Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychol. , 28 March 2025

Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1484839

Shared reading as an intervention to improve health and well-being in adults: a scoping review

  • 1Department of Psychology, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
  • 2Centre for Languages and Literature, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
  • 3Birgit Rausing Centre for Medical Humanities, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
  • 4Department of Culture, Languages and Media, Malmö University, Malmö, Sweden
  • 5Department of Clinical Sciences, Medical Faculty, Lund University, Lund, Sweden

Background: Shared reading (SR) is a group reading concept consisting of weekly meetings led by a trained facilitator where literary fiction and poetry is read aloud and discussed. SR requires no previous knowledge or homework and has been tried out as a health intervention to different vulnerable populations.

Objectives: The present study undertook a scoping review of research evaluating SR as an intervention to improve health and/or well-being in adults. The aim was to map the existing knowledge, identify research gaps, and suggest how these gaps can be addressed in future research.

Method: We followed the PRISMA guidelines for scoping reviews. Online data bases were searched for publications on SR published between Jan 1, 2005, and Dec 31, 2024. Studies were eligible if they used SR as an intervention for adults, reported outcomes for health and/or well-being, and were published in Danish, English, Norwegian, or Swedish in peer-reviewed journals. Quantitative, qualitative, and mixed-methods studies could be included.

Results: The search returned 179 records. We identified 15 studies, all written in English, that met the inclusion criteria. The studies were conducted in UK (67%; n = 10) and in the Scandinavian countries (33%; n = 5). Participants were mostly vulnerable populations such as people with dementia, mental illness, or chronic pain. The number of included participants varied between 4 and 61. Quantitative data were reported in 47% (n = 7) of the studies, showing improvements in quality of life and well-being and reduced symptoms of depression. Most quantitative studies were limited by small sample sizes and lack of comparison. All but one study (n = 14; 93%) reported qualitative outcomes. The qualitative data showed that the group community was a valued aspect, and participants reported positive outcomes related to health and well-being. In-depth analysis found that participation in SR groups may lead to a renewed sense of personal identity and improved capacity for mentalization.

Conclusion: SR was reported to be a feasible and promising intervention for several groups in different settings. However, available evidence is limited, and research gaps exist. Current findings can serve as a foundation for future studies with larger samples and longer follow-up.

1 Introduction

The idea of a relationship between literature and reading and human health dates back to ancient times. In ancient Greece, Apollo was the god of both medicine and poetry. Similarly, Aristotle developed the concept of emotional catharsis among the drama audience of his time, thereby strengthened the connection between literature and personal and psychological growth (Sheppard, 1987). For a long time, books have been used as a treatment for various mental health problems and lifestyle issues, a practice often recognized under the umbrella term bibliotherapy. Bibliotherapy can take many forms, and it can include the use of self-help literature or fiction and poetry, and bibliotherapy can be conducted individually or in group (Canty, 2017). Bibliotherapy using self-help books has shown to be effective to reduce depressive symptoms up to three years after the intervention in adults (Gualano et al., 2017). However, reading books is not equally accessible to all, as it among other things requires reading comprehension and perseverance.

Just after the turn of the millennium the British national charity The Reader in Liverpool developed the group reading concept Shared reading (SR) as an initiative to promote literature and reading (The Reader, 2024). In a SR group meeting, led by a trained facilitator, short stories, novels, and poetry are read out aloud and discussed. No previous preparation is required by the participants – the facilitator hands out the different texts during the session. The participants are thus offered a dialogical interaction in a social context, where they interact with texts written by different authors as well as with their co-participants and the facilitator leading the group sessions. Normally, a SR group consists of 8–12 participants and meets weekly for 1 or 2 h. Although the basic concept is quite clear, there is room for variation and adaptation of the length of the sessions, the session intervals, and number of participants depending on the context.

In a perspectives article published in The Lancet 2009, Jane Davis, the founder of The Reader, highlights the uniqueness of participating in an SR group: “The reading-aloud group model offers something live: the sharing of the experience itself, the reading together, and also the immediate discussion of that complex experience in a social community” (Davis, 2009).

