
94% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.
Find out more
SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article
Front. Psychol. , 06 March 2025
Sec. Developmental Psychology
Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1475387
Play therapy is an effective approach to reduce emotional symptoms, stress and develop emotional sphere in early childhood. But the organization of therapy requires long-term specialized training and a significant number of sessions, making it inaccessible in public institutions. This systematic review aims to analyze the potential and mechanisms of play outside of the therapeutic setting for overcoming emotional symptoms, develop emotional regulation and resilience in preschool-aged children. Using the PRISMA method, we selected 33 articles for qualitative analysis from the 9,639 published articles over the past 5 years found through PubMed, ScienceDirect, and Scopus. The effectiveness of pretend play, play with rules, digital play, and outdoor play for addressing emotional symptoms and improving mental health was revealed. It was shown that play outside of the therapeutic setting is widely used to improve emotional intelligence and its components, decrease anxiety, aggression, and fear. In accordance with the cultural-historical approach, seven generalized blocks of play mechanisms were identified to enhance children’s mental well-being and promote development. Pretend play was found to have the greatest corrective and developmental potential, due to the diverse mechanisms involved. The findings of this review may be used to plan future research and improve educational practice in public kindergartens.
Modern individuals are experiencing increasing pressure from both normative and nonnormative stressful events (Schwarzer and Schulz, 2003). The amount and rate of information change is growing (Toffler, 1970), as is the number and frequency of natural, anthropogenic, and socio-economic threats, such as disasters, and military conflicts (Bennouna et al., 2020; Ivanov et al., 2022; Meshkova et al., 2022; UNDRR, 2020). In many countries, crime and violence rates remain quite high (Moen, 2019; Sánchez-Jáuregui et al., 2023). Together, these factors threaten the harmonious development and well-being of children, who are one of the most vulnerable groups in society (Khakimzyanov and Ryazanov, 2022). Children experience psychological impacts of stressful events—emotional reactions associated with the assessment and experience of what is happening. The experiences that children undergo in stressful events hinder the satisfaction of their basic needs for safety, predictability, connectedness, and development (Bowlby, 1979). Therefore, the emotional sphere is, on the one hand, the most sensitive to stressful events, and on the other, it is the main protective source, which emphasizes the non-direct environmental influences on child development and reactions. Environmental factors affect child development by the principle of refraction rather than by the principle of reflection: any environmental condition is refracted in a child’s mind in unique ways depending on his or her actual abilities and personal attitude to the situation (Gavrilova and Kornienko, 2025; Veresov, 2017).
Emotional intelligence is defined as an individual’s capacity to identify, use, comprehend, and control emotional information (Zhang et al., 2022). According to Trait Emotional Intelligence Model (Petrides, 2010), emotional intelligent contains four different large constructs: emotionality (emotion perception, expression, empathy, relationship, etc.), self-control (emotion regulation, stress management, adaptability, etc.), sociability (social competence, self-esteem, assertiveness, etc.), and well-being (happiness, optimism, etc.). Researchers have found that emotional intelligence serve a central function in psychological resilience (Armstrong et al., 2011; Maulding et al., 2012; Jowkar, 2007), appear to be crucial ability to successfully cope with challenge or misfortune (Guo et al., 2010; Sun et al., 2019; Wagnild and Young, 1993) and critical protective resource (Chen, 2012; Gong and Zhang, 2012). Emotional intelligence develops as children grow older. For instance, in infancy, individual differences in emotion regulation reflect brain-based temperamental factors, such as positive and negative emotionality, which may influence the quality of social interaction between child and caregiver. In children of preschool and elementary school age, language-based regulation of emotion becomes increasingly important. Increasing verbal capabilities allow the child to acquire rules for understanding and expressing emotion, such as “crying is okey” or “good girls do not behave rude,” and for communicating emotion to others (Zeidner et al., 2003). In adolescence cognitive and problem-solving strategies appear to cope with stress and challenges.
However, 5 the most intensive formation period for emotional intelligence is 3 to 7 years (Bozhovich, 1978; Zaporozhets, 1986). The emotional well-being of children at this age is highly sensitive to external factors. As a result, common reactions to stressful events include emotional symptoms such as anxiety, depression, fear, difficulties with emotion regulation, and behavioral problems (Fang et al., 2022; Luby et al., 2006; Platt et al., 2016; Zhang et al., 2022). Insufficiently formed emotional regulation, stress management, and adaptability, among other aspects of emotional intelligence, may not provide sufficient mental resilience.
Considering the dual role of emotional intelligence - on the one hand, heightened sensitivity to stressful factors, and on the other, a protective function - it is crucial to find ways to assist children by addressing negative emotional symptoms while also promoting emotional intelligence development and resilience.
The process of organizing psychological assistance for children must be based on knowledge of the psychological patterns of development. The patterns of child development have their own specificity at each age (Elkonin, 1971; Vygotsky, 1983; Zakharova and Machinskaya, 2023). Moreover, there are age differences in emotional coping with stress (Skinner and Zimmer-Gembeck, 2007). Together, this indicates the impossibility of creating a unified rehabilitation and preventive program for children of all ages. Therefore, in this study, we focused on finding ways of psychological preventive help and rehabilitation for young children aged 3 to 7 years, as the emotional development of children precisely in that period has the most significant impact on children’s future academic achievements and well-being (Currie and Almond, 2011; Bozhovich, 1978; Heckman, 2011; Zaporozhets, 1986).
Play is a leading activity between 3 and 7 years (Elkonin, 2005). The concept of ‘play’ can denote different types of child’s play activities. One of the most effective approaches to the theoretical analysis of play as a phenomenon is considered a cultural-historical approach (Vygotsky, 2017; Elkonin, 2005). Cultural-historical approach is based on the idea that child development takes place through the acquisition of historically created forms and modes of activity through communication rather than through the adaptation to the environment (Hedegaard and Fleer, 2008; Obukhova, 2012). According to Vygotsky (2017) play is a system of actions related to it. It is a culturally determined and most accessible activity for a child, arising spontaneously and bringing not only pleasure but also benefits for development (Fleer, 2022; Vygotsky, 2017; Zaporozhets and Elkonin, 1956). There are three most important characteristics of child’s play in relation to its role in a child’s psychological development: (1) in play the child creates and acts in an imaginary situation; (2) play is the source of psychological development and creates a zone of proximal development (ZPD) and, most importantly, (3) the development of play is characterized by changes in the relations between roles, rules and play actions (Veresov and Veraksa, 2023). Through play a child manifests their subjectivity and inner activity, endowing play with personal meaning and experiences while simultaneously acting as a subject of social action (Veraksa, 2011). In play, a child is usually focused on the process rather than the product (Veraksa et al., 2023). In play, the child learns what he cannot learn in daily life or education, for example, he masters the social relations of the adult world (Vygotsky, 2017).
From the point of view of the cultural-historical approach, there are four main types of play among children aged 3–7 years: the traditional ones are pretend play (or role play), play with rules, and play with toys (Elkonin, 2005), and recently appeared—digital play (Veraksa et al., 2023a; Veresov and Veraksa, 2023). In different types of play, a child gains fundamentally different experiences: from sharing toys with other players to emotional immersion in roles and enacting complex plots with peers, following complex rules. Additionally, the richness of play and child engagement in it may be influenced by the involvement of an adult who is a carrier of cultural experience and knowledge (Veresov et al., 2021).
