Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychol. , 24 February 2025

Sec. Psychopathology

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1466088

Evaluating attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a review of current methods and issues

  • Faculty of Health Sciences, Universidad del Atlántico Medio, Las Palmas de Gran Canaria, Spain

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a common and complex neurodevelopmental disorder that affects individuals across the lifespan. This review provides an overview of the DSM diagnostic criteria for ADHD and discusses recommended considerations for the diagnosis of ADHD in children, adolescents, and adults. Its complexity requires careful consideration in the diagnostic process due to heterogeneity in clinical presentation and symptom overlap with other conditions. Commonly used assessment tools, including clinical interviews, rating scales and continuous performance tasks are reviewed with a focus on their psychometric qualities. Limitations of current diagnostic techniques, including issues related to gender bias, comorbidities and the importance of differential diagnosis are also reviewed. Improvements in the consistency and accuracy of ADHD diagnosis may be achieved by addressing these factors and evaluating the practical application of diagnostic tools.

1 Background and prevalence of ADHD

Attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is defined as a neurodevelopmental condition by American Psychiatric Association (2013), the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders V (DSM-5), and the World Health Organization International Classification of Diseases 11 (ICD-11) (World Health Organization, 2019). It is characterized by impaired attention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity, resulting in negative outcomes in multiple settings of one’s life. Due to its clinical presentation and vulnerability to comorbid diseases, ADHD is associated with increased risk of substance use, health issues, accidents and behavioral addictions among others (Pozzi et al., 2018; Rosenbloom and Wultz, 2011).

It is one of the most common neurodevelopmental disorders which affects 5.9% of children and adolescents (Faraone et al., 2021) and 3.10% among adults with inattentive presentation as the most common type (Ayano et al., 2023). Studies on ADHD prevalence usually focus on children and adolescents and the data on the adult ADHD is quite limited. This is because ADHD symptoms are more clinically apparent and easily identified in children and adolescents than adults. However, with the recent developments and approaches, studies on adult ADHD data are also on the rise. For instance, a meta-analysis estimated ADHD prevalence among adult based on those who reported ADHD symptoms since childhood (persistent ADHD) and those who reported ADHD symptoms in adulthood, regardless of the childhood onset. The results revealed that the worldwide prevalence for persistent adult ADHD was 2.58% and symptomatic adult ADHD was reported to be 6.76% (Song et al., 2021). While persistent adult ADHD reflects accurate epidemiological data aligned with the DSM criteria, it is important to consider individuals symptomatic adult ADHD due to the changes made in the diagnostic criteria over the years.

2 Clinical presentation

The etiology of ADHD highlights three impaired fundamental functions that affect individuals with ADHD at both cognitive and behavioral levels: inattention, hyperactivity and impulsivity. Attention refers to a set of both executive and non-executive functions such as alertness, the ability to select information and signal processing and it operates across both perceptual and non-perceptual domains (Oberauer, 2019). Impaired attention is one of the key indicators of ADHD along with poor planning, listening, organization and concentration abilities and distractibility. For individuals with ADHD, sustaining attention may be challenging, although they could easily hyperfocus on activities which they may find interesting (Bijlenga et al., 2019). Similarly, they exhibit reduced divided and selective attention compared to neurotypical individuals (Tucha et al., 2017). Hyperactivity is defined as excessive verbal behavior and physical movement (Sarver et al., 2015). In children with ADHD, hyperactivity presents itself with behaviors such as being impatient for waiting turn, interrupting conversations, fidgeting and excessive physical activity. Whereas in adults, hyperactivity may present itself differently such as exercising excessively and inability to relax or sleep (Bijlenga et al., 2019). Lastly, impulsivity is characterized by complex factors such as sensation-seeking, difficulties in delaying gratification and acting with limited forethought, which are also present in both internalizing and externalizing disorders (Johnson et al., 2017).

Beyond these three established symptom domains, nuanced impairments in executive functions (EF) are observed in ADHD (Pineda-Alhucema et al., 2018). EFs are characterized as the set of skills such as planning, reasoning, exhibiting goal-directed behavior, monitoring, sustained attention and problem-solving. These neurocognitive abilities are impaired in the ADHD pathophysiology and are common across all ages in ADHD due to delayed fronto-cerebral network development. Additionally, they are more pronounced in children and adolescents due to the ongoing maturation of the brain, with greater impairments observed among those with comorbidities (Sadozai et al., 2024).

According to the unifying theory of ADHD established by Barkley (1997), EF dysfunction in ADHD is evident across four domains: behavioral inhibition, working memory, internalized speech, and the regulation of emotions, motivation, and arousal. These functions interact to support goal-directed behavior and the prefrontal cortex has a key role in coordinating and integrating these processes. The model highlights that EFs are not just about impulse inhibition but also about generating new responses to challenges, making decisions, and adapting to complex situations, implying cognitive flexibility and self-regulation.

Working memory deficits which are responsible for storing and processing information, are frequently associated with poor organizational skills in children with ADHD aged 8–13 (Kofler et al., 2018). These impairments may decrease with age due to brain maturity, though attention difficulties can also interfere with working memory performance (Ramos et al., 2020). Moreover, impairments in domain-general central executive working memory, rather than its individual subcomponents, have been linked to ADHD severity, indicating that broader working memory mechanisms may significantly influence ADHD symptoms (Fosco et al., 2020). Inhibitory control, which is also critical for self-regulation, is a central feature of deficits in ADHD, as highlighted by Barkley’s EF theory (Barkley, 2025). Barkley argues that response inhibition is a central aspect of EF, and that it is essential for the adequate functioning of EFs. On the other hand, the ability to develop strategies, problem-solving and decision-making are considered as part of cognitive flexibility. In particular, the ability of task-switching and goal-directed behavior are determined by the level of cognitive flexibility. Compared to healthy controls, cognitive flexibility is considerably lower in those with ADHD (Roshani et al., 2020).

Following Mahone and Denckla’s debate on early neuropsychological theories of ADHD based their considerations on disturbances in EF (Mahone and Denckla, 2017), the most integrated model by Barkley (1997) indicates that behavioral inhibition to be a core impairment that consequently gives rise to difficulties with working memory, self-regulation, and motor control. The authors follow by explaining more recent theories have expanded this notion by incorporating additional concepts such as state regulation, delay aversion, and response variability. And most importantly, they underline the dynamic heterogeneity of ADHD, as stated in the dual-pathway model (Sonuga-Barke, 2003). This model links executive dysfunction and reward processes, particularly delay aversion. It specifies that ADHD is caused by dysregulation in two interconnected pathways: the executive circuit and the reward circuit. Dysregulation of the executive circuit, especially inhibitory control, causes dysfunction in the EF and negatively influences self-regulation and decision-making skills. Concurrently, abnormal functioning of the reward circuit results in delay aversion. Delay aversion, also known as temporal discounting, refers to the preference for immediate over delayed rewards, often driven by dopaminergic dysregulation (Mahone and Denckla, 2017; Kanarik et al., 2022). This is highly pronounced in ADHD and sheds light on the tendency to form maladaptive habits, such as smoking, gambling or impulsive spending, which all provide instant gratification (Weinsztok et al., 2021). Conversely, if they experience motivational challenges (e.g., performing long or not stimulating tasks), this may also exacerbate the ADHD symptoms (Posner et al., 2020). These frameworks explain how impulsivity and impaired self-regulation contribute to ADHD-related negative outcomes (Sonuga-Barke, 2003; Shen et al., 2020). Nevertheless, it should be noted that the impact of executive dysfunctions on daily functioning may also vary significantly among individuals with ADHD and that many factors such as presence of secondary psychiatric conditions and age may contribute negatively to these impairments (Crisci et al., 2021).

3 DSM diagnostic criteria and classification

3.1 DSM over the years

Acknowledgement of ADHD reaches back to 1775 in medical literature (Faraone et al., 2021). Nevertheless, there has been debate over the years regarding its classification system. As a result of the discrepancies, the diagnostic criteria have undergone revisions multiple times. Updates in the DSM manual for the diagnostic criteria for ADHD reflect all the improvements and advancements regarding understanding this disorder.

The diagnostic value of ADHD first took place in 1968 when it was named as the “Hyperkinetic Reaction of Childhood in DSM-II (American Psychiatric Association, 1968). As per symptoms of the disorder, hyperactivity, short attention span, restlessness, and distractibility were included in the criteria, much as they are in the current description of ADHD (Barkley, 2011). In 1980, the focus on the symptoms shifted from hyperactivity to inattention. ADHD gained popularity as inattention was discovered to be another symptom. DSM-III (American Psychiatic Association, 1980) reclassified ADHD under two divisions: ADD with hyperactivity and ADD without hyperactivity. In 1987, hyperactivity was reintroduced as a core symptom in the revised version of DSM (DSM III-R) (American Psychiatric Association, 1987). DSM III-R defined the disorder with the symptoms (inattention, hyperactivity and combined) all together without any subtypes. Additionally, ADHD was titled as Disruptive Behavior Disorder (DBD). Later in 1994, DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) brought back the subtypes as predominantly inattentive, predominantly hyperactive and combined. This version slightly broadened the definition of ADHD by incorporating instances related to social, vocational, and academic settings in the diagnostic criteria, implying that ADHD was accepted to be not just a disorder affecting children. The revised version of DSM-IV (DSM-IV-R) (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) was later released in 2000 and a fourth sub-category named “not otherwise specified” was added. The impairment had to be present before the age of 7.

3.2 DSM-5 criteria for ADHD

DSM-5 was published by the American Psychiatric Association in May 2013 (Steinau, 2013; Surís et al., 2016). DSM-5 is the latest edition of the DSM and replaces the DSM- IV-TR published in 2000. Although new disorders were generally included in DSM-5, some changes were made such as changing the diagnostic criteria of some existing disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Kendler, 1017). As the latest updated version of the DSM, DSM-5 criteria require that symptoms exacerbate or degrade the quality of social, vocational, and intellectual functioning and affect two or more areas of daily functioning to diagnose an individual (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Clinically, ADHD involves three presentations: inattention (ADHD-I), hyperactive/impulsive (ADHD-HI) and combined (ADHD-C). Inattention presentation includes the inability to pay or sustain attention, impaired thinking, inability to finish tasks, disorganized, distracted easily, and forgetfulness. The hyperactive/impulsive presentation includes the following symptoms: fidgeting, excessive talking, frequently interrupting, excessive physical activity, and inability to wait or take turns. Individuals who exhibit both inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms are classified under the ADHD combined presentation (Rigler et al., 2016). In the previous versions, these presentations were referred to as “subtypes.” However, this terminology has been updated as “presentations” in DSM-5 in order to reflect that the symptoms are not stable and may change over time (Epstein and Loren, 2013; Leffa et al., 2022).

Considering all the developments in the history of ADHD, the DSM-5 was issued in 2013 with significant changes in the criteria. ADHD has become recognized as a neurodevelopmental condition rather than Disruptive Behavior Disorder in the DSM-5. ADHD symptoms are classified as severe, moderate, or mild, with more flexibility regarding the absence or presence of symptoms in social, occupational or academic settings (Steinau, 2013; Surís et al., 2016). While symptom changes may be observed due to maturity, the DSM-5 notes that problems with hyperactivity, poor attention and impulse control persist and that many young individuals with ADHD persist substantially impaired even throughout adulthood. According to the symptomology presentation indicated by DSM-5, the criteria suggest that the individual should be experiencing (1) inattention or (2) hyperactivity and impulsivity or both patterns together for at least before the age of 12 without any psychotic disorder background. These symptoms must be present in two or more settings (social, academic and occupational life), resulting in impairment (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).

Some of the changes in the DSM have caused concerns from a clinical point of view. These changes have paved the way for adult diagnosis (Young and Goodman, 2016). For ADHD diagnosis in adults, DSM-IV required the presence of at least six symptoms, while DSM-5 reduced that criterion to five for adults (Maltezos et al., 2020). The reduction of the ADHD cut-off points in DSM-5 compared to DSM-IV has increased the likelihood of receiving the diagnosis of ADHD. Nevertheless, both versions include 18 items in total, the first nine items are related to inattention and the other nine items address hyperactivity and impulsivity symptoms. However, unlike DSM-IV, the ADHD criteria of DSM- 5 are no longer only for children. On one hand, this may be recognized as an ethical problem because the cut-off scores for the diagnostic criteria have been reduced. On the other hand, it has enabled many people to receive the diagnosis, access the adequate intervention and thereby improve their quality of life. Thus, the changes to the DSM-5 criteria for ADHD more accurately reflect the current scientific understanding of the disorder and help to better capture the diversity of symptoms and impairments experienced by individuals with ADHD.

While the DSM-5 introduced several changes, it is still recommended to consider factors such as age of onset and areas affected by symptoms (Rigler et al., 2016). Age of onset of ADHD is one of the most discussed concepts in the DSM. Debate over whether ADHD is an early age-onset or “late-onset” is still being debated (Caye et al., 2017; Cooper et al., 2018). The concept of late-onset does not emphasize the later onset of the disease, but the manifestation of the symptoms later in life. Some individuals with ADHD may compensate the cognitive and behavioral impairments by developing coping strategies in childhood or it could be masked or misdiagnosed by other conditions such as anxiety or depression (Riglin et al., 2022; Onandia-Hinchado et al., 2021). In such cases, not only clinical presentation but also socioeconomic and cultural differences are of great importance.

