SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 10 April 2025

Sec. Movement Science

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1373824

This article is part of the Research TopicExploring Motor Imagery Across the Lifespan – Gaps Between Practical Applications and Theoretical FrameworksView all 6 articles

Effects of different long-term exercise interventions on working memory in children and adolescents: a network meta-analysis

Xuejun Guo&#x;Xuejun Guo1Junyu Wang&#x;Junyu Wang2Jie LiangJie Liang1Ting XieTing Xie3Lin Zhang
Lin Zhang4*
  • 1Officers College of PAP, Chengdu, China
  • 2School of Exercise and Health, Shanghai University of Sport, Shanghai, China
  • 3Chengdu Xinqiao Primary School, Chengdu, China
  • 4Department of Rehabilitation, West China Hospital Sichuan University Jintang Hospital, Chengdu, China

Objectives: To compare the relative efficacy of different exercise modalities on working memory accuracy and reaction time in healthy children and adolescents.

Methods: A systematic review and network meta-analysis was conducted in accordance with PRISMA guidelines (CRD420251005303). PubMed, Medline, Embase, the Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials, and Web of Science were searched from inception to March 1, 2025. Randomized controlled trials examining the impact of any exercise intervention (e.g., aerobic exercise, dance, high-intensity interval training, sports games, mixed exercise) versus control on working memory accuracy and/or reaction time were eligible. Standardized mean differences (SMDs) with 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were calculated, and a random-effects model was applied to account for between-study heterogeneity. The surface under the cumulative ranking curve (SUCRA) was used to determine the relative ranking of each modality.

Results: Thirty-three studies met inclusion criteria for working memory accuracy, and eight studies contributed data on reaction time. Dance demonstrated the highest SUCRA ranking for accuracy (87.8%), and was significantly superior to control (SMD = 0.67, 95% CI 0.13 to 1.21). Aerobic exercise ranked first for reaction time (93.6%), outperforming control (SMD = −0.40, 95% CI −0.69 to −0.11 for accuracy; SMD = −0.82, 95% CI −1.26 to −0.38 for reaction time compared with mixed exercise). Mixed exercise consistently showed lower rankings for both outcomes.

Conclusion: Distinct exercise modalities differentially affect working memory components in young populations. Dance and aerobic exercise appear most beneficial—dance maximizes accuracy, while aerobic exercise optimizes reaction time. Tailoring exercise interventions to specific cognitive targets may enhance working memory development and inform practical, evidence-based strategies in educational and clinical settings.

Systematic review registration: RD420251005303.

1 Introduction

Working memory refers to the cognitive system responsible for temporarily storing and manipulating information necessary to perform complex cognitive tasks such as reasoning, decision-making, and problem-solving (Diamond, 2013). It serves as a critical component of executive functions, enabling individuals—particularly children and adolescents—to manage cognitive loads, sustain attention, and adapt flexibly to novel or demanding tasks (Cowan, 2022). During childhood and adolescence, working memory undergoes significant developmental changes, directly influencing academic achievement, language development, and overall cognitive capacity. Insufficient development or deficits in working memory during these formative periods can negatively affect academic performance, leading to difficulties in reading comprehension, mathematics, and information processing, and ultimately impacting long-term educational and occupational outcomes (Hessl et al., 2019; Zhang Y. et al., 2023). Consequently, identifying effective strategies for enhancing working memory in children and adolescents is essential for educators, healthcare professionals, and policymakers concerned with promoting optimal cognitive and developmental trajectories.

A variety of factors contribute to variability in working memory performance among children and adolescents. These factors include genetic predispositions, socioeconomic status, educational quality, nutritional status, and physical activity levels. Among these determinants, physical activity has increasingly been recognized as a modifiable and practical intervention with substantial potential for improving cognitive functioning, including working memory (Xu et al., 2024). Exercise interventions can influence working memory through several biological mechanisms, such as increased neurogenesis, enhanced synaptic plasticity, augmented cerebral blood flow, and improved functional connectivity between brain regions involved in executive functioning (Hatch et al., 2021; Kao et al., 2023; Zhu et al., 2021). Given these neurobiological benefits and ease of implementation, exercise interventions represent a promising, non-pharmacological approach not only for immediate cognitive enhancement but also for fostering lifelong healthy habits in children and adolescents.