The SR concept has, thus, been suggested to facilitate the recognition of thoughts and feelings. This could in turn positively affect participants’ well-being without directly trying to address a specific problem. As a result, SR has in recent years been tried out as an intervention to promote health and well-being in different populations and settings, and beneficial outcomes such as improved quality of life, less pain and better sleep have been reported (Billington et al., 2013; Longden et al., 2016; Billington et al., 2017). In contrast to several other reading interventions directed toward vulnerable groups, the literature is chosen on its value to the human existence in a broad sense for all groups and is not specifically targeting the illness or problem the participants are facing.

Today, many societies are burdened by high prevalence of mental disorders as well as loneliness (GBD 2019 Mental Disorders Collaborators, 2022). The health care systems are keen to find cost-effective interventions beyond pharmacological and psychotherapeutic treatments. SR has several advantages, as SR is scalable, comes with a low cost, and meets several different needs: social, cognitive, cultural, and existential. There is a low threshold to participate in an SR group as it requires no preparation or prior knowledge. Participants are invited and encouraged to reflect on the literature, but there is no pressure to perform or say anything. SR could also serve as a way for people with cognitive impairments or fatigue to get access to literary experiences (Billington et al., 2013; Longden et al., 2016; Andersen, 2022). Libraries constitute an existing infrastructure for conducting SR in terms of facilities, personnel, and material (books).

Other reading interventions aiming to improve mental health and well-being have been studied for a longer time and enough data have been reported to conduct systematic reviews and meta-analysis (Gualano et al., 2017; Wang et al., 2020; Zanal Abidin et al., 2021). However, since the specific SR-concept is a relatively new intervention, the number of publications is limited, and the existing studies have been conducted in various scientific disciplines (psychology, anthropology, literary studies etc.) making it difficult to synthesize the data in a systematic review. Scoping review is a form of review suitable to map the evidence in relatively new areas, where there is a diversity of methods, and an overview is needed to identify research gaps. Therefore, we undertook this scoping review to map the existing SR evidence and identify areas appropriate for further research by addressing the following questions: For whom and in what settings has SR been used? How have the interventions been carried out? What are the outcomes reported? How have outcomes been conceptualized and assessed? What further research is needed to better capture the effects of SR?

2 Methods

We followed the PRISMA guidelines for Scoping reviews (Tricco et al., 2018). However, there was no preregistered review protocol for this scoping review.

2.1 Eligibility criteria

To be included in the review, papers needed to study SR as an intervention to promote health and/or well-being in adults (age ≥ 18 years) and report original data. Both quantitative, qualitative, and mixed-methods papers could be included. Papers should be written in Danish, English, Norwegian, or Swedish, as these are the languages the authors of this scoping review are proficient in. To be included, the results should be published in peer-reviewed journals between Jan 1, 2005, and Dec 31, 2024. The starting date was chosen in relation to when the SR-concept was introduced.

Studies were excluded if they studied reading interventions other than the SR concept developed by The Reader as this scoping review focuses on this specific method. SR studies not reporting health or well-being outcomes were also excluded.

2.2 Search strategy

To identify potentially relevant studies, a search was conducted in LUBsearch, the collective entry point to all the Lund University Libraries’ joint resources. For a full list of indexed databases in LUBsearch, see Table 1. PubMed was also searched. The search was made with the help of university librarians. The final search strings are presented in Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Indexed data bases in LUBsearch and the final search strings.

The number of studies identified in the search is shown in Figure 1. The final search was conducted on Jan 22, 2025.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow chart of identified, excluded, and included studies for the scoping review.

2.3 Selection of sources of evidence

All papers were reviewed (first title and abstract, then full text) by two members of the research team (KJ and TF). Divergent views were resolved through discussion.

2.4 Data charting process

A table was created as a charting form, and information from each paper was listed under the headings: country, setting, participants/population, intervention (how SR was delivered, literature used, number of sessions, frequency, closed or open groups etc.), assessment (how was the effect assessed), outcomes (including attrition), limitations, and funding. All co-authors of the present paper independently reviewed the papers (two reviewers for each paper) and added data to the table. One paper was authored by several authors of the present scoping review (Ohlsson et al., 2018), and was therefore deliberately reviewed by two co-authors (MM and JW) not involved in the study by Ohlsson et al.

2.5 Synthesis of the results

We have grouped the studies based on the following charting terms: Country, setting, funding, participants, intervention, quantitative and qualitative assessment, quantitative and qualitative outcomes, and reported limitations.

