visitors prioritized the risks of injury and the importance of maintaining a safe distance from wild animals. A high number of visitors also acknowledge the risks of disease and the importance of taking health precautions such as vaccinations and using masks. However, fewer participants prioritize testing for COVID-19 or are willing to pay extra for safer viewing conditions.Regarding sex and age class differences among participants in regards to their connection towards animals, we found sex-related differences in connection to favorite animals with the mean Connection rates for males being 0.35 points higher (95% CI: 0.10 to 0.61) compared to females. However, no significant differences were noted across generations when comparing "Baby boomer" to: Generation X Estimate: -0.10, 95% CI: -0.48 to 0.27; Generation Z Estimate: 0.04, 95% CI: -0.37 to 0.44, and Millennials Estimate: -0.29, 95% CI: -0.66 to 0.08). Moreover, we found no significant differences in Connection rates related to the duration of animal observation per visit (compared to "Long" time, "Medium" Estimate: -0.02, 95% CI: -2.44 to 2.36; "Short" Estimate: -0.02, 95% CI: -2.46 to 2.42), neither in the levels of interaction with the animals when comparing "Long" interactions with "No" Type Estimate: 0.00, 95% CI: -2.38 to 2.40, and "Short" Type Estimate: -0.04, 95% CI: -2.46 to 2.43 (Figure 2, see also Table 5 and 6 in Supplementary Material for descriptive results).Although we found a positive correlation between the connection rate to the animals and the Likert scale items in each questionindicating that a stronger connection with animals promotes a deeper desire to learn about and protect species (Carr, 2016;Howell et al., 2019) -the participants in this study often disagreed with the risks associated with approaching wild animals. In fact, more people answered that "getting close to wild animals may cause injury" (179 respondents) and "disease transmission" (179) while 252 respondents replied that this would grant them a clearer observation of the animal. This shows that despite being supportive of conservation and educational causes, visitors may still romanticize their connection to animals, underestimating the risk of close encounters with animals given their lack of experience beyond observing animals from the safety of a glass or enclosure's grid. Nevertheless, and considering that the respondents could choose more than one option, 223 respondents admitted that they would get a vaccine to observe wild animals, 258 would avoid approaching wild animals while feeling sick, and, importantly, 547 respondents said they would keep their distance from the wild animals. But only 79 respondents would test for COVID-19 and only 162 would wear masks, even though mask usage in Japan was commonplace even before the pandemic outbreak of 2020.Zoos provide the chance for a great number of tourists from all ages, levels of education, and socioeconomic backgrounds to experience an up-close wildlife encounter, without compromising natural habitats (Doodson et al., 2023) and simultaneously being a source of domestic tourism which is expected to recover faster than international tourism (Toyama, 2022;Kruger and Viljoen, 2022). While we categorized age and sex as variables in our design, we acknowledge that other factors (social context, tradition, demography, etc.) may have impacted visitors' conservation awareness or wildlife connection (Kruger and Viljoen, 2022). Nevertheless, age and sex are important factors to be considered when studying peoples' perceptions and attitudes towards wildlife, as they may influence results to a greater extent than the actual animals' welfare, health, and living environment (Alba et al., 2023). Some previous research has found females and younger age groups to have a more positive perception of wildlife, with women being more concerned about individual welfare and men being more concerned about species conservation (Figueredo et al., 2022;Alba et al., 2023). In addition, younger generations tend to be more critical of captivity in relation to animal welfare (Alba et al., 2023;Gurusamy et al., 2015) particularly in zoos, considering the limitations imposed on animals in exhibiting their natural behavioral repertoire (Pacheco and Madden, 2021) that often lead to increased stress signals and abnormal behavior (Rose et al., 2017) detected by visitors. However, Kruger and Viljoen (2022) found no significant relationship between age and sex in relation to attitudes toward conservation; similarly, a recent meta-analysis by McNally et al. (2024) across 56 studies revealed that age has no significant influence on attitudes toward wildlife. Nevertheless, McNally et al. found that samples with a higher proportion of females reported significantly smaller effect sizes compared to those with fewer females. These findings align with some of our observations, as we did not find marked cross-generational differences. Interestingly, our study also found that men reported higher levels of connection to animals, which contrasts with most findings from Western populations. In this particular case, it may be related to the lack of generational differences in our sampled population. Older Japanese generations may report a connection to the animals according to their religious beliefs, while younger generations may report connection levels in relation to the level of environmental education they have received, regardless of their beliefs. However, we cannot further explore this in our data set, due to the volunteer nature of the