Given the accumulating evidence highlighting the cognitive benefits of physical activity, numerous studies have explored the impact of different exercise modalities on working memory in healthy youth. For example, aerobic exercise has consistently demonstrated positive effects on cognitive performance, presumably via enhanced cardiovascular fitness and neurotrophic factor release (VANR et al., 2023; Pickersgill et al., 2022). Similarly, HIIT has been associated with improvements in cognitive processing speed and attentional control, potentially due to the induction of greater neurophysiological adaptations in shorter periods (Yuan et al., 2025). Prior systematic reviews and meta-analyses have generally confirmed the positive effects of exercise on cognitive outcomes; however, these analyses have predominantly focused on comparisons between exercise interventions and non-active control groups (De Greeff et al., 2018; Wang et al., 2023). Thus, there remains a critical knowledge gap regarding the comparative efficacy of various exercise modalities in enhancing working memory specifically. Addressing this gap is essential, as identifying optimal exercise modalities could help maximize cognitive benefits through targeted physical activity interventions.

A network meta-analysis (NMA) offers a robust methodological approach to address this knowledge gap by allowing multiple interventions to be compared in a single, integrated analysis. Unlike traditional pairwise meta-analyses, an NMA enables both direct and indirect comparisons among different exercise modalities and control conditions, yielding a comprehensive ranking of effectiveness (Leucht et al., 2016). Building on this advantage, the present study seeks to synthesize high-quality evidence from randomized controlled trials (RCTs) examining the effects of various exercise interventions on the working memory of healthy children and adolescents. By systematically evaluating these interventions and identifying the most promising exercise types, the findings are expected to inform evidence-based recommendations for exercise programs, guide implementation strategies in educational and community settings, and illuminate avenues for future research.

2 Methods

This systematic review and network meta-analysis was conducted in accordance with the 2020 Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) and the PRISMA extension statement for network meta-analyses of health care interventions (Hutton et al., 2015; Page et al., 2021). Ethical approval and informed consent were not required, as this study was a meta-analysis of previously published RCTs. The protocol was prospectively registered (CRD420251005303).

2.1 Data sources and search

Relevant articles were identified by searching PubMed, Medline, Embase, the Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials, and Web of Science from their inception to March 1, 2025. The search strategy combined terms related to “exercise” or “physical activity” (e.g., “aerobic exercise,” “resistance training,” “basketball,” “high-intensity interval training”) and “working memory.” Full details of the database-specific search strategies and term combinations are provided in Supplementary Table 2. In addition to the electronic searches, reference lists of all included studies and bibliographies of systematic reviews published in the past 5 years were screened to identify potentially eligible studies not captured by the initial search. Two independent reviewers (JYW and TX) conducted both title/abstract screening and full-text review in parallel; disagreements were resolved by discussion or by consultation with a third reviewer (XJG).

2.2 Eligibility criteria

Studies were included if they met the following criteria: (1) Participants were healthy children (6–12 years) or adolescents (13–18 years); (2) Any type of exercise served as the intervention; (3) Control groups received no intervention, usual care, knowledge-based education, or an alternative exercise type different from that of the experimental group; (4) Intervention duration exceeded four weeks (Kenney et al., 2012); (5) The primary outcome was working memory (i.e., “updating”), measured either by accuracy (higher accuracy indicating better performance) or reaction time (shorter reaction time indicating better performance); (6) The study was a published RCT; and (7) The article was written in English and published in a peer-reviewed journal.

Studies were excluded if they (1) included participants diagnosed with obesity, depression, or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), as such conditions might introduce bias; (2) assessed only acute effects of exercise; (3) used multifaceted interventions (e.g., exercise combined with nutritional interventions); (4) did not clearly specify the type of exercise used; or (5) did not report means and standard deviations, and the authors did not respond to data requests within the designated time. Two independent reviewers (JYW and TX) screened potentially relevant articles by assessing the title and abstract, followed by full-text examination, to confirm eligibility. Any disagreement at either stage was resolved through discussion or by consultation with a third reviewer (XJG).

