Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol. , 17 March 2025

Sec. Educational Psychology

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1453193

Preschool children's social skills, problem behaviors, academic self-esteem and teacher-child relationship: a serial mediation model

  • 1Department of Preschool Education, Faculty of Education, Bayburt University, Bayburt, Türkiye
  • 2Department of Child Care and Youth Services, Child Development Program, Health Services Vocational School, Akdeniz University, Antalya, Türkiye

Introduction: Research has shown that children's social skills have effects on their problem behaviors. However, the mechanisms through which social skills contribute to reducing problem behaviors need further clarification.

Methods: This study examined the relationship between preschool children's social skills and problem behaviors, as well as the independent and serial mediating roles of academic self-esteem and teacher-child relationship in this relationship. The study included 382 children aged 3–5 years, randomly selected in Türkiye.

Results and discussion: As a result of the study: (1) there was a significant and negative relationship between problem behaviors and social skills, academic self-esteem and teacher-child relationship; (2) the mediating role of academic self-esteem and teacher-child relationship in the relationship between social skills and problem behaviors was supported; and (3) the serial mediating role of academic self-esteem and teacher-child relationship in the effect of social skills on problem behaviors, respectively, was found. These findings indicated that children's social skills may help to increase their academic self-esteem, improve the teacher-child relationship, and thus reduce their problem behaviors. Therefore, these results have important implications for designing interventions to increase preschool children's social skills, academic self-esteem, and teacher-child relationship, as well as to prevent the early development of problem behaviors.

Introduction

A growing body of research has focused on elucidating the mechanisms underlying children's PB (Problem behaviors) (e.g., Baardstu et al., 2022b; Choi et al., 2022; Rademacher et al., 2022; Tan et al., 2023). PB are evaluated in two categories: (1) externalizing behaviors, which consist of criminal and aggressive behaviors such as fighting, bullying, kicking, biting, hitting, teasing, and threatening; and (2) internalizing behaviors, which reflect internal states such as anxiety, depression, and withdrawal (Baardstu et al., 2022b; Berry and O'Connor, 2010; Walker, 2006). These PB usually emerge early in life and may lead to permanent PB if necessary interventions are not made. For example, in a study examining the effect of early behavioral disorders on academic achievement in high school, it was determined that children's attention, internalizing behavior and externalizing problems at the age of 6 significantly predicted their math and reading achievement at the age of 17 (Breslau et al., 2009). In addition, many studies have shown that children's PB are associated with school readiness (Yong et al., 2022), school success (McDermott et al., 2022), reading difficulties (Trzesniewski et al., 2006), low level of self-regulation (Rademacher et al., 2022), the quality of the TCR (Wang et al., 2022), academic achievement (Kremer et al., 2016), and later delinquency (Hay and Pawlby, 2003), substance abuse, financial, work, and mental health problems (Moffitt et al., 2002). However, limited research has been conducted on the mechanisms underlying this relationship. Examining the development of such problems before PB become persistent in the early years can help design effective interventions targeting preschool children who are most likely to benefit. The purpose of this study was to examine the relationship between preschool children's social skills (SS) and PB, as well as the potential independent and serial mediating roles of academic self-esteem (ASE) and teacher-child relationship (TCR) in this relationship.

The effect of SS on PB

SS, which include specific learned behaviors, consist of both initiating communication and responding appropriately, and require interaction with others (Little et al., 2017). Children who are considered social in this respect are defined as friendly, responsible and socially capable individuals (Basharpoor et al., 2022). Because of the critical role that SS play in children's socially adaptive behaviors, they are often studied in relation to PB (e.g., Elliott et al., 2019; Montroy et al., 2014). Existing evidence has also revealed that different negative outcomes such as PB (Maag, 2005), guilt, depression, social withdrawal (Cook et al., 2008), school dropout (Elksnin and Elksnin, 1998), academic failure (Malecki and Elliot, 2002) are risk factors for children's SS deficits. SS deficits include not making eye contact consistently when talking to someone, interrupting others, aggression, and other maladaptive behaviors (Little et al., 2017) and can have detrimental effects on a child's current and future level of functioning (Danielson and Phelps, 2003). For example, it has been found that children with SS deficits have higher rates of juvenile delinquency (Elksnin and Elksnin, 1998) and are more likely to have low frustration tolerance, emotional dysregulation (Beaumont et al., 2017), low academic performance (Cook et al., 2008), and there is a strong negative correlation between PB and social competence (Hukkelberg et al., 2019). In addition, previous research has indicated that both SS and PB dimensions represent not only interrelated but also independent components of social competence (Heyman et al., 2018; Montroy et al., 2014). Therefore, early childhood is an important focus for developing children's SS and reducing PB.

Potentially mediating role of ASE

One possible mechanism underlying the relationship between SS and PB is children's ASE. ASE, which involves children's understanding of their academic skills from both their own and others' perspectives, is a non-cognitive factor (Cámara-Martínez et al., 2023; Giofr et al., 2017; Warash and Markstrom, 2001). Indeed, previous research has examined the links between ASE and academic self-efficacy and academic achievement (Ahmadi, 2020), school adjustment (Basharpoor et al., 2022), academic self-concept (Basith, 2021), intelligence, working memory, academic achievement (Giofr et al., 2017) and SS (Kiliç et al., 2021). The results showed that there is a significant relationship between ASE and academic self-concept (Basith, 2021), and ASE has an indirect effect on academic achievement mediated by intelligence (Giofr et al., 2017). In addition, it has shown that school adjustment is positively related to academic self-concept, and that children's high social acceptability can affect school adjustment both directly and indirectly through school engagement and academic self-concept (Basharpoor et al., 2022). It was also found that low self-esteem is associated with children's use of maladaptive achievement strategies, which in turn is associated with their maladjustment at school, internalizing and externalizing PB (Aunola et al., 2000). Theoretically, Hinshaw (1992) proposes a model of reciprocal relationships between problem behaviors and academic achievement. According to this model, academic achievement and problem behaviors occur simultaneously and each area leads to the other. Moreover, the problems of children with academic underachievement are not limited to the academic field, self-esteem deficits, problems related to SS, and conflict in TCR are also commonly seen (e.g., Ahmadi, 2020; Basharpoor et al., 2022; Basith, 2021; Giofr et al., 2017). Given the strong associations of ASE with both SS and PB, as mentioned above, the current study tested the mediating role of ASE in the relationship between the two variables.