In contemporary clinical and psychotherapeutic practice, therapeutic play, and in a broader context play therapy, have gained widespread recognition. Play therapy is defined as “a dynamic system of interpersonal relationships between a child and a therapist trained in play therapy procedures, who provides the child with play materials and facilitates the building of a safe relationship so that the child can fully express and explore their own self: feelings, thoughts, experiences, and behaviors, through play—the child’s natural means of communication” (Landreth, 1994). The goal of such therapy is not to change the child or teach them any specific behavioral skills, but to provide an opportunity to be themselves. In a safe play space, the child overcomes traumatic experiences through the symbolic expression of their feelings. Play therapy is used as a method of rehabilitation for children who have been subjected to abuse, serious traumatic events, developmental delays, and behavioral maladjustment, stressful events (LeBlanc and Ritchie, 2001). Research demonstrates the high effectiveness of play therapy and therapeutic play to reduce negative emotional symptoms such as anxiety, depression, fears and others (Baggerly, 2004; LeBlanc and Ritchie, 2001; Stulmaker and Ray, 2015).
Despite the effectiveness of play therapy and therapeutic play, there are a few limitations to these tools. The key constraint is the mandatory, lengthy, specialized training of the therapist. The therapist must be well-versed in both play and group therapy. A play therapist must sensitively understand and reflect the child’s emotional state during play. They must be fully engaged in the child’s play. The professional skill of a play therapist also includes their internal attitudes and ways of personal behavior, such as “the basic idea that the child is inherently motivated to grow and mature... And a deep and firm belief in the child’s ability to constructive self-direction” (Landreth, 1994). Secondly, play therapy and therapeutic play are long-term interventions. According to meta-analyses, the effects are observed after about 30 sessions, or 6 months of rehabilitation (LeBlanc and Ritchie, 2001). Together, this makes play therapy and therapeutic play difficult options for providing psychological help and prevention in public institutions. While parents rarely seek specialized help from psychotherapists for their young children.
To sum up, play is a space where a child realizes personal meanings and experiences, hones self-regulation skills and interactions with peers, and adults. Play has multifaceted influence on a child’s social–emotional development. Play therapy is actively used for addressing negative emotional symptoms in children. However, play therapy requires in-depth specialist training and long-term sessions, making its application in public institutions such as kindergartens difficult. Therefore, there is an important task to determine the effectiveness of play organized outside the therapeutic setting (e.g., therapeutic relationships, individual diagnosis, deep immersion of a specialist in the personal experiences of a child, compliance with therapeutic procedures, therapist training and supervision) to overcome negative emotional symptoms, develop emotional intelligence and stress resilience. For the purposes of this study, play organized outside of a therapeutic setting will be called a non-therapeutic play. Non-therapeutic play includes, for example, pretend play, play with rules, a digital play, free play with toys and other types of play. The spontaneous and child-controlled nature of non-therapeutic play indicates its potential for correcting and preventing emotional symptoms in children. However, this assumption needs confirmation.
One of the most effective approaches to the theoretical analysis of play as a phenomenon is the cultural-historical approach. It has a deep theoretical development of this topic and includes a set of practical tools for analyzing play as a phenomenon (Zaporozhets and Elkonin, 1956; Veraksa N. E. et al., 2022; Vygotsky, 2017). Therefore, the first goal of this review is to assess the possibilities of using play outside of a therapeutic context to reduce negative emotional symptoms such as anxiety, depression, fear, emotional-behavioral problems and develop emotional intelligence in children aged 3–7 years. The second goal is to analyze the features of play from the perspective of the cultural-historical approach, which provide its corrective, preventive, and developmental potential. The analysis of the features of the play in this work implies the identification of characteristics that ensure the effect as explained by the authors of the articles.
This systematic literature review was conducted to collect and analyze the most relevant empirical evidence on the impact of using play outside a therapeutic setting to reduce negative emotional symptoms in children aged 3–7 years. The PRISMA method was used for literature search and review to simplify the revision of the roadmap structure for research purposes (Moher et al., 2015). To this end, a literature search was carried out based on articles published in two databases: MEDLINE/PubMed and ScienceDirect. The selection of publications was carried out over a two-month period, up to and including 07/06/2024 and additionally for 3 weeks until 12/11/2024. To focus on the most current and relevant play practices, we analyzed papers published in the last 5 years (2019–2024). For the search strategy, 14 blocks of descriptors, combined using the Boolean operator AND, were utilized: (1) “play & “emotional intelligence”; (2) “play & stress”; (3) “play & anxiety”; (4) “play & depression”; (5) “play & emotional regulation”; (6) “play & fear” (7) “play & “emotional or behavioral problems” (8) “game & “emotional intelligence”; (9) “game & stress”; (10) “game & anxiety”; (11) “game & depression”; (12) “game & “emotional regulation”; (13) “game & fear” (14) “game & “emotional or behavioral problems.” In ScienceDirect, the descriptor “preschooler” was added to each block, as this database does not have the capability to automatically filter articles by a specific age group. In PubMed, the automatic selection option was used to find articles with sample ages of 2–5 and 6–12 years. In both databases the automatic selection option was used to find research articles.
Articles had to meet the following criteria to be included in the study:
(1) Sample age: sample include normo-typic children between the ages of 3 and 7;
(2) Type of study: empirical studies analyzing the effects, relationships, or mechanisms of play;
(3) Type of study subject: using any type of play except therapeutic play to reduce the negative emotional symptoms or improve emotional intelligence;
(4) Statistical analysis: using qualitative or quantitative statistical tests to derive results.
The exclusion criteria for articles were as follows:
(1) Articles written in languages other than English;
(2) Articles with no access to their full texts;
(3) Withdrawn articles.
Despite the fact that these exclusion criteria are common to PRISMA reviews, they impose limitations on the article selection and narrow the scope of the studied works.
First, the articles were screened based on their titles and abstracts. The selection process involved identifying studies related to the subject and eliminating those that did not meet the inclusion criteria or were unrelated. A complete analysis of the chosen studies was performed by critically reading the full texts. Any studies that did not meet the set criteria, as well as duplicate records, were excluded.
A total of 9,625 articles were identified through the initial database keyword searches. An additional 14 records were identified via Scopus, Google Scholar, and references of meta-analyses on similar subjects. Of these 9,639 articles, 132 duplicates were excluded. Screening for inclusion or exclusion criteria resulted in 101 studies being identified after reading their titles and abstracts. After a critical review of the full texts, 33 studies were included in the further qualitative review analysis (see Figure 1).
This review did not include an assessment of the methodological quality of the selected works because the main goal was to identify the potential and features of play outside a therapeutic setting for overcoming negative emotional states and to improve emotional resilience. To eliminate this limitation, a criterion on statistical data processing was included in the selection criteria, and the methodological characteristics of the selected studies were analyzed.
Types of play used in different play interventions were determined based on cultural-historical approach. For some interventions involving non-directive child activity, several categories have been identified. During the review, a new category “outdoor play” was also added, which emphasizes playing on the playgrounds. Outdoor play may include play with rules (for example, catch-up or hide-and-sick) or a pretend play on playground. However, the descriptions of the interventions were not sufficient to accurately attribute them to one of the initial categories. In addition, outdoor play also has its own unique characteristics, such as body movement, interaction with nature, and others.