2.3 Data extraction

EndNote X9 was used to collate and manage records to avoid duplication. Two reviewers independently extracted information pertaining to publication details (e.g., authors, title, year, journal), participant characteristics (e.g., sample size, age, sex), details of the exercise interventions (duration of a single session, frequency, total intervention duration, exercise modality), and outcome measures (Supplementary Table 3). For effect size calculations, change scores (endpoint minus baseline), standard deviations, and sample sizes were extracted for each group. When change scores or standard deviations were not directly available, they were derived or converted following guidelines in the Cochrane Handbook (Higgins and Green, 2008). If relevant data were incomplete, at least four attempts over 6 weeks were made to contact the corresponding authors.

2.4 Data coding

For the purpose of making structured comparisons of different exercise modalities in the network meta-analysis, exercise interventions were coded into six categories: “aerobic exercise (AE),” “dance (DC),” “mixed exercise (ME),” “high-intensity interval training (HIIT),” “sports games (SG),” and “CON” for controls. Specific definitions and examples of each exercise modality are detailed in Supplementary Table 3.

2.5 Risk of Bias assessment

Two reviewers independently evaluated the risk of bias at the study level using the revised Cochrane risk of bias tool (RoB 2) (Sterne et al., 2019). This tool assesses risk of bias across the following domains: (1) randomization process, (2) deviations from intended interventions, (3) missing outcome data, (4) measurement of the outcome, and (5) selection of the reported results. Any disagreements in judgments were resolved through consultation with a third reviewer.

2.6 Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using Stata software (version 17.0, StataCorp LLC, College Station, TX, United States). A network meta-analysis approach was used to compare the effects of different exercise interventions on working memory outcomes, assuming all included interventions could influence the results. A network plot was generated to illustrate the connections among treatment comparisons, ensuring feasibility of the network structure. Given the anticipated clinical heterogeneity, a random-effects model was applied to account for both within- and between-study variability.

Because the measures of updating varied across studies in terms of scales or units, standardized mean differences (SMD) with 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were calculated. Heterogeneity was evaluated using the I2 statistic, where thresholds of 25, 50, and 75% represent low, moderate, and high heterogeneity, respectively. The “network” and “mvmeta” packages in Stata’s Bayesian framework were applied to facilitate the network meta-analysis. The relative efficacy of each intervention was ranked according to the surface under the cumulative ranking (SUCRA) curves; a higher SUCRA value indicates a more favorable treatment effect.

To assess publication bias, adjusted funnel plots were generated, and Egger’s test was performed; a p-value <0.05 indicated potential publication bias (Chaimani et al., 2013). Prediction interval plots were also used to explore heterogeneity and interpret variability in effect sizes. All tests were two-sided, and p-values <0.05 were considered statistically significant.

3 Results

3.1 Characteristics of the included studies

A total of 6,337 records were initially identified through electronic database searches. After removing 3,948 duplicates, 2,389 articles were screened by title and abstract, and 2,296 were excluded based on relevance. Ultimately, 81 full-text articles were assessed for eligibility, leading to the inclusion of 33 RCTs comprising 20,697 healthy children and adolescents for the systematic review and network meta-analysis (Figure 1; Alesi et al., 2016; Beck et al., 2016; Contreras-Osorio et al., 2022; De Bruijn et al., 2021; Egger et al., 2019; Gentile et al., 2020; Jeon and Ha, 2017; Kamijo et al., 2011; Knatauskaitė et al., 2021; Koutsandréou et al., 2016; Kvalø et al., 2017; Latino et al., 2021; Leahy et al., 2020; Leong et al., 2015; Lind et al., 2018; Lubans et al., 2020; Ludyga et al., 2018; Mavilidi et al., 2020; Meijer et al., 2022; Meijer et al., 2020; Mora-Gonzalez et al., 2024; Park and Jee, 2022; Robinson et al., 2022; Schmidt et al., 2015; St Laurent et al., 2019; Tocci et al., 2022; Torbeyns et al., 2017; Tottori et al., 2019; Van den Berg et al., 2019; Veldman et al., 2020; Wassenaar et al., 2021; Zhang M. et al., 2023; Zinelabidine et al., 2022). Detailed characteristics of the included studies are presented in Supplementary Table 4.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA Flow diagram of the search process for studies.