Potentially the mediating role of TCR

Many influential theoretical frameworks such as Bronfenbrenner's (2005) ecological systems theory, Dodge's social information processing theory Dodge and Rabiner (2004) and Bowlby's (1946) attachment theory, Vygotsky's (1981) sociocultural theory state that children's social, emotional and academic development is directly related to the quality of their interactions with parents, teachers and peers in their environment. In this respect, the TCR in the preschool period is very important for children's social, emotional and academic development. Existing evidence has also revealed that TCR is related to factors such as children's academic achievement, SS and social development, and PB (Bratsch-Hines et al., 2023; Buyse et al., 2008; Chen et al., 2021; Doumen et al., 2008; Glüer and Gregoriadis, 2017; Lei et al., 2023; Palermo et al., 2007; Zhu et al., 2023). In this context, research shows that the quality of TCR shapes children's development in different aspects, including the development of PB (e.g., Buyse et al., 2008; Doumen et al., 2008; Zhu et al., 2023).

In shaping the TCR, a child's externalizing behaviors, such as aggression, can promote maladaptive interactions and conflict with the teacher, which can mutually reinforce the child's PB over time. The persistence of PB may confirm the teacher's negative expectations and thus perpetuate the teacher's negative interactions with the child (Doumen et al., 2008). For example, Paes et al. (2023) found that conflict in the TCR in preschool is associated with poorer SS, particularly lower assertion and lower engagement. Longitudinal studies have also shown that this negative and conflictual relationship has negative effects on children in the long run, such as increased aggressive behaviors, negative school attitudes, less positive school attendance, and lower academic achievement (Bratsch-Hines et al., 2023; Doumen et al., 2008, 2011; Li et al., 2022). For example, Li et al. (2022) found that teacher-child conflict from kindergarten to first grade was associated with academic skills in primary school. In contrast, positive TCR allow children to develop and use effective SS to negotiate and overcome difficulties (Rudasill and Rimm-Kaufman, 2009). For example, Zhu et al. (2023) found that positive TCR buffered the relationship between social avoidance and peer exclusion in preschool children. It is also stated that teachers' use of strategies that support the development of emotional intelligence plays a critical role in reducing children's problem behaviors by increasing their ASE levels (Chaidi and Drigas, 2022). Therefore, it seems reasonable to consider the mediating role of TCR in the link between preschool children's SS and PB.

The serial mediation role of ASE and TCR in the relationship between SS and PB

As discussed above, both ASE and TCR can independently mediate the relationship between SS and PB. However, how these two mediators work together in this relationship has yet to be explored. According to Hayes (2017), when a mediation model includes more than one mediator, these mediators can play a serial mediation role if they are interrelated. Existing studies have proven the close relationship between ASE and TCR (Chen et al., 2021; Cui et al., 2020; Doumen et al., 2011; Lei et al., 2023; van Aalst et al., 2021). For example, Cui et al. (2020) showed that the TCR helps to increase children's self-confidence, which has a positive relationship with academic achievement. Therefore, it is possible that ASE and TCR not only play an independent mediating role in the relationship between SS and PB, but also relate to each other and also play a serial mediating role in this relationship.

Consistent with the ideas expressed by previous studies (Baardstu et al., 2022a; Berry and O'Connor, 2010; Hinshaw, 1992; Kiliç et al., 2021; Wang et al., 2022), the theoretical model in the current study was constructed by taking into account that as children's PB increase, their SS, TCR and ASE are negatively affected by this situation. In addition, in the model proposed in the current study, it is assumed that ASE and TCR have independent and serial mediator roles in the relationship between preschool children's SS and PB. As shown in the literature, children's ASE (e.g., Arnold et al., 2012; Lane et al., 2005) and TCR (e.g., Wang et al., 2022) are important predictors of both SS and PB. Moreover, in line with the propositions of the Bioecological systems perspective (Bronfenbrenner, 2005), TCR and ASE will be associated with children's SS and PB. Therefore, it is reasonable to expect that preschool children's SS and PB may have different effects on ASE and TCR.

The current study

Although researchers have separately conducted extensive research to understand the relationships between preschool children's SS, PB, TCR, and ASE variables (e.g., Bratsch-Hines et al., 2023; Berry and O'Connor, 2010; Choi et al., 2022; Doumen et al., 2008; Rudasill and Rimm-Kaufman, 2009; Zhu et al., 2023), to the best of our knowledge, no study has tested the mediating roles (Hayes, 2017) of the underlying causes of these variables. Therefore, analyzing the different mechanisms that play a role in these relationships will further help to understand the factors that protect children from or put them at risk of these serious problem situations. In addition, this study contributes to the existing literature by including different assessments of both SS and PB than most previous studies. In this respect, the current study was conducted to examine the relationship between preschool children's SS and PB, as well as the independent and serial mediating roles of ASE and TCR in this relationship (the mediation model in Figure 1 was tested). In the current study, the following hypotheses were developed under the guidance of previous studies.

H1: There is a significant relationship between SS, PB, TCR and ASE.

H2: The effect of SS on PB is significant.

H3: The mediating role of ASE in the effect of SS on PB is significant.

H4: The mediating role of TCR in the effect of SS on PB is significant.

H5: The serial mediation role of ASE and TCR is significant in the effect of SS on PB, respectively.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The estimation model suggested in the research.

Methods

Participants

The study included 382 children aged 3–5 years and their teachers (n = 40) randomly selected from 3 kindergartens and 12 preschools in Bayburt, Türkiye. Demographic information about the children and teachers was obtained through the consent form for participation in the study sent to the teachers along with the measurement tools and teacher responses. Accordingly, 197 (51.6%) of the children in the study were girls and 185 (48.4%) were boys. In terms of age, 4 (1%) were 3 years old, 37 (9.7%) were 4 years old and 341 (89.3%) were 5 years old. All of the teachers (n = 40) who assessed children's SS, PB, ASE and TCR were female and their ages ranged between 22 and 42 years (Mage = 30.7, SD = 6.1). In terms of the length of professional service of the teachers, 2 (5%) had been working in preschool education institutions for 6–11 months, 16 (40%) for 1–5 years, 13 (32.5%) for 6–10 years and 9 (22.5%) for more than 10 years.