3 Results

3.1 Selections of sources of evidence

The search yielded 179 papers. Based on title and abstract, 154 papers were excluded. We retrieved 25 studies for full text review, of which ten were excluded. Reasons for exclusion were not containing original data, i.e., commentaries and method papers (n = 7), reporting outcomes not relevant, i.e., not focusing on health or well-being (n = 2), and using a reading aloud intervention outside the SR concept (n = 1). The remaining 15 papers, published between 2007 and 2024, were included in this review. All included papers were written in English. See Figure 1 for a flow chart.

3.2 Synthesis of results

The results are presented in Tables 2 and 3 for overview and further elaborated on in text.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Data extracted from the included studies with information about country, setting, funding, participants, and assessment.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Data extracted from the included studies with information about findings and reported limitations.

3.2.1 Countries

So far, SR as an intervention to improve mental health and well-being has been studied mainly in the UK (ten studies; 67%) (Hodge et al., 2007; Dowrick et al., 2012; Billington et al., 2013; Longden et al., 2015; Billington et al., 2016a; Billington et al., 2016b; Longden et al., 2016; Billington et al., 2017; Watkins et al., 2022; Tangerås, 2022), but also in the Scandinavian countries (33%): Denmark (three studies) (Steenberg et al., 2014; Christiansen and Dalsgård, 2021; Pihl et al., 2024), Norway (one study) (Andersen, 2022), and Sweden (one study) (Ohlsson et al., 2018).

3.2.2 Populations and settings

Targeted groups have generally been different vulnerable populations, such as people with psychiatric disorders (Dowrick et al., 2012; Steenberg et al., 2014; Watkins et al., 2022), patients with chronic pain (Billington et al., 2016a; Billington et al., 2017; Ohlsson et al., 2018), patients with cancer (Andersen, 2022), elderly with dementia (Billington et al., 2013; Longden et al., 2016; Tangerås, 2022), recovering drug addicts (Hodge et al., 2007), and prisoners (Billington et al., 2016b), but SR has also been used to improve well-being in newly retired seniors (Pihl et al., 2024) (Table 2).

The SR sessions have been conducted in a variety of naturalistic settings such as care homes (Billington et al., 2013; Longden et al., 2016; Tangerås, 2022), clinics/hospitals (Dowrick et al., 2012; Billington et al., 2013; Billington et al., 2016a; Billington et al., 2017; Watkins et al., 2022), prisons (Billington et al., 2016b), and libraries (Hodge et al., 2007) (Table 2). Dowrick et al. (2012) reported that the setting had an important influence on SR as it was easier to engage participants at the mental health drop-in center compared to at a GP surgery, where the participants initially regarded the literature as something “prescribed.” In a Norwegian study published in 2022, some of the participants had taken part in SR online (Andersen, 2022).

Most studies included in this review have studied small samples, and five studies had ten or fewer participants (Steenberg et al., 2014; Billington et al., 2016a; Billington et al., 2017; Ohlsson et al., 2018; Watkins et al., 2022). The largest study included 61 participants (Billington et al., 2013). Some studies have purposely studied participants with a specific gender, such as female prisoners (Billington et al., 2016b) or newly retired men (Pihl et al., 2024). Other SR groups were open to all genders but attracted only women (Ohlsson et al., 2018; Andersen, 2022). Participants’ age was not systematically reported in all studies and ranged from 18 to “elderly” (Table 2).

3.2.3 Funding

Funding was reported in 9 out of 15 studies. The studies were primarily funded by health care authorities, research councils, and universities (Table 2).

3.2.4 Intervention

The interventions varied between 6 and 60 sessions, distributed over 2 to 18 months (Table 2). When reported, length of sessions ranged between 1 and 2 h, and were commonly delivered weekly. However, in a study with participants with dementia, two out of four groups were offered daily instead of weekly sessions (Longden et al., 2016).

In most studies, fiction and poetry were used in the reading sessions. In some cases, adaptations were made in relation to the targeted group, such as shorter texts used for people with dementia (Billington et al., 2013) and people with cancer-related fatigue (Andersen, 2022). Other reported adaptions were the facilitator reading in a much louder voice than usual and summarizing the story before discussion when the target group was people with dementia (Billington et al., 2013; Longden et al., 2016).

