
94% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.
Find out more
ORIGINAL RESEARCH article
Front. Psychol. , 12 February 2025
Sec. Developmental Psychology
Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1468944
There is a lack of research on young children’s strengths, likely in part due to limited tools available to identify individual strengths in early childhood. To help address this gap and provide a brief measure for parents, researchers, and practitioners, the 37-item Preschool Strengths Inventory (PSI) was developed. First, focus groups with parents were conducted to identify strengths in early childhood. Based on the results of the focus groups and a review of the research, items were developed, reviewed by experts, and tested through exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis. The five factors identified were: Dynamic, Dependable, Caring, Inspiring, and Organized. Lastly, validity was tested and established with measures of personality traits and social skills, and the PSI’s test–retest reliability was examined. Results provide support for the content structure, reliability, and validity of the PSI. The PSI can provide the ability to study strengths beginning early in life and provide a foundation to develop strengths-focused interventions.
Strengths are central to the field of positive psychology and the strengths literature has established that identifying, using, and developing strengths can ameliorate risk factors and promote well-being (e.g., Ghielen et al., 2018; Lavy, 2019; Louis and Lopez, 2014; Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). Although the benefits of strengths are apparent across a variety of contexts, the literature related to strengths has focused primarily on adults and adolescents, with limited research involving children (Owens and Waters, 2020; Proctor et al., 2011; Quinlan, 2015). Despite the increasing attention to positive psychology principles in early childhood development and education in recent years (e.g., Baker et al., 2017; Lottman et al., 2017; Waters et al., 2022), there has been relatively little focus on the identification of individual-level strengths in young children. Therefore, the purpose of the studies presented in this paper was to develop and validate a brief strengths measure to identify individual, internal strengths present in children 3–5 years of age.
From a prevention and early intervention perspective, focusing attention on the strengths development process early in childhood is advantageous (Galloway et al., 2020; Hage et al., 2007; Nelson et al., 2003; Owens et al., 2018; Owens and Waters, 2020). The strengths development process begins with the identification of strengths (Clifton et al., 2006). Thus, assessments with the goal of identifying individual strengths in young children can help provide the means to begin the strengths development process early in life, likely resulting in a number of beneficial outcomes. For example, a developmentally appropriate framework for reflection on children’s strengths may help parents to provide appropriate forms of praise (e.g., non-inflated praise; Brummelman et al., 2017) or facilitate parent–child conversations about strengths, which can in turn promote positive parent–child relationships (Goodman et al., 2021). In the school setting, teachers could help students learn to use their individual strengths to enhance their educational experience and learning outcomes (Galloway et al., 2020). Related to mental health, developmentally appropriate strengths interventions could be designed to enhance well-being and address challenges, a noticeable gap in the literature (Owens and Waters, 2020).
In the positive psychology literature, there are several conceptualizations and definitions of strengths (Louis and Lopez, 2014). Across definitions, strengths are generally viewed as unique dispositional characteristics that represent what people are good at and promote positive outcomes, such as well-being (Biswas-Diener et al., 2011; Louis and Lopez, 2014). The definition that guided the development of the PSI extends from the trait theory framework (Carr, 2004): strengths represent positive traits or skills that promote optimal functioning (Owens et al., 2018). In line with this framework, strengths are observed to be fairly stable across the lifespan, but they are not fixed and can be strengthened as a result of environmental/contextual factors or interventions (e.g., Biswas-Diener et al., 2011; Bowers et al., 2010; Ghielen et al., 2018; Owens et al., 2018). Strengths can also be characterized into higher-order and lower-order characteristics, similar to the classification of personality traits (Caspi and Shiner, 2006; Putnam et al., 2001).
Although there are commonalities in the conceptualization of strengths with personality traits and key developmental tasks/abilities, there are some important distinctions. Personality traits represent stable ways in which people differ from one another without valence or the goal of achieving desirable outcomes (Funder, 2024). Developmental tasks tend to reflect what is necessary or expected for children of a given age or developmental stage (McCormick et al., 2011). Conversely, strengths are valenced (i.e., focused on positive characteristics resulting in beneficial outcomes) and reflect characteristics that are particularly strong within an individual compared to their peers or other characteristics and abilities they possess (Biswas-Diener et al., 2011; Louis and Lopez, 2014). Existing conceptualizations of strengths also tend to focus on traits, rather than cognitive developmental factors such as intelligence, memory, or executive functioning or specific abilities and talents such as athletic or musical ability (Biswas-Diener et al., 2011; Louis and Lopez, 2014; Shoshani, 2019). For young children in particular, strengths may be incorporated into identity in a way that personality traits and developmental tasks are not, given young children’s tendencies to focus on accomplishments and positive characteristics when thinking about the self (Harter, 1999).
As noted above, there is little research on strengths in early childhood that focuses on identifying and measuring strengths specific to this developmental stage. Our approach to strengths identification incorporates both inductive (i.e., focus groups) and deductive (i.e., review of developmental literature) methods for item generation. Although inductive methods relying on the qualitative responses of parents provided the overall structure for measurement, we expected that some of the strengths identified from the focus groups would likely map onto established constructs from research in early childhood development. For instance, the strength of kindness might overlap with elements of social competence, and the strength of positivity might overlap with the positive emotionality dimension of temperament. Our focus on a trait-like conceptualization of strengths connects to the literature on early childhood temperament and personality (Caspi et al., 2005; Shiner and DeYoung, 2013; Slobodskaya, 2021) and aligns with the individual strengths literature (Carr, 2004; Louis and Lopez, 2014).
Although a number of strengths measures are available for adults (e.g., Lopez et al., 2005; Peterson and Seligman, 2004) and adolescents (e.g., Leffert et al., 1998; Lopez et al., 2005; Park and Peterson, 2006), few measures exist for assessing individual strengths in young children. To the authors’ knowledge, there are currently three extant measures of strengths for preschool-aged children: the Behavioral and Emotional Rating Scale (BERS-2; Buckley and Epstein, 2004; Epstein et al., 2009; ages 5–18), the Devereux Early Childhood Assessment (DECA; LeBuffe and Naglieri, 1999; ages 2–5), and the Character Strengths Inventory for Early Childhood (CSI-EC; Shoshani, 2019; ages 3–6). However, neither the BERS-2 nor the DECA focus on individual, internal strengths of young children from a promotive perspective (i.e., designed to focus on positive characteristics without the presence of problems or risk). Rather, both the BERS-2 and DECA were designed based upon resilience literature. Despite the importance of resilience as a construct, it implies that some form of risk or barrier is present (Zolkoski and Bullock, 2012).
The CSI-EC is a parent report measure that was developed to measure young children’s strengths consistent with the VIA classification of character strengths (Shoshani, 2019). The measure includes 24 strengths clustered into four areas (i.e., interpersonal, intellectual, temperance, and transcendence). Interpersonal strengths include social intelligence, love, kindness, perspective, leadership, bravery, fairness, and teamwork. Intellectual strengths include love of learning, curiosity, appreciation of beauty, and creativity. Temperance strengths include self-regulation, modesty, prudence, forgiveness, open-mindedness, and persistence. Transcendence strengths include humor, zest, hope, authenticity, spirituality, and gratitude. Although the items of the CSI-EC were designed to assess parents’ perceptions of strength in children ages 3–6, the measure was created by writing items to align with the existing 24 character strengths of the VIA classification—strengths that were originally identified with an adult sample. The goal of this study and the PSI was to create a tool specifically designed to identify individual, internal strengths in early childhood.