The previous versions of DSM involved statements oriented to children and adolescents. However, DSM-5 contains more general statements relevant to all age groups. For instance, in the DSM-IV, the statement “often loses things necessary for task activities” applied to toys, school-related activities or items. In DSM-5, these examples were expanded to items which also apply to adults (e.g., wallets, paperwork, keys). Compared to DSM-IV, DSM-5 has allowed the diagnostic utility for adult screening to some extent. In the previous version of DSM, while the diagnostic criterion age was seven, this was increased to 12 in DSM-5. While for those younger than 17 years of age six symptoms from either domain is considered sufficient, for those older than 17 years of age, this criterion is reduced to five. Hence, the change in age of onset is also one of the factors that facilitates the diagnosis of adults (Posner et al., 2020).

3.3 Diagnosing ADHD

Diagnosing ADHD requires a comprehensive assessment that considers both functional and cognitive impairments. It is essential to consider how ADHD affects an individual’s daily life across multiple areas, including academic performance, social relationships, and occupational functioning, rather than focusing solely on meeting the diagnostic criteria (Posner et al., 2020). Cognitive impairments, on the other hand, are important for predicting functional outcomes, understanding symptom presentation and differential diagnosis. Assessing the both of these impairments enables clinicians to develop tailored interventions and pinpoint the specific areas that require special attention.

Clinical interviews conducted by healthcare professionals have been the most acknowledged means of screening to establish an appropriate diagnosis (Danielson et al., 2018). It is substantial to determine the severity, duration, outcomes, genetic predispositions and the potential existence of any co-occurring conditions and ensure that the symptoms match the criteria and do not belong to another disorder. Apart from interviews, clinical experts often conduct multiple methods such as neuropsychological and behavioral assessment tools as well as gathering information from other informants involved in the patient’s life (NICE, 2018; Hall et al., 2016). However, to ensure interview quality, clinical interviews should be conducted first followed by secondary screening tools.

3.3.1 Diagnosing child and adolescent ADHD

In recent years, the recognition of ADHD has greatly improved, especially in child ADHD by family members and educators. This increased awareness has led to a greater number of individuals seeking attention and intervention from professionals, including psychologists, psychiatrists and general practitioners (McGough, 2014). About one in eight children with ADHD receive intervention from a psychologist, while most children with ADHD are cared for by a physician (French et al., 2020). According to National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE), recommended steps for children and adolescent ADHD is through pediatric services where the diagnosis is established by child and adolescent psychiatrists (Danielson et al., 2018). Initial diagnosis of ADHD requires several important types of assessment. These include the child’s family history and a thorough medical evaluation (e.g., neurological examination, laboratory tests). This is because there may be possible pathological conditions that may resemble ADHD. However, a child or adolescent diagnosed with ADHD may also have other disorders that also require further evaluation (Wolraich et al., 2019).

In preschool children, diagnosing ADHD may be challenging due to the overlap with typical distractibility and hyperactivity for their age. Children with ADHD often show excessive hyperactivity and impulsivity, along with difficulties such as sleep disturbances, tantrums, and aggression. Severe cases may persist over time, but early symptoms may improve as the child ages (Hall et al., 2016). For school-aged children, diagnosis largely relies on reports from parents and teachers, although self-report is also considered. This is because hyperactivity is often observable in classroom settings through behaviors such as restlessness, chattering, and difficulty sitting still in class, while the indicators of ADHD-I include challenges such as difficulty focusing, forgetting homework, and losing materials. These issues are often accompanied by social and behavioral problems (Cuffe et al., 2020). Observing the child in natural settings, such as classrooms, provides additional information about ADHD-related behaviors. In this period, common comorbidities could be observed.

An initial diagnosis of ADHD is also common in adolescence, usually due to the emergence of difficulties in the school, social and home environments, and the possible involvement in impulsive or risky behaviors. Adolescent ADHD often includes engagement with risky behaviors, which can lead to negative outcomes such as substance abuse, academic withdrawal, and mental health challenges like suicidal thoughts. External factors like family, peer relationships, and social circumstances further influence symptom severity and associated risks (Eccleston et al., 2019). Diagnosis is usually based on adolescent self-report, with parental and teacher input. Symptoms can vary in this age group, with adolescents reporting inner restlessness, motivation and organizational difficulties (Hall et al., 2016). Comorbidities such as depression, anxiety and substance use disorders may be more common, particularly during significant developmental transitions such as the pubertal period. Therefore, a more comprehensive approach may be required during this period compared to childhood diagnoses.

For child and adolescent assessments, clinical interviews both with the child and a secondary respondent is always strongly recommended. Specifically in the rating scales completed by teachers, respondent bias could be observed in case of very young children compared to older children (Hall et al., 2016). Hence, both objective and subjective measures should be gathered by parents and teachers in order to avoid conflicting information arising from potential biases or differing level of involvement. This would also allow the clinician to obtain broad information regarding the functional and cognitive impairments in multiple settings. By interviewing these secondary respondents, wider perspective of the child’s clinical presentation such as finding out when, in which settings and how the symptoms arise, the child’s developmental history, daily life and relationships with others and the settings where impairments manifest the most (e.g., unable to sit still in the classroom, unable to pay attention to activities) could be obtained. That is why it is commonly advised to access school records, academic performance, and medical history in ADHD diagnosis for children and adolescents. Therefore, in addition to parent interviews, it is advisable to gather information from teachers regarding the child or the adolescent’s behavior in more than one setting alone (NICE, 2018).

There are challenging parts of clinical interviews in pediatric diagnosis (Hall et al., 2016). One of these challenges is that children, unlike adolescents and adults, have lower verbal competences. Therefore, they may not be sufficiently expressive in clinical interviews. Families may be resistant to medication or other interventions to prevent their children from being stigmatized. In teacher interviews, the reporter bias may be high because the child behavior may differ from home to school environment, however it may be able to detect the ADHD-HI or ADHD-C presentation rather well. Besides, in the early ages, potential issues such as social and intellectual maturation, psychological state, or health conditions must be addressed. While working with children, if feasible, it is recommended to discuss their condition with their parents, teachers or any other important figures in their lives to obtain a better understanding of the child (Geddes and Andreasen, 2020).

3.3.2 Diagnosing adult ADHD

ADHD was long believed to be a childhood disorder that children grow out of as they age. However, many issues such as inattention, poor impulse control, subjective restlessness, poor planning, disorganization, poor self-regulation, and other deficits are highly likely to persist into adulthood (Slobodin et al., 2018). Due to that, diagnosis in later in life is also prevalent and common in ADHD and under-diagnosed or misdiagnosed cases of adult ADHD are highly common (Ginsberg et al., 2014).

Adults face significant psychological, social, and academic stressors, which can adversely affect their EF and emotional regulation. These challenges along with poor self-regulation later extends to occupational settings (Harpin et al., 2016). While hyperactivity is physically manifested in childhood, in adulthood it tends to be more internalized. It often appears as risky behaviors, financial difficulties, or restlessness (Anbarasan et al., 2020) accompanied by EF impairments. In older adults, there is limited ADHD research available which could be partly due to the challenges coming along with cognitive decline and behavioral issues associated with it. Co-occurring conditions like anxiety and depression may further complicate the diagnosis (Kooij et al., 2016) and traumatic or stressful experiences could also resemble ADHD-like symptoms (Marshall et al., 2021).

Age-appropriate symptom profile for adult ADHD in line with the DSM-5, the criteria for adult ADHD should be judged very differently from the criteria for child and adolescent ADHD (Maltezos et al., 2020; Asherson et al., 2016). When considering the symptoms of inattention, the behaviors that we may encounter in adult ADHD include distraction while talking and performing a task, inability to finish initiated tasks or projects, failure of performing individual responsibilities (e.g., paying taxes or bills, doing house chores), forgetfulness, vocational issues and inability to manage time. On the other hand, hyperactivity is seen in a very distinct form than in childhood. In adults with ADHD-HI or ADHD-C presentations, behaviors such as inability to sit for long periods of time, having the urge to move often, interrupting others during conversations and excessive talking are highly common (Gentile et al., 2006).

National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE) (Danielson et al., 2018) guidelines suggest that adults without a formal childhood diagnosis presenting with ADHD symptoms should be referred to adult specialist services. These services assess the typical features of ADHD that emerge in childhood and persist into adulthood, the possible co-existence of other psychiatric conditions, and the identification of significant psychological, occupational or social impairment. Also, behavioral and functional impairments related to EF should be carefully assessed as previously suggested by Brown’s EF model of ADHD (Brown, 2008). These functional areas include activation, focus, effort, emotional, memory and action. These domains are specifically sensitive to cognitive and behavioral diagnostic tools.

Late-onset ADHD is also widely discussed in the literature. Caye et al. (2017) highlights that this phenomenon, also referred to as “de novo adults” is highly common in adults where a significant proportion of them do not meet the ADHD criteria in childhood but prevalence rate of late-onset adult ADHD is 4.4%. They explain this through two models: complex phenotype model, and the restricted phenotype model. Complex phenotype model suggests that biological and environmental factors could lead to the emergence of symptom later in life. ADHD symptoms may remain dormant during the early ages or compensated via protective factors such as positive family environment. However, the transition from childhood to adulthood also implicates higher environmental demands which may contribute to the visibility and the aggregation of the symptoms. Whereas the restricted phenotype model argues that other pre-existing conditions such as trauma, substance abuse, hormonal imbalances may be the main cause of ADHD-like symptoms. Determining whether these late-onset symptoms are due to true ADHD cases requires a multimodal approach where behavioral and cognitive indicators, medical history and environmental stressors are explored through assessment. On the other hand, the environmental stressors may aggregate or reduce the symptoms resulting in symptom fluidity. The severity of the symptoms can fluctuate over time as well as the symptom presentations. Inattention is most likely to persist throughout life, while hyperactivity and impulsivity tend to decrease more over time (Emser et al., 2018). However, the neuropsychological deficits such as attentional vigilance and EF start in childhood and could continue to impact everyday life, persisting into adulthood (Moffitt et al., 2015).

4 Evaluation tools

4.1 Clinical interviews

Clinical interviews have always been of greater clinical value than neuropsychological testing, computerized tests, standardized questionnaires and other tools (Hall et al., 2016). They are divided as structured and unstructured. Structured interviews are objective with strictly predefined questions. Unstructured interviews are performed with a sequence of questions asked by the interviewer that are unpremeditated and adjusted based on the course of the session (Marshall et al., 2021). Although they may help the interviewer discover different aspects and the needs of the patient and build comprehensive rapport, they are low in reliability, more subjective and depend on the expertise level of the interviewer. Consequently, structured interviews provide a systematic and standardized framework for clinicians. These interviews help the clinicians obtain the information that rating scales may not uncover, as structured interviews assess the presence of disorders and comorbidities.

A model based on the DSM has been proposed by a previous study, suggesting a 5-point model to facilitate a more accurate assessment within the diagnostic system, consequently enhancing clinical utility (Chang et al., 2016). Firstly, it is argued that assessing how mental health disorders are initially conceptualized is crucial for determining their clinical utility. Secondly, there is emphasis on the importance of the accurate communication of the relevant clinical information from the relevant parties (e.g., patient and patient’s family, practitioners, healthcare professionals). Thirdly, it is reported that the diagnostic criteria should always be used in the clinical interview. Lastly, the right interventions for the patient should be identified, and the patient should be guided adequately considering their current and future needs. Similarly, there are other important aspects in a clinical interview (Barkley, 2011). To obtain a diagnosis of ADHD, firstly, the patient who comes to the consultation must be questioned about any experienced symptoms in childhood and present symptom complaints. Further approach for establishing a diagnosis could be taken by conducting a formal interview with someone close to the patient (e.g., a family member, friend, teacher or partner) to further gain information about their daily functioning and social relationships in addition to behavioral observation, academic or medical reports (Posner et al., 2020). However, the interviewer is also an important determinant for the quality and the variety of the screening. Objective measurement and additional screening tools such as rating scales, computerized tests and many other tools that may support the accuracy of the diagnosis are of great importance (Hall et al., 2016).

4.1.1 Which tools are commonly used?

The Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children (DISC-V) (Shaffer et al., 2000) is a structured interview DSM-IV based screening tool. It involves six modules and approximately 3,000 questions. From these, 358 of them are categorized as stem questions meaning that they are standard questions given to all respondents. Around 1,300 contingent questions are asked to avoid false positive responses directed during stem questions. The information on age of onset is highly considered to determine the clinical standpoint and the duration of the symptoms. To ascertain the level of impairment, the respondents are also asked questions which involve multiple social settings, as stated in the diagnostic criteria. Additionally, questions regarding the medical history and the family history are also included.

The Young DIVA-51 is a structured clinical interview designed to assess ADHD symptoms in children and adolescents aged 5–17 years. Previous versions have shown high diagnostic accuracy and practical use among adults (Ramos-Quiroga et al., 2019). It is based on the DSM-5 criteria, and DIVA for a comprehensive evaluation of ADHD symptoms across different age groups. The Young DIVA-5 adapted to the developmental and behavioral factors specific to children and adolescents. Although there is limited evidence on the Young DIVA-5, the high potential demonstrated by earlier versions suggests that it could also be an ideal tool for assessing ADHD in younger populations.