Measures

The data were obtained using the Social Skills Improvement System-Rating Scales to assess children's social skills and PB, the Academic Self-Esteem Scale to assess their ASE, and the Student-Teacher Relationship Scale-Short Form for the TCR.

Social skills improvement system-rating scales (SSIS-RS) (preschool ages 3–5)

The SSIS-RS (Gresham and Elliott, 2008) was used to capture data on a wide range of students' social behaviors and academic functioning. The SSIS-RS includes two subscales: the SS subscale, and the PB subscale. The preschool version of SSIS-RS included parents' and teachers' ratings. In the current study, children's SS and PB were all assessed by their teachers. The SS subscale consists of 46 items and seven sub-domains: communication, cooperation, assertion, responsibility, empathy, engagement, and self-control. The PB subscale consists of 30 items and five sub-domains: externalizing, bullying, hyperactivity/inattention, internalizing and autism spectrum. The items and scoring of the SS and PB scales use a 4-point frequency scale with Never = 0, Rarely = 1, Often = 2, and Almost Always = 3 as scale anchor points. The SSIS-RS included the following items: “Follows your directions,” “Takes turns in conversations,” “Follows classroom rules,” and “Acts without thinking” (Gresham and Elliott, 2008).

The internal consistency coefficients of the SSIS-RS were as follows: communication 0.84, cooperation 0.89, assertion 0.83, responsibility 0.90, empathy 0.86, engagement 0.84 and self-control 0.86, externalization 0.93, bullying 0.74, hyperactivity/inattention 0.88, internalization 0.78 and autism spectrum 0.84 (Gresham and Elliott, 2008). It was concluded that the internal consistency coefficients of The SSIS-RS teacher form for 3–5-year-old children in Türkiye ranged between 0.70 and 0.86 and that it is a valid and reliable tool for assessing children's SS and PB (Tutkun and Dinçer, 2019).

Academic self-esteem scale

The Academic Self-Esteem Scale (ASES) was developed by Cevher and Buluş (2006) to measure the ASE of preschool children. In the process of developing the scale, the Behavioral ASES developed by Coopersmith and Gilberts (1982) and the Assessment Inventory developed by Hamachek (1995) were used. The scale includes the following statements: “He puts forward new ideas in the classroom. He/she is spontaneously motivated for school work.” The range of points that can be obtained from the 5-point Likert-type ASES is between 22 and 110. The higher the score on the scale, the higher the level of ASE of the children, and the lower the score, the lower the level of ASE. In the internal consistency reliability analysis, Cronbach Alpha (α) of the scale was found to be 0.95.

Student-teacher relationship scale-short form

The Student-Teacher Relationship Scale (STRS) (Pianta and Nimetz, 1991) assesses teachers' levels of closeness and conflict with each child in their classroom. The closeness subscale included 8 items (e.g., This child values his/her relationship with me), and the conflict subscale included 7 items (e.g., This child easily becomes angry at me). For each item, the teacher rates the extent to which the statement reflects the relationship with each child on a scale of 1 (Definitely does not apply) to 5 (Definitely applies). The minimum score for the conflict subscale is 8 and the maximum score is 40. The minimum score on the closeness subscale is 7 and the maximum score is 35. Higher scores indicate higher closeness and lower conflict. The internal consistency coefficient of the scale is between 0.86 and 0.89. The internal consistency coefficients of the STRS form for children in Türkiye were 0.84 for the conflict subscale and 0.76 for the closeness subscale 0.76 for the conflict subscale and 0.84 for the closeness subscale, and it was concluded that it is a valid and reliable tool for assessing the TCR (Asi and Karabay, 2018).

Procedures

Data were collected by the author. Prior to the study, permission was obtained from the schools and teachers. First of all, a document explaining the general purpose of the study and an information form were given to all teachers in the participating classrooms, and all teachers gave signed consent for participation. Teachers were then given the SSIS-RS to assess children's SS and PB, the ASES to assess ASE, and the STRS-Short Form for TCR. Teachers completed the forms for the children in their classrooms within a 1-week period. No remuneration was offered to the teachers who took part in the study.

Correlations were calculated to determine the relationships between variables. Then, the direct and indirect effects of the independent variable on the dependent variable were investigated. The macro program PROCESS v3.5 (Hayes, 2017) was used to test the hypothesized mediation models and the Sobel test was used to confirm the consistency of the results. The Sobel test is another test that measures whether the mediating variable is statistically significant (Sobel, 1982). SPSS PROCESS macro based on Bootstrap sampling was used to determine the significance of mediating variables in this relationship. During this analysis, bias corrected bootstrap confidence intervals (a bootstrap sample of 5,000 was specified) were calculated at 99% confidence level.

Results

Descriptive statistics and correlation analysis

In line with the first hypothesis of the study, the relationships between preschool children's SS, PB, relationships with teachers and ASE were examined (see Table 1). In the study, a negative and statistically significant relationship was determined between preschool children's SS and PB (r = −0.58, p < 0.01). A positive significant relationship was found between children's SS and their ASE (r = 0.71, p < 0.01) and their relationships with their teachers (r = 0.47, p < 0.01). There was also a significant and negative relationship between children's PB and their ASE (r = −0.56, p < 0.01) and their relationships with their teachers (r = −0.62, p < 0.01). There was a positive and significant relationship between children's relationships with their teachers and their ASE (r = 0.53, p < 0.01). In this context, it was concluded that there was a significant relationship between all variables.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlations among study variables.

The effect of SS on PB

In line with the second hypothesis of the study, the effect of preschool children's SS on PB was examined (see Figure 2). As a result of the analysis, it was determined that the total effect of SS of preschool children on PB was significant (β = −0.48, SE = 0.03 p < 0.001, CI = [−0.57, −0.39]). In other words, SS were found to be a significant predictor of PB. Accordingly, the second hypothesis of the study was accepted.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The effect of SS on PB. *p < 0.001.