Identifying and developing strengths has many benefits for individuals across the lifespan; however, those benefits are scarcely represented in the research on young children. Assessment tools with a focus on internal, individual strengths could help further the literature and establish evidenced-based strengths practices for young children.
To develop the PSI, several steps were taken. First, focus groups with parents of young children (ages 3–5) were conducted to identify specific strengths (Study 1). Scale items were created based on data from the focus groups as well as constructs from existing developmental literature, and an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) followed by a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) were conducted of the items generated (Study 2). Finally, test–retest reliability was examined and validity was tested with measures of personality traits and social skills (Study 3).
In study 1, focus groups with parents of preschool-aged children were conducted to inform the development of items for the PSI.
Parents/guardians of 3–5 year old children (M = 3.60, SD = 0.87) were recruited through preschools and day care centers in the Midwestern United States. Participation was voluntary and no compensation was provided to participants. Seventeen parents (65% mothers, 35% fathers, age M = 36.94, SD = 6.27) participated in one of four focus groups. The parent sample was 70% White, 12% Asian, 6% African American/Black, and 12% of another race/ethnicity.
A semi-structured interview protocol was developed by the first author and reviewed by several experts in developmental, school, and counseling psychology. The questions were designed to facilitate discussion about what strengths parents recognized in children across different settings to help identify which strengths to include in the initial item set. Questions included: (a) What is your definition of a strength? (b) Tell me about a time your child was at their best. What did that look like in your child? (c) What are your child’s strengths? What is your child particularly good at? At what age did your child begin showing these strengths? (d) What positive behaviors do you see in your child in different settings (home, school, with friends, with family, alone, etc.)? (e) What positive behaviors do you see in other children that you do not necessarily see in your child? The focus groups were conducted by the first author and were 45–75 min in length.
The focus groups were audio-recorded and later transcribed by the first author and two trained research assistants. The data was analyzed and coded using a basic interpretative approach (Merriam and Grenier, 2019). This method was chosen to discover and understand the perspectives of parents’ perceptions of young children’s strengths. With this method, data are inductively analyzed to identify common themes across the data.
When coding, the researchers listened for and kept an ongoing list of adjectives or traits either described or explicitly labeled by the parents and independently generated a list of characteristics. These characteristics were then organized into broader categories by the researchers based on similarities among the characteristics. Both the individual characteristics and broader themes were compared and discussed among the three researchers until a consensus within the group was reached.
Several qualitative best practice strategies were used throughout the research process (see Brod et al., 2009; Johnson et al., 2020; Whittemore et al., 2001). These included purposive sampling of young children’s parents, using a semi-structured interview, and one researcher leading all of the focus groups. The focus groups were also conducted until it appeared data saturation was reached; saturation was judged as being met when there was repetition of themes across the focus groups and no new additional strengths were identified. In addition, the focus groups were transcribed verbatim, and prior to starting the coding, the researchers engaged in researcher reflexivity by acknowledging and discussing their potential biases with one another. During the coding process, researcher triangulation was achieved by involving three researchers, and early on a literature review was conducted to see how emerging themes aligned with the existing literature. Finally, an auditor and expert checking were used (e.g., Brod et al., 2009; Johnson et al., 2020; Whittemore et al., 2001).
Four broad, overarching themes were identified from the parent focus groups. These included interpersonal strengths (i.e., characteristics that involve interacting with others), intrapersonal strengths (i.e., characteristics within a person), cognitive strengths, and physical strengths. Given the PSI’s goal to measure trait-like strengths, some cognitive strengths (e.g., memory) and physical strengths (e.g., coordination) mentioned in the focus groups were not included in the measure development process, as these abilities were outside of the scope of the intended measure. Examples of the themes identified from the focus groups can be found in Table 1. Overall, these themes are aligned with the theoretical basis of trait theory (Caspi and Shiner, 2006), which suggests there are a number of defined positive characteristics that represent individual differences across people.
Upon completion of the focus groups, the strengths identified by parents were compared to the temperament, personality, and trait literature, which guided the final decisions of which traits were included in the item-development phase, discussed further in Study 2. Within the broad categories of intrapersonal and interpersonal strengths, many characteristics identified by the parents aligned with personality traits such as extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness (Caspi and Shiner, 2006). For example, strengths aligned with the trait of extraversion included descriptors such as active, enthusiastic, positive, gregarious, and leadership; strengths aligned with the trait of conscientiousness included deliberate, focused, organized, persistent, and self-disciplined. However, there were also some strengths that did not directly align with trait categories identified by Caspi and Shiner (2006), such as humorous.
The purpose of study 2 was to build on the themes identified in study 1 to construct a quantitative measure of strengths that could be completed by parents to identify strengths in their young children. Steps for scale construction recommended by Walsh and Betz (2001) were used to guide the development of the PSI. First, a large pool of items was created and reviewed by experts. The items were then administered to an appropriate sample of participants. Finally, reliability estimates were examined.
The themes identified in study 1 were consistent with a trait-based approach to examining strengths in early childhood. We built upon these identified areas of strengths by incorporating related constructs from Caspi and Shiner (2006) proposed taxonomy of higher- and lower-order traits present in childhood and adolescence. The following criteria were used to determine the final list of characteristics included in the item pool: (a) the item must refer to an individual, internal characteristic; (b) consistent with a strengths-based approach, only positive characteristics were included; and (c) to reduce redundancy and create a measure of manageable length, adjectives that were similar in meaning were not included; rather, only one adjective that was most commonly found in the literature was included.
The characteristics included in the development of the PSI that were represented in both the parent focus groups and the developmental trait literature were: active, creative, curious, deliberate, empathic, enthusiastic, flexible, focused, generous, gregarious, helpful, humorous, leadership, open-minded, organized, persistent, positive, self-disciplined, and warm. Additional constructs drawn from the literature were: accepting, altruistic, calm, cooperative, goal-oriented, modest, and trustworthy.
Nine face-valid items were created for each characteristic (i.e., active, accepting, altruistic, calm, cooperative, creative, curious, deliberate, empathic, enthusiastic, flexible, focused, generous, goal-oriented, gregarious, helpful, humorous, leadership, modest, open-minded, organized, persistent, positive, self-disciplined, trustworthy, and warm), resulting in an initial pool of 234 items. Experts in developmental psychology (n = 1), early childhood education (n = 2), and counseling/clinical psychology (n = 3) reviewed the items for content validity and clarity. The experts included four women and two men; five of the experts identified as White and one identified as Latina. Reviewers were asked to examine (a) whether items were conceptually aligned with the strength/trait they were intended to assess and (b) structural features of the items such as clarity of wording, reading level required for comprehension, and ambiguity (DeVellis, 2017). The experts suggested clarifying the wording of the initial instructions, reducing the number of items presented at one time on the online survey screen, and using a progress bar on the online survey screen. The experts indicated they believed each item reflected the strength it was intended to measure and that the items were clear and concise. The suggestions provided by the experts were implemented, and the experts approved of the changes.