In adult clinical diagnosis, similar tools are generally used. One of the most frequently used tools is The Diagnostic Interview for ADHD in Adults (DIVA) (see text footnote 1) (Kooij and Francken, 2010) which also includes a version for children adolescents. The DIVA was first introduced in Dutch in 2007 and recently it has been updated according to the DSM-5 and translated into more than 25 languages. It has three main sections as follows: (1) childhood and adulthood symptoms, (2) the age of onset of symptoms and (3) areas of impairment (Kooij, 2022). There are 18 DSM-oriented items in total and this semi-structured interview involves retrospective questions (about the childhood). Moreover, DIVA is also offered for free and online via an app, which facilitates the access which means that it also helps the individual to do a self-assessment for suspected ADHD. Given that it is a relatively recent version, there has not been extensive work on the clinical utility of this interview tool. The previous versions such as DIVA-2 was revealed to detect ADHD more accurately than neuropsychological testing and rating scales (Pettersson et al., 2018). Nevertheless, studies conducted with both Korean and Persian samples in DIVA-5 show high sensitivity (SN) and specificity (SP) (Hong et al., 2020) and good test–retest and inter-rater reliability (Zamani et al., 2021). These studies show that DIVA-5 is a reliable tool and can discriminate individuals with ADHD from control subjects accurately. Furthermore, Conners Adult ADHD Diagnostic Interview for DSM-IV (CAADID) is a gold standard DSM-based semi-structure tool and is used in the diagnosis of ADHD in both children and adults (Ramos-Quiroga et al., 2019; Epstein and Kollins, 2006). CAADID’s ability to distinguish healthy controls from adults with ADHD and that it has good test–retest reliability.

To summarize in general, clinical interviews are considered as the gold standard for ADHD diagnosis. However, to accurately diagnose ADHD, firstly, a clinical interview based on the diagnostic manual is required. It is crucial to use one or more assessment tools, and to obtain a report from an informant involved in the individual’s life to assess the individual. The reason for this is that ADHD criteria may vary from children and adolescents to adults and functional and cognitive impairments may change with age (Matte et al., 2015). However, its crucial to recognize that there is limited research regarding the accuracy of the clinical interviews (Marshall et al., 2021). Although there are tools with promising psychometric values, it is important to employ a multifaceted approach to ensure an accurate ADHD evaluation. The inclusion of structured DSM- based interviews and other screening instruments can provide a clearer and more reliable profile of the patient’s condition. The use of multiple diagnostic tools is necessary to create a more accurate and reliable ADHD diagnostic system.

4.2 Use of rating scales in the assessment of ADHD

In addition to clinical diagnosis, rating scales have a very important part in diagnosis of ADHD. Rating scales help the clinician to validate the presence of the disorder and to identify possible co-existing conditions. In addition, availability of multiple respondents in some rating scales allows the clinician to evaluate the patient profile with the responses of various people (e.g., family members, teachers). They are also a useful tool that allow the clinician to choose the most suitable treatment approach.

Rating scales are often the most favored evaluation tool after clinical interviews. Although they are secondary, they play a crucial role in addressing the limitations of clinical interviews. In clinical interviews, if the patient is young, the primary interviewee is usually a parent or guardian, although the patient is still interviewed. Conversely, the parents may provide broad perspective, however their impartiality should be considered. Similarly, children’s limited ability to express themselves emotionally and verbally may influence the clinician’s judgment. And in adolescents, they may also exhibit inconsistencies in self-report or deny experiencing problems. This situation highlights the importance of using rating scales with informants as a secondary diagnostic tool in the assessment of ADHD.

From a clinical standpoint, numerous approaches have been developed to evaluate the quality and usefulness of a tool, and several models have been proposed for it. Numerous factors are reported in the literature to be associated with clinical utility (Smart, 2006). These are usually related to many factors such as duration, accessibility, assessment, and the pricing of the tools. According to a previously suggested model, there are six elements to determine clinical utility of the rating scales. These are as follows respectively: (a) ease of use, (b) time, (c) training and qualifications, (d) format, (e) interpretation, and (f) meaning and relevance of information obtained. In general, the criteria considered here are the availability of the tool, its price, the clarity of the language of the instructions given for both the clinician and the patient, the time given and the effect of this time on reliability and validity, the acceptability of the tool for both the patient and the clinician, the expertise of the clinician and the requirements for interpreting the results, and many other minor criteria are encompassed in these six elements. Similarly, a multi-dimensional model indicates that there are four main elements to determine clinical utility: appropriateness, accessibility, practicability, and the acceptability (Smart, 2006). With these four elements, several important factors such as the appropriateness of the tool in general, its clinical significance, price, accessibility, whether it would cause ethical, legal, social or psychological concerns.

Rating scales are valuable in diagnosis and management, having been rigorously evaluated using psychometric indices such as SN, SP predictive power, and area under the curve (AUC) (Conners, 1999). SN is defined as the capacity of a test to detect patients who have illness/disorder which could be thought as percentage of the cases. SP shows how well the test can distinguish between different groups (e.g., ADHD vs. non-ADHD). In addition, AUC is also important in determining the quality of the diagnostic test. AUC value between 0.90 and 1.00 is considered as above excellent, between 0.80 and 0.90 as good, between 0.70 and 0.80 as fair, between 0.60 and 0.70 as poor, and between 0.50 and 0.60 as unsuccessful (Florkowski, 2008).

Use of rating scales involve both advantages and drawbacks. One possible advantage of rating scales is that they are uncomplicated and inexpensive to administer compared to clinical and neuropsychological testing tools. They could be applied in a variety of settings. They aid in screening and diagnosis procedure, the identification and measurement of the target symptoms and behaviors, treatment outcome, the frequency and severity of ADHD symptoms (Krieger and Amador-Campos, 2018). Similarly, rating scales are quick to administer and economically convenient (Rogers et al., 2022) although, they do not provide an elaborative diagnostic value as clinical interviews do. However, one of the issues with the standardized measures is that many studies focus on assessing ADHD by only utilizing rating scales based on threshold values to diagnose patients instead of using gold-standard clinical interviews (Mulraney et al., 2021). Compared to clinical interviews, rating scales are limited in terms of the variety of statement. Due to that, limiting the diagnosis to rating scales only could lead to misdiagnosis. In some cases, rating scales are more efficient at covering symptoms or potential problems than clinical interview tools except it is often achieved in less profundity. This is because rating scales do not always include information about the onset of the disorder, the duration of its existence or the relevant factors that may be contributing to the symptoms. Additionally, respondent bias in rating scales also may be involved (Döpfner et al., 2006).

Although the psychometric properties of a rating scale may be good, it is still a limited tool compared to clinical interviews. In general, rating scales should be considered secondary and used when significant suspicion of ADHD is present due to their limited ability to identify accurately (Chamberlain et al., 2021). Nevertheless, understanding an individual’s specific behavioral and EF challenges in everyday settings identified through rating scales could help clinicians tailor interventions to address those needs directly (Krieger and Amador-Campos, 2018).

4.2.1 Specific rating scales used for child and adolescent ADHD

There are several rating scales that are frequently used in the measurement of child and adolescent ADHD. Scales such as Conners, ASEBA scales, Vanderbilt and Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham (SWAN), Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) are highly relied on especially for observing treatment response such as side effects or behavioral changes (Kemper et al., 2018). The most frequently used rating scales in children and adolescents are the following:

These rating scales are provided in Table 1 including detailed information along with a summary of their psychometric properties reported by empirical studies and systematic review and meta-analyses. Important values such as SN, SP and AUC are primarily reported. When these values were unavailable, other reliability and validity metrics were included where possible.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Commonly used rating scales in child and adolescent ADHD.

The rating scales used to diagnose and assess ADHD (see Table 1) are valuable tools for gathering information from multiple respondents (parent, teacher, self-report). The Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) shows high SN (81%) and moderate SP (70%), with AUC of 0.81, suggesting its utility in identifying ADHD in children. The Teacher Report Form (TRF) is SN (90%) but has lower SP (27%). Similarly SNAP-IV scale yields high SN rates ranging from 87% (parent report) to 90.6% (teacher report), but the SP values are considerably lower. The Vanderbilt ADHD Diagnostic Scales (VADS), shows high reliability, with high reliability values of 0.94 for total ADHD symptoms. On the other hand, limited information exists regarding Conners 4 rating scales, as this version is relatively new.

Overall, while the rating scales are valuable in clinical practice, their psychometric properties may be influenced by factors such as age, ADHD presentation, experience and training of the person administering the test, and the context in which the test is administered.

4.2.2 Specific rating scales used for adult ADHD

The following is an overview of the most commonly used rating scales for the screening of ADHD in adults (see Table 2). The adult ADHD rating scales reviewed below showed variability in psychometric utility, with the Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS-v1.1) showing the highest SN (0.90) and SP (0.88), making it an excellent screening tool (Kessler et al., 2005). The Barkley Adult ADHD Rating Scale-IV (BAARS-IV) and Conners’ Adult ADHD Rating Scales (CAARS) provide robust internal consistency and are reliable for assessing both childhood and current symptoms, although their test–retest reliability varies. Overall, these scales are valuable tools for diagnosing ADHD in adults, with high AUC and psychometric reliability, although combining them with other diagnostic methods would help obtain improved accuracy.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Commonly used rating scales in adult ADHD.

4.2.3 General scales

Beyond the tools specialized in ADHD assessment, three additional factors are worth noting: cognitive, intellectual and emotional components. The utilisation of several scales could facilitate the evaluation of these domains, where conventional tools may exhibit limitations. A wide variety of EF rating scales could also be used for assessing everyday functioning in various domains of life. Among these scales, Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Functions (BRIEF) (Gioia et al., 2000), the Behavior Assessment System for Children 3 (BASC-3) (Reynolds, 2010) could also provide valuable insight for many routes within the ADHD diagnostic and treatment planning. Furthermore, The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-V) (Weschler, 2024) Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-V) (Weschler, 2024) and Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test, Second Edition Revised (KBIT-2-R) (Kaufman and Kaufman, 2004) are often used in ADHD assessments because they evaluate a broad range of cognitive abilities, including verbal comprehension, perceptual reasoning, working memory, and processing speed. These tests do not measure ADHD directly, but they differentiate cognitive difficulties related to ADHD and other potential conditions, such as learning disabilities (LD) or intellectual impairments.

There are other scales that still could be valuable while not specifically targeting ADHD symptomology. Some scales may measure highly correlated constructs and, therefore indirectly measure ADHD. For example, the Self-vs. Externally-Regulated Behavior Scale, a recent scale (de la Fuente et al., 2022) measures self-regulation, dysregulation (such as hyper-response) and re-regulation behavior. Self-regulation, other than being related to emotional factors, is also intertwined with EF as previously indicated by Barkley (2025). In addition, Assessment System for Children and Adolescents (SENA) (Fernández-Pinto et al., 2015) addresses internalized behavioral problems and is one of the scales that can be used for a broader screening. The SENA has a multidimensional offering high reliability (0.70–0.80) that can be used in the initial diagnosis process for the age range of 3–18 years. It offers an assessment that covers areas such as emotional intelligence, awareness, competence, social integration and self-esteem.

Overall, these scales can be used to evaluate the heterogeneous nature of ADHD. And the use of general screening tests is essential for understanding the everyday functioning and identifying the cognitive impairments. This way, the clinicians could also provide a multimodal approach by screening the symptom fluctuations, identifying the functional and cognitive impairments. There is still a need for greater body of research regarding the clinical utility of the screening tools, including their SN, SP, and discriminant validity (Chang et al., 2016). The research and availability of these data is a necessary for understanding diagnostic value of the screening tools and their appropriate application in clinical practice and research.

4.3 Key neuropsychological tests and continuous performance tests

Up to this date, neuropsychological tests have been widely used to demonstrate EF deficits in ADHD. ADHD is linked to impairments in multiple cognitive domains, with notable deficits in EF such as behavioral inhibition, working memory, set-shifting, planning, and organization. These impairments may vary based on task context, with greater challenges observed during monotonous or lengthy activities compared to engaging ones.

Neuropsychological tests can help to identify specific areas of impairment in EF. A systematic review and meta-analysis has demonstrated that Children with ADHD exhibited larger delays in attention, response inhibition, planning, and working memory compared to children with tic disorders (TD) and specific learning disorders (SLD)s (Sadozai et al., 2024). However, while these measures aim to evaluate discrete EF domains, considerable overlap in the neurobiology underlying these domains challenges their functional independence. For example, the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), typically used to assess set-shifting, may also depend on working memory processes (Sadozai et al., 2024).

Assessment tools for EF in ADHD typically fall into two categories: subjective and objective measures. Subjective tools include clinical psychiatric interviews and rating scales or questionnaires, often relying on reports from the individual or informants familiar with their childhood behaviors. Objective tools, on the other hand, involve neuropsychological tests and Continuous Performance Tests (CPTs), providing standardized and quantifiable data (Posner et al., 2020). Some of the commonly employed neuropsychological tools are provided in Table 3:

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Widely Utilized Neuropsychological Tests in ADHD assessment.

These widely utilized tests tests (see Table 3) assess different aspects of cognitive functioning such as attention, memory, inhibition and cognitive flexibility. The Go/No-Go task has high SN (96.7%) and SP (83.3%) for discriminating ADHD from healthy controls. Variability in other on tests such as the Stroop or WCST is often influenced by ADHD presentation, age and other individual factors. This may limit their generalizability across different presentations of ADHD. As a result, the effectiveness of the tests in the diagnosis of ADHD may be variable and they may not always reflect the complexity of the cognitive profile of ADHD.