The categorization was carried out by one expert.
The review included 33 empirical studies aimed at assessing the potential of play to overcome negative emotional symptoms and improve emotional intelligence outside the therapeutic setting. Tables 1, 2 provide detailed information of the included studies in the following areas: methodological characteristics of the included studies (Table 1), play intervention characteristics and features of play providing the effect (Table 2).
Three of the selected works are qualitative studies (Nicolaidou et al., 2022; Meng et al., 2022; Stavrou, 2019), such as case studies, which do not aim to identify the effects of play on the mental development of preschoolers but focus specifically on identifying the features of play providing the impact on the child’s psyche. Among the quantitative studies, the most common experimental design is the randomized controlled experiment - 23 studies, which indicates a relatively high reliability of the obtained results and conclusions. Four studies are quasi-experiments comparing the effectiveness of different types of play and other interventions (Azher et al., 2020; Bargale et al., 2021; Islaeli et al., 2020; Jones et al., 2021). However, the data on the effectiveness of the analyzed types of play and the reliability of the obtained conclusions should be considered with the relativity of the results in mind for these studies. Three included studies provide information on the relationship between play activities and indicators of child emotional development (Bao et al., 2022; Bauer et al., 2021; Shorer et al., 2021). None of the studies included a follow-up post-test, which imposes limitations on conclusions about the sustainability of the results obtained.
The average sample size of the selected studies, excluding qualitative ones, is 122.4 participants, indicating the scale of the selected studies. Thus, the results of the analyzed studies can be considered reasonably reliable. The mean age of participants in the reviewed studies is 5.3 years.
The selected studies showed the effectiveness of the following types of play: pretend play (n = 15), play with rules (n = 9), play with toys (n = 9), digital play (n = 5), adult-involved play (n = 4), adult-initiated play (n = 3), outdoor play (n = 2). Let us clarify that adult-involved play implies his participation as a respectful observer or player, whereas adult-initiated play implies attracting attention of the child, trying to actively involve a child in the game. One intervention can include several types of play, for example, the nurse involves a child to play with a doctor-doll (adult-initiated play + play with toys).
The duration of play interventions varies from one 20-min session to three times a week 15–60-min sessions for 2 years. 18 of 33 studies assessed one-time play sessions, while 15 studies assessed long-term implementation of play. 24 of the proposed play interventions assume the use of special materials (medical toy equipment, electronic devices, bubble balloons, etc.) or availability of special conditions (outdoor play spaces, parent involvement, etc.). However, all the necessary materials are readily available.13 of 33 play interventions involve special training of teachers, parents, nurses, and other assistants. The minimum duration of training is 1 h, the maximum is 20 h.
18 of 33 studies evaluated play in the medical context such as staying at hospitals or dental offices, in conditions of necessary medical interventions, surgeries, or in the presence of medical diagnoses. In one study, play was used to correct the emotional sphere of a child in an orphanage (Meng et al., 2022).
Table 2 provides more detailed description of play intervention characteristics.
From a substantive perspective, the results of the selected studies demonstrate the potential and effectiveness of play in: reducing anxiety (n = 18); developing emotional intelligence and its components (n = 11); declining emotional-behavioral difficulties (n = 8); overcoming fear (n = 3); reducing stress levels (n = 2). However, 2 studies did not find a significant effect of play, particularly adult-initiated play with toys and pretend play, on the mental state of young children (Karaca and Guner, 2022; Petersen and Holodynski, 2020).
Results indicated that is no clear correlation between the type of play used and its impact on a specific psychological parameter. One type of play in different studies may show effectiveness concerning various psychological indicators. Detailed information about the impact of different types of play on child mental state and development according to the selected studies is present in Table 3.
As a result of the qualitative analysis of the selected studies, 28 features through which play impacts the mental state of young children were identified. In accordance with the cultural-historical understanding of the nature of play, we identified 7 key generalized blocks of mechanisms:
1) Dialectical structure of play (imaginary situation, communication through nonverbal, symbolic means, enacting and re-enacting play roles, development of self-regulation, open-ended perspectives of play).
2) Emotional engagement and reflection (expression of feelings and/or thoughts, emotional scaffolding, facilitation of the interaction between raw emotions and conscious feeling state, repetition of emotions, verbalization).
3) Support of inner activity (regaining a sense of control and/or success, gaining initiative and creation).
4) Social interaction (positive social interaction, parental presence, positive child–parent interaction, opportunity to experiment with social behavior, collaborative nature of conversations, involvement of adult as a socialization agent, group responsibility).
5) Creation of a joyful, familiar, predictable and friendly environment (creation of a joyful, familiar, predictable, and friendly, creation of safe environment for interpersonal communication).
6) Active involvement (distraction of the child’s attention, involvement, incentives).
7) Opportunity to include additional information and/or techniques in play (relaxation exercises and/or techniques, familiarization with information about something / educational component included in play, aerobic exercises, exposure to the natural environment, use of special material).
The primary goal of this review was to analyze the potential of play for improving emotional sphere in children aged 3–7 years outside of a psychotherapeutic setting, particularly its potential to reduce negative emotional symptoms and develop emotional intelligence. A total of 33 studies were analyzed in this review. Existing research indicates the potential of play for addressing the mental state of preschoolers, including anxiety, stress, emotional regulation, and emotional-behavioral difficulties. Seven key pathways through which play affects children’s mental state were identified. However, there is no empirical evidence for the sustainability of these effects.
The analysis reveals four trends in studying play in preschool-aged children and its application possibilities. The first trend indicates that play can be effectively applied to develop or address a wide range of psychological parameters. The second trend shows that play can have a comprehensive impact on the mental state of children through various pathways affecting motivational, emotional, cognitive, and behavioral levels. The third trend descripts different types of play and reflects the significance of pretend play for the emotional development of young children, as it is the most frequently suggested intervention to reduce negative emotional symptoms and to improve emotional intelligence and its components. Play with rules, play with toys and digital play have also demonstrated their effectiveness in various studies. The fourth trend concerns the prevalence of clinical studies on the use of play to reduce negative mental states in children undergoing medical procedures compared to other stressful events.
The analysis of the selected studies revealed that play has a multifaceted impact on the mental state of preschoolers. Specifically, the review identified 13 psychological parameters in the emotional and social domains of the child that can be developed or corrected through play. Studies have shown the effectiveness of play in addressing all major psychological consequences of stressful events. Play not only helps relieve tension, reduce stress and anxiety but also fosters the development of emotional regulation and comprehension (Meng et al., 2022; Selzing-Musa et al., 2021), which contributes to stress resilience and coping with challenges (Chen, 2012; Gong and Zhang, 2012; Guo et al., 2010). The possibility of receiving support for subjectivity in the play and sufficient autonomy helps not only to overcome negative feelings but also develop self-help skills for other stressful events.
It is important to emphasize that the analysis of the studies demonstrated the accessibility of using play. Unlike therapeutic play and play therapy, organizing non-therapeutic play does not require extensive additional training for specialists. Non-therapeutic types of play and programs based on them can be implemented either without specialized training for educators, nurses, psychologists, and other professionals who work with children or with a brief introduction to instructions, game structure, and preparation, as well as training for parents. But we should note that organizing even non-therapeutic play requires basic psychological or pedagogic education, knowledge about the patterns of child mental development in different ages. Such types of play as pretend play, play with rules and toys, digital play, outdoor play can be organized both at home and in kindergartens.