The response format for the PSI was designed to address concerns about parents’ documented tendency to overestimate their children’s positive qualities (e.g., Kårstad et al., 2014; Lagattuta et al., 2012). The PSI response scale was modeled on the Perceived Self-Competence Scale (Harter, 1982), which was designed to limit social desirability related to perceived self-competence. With this format, for each item in the Perceived Self-Competence Scale, participants select which description from the two provided best reflects how they perceive themselves and then to what degree (i.e., “somewhat” or “very much”). Similarly, with the PSI, parents select the description that is most like their child (e.g., “Some children are typically pessimistic” or “Some children are typically optimistic”) and to what degree the description is like their child (i.e., “somewhat” or “very much”). Furthermore, the questions were written to describe children in third person and parents were asked to think about their child in the context of the questions; therefore, a sense of distance between the question and the child is established. Items on the PSI were scaled from 1 to 4, with higher scores indicating greater endorsement of the identified strength.
Parents/guardians (N = 302, 51% mothers, 49% fathers) of young children (ages 3–5) were recruited to participate from preschools and day care centers in the Midwestern United States (n = 119) and through the online data collection platform Qualtrics (n = 183). Participants from preschools and day care centers volunteered their time and did not receive any compensation for their participation. Participants recruited from Qualtrics received compensation in Qualtrics cash points ($4), which are exchanged for incentives the participants select. The sample was randomly divided into two groups, with equivalent representation of mothers and fathers in each group, to conduct exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses. This approach is similar to that used in other instrument development studies (e.g., Shoshani, 2019).
For the EFA subsample, participants’ mean age was 36.12 years (SD = 7.72) and consisted of 82% White, 6% Asian, 4% African American/Black, 4% Hispanic/Latino/a/x, 2% Multiracial, and 2% of another race/ethnicity. The parents reported the following level of education: 24.5% high school diploma/GED; 39.1% Bachelor’s degree; 22.5% Master’s degree; 7.3% M.D./Ph.D./J.D.; and 6.6% “other” education. Their children (49% girls; 51% boys) were 3 (33%), 4 (43%), and 5 (24%) years old. The children’s race/ethnicity were: 76% White, 10% Multiracial, 7% Asian, 3% African American/Black, 3% Hispanic/Latino/a/x, and 1% of another race/ethnicity.
For the CFA sample, participants’ mean age was 34.84 years (SD = 7.12). The parent sample consisted of 77% White, 7% Hispanic/Latino/a/x, 6% African American/Black, 5% Asian, 1% Multiracial, 1% of another race/ethnicity, and 2% did not disclose. The parents reported the following level of education: 0.7% less than high school diploma; 23.2% high school diploma/GED; 40.4% Bachelor’s degree; 17.9% Master’s degree; 7.3% M.D./Ph.D./J.D.; and 10.6% “other” education. Their children (56% girls, 44% boys) were ages 3 (35%), 4 (40%), and 5 (25%) years old. The children’s race/ethnicity were: 70% White, 9% Multiracial, 7% African American/Black, 7% Hispanic/Latino/a/x, 5% Asian, 1% of another race/ethnicity, and 1% not reported.
Given that the underlying structure of early childhood strengths from a trait perspective was previously unknown, an EFA was selected as the first method of analysis. The EFA was conducted using the maximum likelihood method with a Promax (oblique) rotation. Several parameters were selected a priori given the sample size alongside data driven strategies to determine the number of factors included in the final model. Eigenvalues greater than one and the scree plot were initially examined; five or six factors were first identified. The factor loadings of the items were then evaluated. Given the sample size for the EFA was between 150 and 200, the recommended practice of retaining item loadings greater than or equal to 0.50 on at least one factor to ensure the item is a good measure of the overall factor (Tabachnick and Fidell, 2019; Worthington and Whittaker, 2006) and less than or equal to 0.25 on any other factor to eliminate high cross loadings was used. This method removed items that were either weakly loaded or cross-loaded on a number of factors. Finally, items were reviewed by the authors for weak internal validity and content. The final model resulted in a five-factor model consisting of 37 items (see Table 2).
Table 2. PSI items, EFA factor loadings, means, and standard deviations and CFA estimates, means, and standard deviations.
Due to the ordered categorical nature of the items (i.e., dichotomous presentation), a robust weighted least squares estimator (WLSMV) was used to conduct the CFA (Muthén and Muthén, 2017). Four indices were used to evaluate the fit of the CFA model, including the comparative fit index (CFI), the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI), the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the Chi Square of Model Fit (Chi Square). Hu and Bentler (1995) empirically examined a number of fit index cutoffs and suggested that in order to minimize Type I and Type II errors, a combination of an absolute fit index (e.g., RMSEA) and relative fit indices (e.g., CFI, TLI) should be used. The Chi-Square Test of Model Fit attempts to fit a model to the observed data, whereas the Chi-Square Test of Model Fit for the Baseline Model represents what the model is expected to be and serves as a null model (Muthén and Muthén, 2017). Therefore, the lower the chi-square value, the better the fit (Tabachnick and Fidell, 2019). As general guidelines, CFI and TLI values of 0.90 or above and RMSEA values of 0.06 or less are considered supportive of good model fit (Hu and Bentler, 1995). However, Browne and Cudeck (1993) caution model selection is subjective in nature and fit indices should not be used as a “mechanical decision process,” but rather as a tool to help guide the decision process (p. 157).
For the final EFA model, the sample size was deemed acceptable given that each factor loading was 0.50 or higher with no high cross loadings (Guadagnoli and Velicer, 1988; Worthington and Whittaker, 2006). See Table 2 for factor loadings, means, and standard deviations. Overall, the five-factor model had strong factor loadings (0.50–0.92), did not have any substantial cross loadings (≤ 0.24), and could be identified by the “bend” in the scree plot.
The items that remained following the implementation of the final decision rules for item retention aligned conceptually based upon trait theory. The five factors identified and corresponding percentage of variance accounted for by each factor included: Dynamic (21%), Dependable (14%), Caring (11%), Organized (8%), and Inspiring (6%). A description of each factor follows. See Table 2 for all items organized by strength and Supplementary Appendix 1 for the full measure.
• Dynamic (10 items) describes a young child who is enthusiastic, positive, creative, flexible, curious, and gregarious.
• Dependable (8 items) describes a young child who is goal-oriented, deliberate, and trustworthy.
• Caring (10 items) describes a young child who is accepting of others, empathic, generous, and helpful.
• Organized (4 items) describes a young child who arranges, categorizes, and organizes.
• Inspiring (5 items) describes a young child who is decisive, directive, influential among a group, and a leader.
Internal consistency was examined for this sample. Cronbach’s alphas ranged from 0.82–0.89 across the 5 subscales, demonstrating good internal consistency. The reliability estimates for each subscale can be found in Table 3.