3.2.5 Study design and data collection

Seven of the 15 studies (47%) collected some form of quantitative data (Dowrick et al., 2012; Billington et al., 2013; Steenberg et al., 2014; Longden et al., 2015; Billington et al., 2016a; Longden et al., 2016; Billington et al., 2017). All studies collecting quantitative data had a prospective design with repeated assessment. In all studies with post-treatment assessment, this assessment was done in close proximity to the end of the intervention, but no study reported long-term follow-up. A few studies used strategies to improve reliability such as double pre-assessment (Billington et al., 2013), and one study had a randomized controlled design (Longden et al., 2016). Quantitative data were collected using questionnaires, pain rating, and heart rate monitoring (Table 2).

All but one study (Longden et al., 2016) collected qualitative data (93%). The data were collected with participant observations, recordings of sessions, diaries, focus groups, and interviews (Table 2). Both participants and facilitators were interviewed. The method for analyzing qualitative data varied between the studies (Table 2). Thematic analysis was the most frequently reported method (Hodge et al., 2007; Dowrick et al., 2012; Billington et al., 2013; Billington et al., 2016a).

3.2.6 Outcomes

3.2.6.1 Quantitative studies

Six studies reported positive effects of SR (Table 3). Positive changes were reported for depressive symptoms (Dowrick et al., 2012), dementia symptoms (Billington et al., 2013), pain (Billington et al., 2016a; Billington et al., 2017), psychological well-being and quality of life (Longden et al., 2015; Billington et al., 2016a; Longden et al., 2016), and positive affect (Longden et al., 2015; Billington et al., 2017). One study reported no reduction in the use of health services after the intervention (Dowrick et al., 2012). No study reported adverse effects of the intervention.

3.2.6.2 Qualitative studies

The qualitative observations had different focuses. Several studies reported on SR’s social function and the participants talked about the group community being helpful and valued (Hodge et al., 2007; Billington et al., 2016a; Billington et al., 2017; Andersen, 2022). Several studies reported improvements of different mental aspects such as memory, attention, well-being, quality of life, mood, less focus on negative effects of pain, and better sleep (Billington et al., 2013; Billington et al., 2016a; Billington et al., 2016b; Billington et al., 2017). Also, more fundamental mental effects were reported, such as a renewed sense of personal identity and improved capacity for mentalization (Billington et al., 2013; Billington et al., 2016b; Ohlsson et al., 2018).

In studies addressing the literary components of the SR intervention, positive educational effects were reported, as well as increased enthusiasm for literature and reading and a richer reading experience gained through SR (Hodge et al., 2007; Andersen, 2022). Diversity and complexity of the texts were appreciated as well as the texts not being related to the participants’ specific problems (Billington et al., 2016a). Beyond this, the literature, as well as the related discussions, were described as promoting a change of perspectives and a sense of connectedness to the outer world (Billington et al., 2016b; Andersen, 2022).

Two studies reported on the importance of the group facilitator, indicating that the facilitator is a key component of the SR intervention (Dowrick et al., 2012; Steenberg et al., 2014).

3.2.7 Reported limitations

Limitations were reported in 12 out of 15 studies. Mentioned limitations were small sample sizes, short follow-up, no assessment at baseline or few symptoms at baseline, no control group, attrition inflicting bias, and no information about other parallel interventions (Table 3).

4 Discussion

For this scoping review, we found 15 papers – published between 2007 and 2024 – reporting data from SR interventions to improve health and well-being in adults. The use of SR in various settings with different groups indicates a willingness to explore culture-based health interventions and is in line with the general upswing of arts in health over the past decades (Fancourt, 2017). In line with other forms of reading interventions to improve mental health and well-being (Canty, 2017; Gualano et al., 2017; Wang et al., 2020), a majority of the SR studies showed promising results, sparking the interest in further exploring SR as an intervention for various groups.

A strength of the reviewed studies is that all were conducted in naturalistic settings and had few exclusion criteria. This indicates a high ecological validity and feasibility. Targeted groups in the reviewed studies were predominantly vulnerable populations of different kinds, showing that SR is feasible for a range of different groups.