recruiting of participants, as male participants or older participants with deeper connections to animals, may have been more likely to accept the questionnaire in the first place, although this may also be true for women and younger participants.Charismatic species that often drive visitors to zoos and contribute to visitors' satisfaction may be used as models in zoos to attain specific information (e.g., conservation measures for the species and habitat protection) because visitors are more likely to engage in activities related to these species (Colleony et al., 2017;Howell et al., 2019;Consorte-McCrea et al., 2019; but see Mariyam et al., 2022 for similar patterns in nature-based tourism). The endangered gorillas, along with chimpanzees, are one of these charismatic species and a favorite animal of most zoo visitors (Carr, 2016) elsewhere and in our study.Visitors often report feeling easily connected to gorillas and wishing to preserve the species (Myers et al., 2004;Parker et al., 2018). Gorillas` flagship status facilitates raising awareness for the conservation of their habitat which also includes other species living within it (Ballantyne et al., 2007). Critically endangered species are thought to have a wider influence on conservation, education, and economy compared to least concern species (Spooner et al., 2023). Aware of such effects, zoos may mediate such species exhibitions to emphasize conservation efforts (Greenwell et al., 2023). For instance, the Maryland Zoo (Unites States) was crucial for the development of the Mountain Gorilla Veterinary Project, operating in the species range countries (Escobar-Ibarra et al., 2021). Other zoos have provided financial support for the development and maintenance of the Mbeli Bai lowland gorilla research field site (e.g., by employing local nationals as rangers or research assistants) and have contributed to local community projects in the same area (Breuer et al., 2018;but Squires et a., 2016;Wilson et al., 2019;Feilen et al., 2018 for examples targeting other species and sites in Africa and South America).Visitors often make assumptions about animal welfare based on personal feelings and connections, which may conflict with scientific analysis (e.g., gorillas being naturally inactive at certain ages; Parker et al., 2018). To foster better understanding, zoos must help visitors interpret animal behavior, as familiarity increases care and conservation interest. However, recent studies show visitors are poorly motivated to engage with traditional zoo education programs (Ballentyne et al., 2021). Effective conservation actions should integrate easily into daily routines and offer visible outcomes (Miller et al., 2020). Zoos can highlight direct links between human actions and animal welfare, such as habitat loss due to palm oil production or cobalt mining impacting orangutans and gorillas. They can also address threats from the pet trade (e.g., lemurs, lorises) and entertainment industries (e.g., chimpanzees, macaques). Zoos can suggest actionable steps, from simple to committed, like sponsoring local initiatives, joining community conservation efforts, partnering with wildlife organizations, or raising awareness. By focusing on local issues, zoos can engage visitors in meaningful, region-specific actions to protect animals.It is important to recognize that our survey may not have delivered all the ways that zoo visitors connect with animals and their understanding of conservation and risks associated with close proximity to animals. We aimed to apply a balanced questionnaire, so its length would not impact the visitors' viewing time of the animals. Besides, one of the sampled zoos housed only primate species which may have influenced the data by potentially increasing scores for favorite primate species due to the higher exposure to these animals compared to others. The variation in the layout and design of the informational signage, which is relatively newer and of better quality at HZ, may have influenced the responses. Additionally, JMC offers two interactive exhibits where visitors can enter the enclosures (squirrel monkeys and ring-tailed lemurs island), whereas HZ does not provide this experience, potentially impacting its visitors' sense of connection differently.We also intended to include a sampled population outside of the zoos for comparison. However, since the Chubu Gakuin sample consists solely of students-most of whom are women and all belonging to Gen Z-its comparison with the more balanced populations from the zoos is also limited. For a better control to the visitor populations, different Nonetheless, this survey is amongst one of the first attempts to study these aspects in Japanese zoos and may serve in the future for a global comparison of zoo visitors' sense of connection with animals. In addition to zoos, future research should also focus on other types of animal housing facilities, such as sanctuaries and recovery centers, as the limited public visiting these locations may have different expectations and learning experiences.
Keywords: Visitors connection, Animal connection, educational programs, wildlife conservation, Animal Welfare, zoos
Received: 26 Oct 2024; Accepted: 12 Feb 2025.
Copyright: © 2025 Costa, Xu, Brandao and Hayashi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
* Correspondence:
Raquel Filomena Pereira Costa, Japan Monkey Centre, Inuyama, 484-0081, Aichi, Japan
Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Research integrity at Frontiers
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.