Table 3. Descriptive statistics and Cronbach’s alphas for the PSI’s five subscales across four samples by children’s age and gender.
As noted previously, a separate sample was used to conduct the CFA. The five-factor model selected met or was just shy of the fit indices’ general guidelines (CFI = 0.90; TLI = 0.89; RMSEA = 0.06), including the Chi-Square Test of Model Fit value (905.64, p < 0.001), which was less than the Chi-Square Test of Model Fit for the Baseline Model (3404.09, p < 0.001). Together, the five-factor model selected was deemed adequate (Browne and Cudeck, 1993). The estimates and descriptives from the CFA can be found in Table 2.
Internal consistency was also examined for this sample. Cronbach’s alphas ranged from 0.83–0.86 across the five subscales, demonstrating good internal consistency (see Table 3).
The purpose of study 3 was to examine reliability and validity of the PSI. To do so, the PSI was administered twice, 1 month apart, and existing, validated measures of personality traits and social skills were administered to a sample of parents of young children.
A sample of 210 parents/guardians of young children (ages 3–5) from across the United States (50% mothers, 50% fathers) was recruited via Qualtrics. Participants received compensation for their participation in Qualtrics cash points ($4). The sample had a mean age of 33.31 years (SD = 5.26) and consisted of 82% White, 8% Hispanic/Latino/a/x, 5% African American/Black, 2% Asian, 2% Multiracial, and 1% Native American. The parents reported the following level of education: 0.5% less than high school diploma; 30.5% high school diploma/GED; 36.7% Bachelor’s degree; 17.1% Master’s degree; 4.8% M.D./Ph.D./J.D.; 9.5% “other” education; and 1% did not report. Family income levels were reported as: 2.9% with the range of $10,000–$14,000; 7.1% with the range of $15,000–$24,999; 6.7% with the range of $25,000–$34,999; 15.2% with the range of $35,000–$49,999; 25.2% with the range of $50,000–$74,999; 20.5% with the range of $75,000–$99,999; 21.9% with the range of $100,000 and above; and 0.5% did not report. Their children (51% girls; 49% boys) were ages 3 (39%), 4 (37%), and 5 (23%) years old; 1% did not report their child’s age. The children’s race/ethnicity were: 79% White, 9% Hispanic/Latino/a/x, 6% Multiracial, 3% African American/Black, 2% Asian, and 1% Native American.
From the Study 3 sample, 98 parents/guardians participated in the PSI test–retest administration by completing the PSI a second time 1 month later. These participants also received compensation for their participation in Qualtrics cash points ($4). Of the test–retest subset of the sample, 69.7% were mothers and 29.3% were fathers. The mean age was 34.10 years (SD = 5.02) and 87.9% were White, 6.1% Hispanic/Latino/a/x, 1% African American/Black, 1% Asian, 2% Multiracial, and 1% Native American. The parents reported the following level of education: 29.2% high school diploma/GED; 33.3% Bachelor’s degree; 23.2% Master’s degree; 5.1% M.D./Ph.D./J.D.; 8.1% “other” education; and 1% did not report. Family income levels were reported as: 1.0% with the range of $10,000–$14,000; 4.0% with the range of $15,000–$24,999; 5.1% with the range of $25,000–$34,999; 16.2% with the range of $35,000–$49,999; 24.2% with the range of $50,000–$74,999; 19.2% with the range of $75,000–$99,999; 29.3% with the range of $100,000 and above; and 1% did not report. Their children (54.5% girls; 44.4% boys) were ages 3 (46.5%), 4 (37.4%), and 5 (15.2%) years old; 1% did not report their child’s age. The children’s race/ethnicity were: 82.8% White, 6.1% Hispanic/Latino/a/x, 7.1% Multiracial, 1% African American/Black, and 2% Asian; 1% did not report their child’s race/ethnicity.
Scores from the five subscales of the PSI were compared to existing, validated measures of two related constructs: personality traits and social skills. Related, yet distinct, constructs were of interest to examine validity. Specifically, personality traits and social competence were selected given the identification of interpersonal and intrapersonal strengths in the PSI. Moreover, given the ongoing discussion of the similarities and differences between strengths and personality traits in existing research literature (e.g., Dametto and Noronha, 2021; McGrath et al., 2020; Najderska and Cieciuch, 2018; Ruch et al., 2023), examining their relations was also of particular interest. It was expected that the five factors identified by the PSI would correlate with the personality traits of openness, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism in construct-specific ways. For example, it was expected that the Dynamic subscale would be positively correlated with openness and extraversion, the Dependable and Organized subscales would be positively correlated with conscientiousness, and the Caring subscale would be positively correlated with agreeableness. It was expected that the PSI factors involving more interpersonal characteristics (e.g., Caring) would positively correlate with social skills.
Personality traits were measured using the Big Five Inventory (BFI; John and Srivastava, 1999). The parent report version of the BFI is a 44-item measure that assesses parents’ perceptions of their children across the Big Five Factors of personality (John et al., 1991; John et al., 2008). It includes five subscales: Openness, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Neuroticism. The average test–retest reliability was 0.83 across 8 weeks in an English sample and 0.85 across 6 weeks in a German sample (Rammstedt and John, 2007). On average, in past studies, the convergent validity correlations between the BFI and the NEO-PI-R was 0.78. For this study’s sample, internal consistencies were 0.83 for the Extraversion subscale, 0.78 for the Agreeableness subscale, 0.86 for the Conscientiousness subscale, 0.83 for the Neuroticism subscale, and 0.74 for the Openness subscale.
Social skills were measured using the Social Competence Scale—Parent Version (SCS; Corrigan, 1995). The SCS is a 12-item measure with two subscales: Prosocial/Communication Skills and Emotional Regulation Skills. The SCS was originally designed for elementary-school-aged children but has been validated with a preschool-aged sample (Gouley et al., 2008). With the past preschool-aged sample, internal consistency ranged from 0.87 to 0.92. The SCS demonstrated concurrent validity with the Social Skills Rating Scale—Preschool Version (Gresham and Elliott, 1990), Emotion Regulation Checklist (Shields and Cicchetti, 1997), Penn Interactive Peer Play Scale (Fantuzzo et al., 1995), New York Rating Scale (Miller et al., 1995), Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach, 1999), the difficult child subscale from the Parenting Stress Index—Short Form (Abidin, 1995), and cognitive ability using the Differential Abilities Scale (Elliott, 1990). For this study’s sample, the internal consistencies were 0.80 for the Prosocial/Communication Skills subscale and 0.81 for the Emotional Regulation Skills subscale.
Regarding validity, the results of the correlational analysis between the subscales of the PSI, BFI, and the SCS are included in Table 4. Correlations ranged from −0.60 to 0.68, and relations between variables were generally in the expected directions and comparable in size to relations in the literature (e.g., Asendorpf and Van Aken, 2002; Barbaranelli et al., 2003; John and Gross, 2007; Purnamaningsih, 2017). For example, the Dynamic subscale of the PSI was positively correlated with the openness and extraversion subscales of the BFI, r = 0.36, p < 0.001 and r = 0.68, p < 0.001, respectively. The Dependable and Organized subscales of the PSI were positively correlated with the conscientiousness subscale of the BFI, r = 0.65, p < 0.001 and r = 0.47, p < 0.001, respectively. The Caring subscale of the PSI was positively correlated with the agreeableness subscale of the BFI, r = 0.54, p < 0.001, as well as the prosocial/communication skills and emotion regulation skills subscales of the SCS, r = 0.63, p < 0.001 and r = 0.37, p < 0.001, respectively.