Overall, the analyzed studies showed high fidelity to the SR concept, such as having a reading group with a facilitator, reading aloud, and discussing poetry and short stories for 1 or 2 h. However, a great variation in the number of sessions, number of participants, and length of the interventions makes it difficult to assess more precisely the frequency of SR necessary to be effective. It is not known if a single, deeply profound SR session could elicit significant change. However, in the study by Watkins et al. (2022), analyzing the effect of SR on the participants’ discourse, the changes were discernible from around 6 months. As a result, it is difficult to estimate the resources necessary to deliver an effective SR intervention. Still, SR and other reading interventions (Gualano et al., 2017), in comparison with many other types of health interventions (nature-based interventions, art therapy, animal assisted therapy, virtual reality therapy etc.) may be a potentially cost-effective and scalable intervention.

The analyzed SR interventions were carried out in the UK and the Scandinavian countries. This may be due to the selection criteria. Only studies published in English, Danish, Norwegian, and Swedish could be included in this review, although only studies in English were found. There may be papers trying out SR as an intervention to improve health and well-being in adults published in other languages that were not included in this scoping review. The fact that a majority of the studies were conducted in the UK is, however, in line with SR being created and developed at the University of Liverpool. Several of the reviewed studies with data from the UK have been conducted by researchers connected to the founding SR organization (Hodge et al., 2007; Billington et al., 2013; Longden et al., 2015). Likewise, most Scandinavian studies have been carried out by researchers who have introduced SR – or are collaborating with the introducers – in the respective countries (Ohlsson et al., 2018; Christiansen and Dalsgård, 2021; Tangerås, 2022). Researchers trying out interventions they have developed or interventions they are highly invested in can be expected to be more dedicated, adherent, and enthusiastic than other practitioners, which may influence the outcomes. Such a dedication effect is something else than the intrinsic effect of the intervention per se, and in the future, SR needs to be evaluated by researchers less invested in the concept (Durlak and DuPre, 2008). In relation to funding, the studies included in this review were funded by health care authorities, research councils, and universities. We have not found any specific commercial interests that would benefit from the SR concept being implemented or promoted.

The existing studies have laid the ground for upcoming studies, by showing feasibility for different groups in different settings and promising effects on mental health and well-being (Dowrick et al., 2012; Billington et al., 2016a; Longden et al., 2016; Billington et al., 2017) as well as more fundamental mental effects such as a renewed sense of personal identity and improved capacity for mentalization (Billington et al., 2013; Billington et al., 2016b; Ohlsson et al., 2018). However, future studies need more scientific rigorousness, and the findings need to be replicated, and other forms of reading interventions have more solid evidence at the moment (Gualano et al., 2017). Most studies included in this review were limited by small sample sizes and lacked a control group. Attrition was high in some studies (e. g. 56% in Dowrick et al., 2012 and 60% in Watkins et al., 2022) or not reported. Post-intervention assessments were mostly done in connection to the end of intervention, and little is known about the long-term effects of participating in an SR group. However, Longden et al. (2016) reported sustained effects in people with dementia 3 months after the end of intervention. The effectiveness of different intensities and length of SR should also be compared. Further, SR interventions also need to be compared with other types of social interventions where people meet and interact around a common task, which so far has only been done by Longden et al. (2015). Effective therapeutic interventions potentially have side effects such as deterioration of symptoms, or onset of new symptoms (Rozental et al., 2016), and future studies also need to address potential adverse outcomes.

Other reading interventions have successfully been conducted online (Hoover et al., 2023). In the reviewed studies only one used SR digitally (Andersen, 2022), and it is not known whether SR has the same effect on mental health and well-being when delivered online compared to in-person. In a Swedish study on an online SR group during the COVID pandemic, the participants did not perceive the online format as negatively affecting the group cohesion, instead highlighting that it allowed for more focused attention on the text and the discussion (Gustafsson et al., 2023). However, this group was not aiming at improving the participants’ health and well-being. Some groups could especially benefit from participating digitally, e.g., if participants in a targeted group are geographically spread or if showing up in real life is experienced as too exhausting. Future studies could compare the outcomes between SR delivered in-person versus digitally, potentially in a randomized controlled trial.