A special role in using play to overcome negative emotional states and develop emotional intelligence belongs to toys. It is important that toys and play materials (e.g., costumes and props) correspond to the themes of the child’s experiences and are sufficiently varied (Goldstein, 1994). For instance, to address anxiety and fears related to adapting to a new kindergarten, it is necessary to have a sufficient number of toys that can represent children and educators in the play (Gorshkova and Lvova, 2023). At the same time, the main advantage of play is opportunity to use symbolic toys and open play materials (ribbons, fabrics, sticks, etc.), which facilitates the task of selecting toys (Veraksa A. N. et al., 2022).
Finally, the studies reviewed demonstrated effectiveness of play in different timeframes. One-time play sessions are commonly used to reduce situational emotional symptoms (Akhtar et al., 2023; Jones et al., 2021). Systematic play sessions contribute both to the negative symptoms reduction and emotional development (Jaggy et al., 2023; Stavrou, 2019). Prolonged non-therapeutic play exposure leads to an effect after only 6 weekly sessions. This indicates the potential of using non-therapeutic play in educational practice.
Thus, play outside a therapeutic setting can be used for overcoming negative emotional symptoms and develop emotional intelligence. In the reviewed studies, play outside the therapeutic context was used in education, parenting practices, before medical procedures, and to adapt to an orphanage. At the same time, the accessibility of its organization, corrective and developmental potential indicates the possibility of its use for children faced with various stressful situations. However, this assumption needs further research.
Play can have a comprehensive impact on the mental state of children through various pathways affecting motivational, emotional, cognitive, and behavioral levels. Seven blocks of identified pathways can be used separately or simultaneously, increasing the development potential of the play. Different types of play have different characteristics and combine different pathways of influence on the child emotional sphere.
In contemporary preschool education, significant importance is placed on pretend play as a special type of children’s activity, which is reflected in the number of studies where this type of play is used for development or correction. From the perspective of the cultural-historical approach, pretend play has the most essential impact on psychological development during preschool years (Hakkarainen and Bredikyte, 2010). Pretend play is a culturally conditioned type of activity where children, through imaginary situations, reproduce various aspects of adult life, thus learning social roles and relationships by enacting different plots (Elkonin, 2005). The uniqueness of pretend play lies in the emergence of a special relationship between the child and the situation, characterized by the dual nature of children’s experiences and actions (Veraksa, 2011; Vygotsky, 2017). In pretend play, the child operates on two levels—perceptual and symbolic—which together form the dialectical structure of play (Veraksa, 2022). For example, when a child builds a house out of pillows in play, they consider the real properties of the pillows but imagine them as a roof or bricks. While playing “hospital,” the child simultaneously cries like a patient and rejoices as a player. In other words, the child progresses to an activity centered on social relations and guided by an internal plan, leaving behind the play centered on and dominated by objects. Vygotsky conceptualizes this transition as a progressive separation between the visual field and the field of meaning (Hakkarainen and Brėdikytė, 2015; Hakkarainen et al., 2017; Vygotsky, 1978).
Therefore, according to the cultural-historical approach, pretend play serves as zone of proximal development for the child: what is challenging for the child to achieve through direct adult instruction becomes attainable in play, such as controlling aggressive behavior and regulating emotions (Vygotsky, 2017). A similar view on the developmental potential of pretend play can be found in other approaches. For example, Winnicott (1951) considered play as a transitional space because children use both objects and phenomena form the external world (e.g., toys) and aspects derived from their inner world (dreams or fantasies). In the transitional space, the child experiences his “omnipotence,” builds trust to the world, gets a new experience, and takes the next step toward development (Sagan, 2011). Additionally, in pretend play, the child reproduces models of social relationships (Elkonin, 2005). The child takes on various social roles, becoming an obedient student, a teacher, a hero, etc., and gains experience in partnership relationships with playmates. In play children exercises shared regulation, including emotional regulation, toward others because children must regulate their own behavior and, at the same time, control the behavior of playmates (Brėdikytė and Hakkarainen, 2017). These features of pretend play determine the variety of pathways impacting the child’s psyche. It is noteworthy that the review of studies showed that all 7 generalized blocks of mechanisms are characteristic of pretend play, which indicates its potential to address negative emotional symptoms and develop emotional intelligence in young children.
The review also showed that play with rules can contribute to reducing emotional symptoms in children. Play with rules is a type of play that imposes external rules that must be followed throughout the play (Veraksa et al., 2023). The main advantage of this type of play is that it allows to vary the complexity of the tasks for emotional regulation by adding new game components or exercises, for instance relax techniques or aerobic exercises (Azher et al., 2020; Bargale et al., 2021). In play with rules, through the addition of various components, an adult can transmit to a child cultural way of mastering their own internal processes. Following the rules of the game promotes the development of self-regulation, including emotional and social self-regulation (Savina and Wan, 2017). However, research showed that play with rules is effective for the development of older preschool children (6–7 years), while for younger preschoolers, following external rules remains a difficult task (Elkonin, 2005). For children under the age of 5–6 years, it is easier to follow the rules related to their self-chosen role in the pretend play. As well as pretend play, play with rules unfolds in imaginary situation, but commonly there is no plot and role development in play with rules, children do not use symbolic means. When playing with rules, children are more focused on following the rules or achieving a goal than on expressing their own emotions and experiences. To sum up, mentioned features limit the range of pathways through which play with rules can influence the child mental state and development. Main developmental mechanisms of play with rules are active involvement, opportunity to include additional information and/or techniques, imaginary situation.
It was revealed that play with toys can reduce negative emotional symptoms in children. The effect of playing with toys on a child mental state depends on the degree to which the child is immersed in an imaginary situation in play. When a child simply manipulates a toy, the effect either does not reach (Karaca and Guner, 2022) or through distracting the child’s attention helps to reduce anxiety (Arıkan and Esenay, 2020; Islaeli et al., 2020). In studies where a child creates a plot in a game with toys, indeed playing a pretend play, not only negative symptoms are reduced, but emotional regulation also develops (San et al., 2021). Thus, the number of pathways impacting the child’s mental state and development in play with toys depends on the quality and level of the certain game.
Digital play in the reviewed studies is also considered as a beneficial activity. Digital games are programs that organize and direct the play process on various electronic devices. In most studies digital play is used as a distractor or to introduce new information to a child (Dwairej et al., 2020; Matthyssens et al., 2020), which reduces anxiety, stress or fear. In one study digital play based on fairy-tale was proved to develop emotional regulation (Nicolaidou et al., 2022). However, first disadvantage of using digital play is social isolation while playing. Most digital games do not involve interaction with an adult or a peer, while positive social interaction is an important pathway to reduce negative emotional symptoms and develop emotional intelligence (Fonseca et al., 2021; Lozada et al., 2014; Smith and Carlson, 1997). Secondly, research have revealed that positive impact on child development from digital play is unsustainable (Veraksa et al., 2023b). Digital play does not contribute to systematic, qualitative changes in child development. Although some research uses digital play to address emotional symptoms in children, its corrective and developmental potential is limited.