Results also indicated areas of both similarity and distinctiveness across the PSI subscales. For example, both Dynamic and Inspiring were positively correlated with the personality traits of extraversion and openness, but the relations were stronger for Dynamic than for Inspiring. Dynamic was also positively correlated with agreeableness and negatively correlated with neuroticism, whereas Inspiring was unrelated to these two constructs. These results are consistent with the definition of Dynamic as focused on positive interpersonal relationships, whereas Inspiring is more focused on interpersonal leadership.
Together, these results suggest that strengths represent the positive end of trait continua and are related to expected, similar traits and interpersonal skills. However, the significant correlations previously described were generally moderate in nature. This suggests that although the variables demonstrate some overlap in features, they are discrete variables.
Pearson correlations across each subscale ranged from 0.78–0.88 for a one-month period. The individual subscales were 0.81 (Dynamic), 0.85 (Dependable), 0.86 (Caring), 0.78 (Inspiring), and 0.88 (Organized). Results suggest good consistency over time.
It is apparent from past research that there are many benefits to identifying and fostering strengths across contexts (Ghielen et al., 2018; Lavy, 2019). However, less attention has been given to children, particularly young children, in the strengths literature. This gap is concerning, as young children are at a prime age to acquire positive messages about the self that could promote success and allow them to thrive in the future (Cabaj et al., 2014; Orth, 2018). In addition, although strengths are thought to be relatively stable across the lifespan (Bowers et al., 2010; Owens et al., 2018), a lack of research on strengths in childhood contributes to gaps in understanding of when such stability might emerge. Thus, the aim of this study was to develop a brief, reliable, and valid measure to identify preschool-aged children’s strengths. Such a tool could help build the strengths literature for young children and provide a means to foster young children’s strengths early in life.
The results presented in this paper provide support for the content structure, reliability, and validity of the PSI. Results from the EFA indicated that a five-factor model best described the data. The broad factors identified were Dynamic (i.e., enthusiastic, positive, creative, flexible, curious, gregarious), Dependable (i.e., goal-oriented, deliberate, trustworthy), Caring (i.e., accepting, empathic, generous, helpful), Inspiring (i.e., decisive, directive, influential, shows leadership), and Organized (i.e., children who arrange, categorize, and organize things). The five-factor model identified by the EFA was confirmed by a CFA. Additionally, the psychometrics of the measure, including internal consistency, test–retest reliability over 1 month, and relations with measures of personality traits and social skills, were good.
The primary structure of the PSI was formed based on data from parent focus groups, with additional input from extant research on childhood temperament, personality, and traits. Although the goal of developing the PSI measure was not to measure constructs highlighted within extant developmental research, it is important to note similarities between the strengths identified in the PSI and characteristics identified in existing theoretical and empirical literature. First, a variety of elements of the PSI aligned with the positive end of the trait continuum of the five-factor personality model identified in adolescents and adults. For example, traits under the PSI factors Dependable (goal-oriented, deliberate) and Organized (organizes) parallel lower-order traits typically subsumed by the higher-order factor of conscientiousness (self-control, achievement motivation, orderliness; Caspi and Shiner, 2006). Similarly, traits under the PSI factor Caring (helpful, empathic) parallel the lower-order traits typically under the higher-order factor agreeableness (prosocial tendencies; Caspi and Shiner, 2006). Traits within the PSI factor Dynamic (enthusiastic, gregarious, creative, curious) overlap with lower-order traits of both extraversion (sociability, energy level) and openness to experience (creativity, curiosity; Caspi and Shiner, 2006).
Although the elements identified by the PSI show some overlap with existing constructs, there are also meaningful distinctions and contributions of the measure as a whole. First, as mentioned previously, strengths are intended to measure positive characteristics that are believed to promote optimal functioning (Biswas-Diener et al., 2011; Louis and Lopez, 2014; Owens et al., 2018). Therefore, only positively valenced characteristics were included in the development of the PSI. Second, the traits identified in the development process and their organization within the broad factors of the PSI are not identical to the five factor model of personality. For example, the Dynamic construct shows some overlap with the sociability and positive affect elements of many theories and measures of early childhood temperament, but also includes other constructs such as curiosity and creativity. Elements of the Dependable and Organized factors align with elements of conscientiousness, but Dependable and Organized are each distinct PSI factors that are comprised of additional unique characteristics (e.g., to beginning of parenthetical statement trustworthy, arranges). The Inspiring factor does not parallel a specific domain of the five factor model of personality. Third, the Caring factor has some overlap with social competence; however, it also involves other related, but discrete characteristics, such as acceptance of others. These findings were reflected in the validity study. The constructs measured (i.e. personality and social skills) were generally correlated with the PSI factors in the expected directions to a moderate degree, suggesting they are related, yet distinct from one another. This provides further support for the claim that strengths and personality traits are unique and also suggests social skills are distinct from interpersonal strengths. Finally, strengths may also influence how young children engage with developmental tasks (Mahatmya et al., 2012). For example, Caring and Inspiring may reflect different approaches to the developmental task of engaging in peer play, whereas Dynamic and Dependable may reflect strategies for succeeding in preschool environments that reward goal-oriented behavior, intellectual curiosity, and flexibility in adapting to new situations.
While one strengths measure has been previously developed for young children—the CSI-EC (Shoshani, 2019)—the PSI addresses some of its limitations and offers a different measure with distinct strengths of its own. With the CSI-EC, the 24 character strengths that are part of the VIA character strengths model—initially developed for and with adults—were generalized to a young child sample. As noted previously, in developing the PSI, the researchers utilized a bottom-up approach in which parent focus groups were conducted to identify strengths present in young children and developmental literature was later reviewed to be as thorough as possible. This research approach resulted in different traits and factors being identified than the CSI-EC. The CSI-EC interpersonal strengths of kindness and love would likely be similar to traits encompassed by the Caring factor in the PSI (e.g., generous, helpful). The CSI-EC interpersonal strength of leadership aligns with some of the traits present within the PSI Inspiring factor (e.g., leader). The CSI-EC intellectual strengths of curiosity and creativity appear similar to the traits within the PSI Dynamic factor (i.e., creative, curious). The CSI-EC transcendence strength of zest parallels the trait of enthusiasm present in the Dynamic factor of the PSI. The remaining traits represented in the PSI (listed previously in the Factors and Factor Interpretability section; e.g., positive, flexible, goal-oriented, organized, decisive) appear distinct from the strengths in the CSI-EC. Finally, the PSI was also designed to be brief in nature, consisting of 37 items; the CSI-EC has 97 items. A brief measure will hopefully allow parents, practitioners, teachers, and researchers alike to assess young children’s strengths efficiently. Thus, although there is some overlap in the strengths represented in the CSI-EC and PSI, the differences present in the PSI and the approach in which the PSI was developed offer a unique contribution.