So far, there is a lack of common terminology and a methodological consensus within the SR studies. This makes comparison between studies difficult. Consistency in qualitative and quantitative methods over several studies to compare results would be preferable. As an example, the same questionnaires could be used between studies to assess mental health and well-being before and after the SR group. Longden et al. (2015) have suggested that it is necessary to use specific and sensitive questionnaires, since SR may affect specific aspects of well-being such as purpose in life. There is also a need for replicated studies on specific target groups. Only one of the reviewed studies had used a biological marker to study the effect on the participants (Steenberg et al., 2014), and future studies could potentially study SR’s effect on brain connectivity (Berns et al., 2013). The qualitative studies on SR have been conducted in different academic disciplines and on different levels, using different methods for collecting and analyzing the data. A strength is that different perspectives have been explored, but different research practices and traditions make it difficult to evaluate the studies against the same quality criteria and perform a qualitative synthesis of the results.

The specific literary components of the SR intervention were addressed in some studies, but not on a content level so much as regarding the format: in a study with patients diagnosed with cancer the chosen stories were short due to fatigue being a common symptom among the patients (Andersen, 2022). In a similar manner, poetry has been the preferred choice in interventions for elderly people with mild to moderate dementia (Billington et al., 2013; Longden et al., 2016). However, most studies lack a thorough discussion of the literary texts and the specific way they challenge and affect the participants. In some studies, all literature used is listed, e.g., Billington et al. (2017), whereas other studies do not mention the titles of the texts used. In such studies, the literature is an unknown ‘stimulus’ the participants are responding to rather than a central agent.

As acknowledged above, using literature to improve mental health and well-being is not just a recent phenomenon (Canty, 2017), and SR has not been evaluated against other reading interventions. So far, other forms of bibliotherapy have more scientific evidence, with data from several randomized controlled trials and published systematic reviews (Gualano et al., 2017; Zanal Abidin et al., 2021) and also meta-analysis (Wang et al., 2020). In more traditional bibliotherapy the literary content is often specifically chosen for the participants as a therapeutic tool and self-help books can be used (Canty, 2017; Jack and Ronan, 2008). Participants with different health issues or in certain circumstances may find targeted SR groups especially valuable, even if the literature read and discussed during SR is not focusing on the specific illness or life circumstances. It may be valuable to be able to meet with people in the same situation, without having to interact by talking about the common problem (Andersen, 2022). Sharing and discussing poetry and fiction with others can be an appreciated mental challenge and improve self-esteem beyond what a support group can accomplish (Billington et al., 2016a).

It is not only the chosen and read aloud fictional texts that may affect the result of the intervention, but also the facilitator. A few studies explored the importance of the facilitator in the SR concept, and it is not known if a skilled reader leader gets a significantly better result than a less skilled one, and what qualities of the facilitator that are most important. Thus, the facilitator’s role needs to be addressed in future studies, analyzing the actions taken by the facilitator in relation to the group and the texts. Both the literature chosen and the role of the facilitator can be described as mechanisms within the SR group which produce the potential effects. Another such mechanism to explore in future studies is the interaction between the participants necessary to create an effect, as SR may offer unique opportunities to practice perspective taking and mentalization (Gustafsson et al., 2023).

4.1 Limitations

This scoping review has some limitations. With the focus on SR only, we have not included other read aloud interventions for adults aiming at improving mental health and well-being not labeled as SR, (e.g., Blundell and Poole, 2023). Including only papers from peer-reviewed journals may have excluded data reported in other types of publications, such as books and dissertations. There might be publications in other languages that were not included in this scoping review. A strength is that we followed the PRISMA guidelines for Scoping reviews as far as possible, which contributed to the work being carried out in a systematic way.

5 Conclusion

This scoping review gives an updated overview of where the SR research focusing on health and well-being stands today and can serve as a foundation when future studies are designed. SR so far appears to be feasible and appreciated by participants and facilitators, and is potentially an effective and cost-effective intervention, filling an important societal need. However, this scoping review shows that more research is necessary before implementation in clinical care. At present, there is a lack of large, controlled studies comparing SR to other potential interventions to the targeted groups. Also, long-term studies showing lasting benefits from SR are lacking. A key future research question, beyond the scope of this review, is also if SR could be used to prevent mental illness.