The mediating role of ASE in the effect of SS on PB

In line with the third hypothesis of the study, the mediating role of ASE in the effect of preschool children's SS on their PB was examined (see Table 2). In the analyses, it was determined that the effect of preschool children's SS on ASE, which was determined as the mediating variable, was significant (β = 1.10, SE = 0.05 p < 0.001). In addition, the effect of children's ASE on PB was also found to be significant (β = −0.15, SE = 0.03 p < 0.001). When ASE, which was determined as a mediating variable, was controlled, the effect of SS on PB decreased (β = −0.31, SE = 0.04 p < 0.001). The indirect effect of children's SS on PB through ASE was significant (β = −0.17 SE = 0.03 CI [−0.25, −0.08]). At the same time, the fully standardized indirect effect size of children's SS on PB through ASE (K2 = −0.20) was significant and high (see Figure 3). The results obtained from the Sobel test also proved that the mediating role of ASE in the effect of SS on PB was significant (Sobel z = −4.94, p < 0.001). In line with the findings, the third hypothesis of the study was accepted.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The mediating role of ASE in the effect of SS on PB.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The mediating role of ASE in the effect of SS on PB. *p < 0.001.

The mediating role of TCR in the effect of SS on PB

In line with the fourth hypothesis of the study, the mediating role of preschool children's SS on their PB was examined (see Table 3). The results showed that the effect of preschool children's SS on their relationships with their teachers was significant (β = 0.53, SE = 0.05 p < 0.001). In addition, the effect of children's relationship with their teacher on PB was also significant (β = −0.32, SE = 0.03 p < 0.001). When the child's relationship with the teacher, which was determined as the mediating variable, was controlled, the effect of SS on PB (direct effect) decreased (β = −0.31, SE = 0.03 p < 0.001). The indirect effect of children's SS on PB through their relationship with their teachers was significant (β = −0.17 SE = 0.02 CI [−0.25, −0.10]). At the same time, the fully standardized indirect effect size of children's SS on PB through their relationships with their teachers (K2 = −0.20) was significant and high (see Figure 4). The result obtained from the Sobel test also proved the mediating role of the TCR in the effect of SS on PB (Sobel z = −7.44, p < 0.001). Accordingly, the fourth hypothesis of the study was accepted.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. The mediating role of TCR in the effect of SS on PB.

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. The mediating role of TCR in the effect of SS on PB. *p < 0.001.

The serial mediation role of ASE and TCR in the effect of SS on PB, respectively

In line with the fifth hypothesis of the study, the serial mediation role of ASE and relationships with teachers on the effect of preschool children's SS on their PB was examined (see Table 4). The results showed that the effect of children's SS on ASE, which was identified as the first mediating variable, was significant (β = 1.10, SE = 0.05 p < 0.001). It was also determined that the effect of children's ASE on the child's relationship with the teacher, which was determined as the second mediating variable, was also significant (β = 0.28, SE = 0.04 p < 0.001). It was also found that children's relationship with their teachers had a significant effect on PB (β = −0.30 SE = 0.03 p < 0.001). It was concluded that ASE and relationship with the teacher played a serial mediating role in the effect of children's SS on PB, respectively (β = −0.09 SE = 0.01 CI = [−0.15, −0.05]). At the same time, the full standardized indirect effect size (K2 = −0.11) of preschool children's SS on PB through serial mediation of their ASE and their relationship with their teachers, respectively, was determined to be significant and moderate (see Figure 5). Moreover, the result obtained from the Sobel test proved the serial mediation role of ASE and TCR, respectively, in the effect of SS on PB (Sobel z = −5.49, p < 0.001). In line with the findings, the fifth hypothesis of the study was accepted.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. The serial mediation role of ASE and TCR in the effect of SS on PB, respectively.

Figure 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. The serial mediation role of ASE and TCR in the effect of SS on PB, respectively. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, and ***p < 0.001.

Discussion

The current study examined the relationship between SS and PB of preschool children, as well as the independent and serial mediator roles of ASE and TCR in this relationship. As a result of the study: (1) there was a significant and negative relationship between PB and SS, ASE and TCR; (2) SS were found to be a significant predictor of PB; (3) the mediating role of ASE and TCR in the relationship between SS and PB was supported; and (4) the serial mediating role of ASE and TCR in the effect of SS on PB, respectively, was found.

As a result of the study, negative relationships were found between preschool children's PB and their SS, ASE and TCR. In addition, SS were found to be a significant predictor of PB. These results supported the first and second hypotheses of the study. Accordingly, as children's SS, ASE and TCR increased, their PB decreased. Similarly, children with high levels of SS, ASE, and TCR exhibit fewer PB and contribute to the significant evidence that PB have a negative relationship with SS, ASE, and TCR (Elliott et al., 2019; Heyman et al., 2018; Maag, 2005; Kremer et al., 2016). This relationship can be justified as follows: As children's relationships with their teachers improve, their ASE and SS increase, their PB decrease: PB make it difficult for children to establish healthy relationships with their environment and adapt to the social environment they are in. This situation negatively affects his/her relationships with teachers and peers. As a matter of fact, children's exhibiting PB due to lack of SS is likely to create a negative belief on their ASE. ASE, which includes children's feelings about their academic success and ability (Warash and Markstrom, 2001) and is considered as a non-cognitive factor (Giofr et al., 2017), is also affected by this negative relationship and may prevent children from participating more actively in learning processes and being successful. For example, Walker (2006) found that ASE and aggressive behaviors of preschool children are interrelated. Early PB predicted future academic success and difficulties (Breslau et al., 2009; Miner and Clarke-Stewart, 2008). In this respect, children with PB and a history of academic failure are more likely to develop lower ASE than children with fewer experiences of academic failure and SS.