The review of studies showed that outdoor play is another type of play to lessen emotional symptoms and improve emotional resilience. In the reviewed studies outdoor play implies the child’s play on the playground (Bao et al., 2022; Sobko et al., 2020). Thus, it may include elements from pretend play, play with rules or toys. In research it is highlighted that outdoor play commonly include physical activity (running, jumping, physical exercises, etc.), which helps to reduce tension (Wickel and Belton, 2016). Exposure to the natural environment also serves to overcome negative emotions (Sobko et al., 2020). Another, important mechanism in outdoor play is intensive social interaction with peers, which leads to socio-emotional development (Little and Wyver, 2008). Despite the mentioned pathways impacting the child’s mental state and development in outdoor play, its effectiveness depends on the specific child activity in it.
To sum up, the reviewed studies report that different types of play improve the emotional sphere of children. Pretend play involves the greatest number of pathways through which play influence the child psyche. At the same time, it is important to clarify that the corrective and developmental effect in non-therapeutic play is achieved not only through the play itself. Some features of the play providing the impact (e.g., emotional scaffolding, verbalization of emotions, parental presence, positive child–parent interaction, incentives, use of special material and additional information) depend on the involvement of a sensitive adult in the play. As a bearer of cultural experience and knowledge, the adult can enrich the child’s play, influence its depth and development, and provide models of actions, roles, and plots (Fleer, 2022). The participation of an adult in play ensures that the child gains unique experiences, thereby potentially determining the developmental potential of the play (Veresov et al., 2021). Thus, when implementing non-therapeutic play, it is important to consider not only the type of play and its features providing the effect but also the quality of adult involvement.
The main limitation of this review relate to the selection of publications. The selection process from search databases was conducted mainly without the use of automated algorithms. The search based on selected keywords might have excluded relevant studies, which reduced the number of reviewed studies. Additionally, this review did not include an assessment of the methodological quality of the studies using specialized tools (e.g., NHMRC framework, QualSyst, etc.). However, to eliminate this limitation methodological characteristics and statistical tests were analyzed in the review process.
The following summarizes our findings about implementation of non-therapeutic play to reduce emotional symptoms and develop emotional intelligence in young children.
1. To reduce emotional symptoms and develop emotional intelligence in non-therapeutic play, several blocks of pathways identified in this review should be combined to ensure correctional and developmental potential. Primary attention should be paid to creating dialectical structure of the play creating an imaginary situation, as well as providing emotional scaffolding, supporting the child’s inner activity, and ensuring group interaction between children (Bauer et al., 2021; Landreth, 1994; Vygotsky, 2017).
2. Involving a sensitive adult (parents, teachers or other assistance) in the game as an equal player increases correctional and developmental potential of intervention (Jones et al., 2021; Leung et al., 2022; Veresov et al., 2021).
3. To reduce situational emotional symptoms one-time play sessions in an unfamiliar environment up to 60 min can be organized. The play should include familiarization with a new environment, for example, the use of new objects in a play with rules, the use of a new space for pretend play, mastering new information in a digital play (Akhtar et al., 2023; Nicolaidou et al., 2022).
4. To foster emotional development systematic play sessions can be organized at least weekly for 6 weeks (in the reviewed studies 6 weeks of play sessions were the minimum option that had a positive effect). Play sessions should include usage of symbolic means, enacting and re-enacting play roles, emotional engagement and reflection, support of inner activity, positive social interaction (Jaggy et al., 2023; Selzing-Musa et al., 2021).
5. Symbolic toys, changeable toys (e.g., Lego), and improvised materials (ribbons, boxes, sticks, leaves, fabric of different colors and sizes, plastic tableware etc.) can be used to organize the game, which will enhance emotional engagement and support an imaginary situation (Kırkan and Kahraman, 2023; Selzing-Musa et al., 2021; Stavrou, 2019).
A future study should focus on a narrower topic to undertake a comprehensive analysis, especially the corrective effect of non-therapeutic play in children with high level of emotional or behavioral symptoms. An alternative approach would include evaluating the implementation of non-therapeutic play for children faced with different stressful events other than medical procedures (e.g., relocation, family difficulties). Finally, it is necessary to conduct research to determine whether non-therapeutic play have a long-lasting sustainable effect.
This review analyzed 33 studies published in the last 5 years. It was shown that play outside the therapeutic setting holds significant emotional corrective and developmental potential in early childhood. According to the cultural-historical approach, 7 generalized blocks of play features providing the impact on children’s mental state and development were identified: dialectical structure of play, emotional engagement and reflection, support of inner activity, social interaction, creation of a joyful, familiar, predictable, and friendly environment, active involvement, and the opportunity to include additional information and/or techniques in play. Among the different types of play, pretend play showed the greatest potential. However, it is important to consider the benefits of other types of play to engage the maximum number of mechanisms impacting the child’s psyche and to maximize its effectiveness. The variety of pathways of impact and accessibility for implementation indicate the potential for organizing psychological support programs for children, and particularly for children facing with stressful events, based on play. The results of this work can be used for practical purposes, such as developing psychological support programs based on non-therapeutic play activities.
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.
AV: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. VP: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – original draft. VS: Methodology, Writing – original draft. DK: Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing. NR: Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing.
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. The study was supported by The Ministry of Science and Higher Education of the Russian Federation (the research project 075–15–2024–526).
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Akhtar, A. B., Najeeb, H. N., and Rehman Ghafoor, A. U. (2023). Impact Of Incentive-Based Game Therapy And Transporting Children On Toy Vehicle To Operation Theatre On Reducing Anxiety Prior To Induction Of Anesthesia. J Pak Med Assoc. 73, 1388–1392. doi: 10.47391/JPMA.6630
Arıkan, A., and Esenay, F. I. (2020). Active and passive distraction interventions in a pediatric emergency department to reduce the pain and anxiety during venous blood sampling: a randomized clinical trial. J. Emerg. Nurs. 46, 779–790. doi: 10.1016/j.jen.2020.05.004
Armstrong, A. R., Galligan, R. F., and Critchley, C. R. (2011). Emotional intelligence and psychological resilience to negative life events. Personality and individual differences, 51, 331–336. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.03.025
Azher, U., Srinath, S. K., and Nayak, M. (2020). Effectiveness of bubble breath play therapy in the dental management of anxious children: a pilot study. J. Contemp. Dent. Pract. 21, 17–21. doi: 10.5005/jp-journals-10024-2741
Baggerly, J. (2004). The effects of child-centered group play therapy on self-concept, depression, and anxiety of children who are homeless. Int. J. Play Therapy 13, 31–51. doi: 10.1037/h0088889
Bao, Y., Gao, M., Luo, D., and Zhou, X. (2022). The influence of outdoor play spaces in urban parks on children’s social anxiety. Front. Public Health 10:1046399. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2022.1046399
Bargale, S., Khandelwal, J. R., Dave, B. H., Deshpande, A. N., Shah, S. S., and Chari, D. N. (2021). Comparative evaluation of effect of two relaxation breathing exercises on anxiety during buccal infiltration anesthesia in children aged 6–12 years: a randomized clinical study. J. Indian Society of Pedodontics and Preventive Dentistry 39, 284–290. doi: 10.4103/jisppd.jisppd_501_20
Bauer, R. H., Gilpin, A. T., and Thibodeau-Nielsen, R. B. (2021). Executive functions and imaginative play: exploring relations with prosocial behaviors using structural equation modeling. Trend Neurosci. Educ. 25:100165. doi: 10.1016/j.tine.2021.100165
Bawaeda, O., Wanda, D., and Aprillia, Z. (2023). Effectiveness of pop-it therapeutic play on children’s anxiety during inhalation therapy in children’s wards. Pediatr. Med. Chir. 45. doi: 10.4081/pmc.2023.315
Bennouna, C., Stark, L., and Wessells, M. (2020). “Children and adolescents in conflict and displacement” in Child, adolescent and family refugee mental health. eds. S. J. Song and P. Ventervogel (Berlin: Springer), 17–36.