All included RCTs were published between 2011 and 2023, with a median publication year of 2020. Sample sizes ranged from 33 to 16,017 participants (median 73). The mean age of participants varied between 6.2 and 16.2 years, with a median of 10.0 years. Regarding the exercise interventions, 13 studies employed AE, 4 implemented DC, 6 utilized HIIT, 10 incorporated ME, 10 involved SG, and 32 studies used regular physical education or similar activities as the CON. Single-session durations ranged from 4 to 80 min (median 30 min), the frequency of exercise varied from once weekly to five times per week (median three times weekly), and the total intervention duration ranged from 4 to 40 weeks (median 12 weeks).

3.2 The results of network meta-analysis

3.2.1 Working memory accuracy

A network meta-analysis of 33 RCTs (20,697 children and adolescents) evaluated the impact of various exercise modalities on working memory accuracy. Figure 2A illustrates the direct comparisons and sample size distributions among interventions. According to the surface under the cumulative ranking (SUCRA) probabilities (Figure 3A), the top three interventions for improving working memory accuracy were dance (DC, 87.8%), high-intensity interval training (HIIT, 64.2%), and aerobic exercise (AE, 63.6%), whereas the lowest-ranked modality was the control condition (CON).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Network of eligible treatment comparisons for outcomes. (A) Working memory accuracy, (B) Working memory reaction time.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Ranking of exercise strategies based on probability of their effects for outcomes. (A) Working memory accuracy, (B) Working memory reaction time.

As shown in Table 1, DC (SMD = 0.67, 95% CI 0.13 to 1.21) and AE (SMD = 0.40, 95% CI 0.11 to 0.69) significantly enhanced working memory accuracy compared with CON.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. League table of outcomes.

3.2.2 Working memory reaction time

A network meta-analysis of 8 RCTs (17,940 children and adolescents) evaluated the effects of different exercise modalities on working memory reaction time. Figure 2B depicts the direct comparisons and sample size distributions among interventions. Based on SUCRA (Figure 3B), the top three interventions for reducing working memory reaction time were AE (93.6%), SG (71.4%), and HIIT (45.9%), while mixed exercise (ME) demonstrated the lowest ranking (5.6%).

Table 1 shows that, compared with ME, all other modalities produced a significantly greater reduction in working memory reaction time. Specifically, relative to ME, AE (SMD = −0.82, 95% CI −1.26 to −0.38), SG (SMD = −0.66, 95% CI −1.06 to −0.25), HIIT (SMD = −0.49, 95% CI −0.80 to −0.17), and CON (SMD = −0.46, 95% CI −0.72 to −0.21) were all associated with significantly improved (i.e., shortened) reaction times.

3.3 Risk of Bias and publication Bias

Among the 33 RCTs, 17 were judged as low overall risk of bias, 13 showed some concerns, and 3 were rated as high risk. Regarding the randomization process, 27 trials exhibited low risk, and 6 had some concerns. For deviations from intended interventions, 29 trials had low risk, and 4 had some concerns. In terms of missing outcome data, 24 trials exhibited low risk, 7 had some concerns, and 2 were considered high risk. With respect to outcome measurement, 32 were rated as low risk, and 1 presented some concerns. Finally, for selective reporting, 31 were rated as low risk, 1 showed some concerns, and 1 was deemed high risk (Supplementary Table 5).

Funnel plots were used to examine potential publication bias (Supplementary Table 6). The scatter distribution around the vertical axis showed varying degrees of asymmetry, indicating the possibility of publication bias. In particular, Supplementary Figures 1A,B demonstrated notable asymmetry. However, Egger’s tests for both working memory accuracy and working memory reaction time produced p-values greater than 0.05, suggesting no statistically significant evidence of publication bias in the overall analysis.