The independent mediating role of ASE in the relationship between preschool children's SS and PB was supported in this study. The present results support and extend the evidence that social and academic development are interconnected from early on. Previous studies have shown that kindergarten children's learning achievement is most strongly related to SS (Offer-Boljahn et al., 2022) and that there is a high positive relationship between SS and academic achievement (Okeke et al., 2022). On the other hand, PB such as aggression, introversion, bullying, hyperactivity/inattention and internalizing disorders negatively affect children (Adetunji et al., 2022), prevent young children from succeeding in the classroom (McDermott et al., 2022), that poor SS are associated with short-, medium-, and long-term adjustment difficulties (Gresham, 2016), that this has a lasting effect in the future (Kremer et al., 2016; Moffitt et al., 2002), that consistently low SS increase the risk of poor school performance, while consistently high SS increase the chances of good school performance in primary school (Frogner et al., 2022). These results suggest that increasing children's SS and ASE at an early stage may be helpful, especially in terms of preventing or reducing the emergence of PB.

The current study also found that the TCR may be an important mediator of the relationship between SS and PB. This finding supported Bronfenbrenner's (2005) ecological systems theory, which states that children develop by being influenced by their interactions with individuals in their immediate environment. This finding also confirms that TCR play a role in helping children acquire the social and academic skills necessary for success in school and in reducing PB. In addition, previous research has shown that children's aggressive behaviors lead to an increase in teacher-child conflict, which in turn leads to an increase in aggressive behaviors (Doumen et al., 2008); children's PB negatively affect teachers' closeness with children, while children's SS positively affect teachers' closeness with children (Glüer and Gregoriadis, 2017); externalizing and internalizing problems are negatively associated with high teacher-child closeness (Wang et al., 2022); that teachers have more conflictual relationships with children with behavior problems (Buyse et al., 2008); and that TCR is a moderator of the relationships between classroom interactions and children's classroom behavior (Lippard et al., 2018). The current study also found that the TCR may be an important mediator of the relationship between SS and PB. SS can reduce children's PB, which can help children build better relationships with their teachers within the classroom atmosphere, thus improving the TCR.

The serial mediating role of ASE and TCR in the relationship between preschool children's SS and PB is also significant in this study. Such a finding, based on the independent mediating role of TCR, revealed that ASE is closely related to SS and PB simultaneously, thus, it forms a serial mediation pathway between SS and PB together with TCR. This finding also extends and supports previous research using structural equation modeling (Ahmadi, 2020; Baardstu et al., 2022b; Chen et al., 2021; Cui et al., 2020; Paes et al., 2023; Palermo et al., 2007; van Aalst et al., 2021; Wu et al., 2022; Zhu et al., 2023). For example, Paes et al. (2023) found that teacher-child conflict was associated with poorer SS and PB in children; Zhu et al. (2023) found that teacher-child conflict exacerbated the relationships between social avoidance and peer exclusion and anxious-fearful behavior; and self-confidence in learning mediated significant relationships between TCR and students' academic achievement (Cui et al., 2020). In addition, it was found that teacher-child closeness mediated the relationship between school engagement and shyness (Wu et al., 2022), social avoidance and peer exclusion (Zhu et al., 2023), children's behavior and peer exclusion mediated the relationship between TCR and academic readiness (Palermo et al., 2007), and ASE was directly related to academic self-efficacy and indirectly related to academic achievement (Ahmadi, 2020). In the light of all these results, it is important to consider mechanisms such as ASE and TCR that affect the relationship between preschool children's SS and PB when designing educational interventions.

Conclusions, limitations and implications

In conclusion, this study examined the relationship between preschool children's SS and PB, as well as the independent and serial mediating roles of ASE and TCR in this relationship. The results showed that preschool children's ASE and their relationship with their teachers were directly and indirectly related to their SS and PB through the independent and serial mediator role.

The current study has some limitations. First, although the current study revealed possible contemporaneous relationships between all study variables, it could not demonstrate long-term, causal relationships between them without a longitudinal study. Second, the data for each study variable in the current study was based on teacher ratings. Future studies should try to include multiple sources of information, especially parents and/or teachers, and direct observations of children. This is because existing evidence has revealed inconsistencies in parent and teacher ratings (Gresham et al., 2010; Heyman et al., 2018; Wang et al., 2022). Third, the current study did not address other variables such as child age, gender, temperament, or parental attitudes in ASE, TCR, SS, and PB. How and in what ways such variables affect children's ASE, TCR, SS and PB can be further investigated. On the other hand, cultural differences can affect individuals' behaviors, socialization processes, and the way they perceive the world in various ways. Given the variety of indirect or direct effects of culture (Vaughn et al., 2014), this study focuses on preschool children growing up in Turkish culture. However, future studies may examine children from different cultural contexts to assess the effects of culture on ASE, TCR, SS and PB from a broader perspective. Indeed, existing evidence from previous studies has also revealed that there are cultural differences in child and teacher contexts (e.g., Chen and Roorda, 2025; Xu et al., 2023). In this context, conducting similar studies with children in different cultural contexts is important for generalizability.

The current research theoretically contributed to the understanding of the mechanisms underlying the impact of SS and PB, such as children's ASE and TCR, and identified a valuable direction for future studies to analyze their causes. Practically, the predictive models revealed in the current study will be useful for educators and other professionals to successfully develop interventions and programs based on available evidence to improve children's SS, ASE, and relationships with their teachers and to reduce PB. At the same time, understanding the determinants of TCR can inform teacher preparation and professional development and increase teachers' awareness of the characteristics that can influence children's development and achievement. In addition, by having a deeper understanding of how to prevent children's PB and improve their relationships with children, teachers can promote and develop high-quality relationships with more children.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Bayburt University Rectorate's Ethics Committee Unit. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

CT: Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. SE: Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Adetunji, A. A., Aliu, A. H., and Salami, T. A. (2022). Psychometric characteristic of social competence and behaviour evaluation (SCBE-30) in a sample of Nigerian preschoolers. Randwick Int. Soc. Sci. J. 3, 116–126. doi: 10.47175/rissj.v3i1.381

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahmadi, S. (2020). Academic self-esteem, academic self-efficacy and academic achievement: a path analysis. J. Foren. Psy. 5, 10–16. doi: 10.35248/2475-319X.19.5.155

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnold, D. H., Kupersmidt, J. B., Voegler-Lee, M. E., and Marshall, N. A. (2012). The association between preschool children's social functioning and their emergent academic skills. Early Child. Res. Q. 27, 376–386. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.12.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Asi, D. S., and Karabay, S. O. (2018). Ögrenci-ögretmen ilişki ölçegi-kisa formunun Türkçe'ye uyarlanmasi [Turkish adaptation of student-teacher relationship scale-short form]. Ege Egitim. Dergisi. 19, 67–82. doi: 10.12984/egeefd.322358