Bowlby, J. (1979). The bowlby-Ainsworth attachment theory. Behav. Brain Sci. 2, 637–638. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00064955
Bozhovich, L. I. (1978). Towards the development of a person’s affective-need sphere. Problems of General, Age, and educational Psychology. 4, 168–182.
Bredikyte, M., and Hakkarainen, P. (2017). Self-regulation and narrative interventions in children’s play. In The Routledge international handbook of early childhood play, Routledge. 246–257.
Chaurasia, B., Jain, D., Mehta, S., Gandhi, K., and Mathew, P. J. (2019). Incentive-based game for allaying preoperative anxiety in children: a prospective, randomized trial. Anesth. Analg. 129, 1629–1634. doi: 10.1213/ANE.0000000000003717
Chen, X. (2012). Social capital and the deviant behavior of rural left-behind children. Zhejiang. Sociol. Sci. 5, 86–93.
Currie, J., and Almond, D. (2011). “Human capital development before age five” in Handbook of labor economics, Eds. David Card, Orley Ashenfelter, Handbook of Labor Economics. The Netherlands: Elsevier. (Elsevier), 4, 1315–1486.
Dwairej, D. A. A., Obeidat, H. M., and Aloweidi, A. S. (2020). Video game distraction and anesthesia mask practice reduces children’s preoperative anxiety: a randomized clinical trial. J. Spec. Pediatr. Nurs. 25:e12272. doi: 10.1111/jspn.12272
Elkonin, D. B. (1971). On the problem of periodization of mental development in childhood. Questions of psychology, 4, 32–39.
Elkonin, D. B. (2005). The psychology of play. Journal of Russian & East European Psychology, 43, 11–21. doi: 10.1080/10610405.2005.11059245
Fang, Y., Raat, H., Windhorst, D. A., Fierloos, I. N., Jonkman, H., Hosman, C. M. H., et al. (2022). The association between stressful life events and emotional and behavioral problems in children 0–7 years old: the CIKEO study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:1650. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19031650
Fleer, M. (2022). How conceptual PlayWorlds create different conditions for Children’s development across cultural age periods – a programmatic study overview. New Ideas in Child and Educ. Psychol. 2, 3–29. doi: 10.11621/nicep.2022.0201
Fonseca, X., Slingerland, G., Lukosch, S., and Brazier, F. (2021). Designing for meaningful social interaction in digital serious games. Entertainment Computing 36:100385. doi: 10.1016/j.entcom.2020.100385
Gavrilova, M., and Kornienko, D. (2025). Perezhivanie as a source of children’s development: case of emotional development intervention through visual arts. Front. Psychol. 15:1476973. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1476973
Goldstein, J. H. (1994). Toys, play, and child development. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Gong, W., and Zhang, J. (2012). A Meta-analysis of the relationship between individual perception stress and emotional intelligence. J. Southwest China Norm. Univ. 37, 146–151.
Gorshkova, E. V., and Lvova, N. V. (2023). Adaptation of 3rd and 4th years of life children to the conditions of preschool educational organization by means of game exercises with non-verbal communication elements. Preschool Educ. Today. 3, 36–43. doi: 10.24412/2782–4519-2023-3117-36-43
Goyel, V., Mathur, S., Dhingra, N., Nair, U., Singh, S., and Phukan, A. H. (2022). Evaluation of different pre-treatment behaviour modification techniques in 4–7-year olds: a randomised controlled trial. Indian J. Dent. Res. 33, 58–62. doi: 10.4103/ijdr.ijdr_373_21
Guo, R., He, Q., Shi, J., Gong, J., Wang, H., and Wang, Z. (2010). Short-term impact of cognition-motivation-emotional intelligence-resistance skills program on drug use prevention for school students in Wuhan, China. J. Huazhong Univ. Sci. Technol 30, 720–725. doi: 10.1007/s11596-010-0647-6
Hakkarainen, P., and Bredikyte, M. (2010). Strong foundation through play-based learning. Psycholog. Sci. Educ. 15, 58–64.
Hakkarainen, P., and Brėdikytė, M. (2015). “How play creates the zone of proximal development” in The Routledge international handbook of young Children’s thinking and understanding. eds. S. Robson and S. Flannery (Abingdon, UK: Routledge), 31–42.
Hakkarainen, P., Brėdikytė, M., and Safarov, I. (2017). “Pretend play and child development” in The routledge international handbook of early childhood play. eds. T. Bruce, P. Hakkarainen, and M. Brėdikytė (Abingdon, UK: Routledge), 70–84.
Heckman, J. J. (2011). The economics of inequality: the value of early childhood education. Am. Educ. 35:31.
Hedegaard, M., and Fleer, M. (2008). Studying children: A cultural-historical approach : McGraw-Hill Education (UK). Maidenhead: Open University Press.
Horoz, N., Buil, J. M., Koot, S., van Lenthe, F. J., Houweling, T. A., Oude Groeniger, J., et al. (2022). Moderators of an intervention on emotional and behavioural problems: household-and school-level parental education. Eur. J. Pub. Health 32, 864–870. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckac143
ICD-11 (2019). International classification of diseases 11th revision. The global standard for diagnostic health information. Available omline at: https://icd.who.int/en/ (Accessed June 20, 2024).
Islaeli, I., Yati, M., and Fadmi, F. R. (2020). The effect of play puzzle therapy on anxiety of children on preschooler in Kota Kendari hospital. Enferm. Clin. 30, 103–105. doi: 10.1016/j.enfcli.2019.11.032
Ivanov, M. V., Boksha, I. S., Balakireva, E. E., and Klyushnik, T. P. (2022). Epidemiological study on the early detection of mental disorders in young children in Russia. Consortium Psychiatricum 3, 18–26. doi: 10.17816/CP208
Jaggy, A. K., Kalkusch, I., Bossi, C. B., Weiss, B., Sticca, F., and Perren, S. (2023). The impact of social pretend play on preschoolers’ social development: results of an experimental study. Early Child. Res. Q. 64, 13–25. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2023.01.012
Jones, M. T., Kirkendall, M., Grissim, L., Daniels, S., and Boles, J. C. (2021). Exploration of the relationship between a group medical play intervention and children’s preoperative fear and anxiety. J. Pediatr. Health Care 35, 74–83. doi: 10.1016/j.pedhc.2020.08.001
Jowkar, B. (2007). Resilience mediating role in the relationship between emotional intelligence and general intelligence and life satisfaction. Contemporary Psychology, 2, 3–12.