Although strengths are generally stable across time, external factors, such as role models, experiences, and interventions, can influence the overall impact of strengths (e.g., Biswas-Diener et al., 2011; Ghielen et al., 2018; Owens et al., 2018). Strengths interventions are designed to promote awareness of strengths and encourage individuals to find ways to use and strengthen their existing strengths (Ghielen et al., 2018; Louis and Lopez, 2014; Quinlan et al., 2012). Often, the first step in such a process is to identify the strengths present within the person (Clifton et al., 2006). Everyone has their own unique constellation of strengths, with no set of strengths being better or worse than another (Clifton et al., 2006; Peterson and Seligman, 2004). The PSI will help address this important first step by systematically establishing what strengths are present in young children. Once identified, these strengths can be used to promote well-being through activities, programs, and interventions in a variety of contexts, such as home, school, therapy, and recreational programs. For example, awareness of student strengths and the ability to assess them easily could help early childhood educators to provide the kind of personalized, student-centered instruction that is most beneficial for students (Darling-Hammond et al., 2023). Awareness of individual strengths could also help to promote a better understanding of well-being and strategies for promoting well-being among young children (Lottman et al., 2017; Waters et al., 2022).
A recent review of positive psychological interventions (PPIs) for children and adolescents highlighted the scarcity of interventions and programs that focus specifically on strengths, particularly for early childhood populations and settings (Owens and Waters, 2020). With the PSI, greater attention to strengths in young children may be possible, furthering the potential for strengths-based preventative and intervention efforts, and subsequent beneficial outcomes (e.g., enhanced well-being, decreased mental health concerns) early in life. Future research could also use the PSI to assess the impact of interventions; for example, would executive functioning interventions lead to increases in the dimensions of Dependable or Organized?
The creation of the PSI will also help further research endeavors. As noted previously, the PSI could be used to identify strengths that are the focus of strength development programs and interventions. The efficacy of such interventions could then be assessed through randomized clinical trials. By doing so, evidence-based approaches to developing strengths could be established and later used in applied contexts. Strengths identified from the PSI can also be examined in relation to other outcomes or mediating and moderating variables of interest in cross-sectional and longitudinal research. In addition, the developmental trajectory of strengths can be examined from a trait perspective starting early in life. Together, the PSI provides a tool to help further the research base in young children’s strengths.
A few limitations and future directions are important to consider. One limitation is that the EFA and CFA analyses had a less than ideal sample size. Given this consideration, steps were taken to address this or limit its impact. First, fairly equivalent samples of mothers and fathers across the United States were intentionally sought during recruitment to increase the generalizability and applicability of the PSI. Second, when conducting the EFA, a priori decision rules—following best practice guidelines—were used so that the items selected for retention were appropriate for the sample size; items with factor loadings under 0.50 or with cross-loading at or above 0.25 were excluded. Concerns regarding the sample size were also reduced when the factor structure from the EFA was supported by the CFA.
Relatedly, while gender (mothers and fathers for all samples), education levels (for all samples), and family income levels (for samples 2 and 3) were fairly well distributed, another limitation across all samples was the limited diversity related to race and ethnicity. Future research could include additional validity studies with the goal of expanding the diversity of the parents and children represented, particularly related to race and ethnicity. Additionally, examining the factor structure of the PSI with different cultural groups would determine whether or not the strengths identified in the current measure are applicable to other communities and what adaptations are needed.
Finally, the current study relied on parents to report their perceptions of their children’s strengths. Although parents are a valid source of data about young children, parents’ views of what is a strength in early childhood may differ from the views of other important adults (such as early childhood educators) or of children themselves. Thus, creating a teacher version of the PSI for use by early childhood educators would provide another important perspective on young children’s strengths, as children may display different strengths in home and school contexts. Similarly, asking children to report on their own self-perceived strengths could help to better foster the aspects of the self that children are most interested in and passionate about (Galloway and Reynolds 2015).
With research on young children’s strengths in its infancy, from both a research and practice standpoint, the PSI fills a gap in the literature. It provides a brief measure to systematically identify young children’s strengths. The use of this instrument could be helpful in developing evidence-based strengths programs and interventions as well as examining strengths longitudinally over time, which holds the potential to enhance young children’s lives.
The data that support the findings of this article can be made available to qualified individuals on reasonable request from the corresponding author.
The studies involving humans were approved by the University of Kansas Human Research Protection Program and University of Wisconsin – Stevens Point’s Institutional Review Board. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
RO: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft. MP: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. KM: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Funding in support of the publication of this article was provided by the University of Kansas. Funding to assist with participant recruitment was provided by the University of Kansas and the University of Wisconsin – Stevens Point.
RO would like to thank the following people for their assistance or consultation: Jade Bender, Thomas Motl, Karen Multon, Eden Owen, Meagan Patterson, and Graham Rifenbark. This paper is dedicated to my parents (Paula and David), Thomas, Olive, and Olin. - RO.
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1468944/full#supplementary-material
Abidin, R. R. (1995). Parenting Stress Index, Third Edition: Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.
Achenbach, T. M. (1999). The Child Behavior Checklist and related instruments. In The use of psychological testing for treatment planning and outcomes assessment ed. M. E. Maruish (2nd ed., pp. 429–466). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Asendorpf, J. B., and Van Aken, M. A. G. (2002). Validity of big five personality judgements in childhood: a 9 year longitudinal study. Eur. J. Personal. 17, 1–17. doi: 10.1002/per.460
Baker, L., Green, S., and Falecki, D. (2017). Positive early childhood education: expanding the reach of positive psychology into early childhood. Eur. J. Appl. Positi. Psychol. 1, 1–12.
Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., Rabasca, A., and Pastorelli, C. (2003). A questionnaire for measuring the big five in late childhood. Personal. Individ. Differ. 34, 645–664. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00051-X
Biswas-Diener, R., Kashdan, T. B., and Minhas, G. (2011). A dynamic approach to psychological strength development and intervention. J. Posit. Psychol. 6, 106–118. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2010.545429
Bowers, E. P., Li, Y., Kiely, M. K., Brittian, A., Lerner, J. V., and Lerner, R. M. (2010). The five Cs model of positive youth development: a longitudinal analysis of confirmatory factor structure and measurement invariance. J. Youth Adolesc. 39, 720–735. doi: 10.1007/s10964-010-9530-9
Brod, M., Tesler, L. E., and Christensen, T. L. (2009). Qualitative research and content validity: developing best practices based on science and experience. Qual. Life Res. 18, 1263–1278. doi: 10.1007/s11136-009-9540-9
Browne, M. W., and Cudeck, R. (1993). “Alternative ways of assessing model fit” in Testing structural equation models. eds. K. A. Bollen and J. S. Long (Newbury Park, CA: Sage), 136–162.