Author contributions

KJ: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AO: Formal analysis, Project administration, Writing – review & editing. KB: Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing. AG: Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing. PJ: Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing. MM: Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing. JW: Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing. TF: Formal analysis, Project administration, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank librarians Daniel Lind and Maja Carlson at the Social Sciences Faculty Library, Lund University, Sweden for support with the systematic search for studies.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Andersen, T. R. (2022). Regaining autonomy, competence, and relatedness: experiences from two shared Reading groups for people diagnosed with cancer. Front. Psychol. 13, 1–18. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1017166

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Berns, G. S., Blaine, K., Prietula, M. J., and Pye, B. E. (2013). Short- and long-term effects of a novel on connectivity in the brain. Brain Connect. 3, 590–600. doi: 10.1089/brain.2013.0166

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Billington, J., Carroll, J., Davis, P., Healey, C., and Kinderman, P. (2013). A literature-based intervention for older people living with dementia. Perspect. Public Health 133, 165–173. doi: 10.1177/1757913912470052

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Billington, J., Farrington, G., Lampropoulou, S., Lingwood, J., Jones, A., Ledson, J., et al. (2017). A comparative study of cognitive behavioural therapy and shared reading for chronic pain. Med. Humanit. 43, 155–165. doi: 10.1136/medhum-2016-011047

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Billington, J., Humphreys, A. L., Jones, A., and McDonnell, K. (2016a). A literature-based intervention for people with chronic pain. Arts Health 8, 13–31. doi: 10.1080/17533015.2014.957330

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Billington, J., Longden, E., and Robinson, J. (2016b). A literature-based intervention for women prisoners: preliminary findings. Int. J. Prison. Health 12, 230–243. doi: 10.1108/ijph-09-2015-0031

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blundell, J., and Poole, S. (2023). Poetry in a pandemic. Digital shared reading for wellbeing. J. Poet. Ther. 36, 197–209. doi: 10.1080/08893675.2022.2148135

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Canty, N. (2017). Bibliotherapy: its processes and benefits and application in clinical and developmental settings. Logos 28, 32–40. doi: 10.1163/1878-4712-11112133

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Christiansen, C. E., and Dalsgård, A. L. (2021). The day we were dogs: mental vulnerability, shared Reading, and moments of transformation. Ethos 49, 286–307. doi: 10.1111/etho.12319

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, J. (2009). Enjoying and enduring: groups reading aloud for wellbeing. Lancet 373, 714–715. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(09)60426-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dowrick, C., Billington, J., Robinson, J., Hamer, A., and Williams, C. (2012). Get into Reading as an intervention for common mental health problems: exploring catalysts for change. Med. Humanit. 38, 15–20. doi: 10.1136/medhum-2011-010083

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Durlak, J. A., and DuPre, E. P. (2008). Implementation matters: a review of research on the influence of implementation on program outcomes and the factors affecting implementation. Am. J. Community Psychol. 41, 327–350. doi: 10.1007/s10464-008-9165-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fancourt, D. (2017). Arts in health: Designing and researching interventions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

GBD 2019 Mental Disorders Collaborators (2022). Global, regional, and national burden of 12 mental disorders in 204 countries and territories, 1990-2019: a systematic analysis for the global burden of disease study 2019. Lancet Psychiatry 9, 137–150. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(21)00395-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gualano, M. R., Bert, F., Martorana, M., Voglino, G., Andriolo, V., Thomas, R., et al. (2017). The long-term effects of bibliotherapy in depression treatment: systematic review of randomized clinical trials. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 58, 49–58. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.09.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gustafsson, A. W., Johnsson, P., Järvholm, K., Bernhardsson, K., Forslid, T., and Ohlsson, A. (2023). An intricate dance of intersubjectivity: the social and cognitive benefits of a digital shared reading group. Sci. Study Lit. 12, 103–132. doi: 10.1075/ssol.22010.gus

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodge, S., Robinson, J., and Davis, P. (2007). Reading between the lines: the experiences of taking part in a community reading project. Med. Humanit. 33, 100–104. doi: 10.1136/jmh.2006.000256

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoover, E., Bernstein-Ellis, E., and Meyerson, D. (2023). Using bibliotherapy to rebuild identity for people with aphasia: a book club experience. J. Commun. Disord. 105:106363. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2023.106363