One important limitation is that ADHD encompasses a range of cognitive profiles, with some individuals exhibiting deficits in EF, while others show difficulties in non-executive processes like memory, temporal processing, and motivation regulation (Barkley, 2025). Conversely, although EF impairments within the clinical presentation of ADHD have been widely implicated, it is critical to recognize that there may be individual variability in the levels of cognitive performance deficits. Recent systematic review has revealed that several studies support this variability; some studies report minimal EF differences in ADHD patients compared to controls, while others emphasize significant differences which could be due to the discrepancies between methods used (self-report and objective measures) for measuring EF (Onandia-Hinchado et al., 2021). This contrast underlines the complexity of diagnosing and assessing EF impairments in ADHD, as both subjective and objective measures can provide valuable but different perspectives. Additionally, it is essential to acknowledge the heterogeneous nature of ADHD. Cognitive impairments and their neurophysiological correlates may vary with age and the presence of comorbid disorders. Some struggle in many areas, while others have very specific problems and function well in other areas (Posner et al., 2020). This also highlights the need for further research to explore how the sensitivity of neuropsychological measures such as the N-Back or Go/No-Go task may vary in different ADHD subgroups (Breitling-Ziegler et al., 2020). Notably, neuropsychological tests alone are not sufficient to diagnose ADHD. A diagnosis should be made by a qualified professional using a multifaceted approach that incorporates various sources of information (Krieger and Amador-Campos, 2021).

Continuous Performance Tests (CPTs) are one of the complementary screening methods that are involved in the diagnosis of ADHD. The purpose of the CPTs overall is to measure attention, vigilance, and inhibition. To measure these functions, there are two types of stimuli involved in the test. These are referred as target and non-target stimuli. Participants are assigned a target at the beginning of the test. This target may be a specific symbol or any stimuli and the scoring is performed by using omission and commission errors. The scoring would be done through omission and commission errors. Comission errors refer to pressing the button for a symbol other than the target and omission errors occurs when the target is missed (Sparrow, 2010). Through these values, certain functions such as attention, vigilance, inhibition and working memory could be measured (Harpin et al., 2016).

A recent systematic review and meta-analysis reviewed 74 studies using various neuropsychological measures, with most focusing on CPTs to evaluate attention, impulsivity, and response time variability. As seen in most CPT measurements, the CPTs analyzed commonly tracked variables such as omission errors (indicating inattention), commission errors (impulsivity), and reaction time variability. The SN of these tests varied widely, from 22 to 100%, with SP also ranging between 22 and 100%. The AUC values ranged from 0.59 to 0.93, showing the mixed performance of these tools (Peterson et al., 2024).

Just as any other tool, CPTs come with some limitations. One of these limitations particularly embodies validity and reliability issues. These tools, although being practical, may lack reliability due to technical factors, such as issues with the software or hardware used during administration (Roebuck-Spencer et al., 2017). Some CPTs can be financially burdening as they often require specialized equipment, and training. Additionally, CPTs measure limited abilities or only a sample of behavior under a controlled environment. And the CPT results of an individual may still not be reproducible in the same contexts due to several other uncontrollable factors such as motivation, attention or the type of existing disorder. Hence, the CPT results may not necessarily reflect the ADHD symptoms or may be influenced by other factors which are not directly related to the disorder. These factors could also introduce false positives that are either reporter or context-dependent which also indicates the importance of using appropriate normative data to ensure reliable interpretation (Rabin et al., 2014). Additionally, the application of multiple tasks may be particularly challenging for individuals with ADHD, who may have difficulty maintaining focus and remaining still for long periods of time (Breitling-Ziegler et al., 2020). Additionally, CPT’s ability to identify a wide range of deficits may be limited, as most of them are sensitive to identify specific types of functions such as attention and inhibition (Onandia-Hinchado et al., 2021). Collectively, these limitations highlight the importance of the clinical utility of these tools, as illustrated in Table 4 through SN and SN values of some of the most employed CPTs in ADHD:

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Common CPTs used in ADHD assessment: key characteristics and diagnostic accuracy.

MOXO, TOVA, QbTest, Conners CPT-3 and IVA-2 show varying psychometric utility (see Table 4) based on meta-analyses and experimental studies. In particular, the MOXO and TOVA show high SE (82 and 77%, respectively) and SP (87 and 77%), indicating that they are reliable in assessing ADHD-related symptoms such as attention, impulsivity and hyperactivity. But these findings are based on pooled data, which may not always reflect the diversity of real-world clinical settings. The QbTest provides significant utility in some subscales, however SN and SP vary across its subscales. Conners CPT-3, despite its high SP (≥90%), has a low SN (19–41%), suggesting that it may be more useful in ruling out ADHD diagnosis. The IVA-2 provides valuable insight into both visual and auditory attention, although its moderate SN and SP (75 and 68%) may limit its role in screening.

CPTs could be recommended to be used as a complementary tool, such as for monitoring treatment progress or as part of the diagnostic process alongside rating scales or previous medical and academic reports, can strengthen the evaluation process. For instance, QbTest have shown that attention measure of the QbTest is linked to overall impairment profile in ADHD and demonstrates sensitivity to the effects of ADHD medication (Emser et al., 2018). Finally, the applicability of CPTs for individuals with mental health challenges or disabilities requires further investigation to determine its effectiveness across diverse populations (Lancaster et al., 2009).

CPTs can be a useful tool for evaluating EF in ADHD (Park et al., 2019) when interpreted with caution, and the clinical utility of these tools is carefully considered. Besides, they may fail to represent the full EF profile since these tests are conducted in structured environment (Krieger and Amador-Campos, 2018). It is fundamental to emphasize that EF impairments are not exclusive to ADHD and may also occur in other disorders. While the CPT is effective in identifying cognitive differences between healthy individuals and those with ADHD, it may not be as reliable for distinguishing ADHD from other psychopathologies. While CPTs are useful for identifying cognitive deficits in ADHD, their variability suggests that they should be combined with other diagnostic methods, as they alone should not be used as a diagnostic tool (Peterson et al., 2024; Arrondo et al., 2024). CPTs could be recommended to be used as a complementary tool, such as for monitoring treatment progress or as part of the diagnostic process alongside rating scales or previous medical and academic reports, can strengthen the evaluation process. For instance, QbTest have shown that attention measure of the QbTest is linked to overall impairment profile in ADHD and demonstrates sensitivity to the effects of ADHD medication (Emser et al., 2018). Finally, the applicability of CPTs for individuals with mental health challenges or disabilities requires further investigation to determine its effectiveness across diverse populations (Lancaster et al., 2009).

In summary, both neuropsychological tests and CPTs are widely used and acknowledged in the ADHD assessment. Nevertheeless, there are important differences in ADHD presentation between children, adolescents and adults in terms of symptom expression, which requires different diagnostic measure considerations. In children, as hyperactivity is more overt and observable, with objective CPT measures like movement patterns and reaction times may be very effective for assessing hyperactivity. In contrast, hyperactivity presentation in adults implies more internal restlessness rather than a noticeable behavior. Hence, for adult assessment, rating scales may be more ideal and relevant (Emser et al., 2018). More so, ADHD encompasses a range of impairments with different intensities due to its heterogeneous nature. Some individuals may exhibit severe deficits in EF, while others show difficulties in non-executive processes like memory, temporal processing, and motivation regulation (Faraone et al., 2015). These findings not only contradict the notion that executive dysfunction is a core feature, but also may attention regarding the accuracy of tests focusing solely on cognitive measures.

5 Potential issues in ADHD diagnosis

5.1 The DSM

One of the most discussed problems with the DSM is the age of onset of ADHD. It has been widely discussed that ADHD is an early age onset disorder, but the concept of “late-onset ADHD” is also debated in the literature (Caye et al., 2017; Cooper et al., 2018). The previous versions of DSM involved statements more likely to apply to children and adolescents. However, the most recent version of DSM (DSM-5) has expanded these statements and given examples to the statements involving all age groups. For instance, in the DSM-IV, the statement “often loses things necessary for task activities” applied to toys, school- related activities or items. In DSM-5, these examples were expanded to items which also apply to adults (e.g., wallets, paperwork, keys). Conversely, a significant number of adults with ADHD do not receive a diagnosis until later in life (Johnson et al., 2020) which could be due to the age criteria in the previous versions. Nevertheless, since the DSM-5 transition, ADHD diagnoses in young adults have risen by 27% (Matte et al., 2015). The rise in the number of ADHD diagnoses may be positive indicator for improved access for clinical support. Regardless, it also raises concerns about potential “over-diagnosis.” The findings of a scoping review further reinforced this idea, showing increase in diagnoses and pharmacological treatment (Kazda et al., 2021), With the changes in DSM-5 and changing trends in ADHD, it is difficult to determine whether ADHD is over-diagnosed, overtreated or misdiagnosed. Such a condition may have serious consequences, such as potential substance abuse as a result of pharmacological treatment, economic burden on the society as well as the need for new regulations and an updated diagnostic system (Manos et al., 2017).

Considering the changes in the age-onset criteria, diagnosing ADHD in adulthood could be challenging. This is particularly due to the self-report assessment of the childhood symptoms. Adults could struggle recalling the onset of the symptoms, as this part of the assessment heavily relies on retrospective memory (Pallanti and Salerno, 2020).

Moreover, Posner et al. (2020) reported that ADHD is becoming recognized as a dimensional condition, rather than being categorically separated as mild, moderate or severe from non-ADHD individuals, as the DSM-5 states. They further discuss the absence of dimensional models and the questionable practicality of the categorical framework. Additionally, they emphasize that the DSM-5 overlooks the changes in the ADHD presentation across developmental stages. Similarly, inattention symptoms persist into adulthood while visible signs of ADHD often tend to lessen although it is unclear whether this is due to the diagnostic criteria or absolute remission of the symptoms (Faraone et al., 2021).

A recent review by Sadek (2023) emphasized another important limitation of the DSM. The criteria for ADHD in the DSM-5 do not specify that the symptoms are not “substance-induced or are attributable to the physiological effects of another medical condition,” as is written in the criteria for Major Depressive Disorder (MDD). Conditions such as sleep-disordered breathing, thyroid dysfunction, diabetes and typical absence seizures could resemble some ADHD symptoms, potentially resulting in serious consequences. Therefore, consulting a medical practitioner and conducting blood tests to rule out any, the importance of consulting a medical practitioner and running blood tests to rule out any underlying medical conditions is essential.

5.2 Impact of comorbidities

5.2.1 Comorbidity and ADHD

Adult ADHD is highly susceptible to misdiagnosis or a secondary diagnosis due to comorbidity or resemblance to other conditions. ADHD symptoms in early life may be masked due to the similarity to anxiety, mood or substance use disorders (Anbarasan et al., 2020). Results from a longitudinal study indicated that ADHD is a challenging disorder with a high rate of comorbidity (Reale et al., 2017). Most comorbid cases in ADHD include conduct disorder (CD), oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), learning disorders (LD), anxiety, mood disorders (MD), autism spectrum disorders (ASD), and tic disorders (TD) (Elwin et al., 2020).

There is a notable prevalence of co-occurring ADHD symptoms in children diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD). This comorbidity is linked to an elevated risk of amplified psychosocial difficulties. Both ASD and ADHD present commonalities in their clinical profiles, including communication difficulties, restricted behaviors, and attention problems (Harkins et al., 2022). EF deficits are recognized as transdiagnostic factors contributing to both ASD and ADHD. Inhibition emerges as a key area of difficulty, potentially linked more strongly to ADHD symptoms. Similarly, working memory deficits also may be present as one of the challenges in ASD (Luderer et al., 2021). Moreover, both disorders exhibit a higher prevalence in boys compared to girls (Coulacoglou and Saklofske, 2017).

Diagnosing ADHD and ASD together poses unique difficulties due to overlapping symptoms and limitations in standard diagnostic tools. While ADHD rating scales and structured interviews are effective for ADHD, they may not reliably distinguish ADHD symptoms in individuals with ASD (Bölte et al., 2018). To prevent these overlaps, several tools like the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ) and the Autism Mental Status Examination (AMSE) have shown better accuracy (SN: 0.83, SP: 0.90) in differentiating ADHD from ASD and their comorbid presentations. On the other hand, Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School Aged Children Lifetime Version (KSADS-PL) and behavioral observations could aid in the diagnostic process (Antshel and Russo, 2019). However, clinical judgment remains critical in interpreting these assessments, as the distinctions between ADHD and ASD symptoms are often blurred.

Conduct Disorder (CD), has been identified as one of the most prevalent comorbid conditions, with ADHD in 10–20% of cases (Sadek, 2014). The family history of ADHD and CD comorbidity increases the risk of recurrence. Similarly, at the cognitive and neuronal level, both CD and ADHD share common challenges such as inability to regulate and process emotions and poor decision-making which supports the concurrent occurrence. Additionally, ADHD and CD have similarities in negative personality traits as low self-regulation skills in individuals with ADHD lead to negative emotions such as irritability and anxiety and these negative traits are also very common in CD. Finally, CD is more likely to be reported than ADHD due to externalized symptoms (Thapar and van Goozen, 2018). However, that the risk of CD comorbidity depends on the severity of ADHD symptoms.

Alternatively, oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) is an externalized disorder is frequently reported in ADHD. The approximate prevalence of ODD comorbidity in ADHD is 60% and for CD the prevalence is around 20%. The co-occurrence of ODD and CD in ADHD is highly dependent on age and sex (Hudec and Mikami, 2017). Both CD and ODD comorbidity in childhood ADHD is very frequently reported and more common among boys (Azeredo et al., 2018). ADHD, ODD, and CD symptoms develop together from late childhood to adolescence and that there is a high likelihood of experiencing further escalation in ADHD, ODD, and CD symptoms with time (Atherton et al., 2020). Although early diagnosis of ADHD, ODD, and CD in the preschool period is difficult due to the symptom similarities of these disorders. However early diagnosis can positively change the developmental course of these disorders.