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Aunola, K., Stattin, H., and Nurmi, J. E. (2000). Adolescents' achievement strategies, school adjustment, and externalizing and internalizing problem behaviors. J. Youth Adolesc. 29, 289–306. doi: 10.1023/A:1005143607919

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baardstu, S., Coplan, R. J., Eliassen, E., Brandlistuen, R. E., and Wang, M. V. (2022a). Exploring the role of teacher–child relationships in the longitudinal associations between childhood shyness and social functioning at school: a prospective cohort study. Schl. Mental Health 14, 984–996. doi: 10.1007/s12310-022-09518-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baardstu, S., Wang, M. V., and Brandlistuen, R. E. (2022b). The role of ECEC teachers for the long-term social and academic adjustment of children with early externalizing difficulties: a prospective cohort study. Eur. Early Childhood Educ. Res. J. 30, 606–623. doi: 10.1080/1350293X.2021.1981961

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Basharpoor, S., Heidari, F., Narimani, M., and Barahmand, U. (2022). School adjustment, engagement and academic self-concept: family, child, and school factors. J. Psychol. Counsel. Schl. 32, 23–38. doi: 10.1017/jgc.2020.6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Basith, A. (2021). The relationship among academic self-concept, academic self-esteem, and academic achievement. Konselor 10, 36–42. doi: 10.15294/jubk.v9i1.45373

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaumont, R., Hariton, J., Bennett, S., Miranda, A., and Mitchell, E. S. (2017). “Social learning instructional models,” in: Handbook of Social Behavior and Skills in Children, ed J. Matson (Cham: Springer), pp. 133–147. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-64592-6_9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Berry, D., and O'Connor, E. (2010). Behavioral risk, teacher–child relationships, and social skill development across middle childhood: a child-by-environment analysis of change. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 31, 1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2009.05.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowlby, J. (1946). Forty-four juvenile thieves: Their characters and home-life. Int. J. Psychoanal. 25, 19–52.

Google Scholar

Bratsch-Hines, M., Cohen-Vogel, L., Little, M., Lindsay, C. A., and Carr, R. (2023). Teacher-child racial/ethnic match from prekindergarten through first grade: understanding early exposure and outcomes. Early Child. Res. Q. 63, 145–155. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2022.12.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Breslau, J., Miller, E., Breslau, N., Bohnert, K., Lucia, V., and Schweitzer, J. (2009). The impact of early behavior disturbances on academic achievement in high school. Pediatrics 123, 1472–1476. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-1406

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bronfenbrenner, U. (2005). Making Human Beings Human: Bioecological Perspectives on Human Development. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Google Scholar

Buyse, E., Verschueren, K., Doumen, S., Van Damme, J., and Maes, F. (2008). Classroom problem behavior and teacher-child relationships in kindergarten: the moderating role of classroom climate. J. Sch. Psychol. 46, 367–391. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2007.06.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cámara-Martínez, A., Ruiz-Ariza, A., Suárez-Manzano, S., Cruz-Cantero, R. M., and Martínez-López, E. J. (2023). Effect of an integrated active lessons programme through playful maths games on self-concept, self-esteem and social skills in preschool children. Behav. Sci. 13, 260–275. doi: 10.3390/bs13030260

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cevher, F. N., and Buluş, M. (2006). Okul öncesi egitim kurumlarina devam eden 5-6 yaş çocuklarinda akademik benlik saygisi [Academic self-respect in children aged 5-6 who continues pre-school education]. Dokuz Eylül Üniversitesi Buca Egitim Fakültesi Dergisi 20, 28–39.

Google Scholar

Chaidi, I., and Drigas, A. (2022). Emotional intelligence and learning, and the role of ICTs. Tech. Soc. Sci. J. 35, 56–78. doi: 10.47577/tssj.v35i1.7249

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, M., and Roorda, D. L. (2025). Psychometric quality of student–teacher relationship drawings among young children in the Netherlands and China. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2025, 1–12. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2025.2455150

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, P., Bao, C., and Gao, Q. (2021). Proactive personality and academic engagement: the mediating effects of teacher-student relationships and academic self-efficacy. Front. Psychol. 12:652994. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.652994

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, J. K., Hatton-Bowers, H., and Shin, J. (2022). Predicting toddlers' problematic behaviors: the role of poverty, parenting, and childcare. Early Child Dev. Care 192, 313–330. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2020.1756791

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cook, C. R., Gresham, F. M., Kern, L., Barreras, R. B., Thornton, S., and Crews, S. D. (2008). Social skills training for secondary students with emotional and/or behavioral disorders: a review and analysis of the meta-analytic literature. J. Emot. Behav. Disord. 16, 131–144. doi: 10.1177/1063426608314541

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Coopersmith, S., and Gilberts, R. (1982). Behavioral Academic Self-Esteem. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Google Scholar

Cui, T., Liu, Q., Liu, J., and Wang, C. (2020). “The relationship between teacher-student relationship, self-confidence, and academic achievement in the Chinese context,” in: Education and New Developments 2020, ed M. Carmo (Lisbon: InScience Press), 24−28. doi: 10.36315/2020end006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Danielson, C. K., and Phelps, C. R. (2003). The assessment of children's social skills through self-report: a potential screening instrument for classroom use. Measure. Evaluat. Counsel. Dev. 35, 218–229. doi: 10.1080/07481756.2003.12069068

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dodge, K. A., and Rabiner, D. L. (2004). Returning to roots: on social information processing and moral development. Child Dev. 75, 1003–1008. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00721.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Doumen, S., Buyse, E., Colpin, H., and Verschueren, K. (2011). Teacher–child conflict and aggressive behaviour in first grade: the intervening role of children's self-esteem. Infant Child Dev. 20, 449–465. doi: 10.1002/icd.725