Karaca, T. N., and Guner, U. C. (2022). The effect of music-moving toys to reduce fear and anxiety in preschool children undergoing intravenous insertion in a pediatric emergency department: a randomized clinical trial. J. Emerg. Nurs. 48, 32–44. doi: 10.1016/j.jen.2021.10.004
Khakimzyanov, R. N., and Ryazanov, D. A. (2022). Social, psychological and organizational factors affecting the psychological safety of the educational environment. Educ. Self Develop. 17, 303–320. doi: 10.26907/esd.17.4.21
Kırkan, Ç., and Kahraman, A. (2023). Effect of therapeutic play using a toy nebulizer and toy mask on a child’s fear and anxiety levels. J. Pediatr. Nurs. 73, e556–e562. doi: 10.1016/j.pedn.2023.10.033
Kumar, A., Das, S., Chauhan, S., Kiran, U., and Satapathy, S. (2019). Perioperative anxiety and stress in children undergoing congenital cardiac surgery and their parents: effect of brief intervention—a randomized control trial. J. Cardiothorac. Vasc. Anesth. 33, 1244–1250. doi: 10.1053/j.jvca.2018.08.187
Landreth, G. L. (1994). Play therapy: The art of relationships. Moscow: International Pedagogical Academy.
LeBlanc, M., and Ritchie, M. (2001). A meta-analysis of play therapy outcomes. Couns. Psychol. Q. 14, 149–163. doi: 10.1080/09515070110059142
Lekhwani, P. S., Nigam, A. G., Marwah, N., and Jain, S. (2023). Comparative evaluation of tell-show-do technique and its modifications in managing anxious pediatric dental patients among 4–8 years of age. J. Indian Society of Pedodontics and Preventive Dentistry 41, 141–148. doi: 10.4103/jisppd.jisppd_242_23
Leung, C., Hui, A. N., Wong, R. S., Rao, N., Karnilowicz, W., Chung, K., et al. (2022). Effectiveness of a multicomponent parenting intervention for promoting social-emotional school readiness among children from low-income families in Hong Kong: a cluster randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatr. 176, 357–364. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2021.6308
Little, H., and Wyver, S. (2008). Outdoor play: does avoiding the risks reduce the benefits? Australas. J. Early Childhood 33, 33–40. doi: 10.1177/183693910803300206
Lozada, M., Carro, N., D’adamo, P., and Barclay, C. (2014). Stress management in children: a pilot study in 7 to 9 year olds. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 35, 144–147. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000026
Luby, J. L., Belden, A. C., and Spitznagel, E. (2006). Risk factors for preschool depression: the mediating role of early stressful life events. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 47, 1292–1298. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01672.x
Maulding, W. S., Peters, G. B., Roberts, J., Leonard, E., and Sparkman, L. (2012). Emotional intelligence and resilience as predictors of leadership in school administrators. Journal of leadership studies, 5, 20–29. doi: 10.1002/jls.20240
Maru, V., Bhise Patil, R. S., Kumari, S., Tiwari, S., and Bapat, S. (2023). Influence of pretreatment exposure to pediatric dental care using the “tiny dentist’game on 4–7 years old children’s pain and anxiety: a parallel randomised clinical trial. J. Clin. Pediatr. Dent. 47, 96–102. doi: 10.22514/jocpd.2023.058
Matthyssens, L. E., Vanhulle, A., Seldenslach, L., Vander Stichele, G., Coppens, M., and Van Hoecke, E. (2020). A pilot study of the effectiveness of a serious game CliniPup® on perioperative anxiety and pain in children. J. Pediatr. Surg. 55, 304–311. doi: 10.1016/j.jpedsurg.2019.10.031
Meng, X., Fleer, M., Li, L., and Hammer, M. (2022). A cultural-historical study of the development of emotion regulation through collective play in an institutional care setting in China. Learn. Cult. Soc. Interact. 32:100590. doi: 10.1016/j.lcsi.2021.100590
Meshkova, N. V., Kudryavtsev, V. T., and Enikolopov, S. N. (2022). To the psychological portrait of the victims of telephone fraud. Vestnik Moskovskogo Universiteta. Seriya 14. Psikhologiya [Moscow University Psychology Bulletin] 1, 138–157. doi: 10.11621/vsp.2022.01.06
Moen, M. (2019). “The wrong direction”: childhood adversity and aggression in young children. Child Abuse Res. S. Afr. 20, 69–81.
Moher, D., Shamseer, L., Clarke, M., Ghersi, D., Liberati, A., Petticrew, M., et al. (2015). Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement. Syst. Rev. 4, 1–9. doi: 10.1186/2046-4053-4-1
Nicolaidou, I., Tozzi, F., and Antoniades, A. (2022). A gamified app on emotion recognition and anger management for pre-school children. Int. J. Child-Computer Interaction 31:100449. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcci.2021.100449
Obukhova, L. F. (2012). Vygotsky and developmental psychology in his and our time. Cultural-Historical Psychol. 1, 51–58.
Petersen, H., and Holodynski, M. (2020). Bewitched to be happy? The impact of pretend play on emotion regulation of expression in 3-to 6-year-olds. J. Genet. Psychol. 181, 111–126. doi: 10.1080/00221325.2020.1734909
Platt, R., Williams, S. R., and Ginsburg, G. S. (2016). Stressful life events and child anxiety: examining parent and child mediators. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 47, 23–34. doi: 10.1007/s10578-015-0540-4
Petrides, K. V. (2010). Trait emotional intelligence theory. Industrial and organizational psychology, 3, 136–139.
Richard, S., Baud-Bovy, G., Clerc-Georgy, A., and Gentaz, E. (2021). The effects of a ‘pretend play-based training’designed to promote the development of emotion comprehension, emotion regulation, and prosocial behaviour in 5-to 6-year-old Swiss children. Br. J. Psychol. 112, 690–719. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12484
Richard, S., Clerc-Georgy, A., and Gentaz, E. (2023). Pretend play-based training improves some socio-emotional competences in 5–6-year-old children: a large-scale study assessing implementation. Acta Psychol. 238:103961. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2023.103961
San, N. M. H., Myint, A. A., and Oo, C. Z. (2021). Using play to improve the social and emotional development of preschool children.Southeast. Asia Early Childhood J. 10, 16–35. doi: 10.37134/saecj.vol10.2.2.2021
Sánchez-Jáuregui, T., Téllez, A., Almaraz, D., Valdez, A., Hinojosa-Fernández, R., García-Balvaneda, H., et al. (2023). Adverse childhood experiences in Mexico: prevalence and association with sociodemographic variables and health status. Psychol. Russia: State of the Art 16, 48–62. doi: 10.11621/pir.2023.0204
Selzing-Musa, G., Nyinyi, S. D., Danbauchi, S. S., and Sunday, W. K. (2021). Impact of pretend play on self-regulation and resilience skills of nursery pupils in plateau state. Prestige J. Educ. 4, 23–30.