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jack, S. J., and Ronan, K. R. (2008). Bibliotherapy: practice and research. Sch. Psychol. Int. 29, 161–182. doi: 10.1177/0143034308090058

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Longden, E., Davis, P., Billington, J., Walsh, E., Corcoran, R., Lampropoulou, S., et al. (2015). Shared reading: assessing the intrinsic value of a literature-based health intervention. Med. Humanit. 41, 113–120. doi: 10.1136/medhum-2015-010704

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Longden, E., Davis, P., Carroll, J., Billington, J., and Kinderman, P. (2016). An evaluation of shared reading groups for adults living with dementia: preliminary findings. J. Public Ment. Health 15, 75–82. doi: 10.1108/jpmh-06-2015-0023

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohlsson, A., Ingemansson, M., Forslid, T., Grelz, H., Gustafsson, A. W., Järvholm, K., et al. (2018). Book talks among people with chronic non-cancer pain: literary meaning making in a shared reading group. Working papers. Med. Humanit. 4, 1–22.

Google Scholar

Pihl, M. E. L., Kristensen, M. M., Folker, A. P., and Simonsen, P. (2024). Approaching literary connectivity: early reflections on a “shared Reading” intervention in the light of postcritical thought. Orb. Litt. 79, 145–153. doi: 10.1111/oli.12380

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rozental, A., Kottorp, A., Boettcher, J., Andersson, G., and Carlbring, P. (2016). Negative effects of psychological treatments: an exploratory factor analysis of the negative effects questionnaire for monitoring and reporting adverse and unwanted events. PLoS One 11:e0157503. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0157503

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheppard, A. D. R. (1987). Aesthetics: An introduction to the philosophy of art. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Steenberg, M., Bräuner, P., and Wallot, S. (2014). Text technology: building subjective and shared experience in reading. J. Cogn. Cult. 14, 357–372. doi: 10.1163/15685373-12342131

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tangerås, T. M. (2022). Moments of meeting: a case study of shared Reading of poetry in a care home. Front. Psychol. 13, 1–7. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.965122

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

The Reader. (2024). Bringing people together and books to life [online]. Available online at:https://www.thereader.org.uk/ [Accessed June 27 2024].

Google Scholar

Tricco, A. C., Lillie, E., Zarin, W., O'Brien, K. K., Colquhoun, H., Levac, D., et al. (2018). PRISMA extension for scoping reviews (PRISMA-ScR): checklist and explanation. Ann. Intern. Med. 169, 467–473. doi: 10.7326/m18-0850

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, S. S., Bressington, D. T., Leung, A. Y. M., Davidson, P. M., and Cheung, D. S. K. (2020). The effects of bibliotherapy on the mental well-being of informal caregivers of people with neurocognitive disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Nurs. Stud. 109:103643. doi: 10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2020.103643

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Watkins, M., Naylor, K., and Corcoran, R. (2022). Beyond the sentence: shared reading within a high secure hospital. Front. Psychol. 13:1015498. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1015498

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zanal Abidin, N. S., Shaifuddin, N., and Wan Mohd Saman, W. S. (2021). Systematic literature review of the Bibliotherapy practices in public libraries in supporting communities’ mental health and wellbeing. Public Libr. Q. 42, 124–140. doi: 10.1080/01616846.2021.2009291

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: literature, culture and health, quality of life, depression, group intervention, community

Citation: Järvholm K, Ohlsson A, Bernhardsson K, Gustafsson AW, Johnsson P, Malmström M, Wistrand J and Forslid T (2025) Shared reading as an intervention to improve health and well-being in adults: a scoping review. Front. Psychol. 16:1484839. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1484839

Received: 22 August 2024; Accepted: 17 March 2025;
Published: 28 March 2025.

Edited by:

Gianluca Castelnuovo, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Italy

Reviewed by:

Federico Batini, University of Perugia, Italy
Tine Riis Andersen, University of Stavanger, Norway

Copyright © 2025 Järvholm, Ohlsson, Bernhardsson, Gustafsson, Johnsson, Malmström, Wistrand and Forslid. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kajsa Järvholm, a2Fqc2EuamFydmhvbG1AcHN5Lmx1LnNl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Research integrity at Frontiers

Man ultramarathon runner in the mountains he trains at sunset

95% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good

Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.


Find out more