Learning disorders (LD) are also known to challenge individuals with ADHD throughout their educational and professional life. LD involves disorders such as dyslexia (reading difficulties), dyscalculia (difficulties in mathematical skills) and dysgraphia (writing difficulties). Children with LD often feel helpless and act out and are often unable to regulate their emotions. The rate of co-occurrence of LD with ADHD is very high (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2011).

ADHD is also very frequently accompanied by mood disorders such as bipolar disorder, depression, and dysthymia as well as anxiety disorders. The prevalence of depression and ADHD was reported to be at an average of 7.8% (Katzman et al., 2017). Comorbid depression occurs in ADHD in a regardless of gender and has a more negative impact on quality of life and cognitive functioning than in individuals with ADHD alone (Roy et al., 2017).

The prevalence of comorbid anxiety is significantly high in ADHD. A study reported that 55% of college students have one and 31.8% have two more comorbid disorders predominantly being anxiety and mood disorders (Anastopoulos et al., 2018). Although genetics is associated with comorbidities, the influence of environmental factors is quite serious. Psychosocial challenges experienced by individuals with ADHD at the school age may lead to the emergence of mood disorders or anxiety comorbidities. The problems caused by ADHD symptoms usually starting at school age, such as inability to concentrate in class, receiving low grades, extreme physical restlessness, breaking rules due to unrestrained impulsivity and being bullied by peers, lowers school persistence and academic success and may cause low self-esteem, depression and anxiety (Maltezos et al., 2020; Lung et al., 2019). Conversely, self-esteem is significantly lower in individuals with ADHD than in typically developing individuals. The fact that the symptoms interfere with many areas of their lives lead these individuals to be more prone to lack of self-confidence, sensitivity to rejection, social withdrawal which may aggregate the likelihood and vulnerability to developing mood and anxiety disorders (Beaton et al., 2022).

Obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) and ADHD share many similarities in terms of neuropsychological aspects. Underactive EF is observed in both disorders (Rothenberger et al., 2018). As inhibition and impulsivity problems in ADHD have many negative consequences, these key problems also account for repetitive behaviors and intrusive thoughts in OCD. It is also stated that the comorbidity of ADHD with OCD is due to genetic transmission (Brem et al., 2014).

In case of Tic Disorders (TD), the visual dominance and prominence of TD leads to the disregard or oversight of existing ADHD (Rothenberger et al., 2018). The co-occurrence of ADHD and tic disorders is one of the most challenging comorbidities. This is due to the aggravation of tics causing difficulties in social life (Bélanger et al., 2018). Often, this situation may also contribute to the development of mood and anxiety disorders (Huisman-van et al., 2019).

Moreover, individuals with ADHD have a much higher likelihood of engaging in risky behavior than normal individuals. Between 25 to 40% of ADHD cases are involved in substance misuse and besides endangering the life of the individual, substance dependence has been associated with car accidents, engagement with dangerous behavior and suicidal ideation or attempt (Luderer et al., 2021; Wilens et al., 2018). One of the major contributing factors to this is their inability to make rational judgments, which can be explained by impaired EF (Vassileva et al., 2019). Impulsivity, lack of self- inhibition as well as the impaired brain reward system show how easily they may lapse into negative habits and quickly adapt to them (Ivanov et al., 2022; Blevins et al., 2020). Therefore, smoking, alcohol, substance abuse and even addiction are very common in individuals with ADHD. In addition, addiction is frequently reported in individuals with ADHD who use psychostimulants for the purpose of treatment due to taking more than the prescribed dose of the drug (Flores-García et al., 2020).

5.2.1.1 Does comorbidity impact diagnostic accuracy?

It is crucial to acknowledge that ADHD symptoms could be coincident with those indicative of other disorders, due to the fact that numerous disorders exhibit symptoms similar to those of ADHD. And in most cases, this leads to misdiagnosis. It is therefore very important to determine how long the symptoms have been persistent. It is certainly very important to detect how long the symptoms have been present (Maltezos et al., 2020) which is usually recognized during rapport building. To ensure the accuracy of the diagnosis, the risk of comorbidity should be analyzed in line with the structured interview. Additionally, for disorders that are complex to distinguish, such as the comorbidity of anxiety and ADHD, it is useful to utilize rating scales that are reliable to provide a more valid diagnosis (Grogan et al., 2018).

In terms of misdiagnosis, anxiety, depression are usually the most common reported conditions. This is because restlessness, attention problems and forgetfulness are some of the many symptoms seen in both anxiety and depression in ADHD. Additionally, ADHD may sometimes be mistaken for bipolar disorder. However, there are distinctive clinical symptoms. For example, both disorders are characterized by mobility, distractibility, insomnia, irritability, anger outbursts, mood swings and social assertiveness. However, the distinctive features in bipolar disorder are excessive cheerfulness, self-confidence, increased sex drive, unwillingness to sleep, racing thoughts, pleasure to engage in risky activities and suicidal thoughts or attempts. However, these symptoms are not seen in ADHD. Conversely, social interaction, affective problems and speech delay are the features that are indicated in both ASD and ADHD. However, social problems, forming friendships, and cognitive development are much slower in ASD than in ADHD. In order to distinguish these, the level of dysfunction must be assessed (Bölte et al., 2018). It must not be ignored that comorbidity is a very sensitive concept in diagnosis. Although some disorders are interpreted as co-existing, misdiagnosis or under-diagnosis should not be overlooked (Ginsberg et al., 2014).

5.3 Gender bias in diagnosis

Diagnostic criteria for ADHD have traditionally been developed using predominantly male child samples. ADHD in women has been often un-recognized due to gender bias in the diagnostic procedure. Females are less likely to be referred for an assessment and they often receive misdiagnosis (Danielson et al., 2018). Notably, that women are more likely to report inattention symptoms, and less hyperactivity or impulsivity compared to boys (Antoniou et al., 2021). Males overall have a higher diagnosis ratio than females which could be due to the under-diagnosis of ADHD in females (Sadek, 2014).

Even though ADHD is believed to be more common in males, it is emerging to be highly recognized in females (Faheem et al., 2022). ADHD is usually diagnosed at an early age, with early signs in toddlers being noticeable between 2 and 3 years of age through irregular sleep patterns (Posner et al., 2020). However, at early ages, the hyperactivity symptoms tend to be more pronounced, and it is a highly predominant symptom in boys. In girls, on the other hand, internalized symptoms; more inwardness, shyness, and inattention are observed (Attoe and Climie, 2023).

Because of the expression of the symptoms, it is more often observed that boys acquire ADHD diagnosis at a young age, much earlier than girls. Regarding older ages, especially in women, non- diagnosed ADHD is generally identified and treated as anxiety or depression (Hinshaw et al., 2022). Consequently, most women are diagnosed with ADHD at a later age (Fairman et al., 2020) and they are generally misdiagnosed, which may also be due to gender bias arising from the traditional nature of ADHD. In addition, as women with undiagnosed ADHD are more likely to experience heightened hormonal shifts that result in higher levels of irritability, emotional dysregulation, mood swings and poor concentration, they may be more likely to receive another diagnosis (Attoe and Climie, 2023). While neurocognitive profiles of adults with ADHD have been studied, gender differences in these cognitive patterns are often overlooked, despite biological and hormonal factors suggesting potential disparities between males and females. Thus, both men and women may be misdiagnosed due to differences in ADHD presentation and gender-related differences.

Recognizing the potential differences in the presentation of ADHD due to gender differences could help reduce the gender gap, however there is a need for improved diagnostic tools (Slobodin and Davidovitch, 2019). Among other tools, DIVA-5 is currently exploring new methods to improve the identification of ADHD symptom presentations in women. This includes investigating symptom differences between men and women and hormonal factors to improve the diagnostic accuracy for women.

5.4 Differential diagnosis

ADHD should not be diagnosed if symptoms are solely present during psychotic episodes or could be explained by another mental disorder. Individuals with autism or intellectual disabilities may be diagnosed with ADHD if their symptoms exceed the expected severity for their developmental level. Clinicians must distinguish ADHD from other mental disorders, especially those that share overlapping symptoms, by noting the persistence of inattentive or hyperactive–impulsive symptoms outside of discrete episodes of mood or anxiety issues. When both sets of diagnostic criteria are met, comorbidity should be considered (Hall et al., 2016).

As noted previously, some medical conditions like thyroiditis and diabetes may mimic ADHD symptoms (Sadek, 2023). Evidence-based clinical guidelines also suggest that the use of certain drugs or substances can be a leading factor in the appearance of ADHD-like symptoms and should also be addressed in the diagnostic process (May et al., 2023). Referral for a medical assessment (general examination, blood tests, brain imaging) and taking a medical history may also help to assess all of these factors.

In case of co-existing conditions, it is essential to undertake a thorough evaluation of both overlapping and distinguishing symptoms. To differentiate ADHD from other potential disorders, specific tests can be used in addition to interviews. For example, the Woodcock-Johnson IV (WJ IV), which measures achievement, cognitive abilities, and oral language, is an ideal scale for identifying potential learning disabilities (LD) (Schrank and McGrew, 2015). Similarly, for mood and anxiety disorders Child Depression Inventory (CDI) (Kovacs, 2015) and Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale, Second Edition (RCMAS-2) (Reynolds et al., 2008) could be utilized for detecting whether symptoms only mimic each other or there is comorbidity. Conversely, neuropsychological testing should also be considered for mapping out the impairments more accurately.

The functionality of the chosen tools for the assessment is also very important. First, in child and adolescent assessment, informant-based measures (parents and teachers) of EF, typically indicate greater delays in EF compared to performance-based measures. This discrepancy may arise because informant-based measures capture a child’s challenges in real-life contexts, such as school or home, where everyday demands refer to the difficulties in managing attention, impulsivity, and task completion. However, performance-based measures focus on isolated cognitive tasks that may not fully reflect the overall profile. However, using both measures in the diagnostic process to gain a comprehensive understanding of the child’s EF abilities (Sadozai et al., 2024).

Accurate diagnosis of ADHD requires a methodical approach, involving multiple steps that not only aid in identifying ADHD but also distinguish it from conditions with overlapping symptoms. Sibley (2021) outlines seven steps for accurately diagnosing adult ADHD. First step involves a structured diagnostic interview followed by the second step: self-report ratings and informant ratings. In this step, an utmost importance to informant ratings should be given as patients could over-report or under-report symptoms. To address this, the third step applies the “or rule,” which ensures that the symptom reported by the patient also needs to be endorsed by the informant to confirm its presence. The fourth step focuses on the areas of impairment and analyzing any existing evidence to support the impairment (e.g., academic and medical records, legal documents etc.) and the duration of these symptoms are assessed in the fifth step where the age-onset, the symptom timeline and potential stressors are further evaluated. In the sixth step, differential diagnoses are explored to rule out any alternative mental or medical conditions which may resemble ADHD symptoms. And finally, the last step allows for the establishment of a diagnosis. Following these diagnostic steps and addressing the differential diagnosis process, ADHD could be correctly identified, which would facilitate effective treatment and management.

6 Conclusion

In conclusion, diagnosing ADHD is a challenging and multifaceted process that requires gathering information from diverse sources, including standardized assessments and clinical observations, while considering the limitations of these tools as well as the diagnostic guidelines such as the DSM. Factors such as cognitive differences, age, gender, and comorbid conditions must also be accounted for to reduce potential biases in diagnosis.

A comprehensive, multimodal approach that combines subjective measures with objective tools is essential to capture the complexity of ADHD and minimize diagnostic errors. Such an approach not only improves the understanding of individual cases but also addresses concerns about over-diagnosis and under-diagnosis, which can arise from overlapping symptoms with other conditions. Utilizing assessments for EF screening, comorbidities, and functional impairments further enhances the accuracy and utility of the diagnostic process. The debate over the ecological validity and objective reliability of the tools used in ADHD assessment measures persists; while some advocate for performance-based assessments as more objective evaluations of EF, others highlight the value of informant-based measures in predicting real-world functional outcomes (Sadozai et al., 2024).

While this study has not focused on the gender-specific issues or neuroimaging, future research should discover the role and of other neuroimaging techniques such as fNIRS (Poliakova et al., 2023) and contribute to the area of gender-specific presentations of ADHD Additionally, thorough research into the psychometric properties of existing tools is needed to reduce financial burden caused by these tools and ensure reliable, evidence-based practices in ADHD diagnosis.

Finally, advancing our understanding of ADHD and improving diagnostic practices will not only enhance clinical outcomes but also support more targeted and effective interventions for individuals affected by this complex condition.

Author contributions

HM: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Acknowledgments

The author declares that this article has been reviewed and edited using Generative AI DeepL translate & Write to enhance spelling, grammar, and language clarity, ensuring it reflects the fluency of a native speaker.

Conflict of interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

References

Achenbach, T. M. Manual for the teacher’s report form and 1991 profile. (1991a). Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont, Burlington.

Google Scholar

Achenbach, T. M. Manual for the youth self-report and 1991 profile. (1991b). Burlington, VT: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry.

Google Scholar

Achenbach, T. M., and Rescorla, L. A. (2014). “The achenbach system of empirically based assessment (ASEBA) for ages 1.5 to 18 years” in The use of psychological testing for treatment planning and outcomes assessment. ed. M. E. Maruish (Routledge: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers), 179–213.