Shorer, M., Swissa, O., Levavi, P., and Swissa, A. (2021). Parental playfulness and children’s emotional regulation: the mediating role of parents’ emotional regulation and the parent–child relationship. Early Child Dev. Care 191, 210–220. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2019.1612385
Skinner, E. A., and Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J. (2007). The development of coping. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58, 119–144. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085705
Smith, C., and Carlson, B. E. (1997). Stress, coping, and resilience in children and youth. Soc. Serv. Rev. 71, 231–256. doi: 10.1086/604249
Sobko, T., Liang, S., Cheng, W. H., and Tun, H. M. (2020). Impact of outdoor nature-related activities on gut microbiota, fecal serotonin, and perceived stress in preschool children: the Play&Grow randomized controlled trial. Sci. Rep. 10:21993. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-78642-2
Stavrou, P. D. (2019). The development of emotional regulation in pre-schoolers: the role of sociodramatic play. Psychology 10, 62–78. doi: 10.4236/psych.2019.101005
Stulmaker, H. L., and Ray, D. C. (2015). Child-centered play therapy with young children who are anxious: a controlled trial. Child Youth Serv. Rev. 57, 127–133. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2015.08.005
Sun, J., Liu, Q., and Yu, S. (2019). Child neglect, psychological abuse and smartphone addiction among Chinese adolescents: the roles of emotional intelligence and coping style. Comput. Hum. Behav. 90, 74–83. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.08.032
Sagan, O. (2011). Between the lines: The transitional space of learning. In Re-shaping learning: A critical reader: The future of learning spaces in post-compulsory education (pp. 69–79). Rotterdam: SensePublishers.
Savina, E., and Wan, K. P. (2017). Cultural pathways to socio-emotional development and learning. Journal of Relationships Research, 8, e19. doi: 10.1017/jrr.2017.19
UNDRR. (2020). Human cost of disasters 2000-2019. Available online at: https://www.undrr.org/publication/human-cost-disasters-overview-last-20-years-2000-2019 (Accessed June 20, 2024).
Ünver, S., Güray, Ö., and Aral, S. (2020). Effectiveness of a group game intervention in reducing preoperative anxiety levels of children and parents: a randomized controlled trial. AORN J. 111, 403–412. doi: 10.1002/aorn.12990
Veraksa, N. E. (2022). Dialectical structure of preschool play. Natsional’nyy psikhologocheskiy zhurnal (National Psycholog. J.) 47, 4–12. doi: 10.11621/npj.2022.0302
Veraksa, A. N., Sidneva, A. N., Aslanova, M. S., and Plotnikova, V. A. (2022). Effectiveness of different teaching resources for forming the concept of magnitude in older preschoolers with varied levels of executive functions. Psychol. Russia: State of the Art 15, 62–82. doi: 10.11621/pir.2022.0405
Veraksa, N. E., Veraksa, A. N., and Plotnikova, V. A. (2023). Pretend play and project-based learning as factors in the development of preschool children. RUDN J. Psychol. Pedagogics 20, 431–445. doi: 10.22363/2313-1683-2023-20-3-431-445
Veraksa, N. E., Veresov, N. N., and Sukhikh, V. L. (2022). The play matrix: a tool for assessing role-play in early childhood. Int. J. Early Years Educ. 30, 542–559. doi: 10.1080/09669760.2022.2025582
Veraksa, A. N., Veresov, N. N., Sukhikh, V. L., Gavrilova, M. N., and Plotnikova, V. A. (2023a). Correction to: play to Foster Children’s executive function skills: exploring short-and long-term effects of digital and traditional types of play. IJEC 56, 711–712. doi: 10.1007/s13158-023-00385-8
Veraksa, A. N., Veresov, N. N., Sukhikh, V. L., Gavrilova, M. N., and Plotnikova, V. A. (2023b). Play to Foster Children’s executive function skills: exploring short-and long-term effects of digital and traditional types of play. IJEC. 56, 687–709. doi: 10.1007/s13158-023-00377-8
Veresov, N. (2017). The Concept of Perezhivanie in Cultural-Historical Theory: Content and Contexts. In: Fleer, M., González Rey, F., Veresov, N. (eds) Perezhivanie, Emotions and Subjectivity. Perspectives in Cultural-Historical Research, vol 1. Springer, Singapore. doi: 10.1007/978-981-10-4534-9_3
Veresov, N., and Veraksa, N. (2023). Digital games and digital play in early childhood: a cultural-historical approach. Early Years 43, 1089–1101. doi: 10.1080/09575146.2022.2056880
Veresov, N., Veraksa, A., Gavrilova, M., and Sukhikh, V. (2021). Do children need adult support during sociodramatic play to develop executive functions? Experimental evidence. Front. Psychol. 12:779023. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.779023
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. UK: Harvard University Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1983). Problems of mental development. Collected works. Editor: A.V. Zaporozhets. Vol.4. Moscow: Pedagogy, 338.
Vygotsky, L. S. (2017). Play and its role in the mental development of the child. Almanac of the Institute of Correctional Pedagogy 28, 1–33.
West, N., Christopher, N., Stratton, K., Görges, M., and Brown, Z. (2020). Reducing preoperative anxiety with child life preparation prior to intravenous induction of anesthesia: a randomized controlled trial. Pediatr. Anesth. 30, 168–180. doi: 10.1111/pan.13802
Wickel, E. E., and Belton, S. (2016). School’s out… now what? Objective estimates of afterschool sedentary time and physical activity from childhood to adolescence. J. Sci. Med. Sport 19, 654–658. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2015.09.001
Wagnild, G. M., and Young, H. M. (1993). Development and psychometric. Journal of nursing measurement, 1, 165–17847.
Winnicott, D. W. (1951). 1971. Transitional objects and transitional phenomena, Playing and Reality.
Zakharova, M. N., and Machinskaya, R. I. (2023). Voluntary control of cognitive activity in preschool children: age-dependent changes from ages 3-4 to 4-5. Psychol. Russia: State of the Art 16, 122–131. doi: 10.11621/pir.2023.0309
Zaporozhets, A. V. (1986). “On the question of the genesis, function and structure of emotional processes in a child //selected psychological works” in Mental development of a child. eds. V. V. Davydov and V. P. Zinchenko, Moscow: Pedagogika, 1, 260–275.
Zaporozhets, A. V., and Elkonin, D. B. (1956). General characteristics of children’s mental development. Moscow: Psychology. M.
Zeidner, M., Matthews, G., Roberts, R. D., and MacCann, C. (2003). Development of emotional intelligence: towards a multi-level investment model. Hum. Dev. 46, 69–96. doi: 10.1159/000068580
Zhang, L., Roslan, S., Zaremohzzabieh, Z., Jiang, Y., Wu, S., and Chen, Y. (2022). Perceived stress, social support, emotional intelligence, and post-stress growth among Chinese left-behind children: a moderated mediation model. International journal of environmental research and public health, 19, 1851. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19031851
Keywords: early childhood, cultural-historical approach, psychological help, pretend play, digital play, emotional regulation, anxiety
Citation: Veraksa AN, Plotnikova VA, Sukhikh VL, Kornienko DS and Rudnova NA (2025) Non-therapeutic play to overcome negative emotional symptoms and improve emotional intelligence in children aged 3–7: a systematic review. Front. Psychol. 16:1475387. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1475387
Received: 03 August 2024; Accepted: 21 February 2025;
Published: 06 March 2025.
Edited by:
Evgenia Gkintoni, General University Hospital of Patras, GreeceReviewed by:
Maayan Shorer, Ruppin Academic Center, IsraelCopyright © 2025 Veraksa, Plotnikova, Sukhikh, Kornienko and Rudnova. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Valeriya A. Plotnikova, bGVyLnNoaW5lbGlzQHlhbmRleC5ydQ==
Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Research integrity at Frontiers
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.