Brummelman, E., Nelemans, S. A., Thomaes, S., and Orobio de Castro, B. (2017). When parents’ praise inflates, children's self-esteem deflates. Child Dev. 88, 1799–1809. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12936
Buckley, J. A., and Epstein, M. H. (2004). The behavioral and emotional rating Scale-2 (BERS-2): providing a comprehensive approach to strength-based assessment. Contemp. Sch. Psychol. 9, 21–27. doi: 10.1007/BF03340904
Cabaj, J. L., McDonald, S. W., and Tough, S. C. (2014). Early childhood risk and resilience factors for behavioural and emotional problems in middle childhood. BMC Pediatr. 14, 1–11. doi: 10.1186/1471-2431-14-166
Carr, A. (2004). Positive psychology: the science of happiness and human strengths. London: Brunner-Routledge.
Caspi, A., Roberts, B. W., and Shiner, R. L. (2005). Personality development: stability and change. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 56, 453–484. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141913
Caspi, A., and Shiner, R. L. (2006). “Personality development” in Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development. eds. N. Eisenberg, W. Damon, and R. M. Lerner. 6th ed (Hoboken, New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.), 300–365.
Clifton, D. O., Anderson, E., and Schriener, L. A. (2006). StrengthsQuest: discover and develop your strengths in academics, career, and beyond : Gallup Press.
Corrigan, A. (1995). Social competence scale-parent version, kindergarten/year 1 (fast track project technical report). Available at: http://www.fasttrackproject.org/techrept/s/scp/scp1tech.pdf (Accessed July 19, 2024).
Dametto, D. M., and Noronha, A. P. P. (2021). Study between personality traits and character strengths in adolescents. Curr. Psychol., 40, 2067–2072. doi: 10.1007/ s12144-019-0146-2, doi: 10.1007/s12144-019-0146-2
Darling-Hammond, L., Schachner, A. C., Wojcikiewicz, S. K., and Flook, L. (2023). Educating teachers to enact the science of learning and development. Appl. Dev. Sci. 28, 1–21. doi: 10.1080/10888691.2022.2130506
DeVellis, R. F. (2017). Scale development: Theory and application. 4th Edn. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Elliott, C. D. (ed.). (1990). Differential ability scales. In Administration and scoring manual. (San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation).
Epstein, M. H., Synhorst, L. L., Cress, C. J., and Allen, E. A. (2009). Development and standardization of a test to measure the emotional and behavioral strengths of preschool children. J. Emot. Behav. Disord. 17, 29–37. doi: 10.1177/1063426608319223
Fantuzzo, J., Sutton-Smith, B., Coolahan, K. C., Manz, P. H., Canning, S., and Debnam, D. (1995). Assessment of preschool play interaction behaviors in young low-income children: Peen Interactive Peer Play Scale. Early Child. Res. Q. 10, 105–120. doi: 10.1016/0885-2006(95)90028-4
Galloway, R., and Reynolds, B. (2015). Positive psychology in the elementary classroom: The influence of strengths-based approaches on children’s self-efficacy. Open J. Soc. Sci. 3, 16–23. doi: 10.4236/jss.2015.39003
Galloway, R., Reynolds, B., and Williamson, J. (2020). Strengths-based teaching and learning approaches for children: perceptions and practices. J. Pedagog. Res. 4, 31–45. doi: 10.33902/JPR.2020058178
Ghielen, S. T. S., van Woerkom, M., and Meyers, M. C. (2018). Promoting positive outcomes through strengths interventions: a literature review. J. Posit. Psychol. 13, 1–13. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2017.1365164
Goodman, E. S., Ford, C. A., Friedrich, E. A., Ginsburg, K. R., Miller, V. A., and Mirman, J. H. (2021). Frequency of communication about adolescents’ strengths and weaknesses and the parent–adolescent relationship. Appl. Dev. Sci. 25, 260–271. doi: 10.1080/10888691.2019.1594813
Gouley, K. K., Brotman, L. M., Huang, K., and Shrout, P. E. (2008). Construct validation of the social competence scale in preschool-age children. Soc. Dev. 17, 380–398. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00430.x
Gresham, F. M., and Elliott, S. N. (1990). The Social Skills Rating System. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
Guadagnoli, E., and Velicer, W. F. (1988). Relation of sample size to the stability of component patterns. Psychol. Bull. 103, 265–275. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.103.2.265
Hage, S. M., Romano, J. L., Conyne, R. K., Kenny, M., Matthews, C., Schwartz, J. P., et al. (2007). Best practice guidelines on prevention practice, research, training, and social advocacy for psychologists. Counsel. Psychol. 35, 493–566. doi: 10.1177/0011000006291411
Harter, S. (1982). The perceived competence scale for children. Child Dev. 53, 87–97. doi: 10.2307/1129640
Hu, L. T., and Bentler, P. (1995). “Evaluating model fit” in Structural equation modeling. Concepts, issues, and applications. ed. R. H. Hoyle (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage), 76–99.
John, O. P., Donahue, E. M., and Kentle, R. L. (1991). The big five inventory – versions 4a and 54. Berkeley: University of California.
John, O. P., and Gross, J. J. (2007). “Individual differences in emotion regulation” in Handbook of emotion regulation. ed. J. J. Gross. 1st ed (Guilford Press), 351–372.
John, O. P., Naumann, L. P., and Soto, C. J. (2008). Paradigm shift to the integrative big five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and conceptual issues. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, and L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 114– 158). New York, NY: Guilford Press
John, O. P., and Srivastava, S. (1999). “The big-five trait taxonomy: history, measurement, and theoretical perspectives” in Handbook of personality: theory and research. eds. L. A. Pervin and O. P. John, vol. 2 (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 102–138.
Johnson, J. L., Adkins, D., and Chauvin, S. (2020). Qualitative research in pharmacy education: a review of the quality indicators of rigor in qualitative research. Am. J. Pharm. Educ. 81:7120. doi: 10.5688/ajpe7120
Kårstad, S. B., Kvello, Ø., Wichstrøm, L., and Berg-Nielsen, T. S. (2014). What do parents know about their children's comprehension of emotions? Accuracy of parental estimates in a community sample of pre-schoolers. Child Care Health Dev. 40, 346–353. doi: 10.1111/cch.12071
Lagattuta, K. H., Sayfan, L., and Bamford, C. (2012). Do you know how I feel? Parents underestimate worry and overestimate optimism compared to child self-report. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 113, 211–232. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.04.001
Lavy, S. (2019). A review of character strengths interventions in twenty-first-century schools: their importance and how they can be fostered. Appl. Res. Qual. Life 15, 573–596. doi: 10.1007/s11482-018-9700-6
LeBuffe, P. A., and Naglieri, J. A. (1999). The Devereux early childhood assessment. Lewisville, NC: Kaplan Press.
Leffert, N., Benson, P. L., Scales, P. C., Sharma, A. R., Drake, D. R., and Blyth, D. A. (1998). Developmental assets: measurement and prediction of risk behaviors among adolescents. Appl. Dev. Sci. 2, 209–230. doi: 10.1207/s1532480xads0204_4
Lopez, S. J., Hodges, T., and Harter, J. (2005). Clifton youth StrengthsExplorer technical report: Development and initial validation. Princeton, NJ: The Gallup Organization.