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Doumen, S., Verschueren, K., Buyse, E., Germeijs, V., Luyckx, K., and Soenens, B. (2008). Reciprocal relations between teacher–child conflict and aggressive behavior in kindergarten: a three-wave longitudinal study. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 37, 588–599. doi: 10.1080/15374410802148079

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Elksnin, L. K., and Elksnin, N. (1998). Teaching social skills to students with learning and behavior problems. Interv. Sch. Clin. 33, 131–140. doi: 10.1177/105345129803300301

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Elliott, S. N., Hwang, Y. S., and Wang, J. (2019). Teachers' ratings of social skills and problem behaviors as concurrent predictors of students' bullying behavior. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 60, 119–126. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2018.12.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Frogner, L., Hellfeldt, K., Ångström, A. K., Andershed, A. K., Källström, Å., Fanti, K. A., et al. (2022). Stability and change in early social skills development in relation to early school performance: a longitudinal study of a Swedish cohort. Early Educ. Dev. 33, 17–37. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2020.1857989

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Giofr,è, D., Borella, E., and Mammarella, I. C. (2017). The relationship between intelligence, working memory, academic self-esteem, and academic achievement. J. Cogn. Psychol. 29, 731–747. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2017.1310110

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Glüer, M., and Gregoriadis, A. (2017). Quality of teacher–child relationship and preschoolers' pro-social behaviour in German kindergartens. Education 45, 558–571. doi: 10.1080/03004279.2016.1140802

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gresham, F., and Elliott, S. N. (2008). Social Skills Improvement System (SSIS) Rating Scales. Bloomington, MN: Pearson Assessments. doi: 10.1037/t02650-000

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gresham, F. M. (2016). Social skills assessment and intervention for children and youth. Cambridge J. Educ. 46, 319–332. doi: 10.1080/0305764X.2016.1195788

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gresham, F. M., Elliott, S. N., Cook, C. R., Vance, M. J., and Kettler, R. (2010). Cross-informant agreement for ratings for social skill and problem behavior ratings: an investigation of the social skills improvement system—rating scales. Psychol. Assess. 22, 157–166. doi: 10.1037/a0018124

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamachek, D. (1995). Self-concept and school achievement: Interaction dynamics and a tool for assessing the self-concept component. J. Couns. Dev. 73, 419–425. doi: 10.1002/j.1556-6676.1995.tb01775.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hay, D. F., and Pawlby, S. (2003). Prosocial development in relation to children's and mothers' psychological problems. Child Dev. 74, 1314–1327. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00609

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2017). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. New York, NY: Guilford Publications.

Google Scholar

Heyman, M., Poulakos, A., Upshur, C., and Wenz-Gross, M. (2018). Discrepancies in parent and teacher ratings of low-income preschooler's social skills. Early Child Dev. Care 188, 759–773. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2016.1236257

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hinshaw, S. P. (1992). Externalizing behavior problems and academic underachievement in childhood and adolescence: causal relationships and underlying mechanisms. Psychol. Bull. 111, 127–155. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.111.1.127

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hukkelberg, S., Keles, S., Ogden, T., and Hammerstrøm, K. (2019). The relation between behavioral problems and social competence: a correlational Meta-analysis. BMC Psychiatry 19, 1–14. doi: 10.1186/s12888-019-2343-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiliç, M., Tutkun, C., and Sahin, F. T. (2021). The mediating role of social skills in the relationship between preschool children's academic self-esteem and problem behaviours. J. Innov. Res. Teach. Educ. 4, 393–409. doi: 10.29329/jirte.2023.616.3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kremer, K. P., Flower, A., Huang, J., and Vaughn, M. G. (2016). Behavior problems and children's academic achievement: a test of growth-curve models with gender and racial differences. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 67, 95–104. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2016.06.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lane, K. L., Menzies, H. M., Barton-Arwood, S. M., Doukas, G. L., and Munton, S. M. (2005). Designing, implementing, and evaluating social skills interventions for elementary students: step-by-step procedures based on actual school-based investigations. Prevent. Schl. Fail. 49, 18–26. doi: 10.1080/1045988X.2005.10823217

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lei, H., Wang, X., Chiu, M. M., Du, M., and Xie, T. (2023). Teacher-student relationship and academic achievement in China: evidence from a three-level meta-analysis. Sch. Psychol. Int. 44, 68–101. doi: 10.1177/01430343221122453

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, L., Valiente, C., Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Johns, S. K., Berger, R. H., et al. (2022). Longitudinal associations among teacher–child relationship quality, behavioral engagement, and academic achievement. Early Child. Res. Q. 61, 25–35. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2022.05.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lippard, C. N., La Paro, K. M., Rouse, H. L., and Crosby, D. A. (2018). A closer look at teacher–child relationships and classroom emotional context in preschool. Child Youth Care Forum 47, 1–21. doi: 10.1007/s10566-017-9414-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Little, S. G., Swangler, J., and Akin-Little, A. (2017). “Defining social skills,” in: Handbook of Social Behavior and Skills in Children. Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, ed J. Matson, J. (Cham: Springer), 9–17. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-64592-6_2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Maag, J. W. (2005). Social skills training for youth with emotional and behavioral disorders and learning disabilities: problems, conclusions, and suggestions. Exceptionality 13, 155–172. doi: 10.1207/s15327035ex1303_2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Malecki, C. K., and Elliot, S. N. (2002). Children's social behaviors as predictors of academic achievement: a longitudinal analysis. Schl. Psychol. Quart. 17, 1–23. doi: 10.1521/scpq.17.1.1.19902

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McDermott, P. A., Rovine, M. J., Gerstner, C. C. E., Weiss, E. M., and Watkins, M. W. (2022). Latent profile analysis of classroom behavior problems in an American national sample of prekindergarten children. Soc. Dev. 2022:12606. doi: 10.1111/sode.12606

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Miner, J. L., and Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (2008). Trajectories of externalizing behavior from age 2 to age 9: relations with gender, temperament, ethnicity, parenting, and rater. Dev. Psychol. 44, 771–786. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.771

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Harrington, H., and Milne, B. J. (2002). Males on the life-course-persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial pathways: follow-up at age 26 years. Dev. Psychopathol. 14, 179–207. doi: 10.1017/S0954579402001104

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Montroy, J. J., Bowles, R. P., Skibbe, L. E., and Foster, T. D. (2014). Social skills and problem behaviors as mediators of the relationship between behavioral self-regulation and academic achievement. Early Child. Res. Q. 29, 298–309. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.03.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Offer-Boljahn, H., Hövel, D. C., and Hennemann, T. (2022). Learning behaviors, executive functions, and social skills: a meta-analysis on the factors influencing learning development in the transition from kindergarten to elementary school. J. Pedag. Res. 6, 1–17. doi: 10.33902/JPR.20221175398

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Okeke, N. U., Anene, N. A., and Agu, N. A. (2022). Assessment of over-schooling and social skills as determinants of academic achievement among selected private owned secondary schools in Anambra State-Nigeria. Acad. J. Curr. Res. 9, 19–27.