Google Scholar

American Psychiatic Association (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual. Washington, DC: APA Press.

Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (1968). DSM-II: diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 3rd ed., revised Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th Edn, Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (Ed.) (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th Edn: American Psychiatric Association Publishing.

Google Scholar

Anastopoulos, A. D., DuPaul, G. J., Weyandt, L. L., Morrissey-Kane, E., Sommer, J., Rhoads, L., et al. (2018). Rates and patterns of comorbidity among first-year college students with ADHD. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 47, 236–247. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2015.1105137

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Anbarasan, D., Kitchin, M., and Adler, L. A. (2020). Screening for adult ADHD. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 22:72. doi: 10.1007/s11920-020-01194-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, N. P., Feldman, J. A., Kolko, D. J., Pilkonis, P. A., and Lindhiem, O. (2022). National Norms for the Vanderbilt ADHD diagnostic parent rating scale in children. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 47, 652–661. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsab132

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Antoniou, E., Rigas, N., Orovou, E., Papatrechas, A., and Sarella, A. (2021). ADHD symptoms in females of childhood, adolescent, reproductive and menopause period. Mater Sociomed 33:114. doi: 10.5455/msm.2021.33.114-118

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Antshel, K. M., and Russo, N. (2019). Autism spectrum disorders and ADHD: overlapping phenomenology, diagnostic issues, and treatment considerations. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 21, 1–11. doi: 10.1007/s11920-019-1020-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Arrondo, G., Mulraney, M., Iturmendi-Sabater, I., Musullulu, H., Gambra, L., Niculcea, T., et al. (2024). Systematic review and Meta-analysis: clinical utility of continuous performance tests for the identification of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 63, 154–171. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2023.03.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Asherson, P., Buitelaar, J., Faraone, S. V., and Rohde, L. (2016). Adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: key conceptual issues. Lancet Psychiatry 3, 568–578. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(16)30032-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Atherton, O. E., Lawson, K. M., Ferrer, E., and Robins, R. W. (2020). The role of effortful control in the development of ADHD, ODD, and CD symptoms. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 118, 1226–1246. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000243

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Attoe, D. E., and Climie, E. A. (2023). Miss. Diagnosis: a systematic review of ADHD in adult women. J. Atten. Disord. 27, 645–657. doi: 10.1177/10870547231161533

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ayano, G., Demelash, S., Gizachew, Y., Tsegay, L., and Alati, R. (2023). The global prevalence of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents: an umbrella review of meta-analyses. J. Affect. Disord. 339, 860–866. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2023.07.071

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Azeredo, A., Moreira, D., and Barbosa, F. (2018). ADHD, CD, and ODD: systematic review of genetic and environmental risk factors. Res. Dev. Disabil. 82, 10–19. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2017.12.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol. Bull. 121, 65–94. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barkley, R. A. Barkley Adult ADHD rating scale-IV (BAARS-IV). The Guildford Press. Available at: https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2011-05875-000 (2011) (Accessed December 5, 2024).

Google Scholar

Barkley, R. A. (2011). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a handbook for diagnosis and treatment. 4th Edn. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Barkley, R. A. The important role of executive functioning and self-regulation in ADHD©. J Child Neuropsy. (2025). 113, 41–56.

Google Scholar

Bates, M. E., and Lemay, E. P. (2004). The d2 test of attention: construct validity and extensions in scoring techniques. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 10, 392–400. doi: 10.1017/S135561770410307X

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaton, D. M., Sirois, F., and Milne, E. (2022). Experiences of criticism in adults with ADHD: a qualitative study. PLoS One 17:e0263366. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0263366

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bélanger, S. A., Andrews, D., Gray, C., and Korczak, D. (2018). ADHD in children and youth: part 1—etiology, diagnosis, and comorbidity. Paediatr. Child Health 23, 447–453. doi: 10.1093/pch/pxy109

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bellato, A., Hall, C. L., Groom, M. J., Simonoff, E., Thapar, A., Hollis, C., et al. (2024). Practitioner review: clinical utility of the QbTest for the assessment and diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder – a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 65, 845–861. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13901

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bijlenga, D., Vollebregt, M. A., Kooij, J. J. S., and Arns, M. (2019). The role of the circadian system in the etiology and pathophysiology of ADHD: time to redefine ADHD? ADHD Attent. Def. Hyperact. Disord. 11, 5–19. doi: 10.1007/s12402-018-0271-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blevins, D., Choi, C. J., Pavlicova, M., Martinez, D., Mariani, J. J., Grabowski, J., et al. (2020). Impulsiveness as a moderator of amphetamine treatment response for cocaine use disorder among ADHD patients. Drug Alcohol Depend. 213:108082. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2020.108082

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bölte, S., Poustka, L., and Geurts, H. M. (2018). “Autism spectrum disorder” in Case studies in clinical psychological science: bridging the gap from science to practice, 1–7.

Google Scholar

Braver, T. S., Cohen, J. D., Nystrom, L. E., Jonides, J., Smith, E. E., and Noll, D. C. (1996). A parametric study of prefrontal cortex involvement in human working memory. Neuroimage 5, 49–62. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1996.0247

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Breitling-Ziegler, C., Tegelbeckers, J., Flechtner, H. H., and Krauel, K. (2020). Economical assessment of working memory and response inhibition in ADHD using a combined n-back/Nogo paradigm: an ERP study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:551118. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00322

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brem, S., Grünblatt, E., Drechsler, R., Riederer, P., and Walitza, S. (2014). The neurobiological link between OCD and ADHD. ADHD Attenti. Def. Hyperact. Disord. 6, 175–202. doi: 10.1007/s12402-014-0146-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brevik, E. J., Lundervold, A. J., Haavik, J., and Posserud, M. B. (2020). Validity and accuracy of the adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) self-report scale (ASRS) and the Wender Utah rating scale (WURS) symptom checklists in discriminating between adults with and without ADHD. Brain Behav. 10:e01605. doi: 10.1002/BRB3.1605

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, T. E. (2008). ADD/ADHD and impaired executive function in clinical practice. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 10, 407–411. doi: 10.1007/s11920-008-0065-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bussing, R., Fernandez, M., Harwood, M., Wei Hou,, Garvan, C. W., Eyberg, S. M., et al. (2008). Parent and teacher SNAP-IV ratings of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms: psychometric properties and normative ratings from a school district sample. Assessment 15, 317–328. doi: 10.1177/1073191107313888

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Callan, P. D., Swanberg, S., Weber, S. K., Eidnes, K., Pope, T. M., and Shepler, D. (2024). Diagnostic utility of Conners continuous performance Test-3 for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review. J. Atten. Disord. 28, 992–1007. doi: 10.1177/10870547231223727

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cation, B., and Schoenberg, M. (2024). B - 135 Wisconsin card sorting test 64-card computer version (WCST-64-CV) failure to maintain set (FMS) as an embedded performance validity Indicator in a mixed clinical sample. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 39:1238. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acae067.296

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Caye, A., Sibley, M. H., Swanson, J. M., and Rohde, L. A. (2017). Late-onset ADHD: understanding the evidence and building theoretical frameworks. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 19, 1–10. doi: 10.1007/s11920-017-0858-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chamberlain, S. R., Cortese, S., and Grant, J. E. (2021). Screening for adult ADHD using brief rating tools: what can we conclude from a positive screen? Some caveats. Compr. Psychiatry 106:152224. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2021.152224

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, L. Y., Wang, M. Y., and Tsai, P. S. (2016). Diagnostic accuracy of rating scales for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics 137:e20152749. doi: 10.1542/PEDS.2015-2749/81432

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Conners, C. K. (1999). Clinical use of rating scales in diagnosis and treatment of attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatr. Clin. N. Am. 46, 857–870. doi: 10.1016/S0031-3955(05)70159-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Conners, C. K. (2024). Conners 4 – Conners. 4th Edn: Multi-Health Systems Inc. (MHS).

Google Scholar

Cooper, M., Hammerton, G., Collishaw, S., Langley, K., Thapar, A., Dalsgaard, S., et al. (2018). Investigating late-onset ADHD: a population cohort investigation. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 59, 1105–1113. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12911

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Coulacoglou, C., and Saklofske, D. H. (2017). Executive function, theory of mind, and adaptive behavior. Psychome. Psychol. Assess., 91–130. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-802219-1.00005-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Crisci, G., Caviola, S., Cardillo, R., and Mammarella, I. C. (2021). Executive functions in neurodevelopmental disorders: comorbidity overlaps between attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder and specific learning disorders. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:35. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.594234

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cuffe, S. P., Visser, S. N., Holbrook, J. R., Danielson, M. L., Geryk, L. L., Wolraich, M. L., et al. (2020). ADHD and psychiatric comorbidity: functional outcomes in a school-based sample of children. J. Atten. Disord. 24, 1345–1354. doi: 10.1177/1087054715613437

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Conners, C., Sitarenios, G., and Ayearst, L. E. (2018). Conners’ continuous performance test third edition. Encyclop. Clin. Neuropsychol., 929–933. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-57111-9_1535

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Danielson, M. L., Bitsko, R. H., Ghandour, R. M., Holbrook, J. R., Kogan, M. D., and Blumberg, S. J. (2018). Prevalence of parent-reported ADHD diagnosis and associated treatment among U.S. children and adolescents, 2016. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 47, 199–212. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2017.1417860

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Pachón-Basallo, M., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., Peralta-Sánchez, F. J., Garzón-Umerenkova, A., and Sander, P. (2022). Self- vs. external-regulation behavior ScaleTM in different psychological contexts: a validation study. Front. Psychol. 13:6480. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.922633e

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Döpfner, M., Steinhausen, H.-C., Coghill, D., Dalsgaard, S., Poole, L., Ralston, S. J., et al. (2006). Cross-cultural reliability and validity of ADHD assessed by the ADHD rating scale in a pan-European study. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 15, i46–i55. doi: 10.1007/s00787-006-1007-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eccleston, L., Williams, J., Knowles, S., and Soulsby, L. (2019). Adolescent experiences of living with a diagnosis of ADHD: a systematic review and thematic synthesis. Emot. Behav. Diffic. 24, 119–135. doi: 10.1080/13632752.2019.1582762

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Elosúa, M. R., del Olmo, S., and Contreras, M. J. (2017). Differences in executive functioning in children with attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Front. Psychol. 8:976. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00976

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Elwin, M., Elvin, T., and Larsson, J. O. (2020). Symptoms and level of functioning related to comorbidity in children and adolescents with ADHD: a cross-sectional registry study. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry Ment. Health 14:30. doi: 10.1186/s13034-020-00336-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Emser, T. S., Johnston, B. A., Steele, J. D., Kooij, S., Thorell, L., and Christiansen, H. (2018). Assessing ADHD symptoms in children and adults: evaluating the role of objective measures. Behav. Brain Funct. 14:11. doi: 10.1186/s12993-018-0143-x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Epstein, J. N., and Kollins, S. H. (2006). Psychometric properties of an adult ADHD diagnostic interview. J. Atten. Disord. 9, 504–514. doi: 10.1177/1087054705283575

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Epstein, J. N., and Loren, R. E. A. (2013). Changes in the definition of ADHD in DSM-5: subtle but important. Neuropsychiatry 3, 455–458. doi: 10.2217/npy.13.59

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Faheem, M., Akram, W., Akram, H., Khan, M. A., Siddiqui, F. A., and Majeed, I. (2022). Gender-based differences in prevalence and effects of ADHD in adults: a systematic review. Asian J. Psychiatr. 75:103205. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2022.103205

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fairman, K. A., Peckham, A. M., and Sclar, D. A. (2020). Diagnosis and treatment of ADHD in the United States: update by gender and race. J. Atten. Disord. 24, 10–19. doi: 10.1177/1087054716688534

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Faraone, S. V., Asherson, P., Banaschewski, T., Biederman, J., Buitelaar, J. K., Ramos-Quiroga, J. A., et al. (2015). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nat. Rev. Dis. Primers 1:15020. doi: 10.1038/nrdp.2015.20

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Faraone, S. V., Banaschewski, T., Coghill, D., Zheng, Y., Biederman, J., Bellgrove, M. A., et al. (2021). The world federation of ADHD international consensus statement: 208 evidence-based conclusions about the disorder. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 128, 789–818. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.01.022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández-Pinto, I., Santamaría, P., Sánchez-Sánczhez, F., Carrasco, M. A., and del Barrio, V. Sistema de Evaluación de Niños y Adolescentes: manual técnico: Madrid: TEA Ediciones (2015).