Lottman, T. J., Zawaly, S., and Niemiec, R. (2017). “Well-being and well-doing: bringing mindfulness and character strengths to the early childhood classroom and home” in Positive psychology interventions in practice. ed. C. Proctor (Cham, Switzerland: Springer), 83–105.
Louis, M. C., and Lopez, S. J. (2014). “Strengths interventions: current progress and future directions” in The Wiley Blackwell handbook of positive psychological interventions. eds. A. C. Parks and S. M. Schueller (West Sussex, UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd).
Mahatmya, D., Lohman, B. J., Matjasko, J. L., and Farb, A. F. (2012). “Engagement across developmental periods” in Handbook of research on student engagement. eds. S. Christenson, A. Reschly, and C. Wylie (New York, NY: Springer), 45–63.
McCormick, C. M., Kuo, S. I.-C., and Masten, A. S. (2011). “Developmental tasks across the lifespan” in Handbook of lifespan development. eds. K. L. Fingerman, C. A. Berg, J. Smith, and T. C. Antonucci (New York, NY: Springer), 117–140.
McGrath, R. E., Hall-Simmonds, A., and Goldberg, L. R. (2020). Are measures of character and personality distinct? Evidence from observed-score and true-score analyses. Assessment 27, 117–135. doi: 10.1177/1073191117738047
Merriam, S. B., and Grenier, R. S. (2019). Qualitative research in practice: examples for discussion and analysis. 2nd Edn. Hoboken, NJ: Jossey-Bass.
Miller, L. S., Klein, R. G., Piacentini, J., Abikoff, H., Shah, M. R., Samoilov, A., et al. (1995). The New York Teacher Rating Scale for disruptive and antisocial behavior. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 34, 359–370. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199503000-00022
Muthén, L. K., and Muthén, B. O. (2017). Mplus user’s guide. 8th Edn. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.
Najderska, M., and Cieciuch, J. (2018). The structure of character strengths: variable-and person-centered approaches. Front. Psychol. 9:153. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00153
Nelson, G., Westhues, A., and MacLeod, W. (2003). A meta-analysis of longitudinal research on preschool prevention programs for children. Prev. Treat. 6:np. doi: 10.1037/1522-3736.6.1.631a
Orth, U. (2018). The family environment in early childhood has a long-term effect on self-esteem: a longitudinal study from birth to age 27 years. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 114, 637–655. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000143
Owens, R. L., Baugh, L., Barrett-Wallis, R., Hui, N., and McDaniel, M. (2018). Strengths across the lifespan: a qualitative analysis of developmental trajectories and influential factors. Transla. Issues Psychol. Sci. 4, 265–276. doi: 10.1037/tps0000164
Owens, R. L., and Waters, L. (2020). What does positive psychology tell us about early intervention and prevention with children and adolescents? A review of positive psychological interventions with young people. J. Posit. Psychol. 15, 588–597. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2020.1789706
Park, N., and Peterson, C. (2006). Moral competence and character strengths among adolescents: the development and validation of the values in action inventory of strengths for youth. J. Adolesc. 29, 891–909. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2006.04.011
Peterson, C., and Seligman, M. E. P. (2004). Character strengths and virtues: a classification and handbook. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Proctor, C., Tsukayama, E., Wood, A. M., Maltby, J., Eades, J., and Linley, P. A. (2011). Strengths gym: the impact of a character strengths-based intervention on the life satisfaction and well-being of adolescents. J. Posit. Psychol. 6, 377–388. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2011.594079
Purnamaningsih, E. H. (2017). Personality and emotion regulation strategies. Int. J. Psychol. Res. 10, 53–60. doi: 10.21500/20112084.2040
Putnam, S. P., Ellis, L. K., and Rothbart, M. K. (2001). “The structure of temperament from infancy through adolescence” in Advances in research on temperament. eds. A. Eliasz and A. Angleitner (Lengerich, Germany: Pabst Scientist Publisher), 165–182.
Quinlan, D. M. (2015). How ‘other people matter’ in a classroom-based strengths intervention: exploring interpersonal strategies and classroom outcomes. J. Posit. Psychol. 10, 77–89. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2014.920407
Quinlan, D., Swain, N., and Vella-Brodrick, D. A. (2012). Character strengths interventions: Building on what we know for improved outcomes. J. Happiness Stud. 13, 1145–1163. doi: 10.1007/s10902-011-9311-5
Rammstedt, B., and John, O. P. (2007). Measuring personality in one minute or less: A 10-item short version of the Big Five Inventory in English and German. J. Res. Pers. 41, 203–212. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2006.02.001
Ruch, W., Vylobkova, V., and Heintz, S. (2023). Two of a kind or distant relatives? A multimethod investigation of the overlap between personality traits and character strengths. J. Individ. Differ. 44, 263–270. doi: 10.1027/1614-0001/a000400
Seligman, M. E. P., and Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: an introduction. Am. Psychol. 55, 5–14. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.5
Shields, A., and Cicchetti, D. (1997). Emotion regulation among school-age children: The development and validation of a new criterion Q-sort scale. Dev. Psychol. 33, 906–916. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.33.6.906
Shiner, R. L., and DeYoung, C. G. (2013). “The structure of temperament and personality traits: a developmental perspective” in The Oxford handbook of developmental psychology. ed. P. D. Zelazo, vol. 2. Self and other (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 113–141.
Shoshani, A. (2019). Young children’s character strengths and emotional well-being: development of the character strengths inventory for early childhood (CSI-EC). J. Posit. Psychol. 14, 86–102. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2018.1424925
Slobodskaya, H. R. (2021). Personality development from early childhood through adolescence. Personal. Individ. Differ. 172:110596. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110596
Tabachnick, B. G., and Fidell, L. S. (2019). Using multivariate statistics. 7th Edn. New York, NY: Pearson.
Waters, L., Dussert, D., and Loton, D. (2022). How do young children understand and action their own well-being? Positive psychology, student voice, and well-being literacy in early childhood. Int. J. Appl. Posit. Psychol. 7, 91–117. doi: 10.1007/s41042-021-00056-w
Whittemore, R., Chase, S. K., and Mandle, C. L. (2001). Validity in qualitative research. Qual. Health Res. 11, 522–537. doi: 10.1177/104973201129119299
Worthington, R. L., and Whittaker, T. A. (2006). Scale development research: a content analysis and recommendations for best practices. Couns. Psychol. 34, 806–838. doi: 10.1177/0011000006288127
Keywords: strengths, assets, early childhood, assessment, positive psychology, measure, instrument
Citation: Owens RL, Patterson MM and Multon KD (2025) The preschool strengths inventory: development and validation. Front. Psychol. 16:1468944. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1468944
Received: 22 July 2024; Accepted: 20 January 2025;
Published: 12 February 2025.
Edited by:
Pamela Bryden, Wilfrid Laurier University, CanadaReviewed by:
Rebecca Distefano, University of Minnesota Twin Cities, United StatesCopyright © 2025 Owens, Patterson and Multon. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Meagan M. Patterson, bW1wYXR0ZXJAa3UuZWR1
Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Research integrity at Frontiers
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.