Google Scholar

Paes, T. M., Duncan, R., Purpura, D. J., and Schmitt, S. A. (2023). The relations between teacher-child relationships in preschool and children's outcomes in kindergarten. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 86:101534. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2023.101534

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Palermo, F., Hanish, L. D., Martin, C. L., Fabes, R. A., and Reiser, M. (2007). Preschoolers' academic readiness: what role does the teacher–child relationship play? Early Child. Res. Q. 22, 407–422. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2007.04.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pianta, R. C., and Nimetz, S. (1991). Student-Teacher Relationship Scale (STRS) [Database record]. APA PsycTests. doi: 10.1037/t11872-000

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rademacher, A., Zumbach, J., and Koglin, U. (2022). Cross-lagged effects of self-regulation skills and behaviour problems in the transition from preschool to elementary school. Early Child Dev. Care 192, 631–637. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2020.1784891

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rudasill, K. M., and Rimm-Kaufman, S. E. (2009). Teacher–child relationship quality: the roles of child temperament and teacher–child interactions. Early Child. Res. Q. 24, 107–120. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2008.12.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models. Sociol. Methodol. 13, 290–312. doi: 10.2307/270723

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, R., Guo, X., Chen, S., He, G., and Wu, X. (2023). Callous-unemotional traits and externalizing problem behaviors in left-behind preschool children: the role of emotional lability/negativity and positive teacher-child relationship. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry Ment. Health 17, 82–93. doi: 10.1186/s13034-023-00633-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Trzesniewski, K. H., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Taylor, A., and Maughan, B. (2006). Revisiting the association between reading achievement and antisocial behavior: new evidence of an environmental explanation from a twin study. Child Dev. 77, 72–88. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00857.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tutkun, C., and Dinçer, Ç. (2019). The social skills improvement system-rating scale (3-5 age preschool version) teacher form: validity and reliability study. J. Bayburt Educ. Faculty 14, 301–323. doi: 10.35675/befdergi.467864

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van Aalst, D. A., Huitsing, G., Mainhard, T., Cillessen, A. H., and Veenstra, R. (2021). Testing how teachers' self-efficacy and student-teacher relationships moderate the association between bullying, victimization, and student self-esteem. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 18, 928–947. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2021.1912728

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaughn, M. G., DeLisi, M., and Matto, H. C. (2014). Human Behavior: A Cell to Society Approach. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. doi: 10.1002/9781394260546

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vygotsky, L. S. (1981). “The development of higher forms of attention in childhood,” in: The Concept of Activity in Soviet Psychology, ed J. V. Wertsch (Armonk, NY: M. E. Sharpe, Inc.), 189–240. doi: 10.4324/9781003575429-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker, C. M. (2006). The Association Between Aggressive Behaviors and Academic Self-Esteem of Preschool Children. [Master's thesis, West Virginia University]. Available online at: http://www.umi.com.

Google Scholar

Wang, W., Valiente, C., Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Johns, S. K., Berger, R. H., et al. (2022). The interaction between parental warmth and the teacher-student relationship predicts changes in early elementary children's problem behaviors. Soc. Dev. 31, 248–262. doi: 10.1111/sode.12544

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Warash, B. G., and Markstrom, C. A. (2001). Parental perceptions of parenting styles in relation to academic self-esteem of preschoolers. Education 121, 485–494.

Google Scholar

Wu, Y., Fang, M., Wu, J., Chen, Y., and Li, H. (2022). Shyness and school engagement in Chinese suburban preschoolers: a moderated mediation model of teacher–child closeness and child gender. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19, 4270–4287. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19074270

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, C., Huizinga, M., De Luca, G., Pollé, S., Liang, R., Sankalaite, S., et al. (2023). Cultural universality and specificity of teacher-student relationship: a qualitative study in Belgian, Chinese, and Italian primary school teachers. Front. Psychol. 14:1287511. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1287511

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yong, N. H., Lee, M. N., Abdullah, Y., Rendah, S., Kebangsaaan, J., and Hua, N. (2022). Factors influencing school readiness and behavioral problems of elementary students. J. Cakrawala Pendidikan 41, 225–236.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Zhu, J., Yin, X., Li, X., Dong, X., Zou, S., and Li, Y. (2023). Social avoidance and social adjustment in Chinese preschool migrant children: the moderating role of teacher–child relationships. Front. Psychiatry 14:1149319. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1149319

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: social skills, problem behaviors, academic self-esteem, teacher-child relationship, preschool

Citation: Tutkun C and Eskidemir Meral S (2025) Preschool children's social skills, problem behaviors, academic self-esteem and teacher-child relationship: a serial mediation model. Front. Psychol. 16:1453193. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1453193

Received: 22 June 2024; Accepted: 26 February 2025;
Published: 17 March 2025.

Edited by:

Enrique H. Riquelme, Temuco Catholic University, Chile

Reviewed by:

Irene Sotirios Chaidi, Institute of Informatics and Telecommunications, Greece
Flavio Muñoz-Troncoso, Major University, Chile

Copyright © 2025 Tutkun and Eskidemir Meral. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Cansu Tutkun, Y2Fuc3V0dXRrdW4wNkBob3RtYWlsLmNvbQ==; Seda Eskidemir Meral, c2Vza2lkZW1pckBnbWFpbC5jb20=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Research integrity at Frontiers

Man ultramarathon runner in the mountains he trains at sunset

95% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good

Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.


Find out more