Google Scholar

Flores-García, L., Ytterstad, E., Lensing, M. B., and Eisemann, M. (2020). Exploring personality and readiness to change in patients with substance use disorders with and without ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 24, 1255–1265. doi: 10.1177/1087054716677819

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Florkowski, C. M. (2008). Sensitivity, specificity, receiver-operating characteristic (ROC) curves and likelihood ratios: communicating the performance of diagnostic tests. Clin. Biochem. Rev. 29 Suppl 1, S83–S87

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Fosco, W. D., Kofler, M. J., Groves, N. B., Chan, E. S. M., and Raiker, J. S. Jr. (2020). Which ‘working’ components of working memory aren’t working in youth with ADHD? J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 48, 647–660. doi: 10.1007/s10802-020-00621-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

French, B., Perez Vallejos, E., Sayal, K., and Daley, D. (2020). Awareness of ADHD in primary care: stakeholder perspectives. BMC Fam. Pract. 21, 1–13. doi: 10.1186/s12875-020-01112-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Geddes, J. R., and Andreasen, N. C. (2020). New Oxford textbook of psychiatry. 3rd Edn. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Gentile, J. P., Atiq, R., and Gillig, P. M. (2006). Adult ADHD: diagnosis, differential diagnosis, and medication management. Psychiatry (Edgmont) 3, 25–30

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Gift, T. E., Reimherr, M. L., Marchant, B. K., Steans, T. A., and Reimherr, F. W. (2021). Wender Utah rating scale: psychometrics, clinical utility and implications regarding the elements of ADHD. J. Psychiatr. Res. 135, 181–188. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.01.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ginsberg, Y., Quintero, J., Anand, E., Casillas, M., and Upadhyaya, H. P. (2014). Underdiagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adult patients: a review of the literature. Prim Care Companion CNS Disord 16:23591. doi: 10.4088/PCC.13r01600

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C., and Kenworthy, L. (2000). Behavior rating inventory of executive function. Child Neuropsychol. 6, 235–238. doi: 10.1076/chin.6.3.235.3152

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodman, R. (1997). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a research note. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 38, 581–586. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grant, D. A., and Berg, E. A. (1948). A behavioral analysis of degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigl-type card-sorting problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 404–411 doi: 10.1037/h0059831

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenberg, L. M., and Waldmant, I. D. (1993). Developmental normative data on the test of variables of attention (T.O.V.A.™). J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 34, 1019–1030. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1993.tb01105.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grogan, K., Gormley, C. I., Rooney, B., Whelan, R., Kiiski, H., Naughton, M., et al. (2018). Differential diagnosis and comorbidity of ADHD and anxiety in adults. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 57, 99–115. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12156

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, C. L., Guo, B., Valentine, A. Z., Groom, M. J., Daley, D., Sayal, K., et al. (2020). The validity of the SNAP-IV in children displaying ADHD symptoms. Assessment 27, 1258–1271. doi: 10.1177/1073191119842255

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, C. L., Valentine, A. Z., Groom, M. J., Walker, G. M., Sayal, K., Daley, D., et al. (2016). The clinical utility of the continuous performance test and objective measures of activity for diagnosing and monitoring ADHD in children: a systematic review. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 25, 677–699. doi: 10.1007/s00787-015-0798-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harkins, C. M., Handen, B. L., and Mazurek, M. O. (2022). The impact of the comorbidity of ASD and ADHD on social impairment. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 52, 2512–2522. doi: 10.1007/s10803-021-05150-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harpin, V., Mazzone, L., Raynaud, J. P., Kahle, J., and Hodgkins, P. (2016). Long-term outcomes of ADHD: a systematic review of self-esteem and social function. J. Atten. Disord. 20, 295–305. doi: 10.1177/1087054713486516

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hinshaw, S. P., Nguyen, P. T., O’Grady, S. M., and Rosenthal, E. A. (2022). Annual research review: attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in girls and women: underrepresentation, longitudinal processes, and key directions. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 63, 484–496. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13480

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hong, M., Kooij, J. J. S., Kim, B., Joung, Y. S., Yoo, H. K., Kim, E. J., et al. (2020). Validity of the Korean version of DIVA-5: a semi-structured diagnostic interview for adult ADHD. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 16, 2371–2376. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S262995

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hudec, K. L., and Mikami, A. Y. (2017). “Diagnostic issues for ODD/CD with ADHD comorbidity” in The Wiley handbook of disruptive and impulse-control disorders, 55–71.

Google Scholar

Huisman-van, H., Matthijssen, S. J. M. A., Stockmann, R. T. S., Fritz, A., and Cath, D. (2019). Effects of comorbidity on Tourette’s tic severity and quality of life. Acta Neurol. Scand. 140, 390–398. doi: 10.1111/ane.13155

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ivanov, I., Bjork, J. M., Blair, J., and Newcorn, J. H. (2022). Sensitization-based risk for substance abuse in vulnerable individuals with ADHD: review and re-examination of evidence. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 135:104575. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104575

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jarrett, M. A., Meter, A., Youngstrom, E. A., Hilton, D., and Ollendick, T. (2018). Evidence-based assessment of ADHD in youth using a receiver operating characteristic approach. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 47, 808–820. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2016.1225502

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, J., Morris, S., and George, S. Misdiagnosis and missed diagnosis of adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. BJPsych Advances. (2020). 27, 60–61. doi: 10.1192/bja.2020.34

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, S. L., Tharp, J. A., Peckham, A. D., Carver, C. S., and Haase, C. M. (2017). A path model of different forms of impulsivity with externalizing and internalizing psychopathology: toward greater specificity. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 56, 235–252. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12135

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, G., and Macken, B. (2015). Questioning short-term memory and its measurement: why digit span measures long-term associative learning. Cognition 144, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.07.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kakubo, S. M., Mendez, M., Silveira, J. D., Maringolo, L., Nitta, C., Silveira, D. X., et al. (2018). Translation and validation of the Brown attention-deficit disorder scale for use in Brazil: identifying cases of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder among samples of substance users and non-users. Cross-cultural validation study. Sao Paulo Med. J. 136, 157–164. doi: 10.1590/1516-3180.2017.0227121217

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanarik, M., Grimm, O., Mota, N. R., Reif, A., and Harro, J. (2022). ADHD co-morbidities: a review of implication of gene × environment effects with dopamine-related genes. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 139:104757. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104757

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Katzman, M. A., Bilkey, T. S., Chokka, P. R., Fallu, A., and Klassen, L. J. (2017). Adult ADHD and comorbid disorders: clinical implications of a dimensional approach. BMC Psychiatry 17, 1–15. doi: 10.1186/s12888-017-1463-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaufman, A. S., and Kaufman, N. L. (2004). Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test, Second Edition (KBIT-2) [Database record]. APA PsycTests.

Google Scholar

Kazda, L., Bell, K., Thomas, R., McGeechan, K., Sims, R., and Barratt, A. (2021). Overdiagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents: a systematic scoping review. JAMA Netw. Open 4:e215335. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2021.5335

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemper, AR, Maslow, GR, Hill, S, Namdari, B, LaPointe, NMA, Goode, AP, et al. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: diagnosis and treatment in children and adolescents. (2018).

Google Scholar

Kendler, K. S. A history of the DSM-5 scientific review committee. Psychol. Med. 43, 1793–1800. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713001578

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kercood, S., Lineweaver, T. T., Frank, C. C., and Fromm, E. D. (2017). Cognitive flexibility and its relationship to academic achievement and career choice of college students with and without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J. Postsecond. Educ. Disabil. 30, 329–344.

Google Scholar

Kessler, R., Adler, L., Ames, M., Demler, O., Faraone, S., Hiripi, E., et al. (2005). The World Health Organization adult ADHD self-report scale (ASRS): a short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychol. Med. 35, 245–256. doi: 10.1017/S0033291704002892

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kofler, M. J., Sarver, D. E., Harmon, S. L., Moltisanti, A., Aduen, P. A., Soto, E. F., et al. (2018). Working memory and organizational skills problems in ADHD. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 59, 57–67. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12773

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kooij, J. J. S. (2022). Adult ADHD: Diagnostic assessment and treatment. Cham: Springer International Publishing.

Google Scholar

Kooij, J. J. S., and Francken, M. H. DIVA: Diagnostisch Interview voor ADHD bij volwassenen (printversie) Kenniscentrum ADHD bij volwassenen, PsyQ. (2010).

Google Scholar

Kooij, J. J. S., Michielsen, M., Kruithof, H., and Bijlenga, D. (2016). ADHD in old age: a review of the literature and proposal for assessment and treatment. Expert. Rev. Neurother. 16, 1371–1381. doi: 10.1080/14737175.2016.1204914

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kovacs, M. Children’s depression inventory. PsycTESTS dataset. (2015).

Google Scholar

Krieger, V., and Amador-Campos, J. A. (2018). Assessment of executive function in ADHD adolescents: contribution of performance tests and rating scales. Child Neuropsychol. 24, 1063–1087. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2017.1386781

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Krieger, V., and Amador-Campos, J. A. (2021). Clinical presentations of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in children and adolescents: comparison of neurocognitive performance. Child Neuropsychol. 27, 1024–1053. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2021.1917530

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lancaster, P. E., Schumaker, J. B., Lancaster, S. J. C., and Deshler, D. D. (2009). Effects of a computerized program on use of the test-taking strategy by secondary students with disabilities. Learn. Disabil. Q. 32, 165–179. doi: 10.2307/27740366

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leffa, D. T., Caye, A., and Rohde, L. A. (2022). ADHD in children and adults: diagnosis and prognosis. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci., 1–18. doi: 10.1007/7854_2022_329

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, F., Zheng, Y., Smith, S. D., Shic, F., Moore, C. C., Zheng, X., et al. (2016). A preliminary study of movement intensity during a go/no-go task and its association with ADHD outcomes and symptom severity. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry Ment. Health 10, 1–10. doi: 10.1186/s13034-016-0135-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Luderer, M., Ramos Quiroga, J. A., Faraone, S. V., Zhang-James, Y., and Reif, A. (2021). Alcohol use disorders and ADHD. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 128, 648–660. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.07.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lung, F. W., Shu, B. C., Chiang, T. L., and Lin, S. J. (2019). Prevalence of bullying and perceived happiness in adolescents with learning disability, intellectual disability, ADHD, and autism spectrum disorder: in the Taiwan birth cohort pilot study. Medicine 98:e14483. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000014483

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Logan, G. D., Cowan, W. B., and Davis, K. A. (1984). On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. Journal of experimental psychology. Hum. Percept. Perform. 10, 276–291. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.10.2.276

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahone, E. M., and Denckla, M. B. (2017). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a historical neuropsychological perspective. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 23, 916–929. doi: 10.1017/S1355617717000807

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Maltezos, S., Whitwell, S., and Asherson, P. (2020). ADHD in adults. Oxford Textb. Neuropsychiatr., 411–424. doi: 10.1093/med/9780198757139.003.0034

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Manos, M. J., Giuliano, K., and Geyer, E. (2017). ADHD: overdiagnosed and overtreated, or misdiagnosed and mistreated? Cleve. Clin. J. Med. 84, 873–880. doi: 10.3949/ccjm.84a.15051

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marshall, P., Hoelzle, J., and Nikolas, M. (2021). Diagnosing attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in young adults: a qualitative review of the utility of assessment measures and recommendations for improving the diagnostic process. Clin. Neuropsychol. 35, 165–198. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2019.1696409

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Matte, B., Anselmi, L., Salum, G. A., Kieling, C., Gonçalves, H., Menezes, A., et al. (2015). ADHD in DSM-5: a field trial in a large, representative sample of 18- to 19-year-old adults. Psychol. Med. 45, 361–373. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714001470

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

May, T., Birch, E., Chaves, K., Cranswick, N., Culnane, E., Delaney, J., et al. (2023). The Australian evidence-based clinical practice guideline for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 57, 1101–1116. doi: 10.1177/00048674231166329

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McGough, J. (2014). ADHD : Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Moffitt, T. E., Houts, R., Asherson, P., Belsky, D. W., Corcoran, D. L., Hammerle, M., et al. (2015). Is adult ADHD a childhood-onset neurodevelopmental disorder? Evidence from a four-decade longitudinal cohort study. Am. J. Psychiatry 172, 967–977. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.14101266

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Monden, Y., Dan, I., Nagashima, M., Dan, H., Uga, M., Ikeda, T., et al. (2015). Individual classification of ADHD children by right prefrontal hemodynamic responses during a go/no-go task as assessed by fNIRS. NeuroImage Clinical, 9, 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.06.011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mulraney, M., Arrondo, G., Musullulu, H., Iturmendi-Sabater, I., Cortese, S., Westwood, S. J., et al. (2021). Systematic review and meta-analysis: screening tools for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 61, 982–996. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2021.11.031

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

NICE. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Diagnosis and management NICE guideline. (2018). Available at: www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ng87 (Accessed August 4, 2022).

Google Scholar

Oberauer, K. (2019). Working memory and attention – a conceptual analysis and review. J. Cogn. 2:36. doi: 10.5334/joc.58

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Onandia-Hinchado, I., Pardo-Palenzuela, N., and Diaz-Orueta, U. (2021). Cognitive characterization of adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder by domains: a systematic review. J. Neural Transm. 128, 893–937. doi: 10.1007/s00702-021-02302-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pallanti, S., and Salerno, L. (2020). The burden of adult ADHD in comorbid psychiatric and neurological disorders. Cham: Springer International Publishing.

Google Scholar

Park, J., Kim, C., Ahn, J. H., Joo, Y., Shin, M. S., Lee, H. J., et al. (2019). Clinical use of continuous performance tests to diagnose children with ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 23, 531–540. doi: 10.1177/1087054716658125

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, B. S., Trampush, J., Brown, M., Maglione, M., Bolshakova, M., Rozelle, M., et al. (2024). Tools for the diagnosis of ADHD in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Pediatrics 153:e2024065854. doi: 10.1542/peds.2024-065854

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pettersson, R., Söderström, S., and Nilsson, K. W. (2018). Diagnosing ADHD in adults: an examination of the discriminative validity of neuropsychological tests and diagnostic assessment instruments. J. Atten. Disord. 22, 1019–1031. doi: 10.1177/1087054715618788

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pineda-Alhucema, W., Aristizabal, E., Escudero-Cabarcas, J., Acosta-López, J. E., and Vélez, J. I. (2018). Executive function and theory of mind in children with ADHD: a systematic review. Neuropsychol. Rev. 28, 341–358. doi: 10.1007/s11065-018-9381-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Polgar, J. M., Reg, O. T., and Barlow, I. (2002). “Measuring the clinical utility of an assessment: the example of the Canadian occupational performance measure” in The example of the Canadian occupational performance measure, 114–120.

Google Scholar

Poliakova, E., Conrad, A. L., Schieltz, K. M., and O'Brien, M. J. (2023). Using fNIRS to evaluate ADHD medication effects on neuronal activity: a systematic literature review. Front. Neuroimaging 2:1083036. doi: 10.3389/fnimg.2023.1083036

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Posner, J., Polanczyk, G. V., and Sonuga-Barke, E. (2020). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet 395, 450–462. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(19)33004-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pozzi, M., Carnovale, C., Peeters, G. G. A. M., Gentili, M., Antoniazzi, S., Radice, S., et al. (2018). Adverse drug events related to mood and emotion in paediatric patients treated for ADHD: a meta-analysis. J. Affect. Disord. 238, 161–178. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.05.021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabin, L. A., Spadaccini, A. T., Brodale, D. L., Grant, K. S., Elbulok-Charcape, M. M., and Barr, W. B. (2014). Utilization rates of computerized tests and test batteries among clinical neuropsychologists in the United States and Canada. Prof. Psychol. 45, 368–377. doi: 10.1037/a0037987

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramos, A. A., Hamdan, A. C., and Machado, L. (2020). A meta-analysis on verbal working memory in children and adolescents with ADHD. Clin. Neuropsychol. 34, 873–898. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2019.1604998

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramos-Quiroga, J. A., Nasillo, V., Richarte, V., Corrales, M., Palma, F., Ibáñez, P., et al. (2019). Criteria and concurrent validity of DIVA 2.0: a semi-structured diagnostic interview for adult ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 23, 1126–1135. doi: 10.1177/1087054716646451

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reale, L., Bartoli, B., Cartabia, M., Costantino, M. A., Canevini, M. P., Termine, C., et al. (2017). Comorbidity prevalence and treatment outcome in children and adolescents with ADHD. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 26, 1443–1457. doi: 10.1007/s00787-017-1005-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reitan, R. M. (1955). The relation of the trail making test to organic brain damage. J. Consult. Psychol. 19, 393–394. doi: 10.1037/h0044509

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reynolds, C. R. (2010). Behavior assessment system for children. Corsini Encyclop. Psychol., 1–2. doi: 10.1002/9780470479216.corpsy0114

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reynolds, C. R., Richmond, B. O., Sella, F., Reynolds, C. R., Richmond, B. O., Sella, F., et al. (2008). RCMAS-2: Revised children’s manifest anxiety scale 2. edition: manuale. Los Angeles, CA: OS Giunti.

Google Scholar

Rigler, T., Manor, I., Kalansky, A., Shorer, Z., Noyman, I., and Sadaka, Y. (2016). New DSM-5 criteria for ADHD — does it matter? Compr. Psychiatry 68, 56–59. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.03.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Riglin, L., Wootton, R. E., Livingston, L. A., Agnew-Blais, J., Arseneault, L., Blakey, R., et al. (2022). “Late-onset” ADHD symptoms in young adulthood: is this ADHD? J. Atten. Disord. 26, 1271–1282. doi: 10.1177/10870547211066486

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roebuck-Spencer, T. M., Glen, T., Puente, A. E., Denney, R. L., Ruff, R. M., Hostetter, G., et al. (2017). Cognitive screening tests versus comprehensive neuropsychological test batteries: a National Academy of neuropsychology education paper. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 32, 491–498. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acx021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, E. A., Graves, S. J., Freeman, A. J., Paul, M. G., Etcoff, L. M., and Allen, D. N. (2022). Improving accuracy of ADHD subtype diagnoses with the ADHD symptom rating scale. Child Neuropsychol. 28, 962–978. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2022.2044768

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenbloom, T., and Wultz, B. (2011). Thirty-day self-reported risky driving behaviors of ADHD and non-ADHD drivers. Accid. Anal. Prev. 43, 128–133. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2010.08.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roshani, F., Piri, R., Malek, A., Michel, T. M., and Vafaee, M. S. (2020). Comparison of cognitive flexibility, appropriate risk-taking and reaction time in individuals with and without adult ADHD. Psychiatry Res. 284:112494. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2019.112494

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothenberger, A., Becker, A., Brüni, L. G., and Roessner, V. (2018). “Influence of tics and/or obsessive-compulsive behaviour on the phenomenology of coexisting ADHD” in Case studies in clinical psychological science: bridging the gap from science to practice, 1–7.

Google Scholar

Roy, A., Oldehinkel, A. J., and Hartman, C. A. (2017). Cognitive functioning in adolescents with self-reported ADHD and depression: results from a population-based study. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 45, 69–81. doi: 10.1007/s10802-016-0160-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sadek, J. A clinician’s guide to ADHD. (2014).

Google Scholar

Sadek, J. (2023). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder misdiagnosis: why medical evaluation should be a part of ADHD assessment. Brain Sci. 13:1522. doi: 10.3390/brainsci13111522

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sadozai, A. K., Sun, C., Demetriou, E. A., Lampit, A., Munro, M., Perry, N., et al. (2024). Executive function in children with neurodevelopmental conditions: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2024, 1–10. doi: 10.1038/s41562-024-02000-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandford, J. A., and Sandford, S. E. (2016). IVA- 2TM: integrated visual and auditory continuous performance test. Richmond, VA Available at: www.braintrain.com.

Google Scholar

Sarver, D. E., Rapport, M. D., Kofler, M. J., Raiker, J. S., and Friedman, L. M. (2015). Hyperactivity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): impairing deficit or compensatory behavior? J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 43, 1219–1232. doi: 10.1007/s10802-015-0011-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schrank, F. A., and McGrew, K. S. (2015). Woodcock-Johnson IV tests of early cognitive and academic development. Rolling Meadows, IL: Riverside.

Google Scholar

Senkowski, D., Ziegler, T., Singh, M., Heinz, A., He, J., Silk, T., et al. (2024). Assessing inhibitory control deficits in adult ADHD: a systematic review and Meta-analysis of the stop-signal task. Neuropsychol. Rev. 34, 548–567. doi: 10.1007/s11065-023-09592-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaffer, D., Fisher, P., Lucas, C., Dulcan, M., and Schwab-Stone, M. (2000). NIMH diagnostic interview schedule for children version IV (NIMH DISC-IV): description, differences from previous versions, and reliability of some common diagnoses. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 39, 28–38. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200001000-00014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 298, 199–209. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1982.0082

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, C., Luo, Q., Jia, T., Zhao, Q., Desrivières, S., Quinlan, E. B., et al. (2020). Neural correlates of the dual-pathway model for ADHD in adolescents. Am. J. Psychiatry 177, 844–854. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.19020183

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sibley, M. H. (2021). Empirically-informed guidelines for first-time adult ADHD diagnosis. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 43, 340–351. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2021.1923665

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Skarphedinsson, G., Jarbin, H., Andersson, M., and Ivarsson, T. (2021). Diagnostic efficiency and validity of the DSM-oriented child behavior checklist and youth self-report scales in a clinical sample of Swedish youth. PLoS One 16:e0254953. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0254953

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Slobodin, O., Cassuto, H., and Berger, I. (2018). Age-related changes in distractibility: developmental trajectory of sustained attention in ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 22, 1333–1343. doi: 10.1177/1087054715575066

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Slobodin, O., and Davidovitch, M. (2019). Gender differences in objective and subjective measures of ADHD among clinic-referred children. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:441. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00441

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smart, A. (2006). A multi-dimensional model of clinical utility. Int. J. Qual. Health Care 18, 377–382. doi: 10.1093/intqhc/mzl034

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smyth, A. C., and Meier, S. T. (2019). Evaluating the psychometric properties of the Conners adult ADHD rating scales. J. Atten. Disord. 23, 1111–1118. doi: 10.1177/1087054715624230

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, P., Zha, M., Yang, Q., Zhang, Y., Li, X., and Rudan, I. (2021). The prevalence of adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a global systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Glob. Health 11, 1–9. doi: 10.7189/jogh.11.04009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2003). The dual pathway model of AD/HD: an elaboration of neuro-developmental characteristics. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 27, 593–604. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.08.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sparrow, E. P. (2010). Essentials of Conners behavior assessments. 1st Edn. John Wiley & Sons: Wiley.

Google Scholar

Steinau, S. (2013). Diagnostic criteria in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder – changes in DSM 5. Front. Psych. 4, 4–5. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00049

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 18, 643–662. doi: 10.1037/h0054651

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Surís, A., Holliday, R., and North, C. S. (2016). The evolution of the classification of psychiatric disorders. Behav. Sci. 6. doi: 10.3390/bs6010005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thapar, A., and van Goozen, S. (2018). “Conduct disorder in ADHD” in Oxford textbook of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. eds. T. Banaschewski, D. Coghill, and A. Zuddas (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 193–199.

Google Scholar

Tucha, L., Fuermaier, A. B. M., Koerts, J., Buggenthin, R., Aschenbrenner, S., Weisbrod, M., et al. (2017). Sustained attention in adult ADHD: time-on-task effects of various measures of attention. J. Neural Transm. 124, 39–53. doi: 10.1007/s00702-015-1426-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Turgay, A. (1995). DSM-IV-based adult hyperactivity rating scale (unpublished scale). Toronto.

Google Scholar

U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Data from the National Health Interview Survey. Summary health statistics for US children: National Health Interview Survey (2011). Available at: https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/products/index.htm (Accessed November 29, 2022).

Google Scholar

Unterrainer, J. M., Rahm, B., Loosli, S. V., Rauh, R., Schumacher, L. V., Biscaldi, M., et al. (2020). Psychometric analyses of the tower of London planning task reveal high reliability and feasibility in typically developing children and child patients with ASD and ADHD. Child Neuropsychol. 26, 257–273. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2019.1642317

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vassileva, J., Conrod, P. J., and Justine, S. (2019). Impulsivities and addictions: a multidimensional integrative framework informing assessment and interventions for substance use disorders. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 374:20180137. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2018.0137

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward, M. F., Wender, P. H., and Reimherr, F. W. (1993). The Wender Utah rating scale: an aid in the retrospective diagnosis of childhood attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 150, 885–890

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Weinsztok, S., Brassard, S., Balodis, I., Martin, L. E., and Amlung, M. (2021). Delay discounting in established and proposed behavioral addictions: a systematic review and Meta-analysis. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 15:786358. doi: 10.3389/FNBEH.2021.786358

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Weschler, D. (2024). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. 5th Edn: NCS Pearson.

Google Scholar

Wilens, T., Carrellas, N., and Biederman, J. (2018). The Co-Occurrence of ADHD and Substance use Disorders. Psychiatric Annals, 48, 328–332. doi: 10.3928/00485713-20180613-01

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolraich, M. L., Hagan, J. F., Allan, C., Chan, E., Davison, D., Earls, M., et al. (2019). Clinical practice guideline for the diagnosis, evaluation, and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Pediatrics 144:e20192528. doi: 10.1542/PEDS.2019-2528

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolraich, M., Hannah, J., Baumgaertel, A., and Feurer, I. (1998). Examination of DSM-IV criteria for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a county-wide sample. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 19, 162–168. doi: 10.1097/00004703-199806000-00003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolraich, M. L., Lambert, W., Doffing, M. A., Bickman, L., Simmons, T., and Worley, K. (2003). Psychometric properties of the Vanderbilt ADHD diagnostic parent rating scale in a referred population. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 28, 559–568. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsg046

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization. (2019). International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems (11th ed.. Available at: https://icd.who.int/ (Accessed June 19, 2024).

Google Scholar

Yato, Y., Hirose, S., Wallon, P., Mesmin, C., and Jobert, M. (2019). d2-R test for Japanese adolescents: Concurrent validity with the attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder rating scale. Pediatr. Int.: J. Japan Pediatr. Society, 61, 43–48. doi: 10.1111/ped.13735

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, J. L., and Goodman, D. W. (2016). Adult Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Diagnosis, Management, and Treatment in the DSM-5 Era. The primary care companion for CNS disorders, 18. doi: 10.4088/PCC.16r02000

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zamani, L., Shahrivar, Z., Alaghband-Rad, J., Sharifi, V., Davoodi, E., Ansari, S., et al. (2021). Reliability, criterion and concurrent validity of the Farsi translation of DIVA-5: a semi-structured diagnostic interview for adults with ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 25, 1666–1675. doi: 10.1177/1087054720930816

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: ADHD, diagnosis, evaluation tools, assessment, rating scales

Citation: Musullulu H (2025) Evaluating attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a review of current methods and issues. Front. Psychol. 16:1466088. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1466088

Received: 17 July 2024; Accepted: 03 February 2025;
Published: 24 February 2025.

Edited by:

Concetta Pirrone, Departement of Educational Sciences, Italy

Reviewed by:

Iban Onandia Hinchado, University of the Basque Country, Spain
Hakan Öğütlü, Cognitive Behavioral Psychotherapies Association, Türkiye

Copyright © 2025 Musullulu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Hande Musullulu, aGFuZGUubXVzdWxsdWx1QHBkaS5hdGxhbnRpY29tZWRpby5lcw==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Research integrity at Frontiers

Man ultramarathon runner in the mountains he trains at sunset

94% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good

Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.


Find out more