4 Discussion

This network meta-analysis integrated data from 33 RCTs involving 20,697 healthy children and adolescents to compare the effects of various exercise interventions on working memory. Several noteworthy findings emerged. First, DC was identified as the most effective intervention for improving working memory accuracy, outperforming other exercise modalities in this domain. Second, AE demonstrated the greatest efficacy in enhancing working memory reaction time and also showed a significant benefit in accuracy compared with the control condition. Third, ME ranked lower for both working memory accuracy and reaction time. Taken together, these observations underscore the nuanced effects of different exercise modalities on distinct facets of working memory and highlight the potential value of carefully selecting specific types of exercise interventions to optimize cognitive outcomes among healthy children and adolescents.

Working memory accuracy is crucial for children and adolescents because it enables them to efficiently process, store, and retrieve information in real time, facilitating a wide range of academic and everyday tasks (Diamond, 2013). Although working memory continues to develop throughout adolescence, many young individuals exhibit variability in their capacity to handle cognitively demanding activities, underscoring the importance of interventions that can strengthen these foundational skills (Xue et al., 2019). In the present study, dance emerged as the top-ranked modality for enhancing working memory accuracy among all examined interventions, reinforcing the value of structured, skill-based physical activities in supporting core cognitive functions. This finding largely aligns with earlier studies that reported moderate-to-strong cognitive gains from dance-related programs but departs from research that highlighted aerobic training as the most potent approach for improving executive functions (Burzynska et al., 2017a; May et al., 2021). The distinctive value of dance for children and adolescents may stem from its unique combination of rhythmic movement, coordination of motor skills, and need for sustained attention, all of which can engage sensory, motor, and cognitive systems simultaneously (Mansfield et al., 2018). From a mechanistic perspective, dance may optimize neural circuitry underlying executive functions by promoting neuroplastic changes through complex motor sequencing, fostering social and emotional engagement, and requiring continuous cognitive monitoring of posture, balance, and spatial awareness (Burzynska et al., 2017b). Consequently, this multifaceted stimulus could lead to more pronounced improvements in working memory accuracy than activities focusing predominantly on endurance, strength, or simpler motor tasks.

In addition to accuracy, working memory reaction time provides critical insight into how swiftly children and adolescents can process and respond to cognitive demands. The present study identified aerobic exercise as the top-ranked modality for improving reaction time, and it also yielded notable benefits for working memory accuracy. This aligns with several previous investigations that have highlighted the cognitive advantages of endurance-based activities, although some meta-analyses have reported mixed findings depending on factors such as intervention duration and participant age (Liang et al., 2021). Aerobic exercise, encompassing activities like running, cycling, and swimming, is often more accessible and feasible for children and adolescents in both formal (e.g., school-based programs) and informal (e.g., community or home-based) settings, thereby enhancing its practical appeal. From a mechanistic perspective, aerobic exercise may facilitate improvements in working memory through augmented cerebral blood flow, release of neurotrophic factors (e.g., brain-derived neurotrophic factor), and enhanced neuroplasticity, all of which contribute to more efficient neural processing (Walsh et al., 2020). The repetitive and rhythmic nature of aerobic activities can also support attentional control and self-regulation, enabling individuals to sustain focus over extended periods. Consequently, by simultaneously promoting neural health and fostering key cognitive processes, aerobic exercise is uniquely positioned to exert robust improvements on both the speed and accuracy dimensions of working memory in children and adolescents.

Interestingly, this study revealed that ME ranked relatively lower for both working memory accuracy and reaction time. Among children and adolescents, ME typically involves combining multiple exercise modalities into a single session, such as initiating with aerobic warm-up, transitioning into resistance exercises like squats and lunges, and culminating in sports games focused on skill development and teamwork. While in theory, such multi-component sessions could offer diverse physiological and cognitive stimuli, the present results suggest that this versatility may inadvertently dilute the specific training effects necessary for enhancing core cognitive processes. One possible explanation is that each component of a mixed session, being relatively short, might not reach the threshold intensity or consistency needed to elicit robust neurocognitive adaptations (Costigan et al., 2016). In addition, rapid transitions between disparate activities could impose competing demands on attentional resources, reducing the sustained engagement crucial for working memory improvement. Moreover, heterogeneous protocols classified under the ME label in existing studies may further contribute to inconsistent outcomes, as the intensity, duration, and sequencing of different activities can vary considerably (Mezcua-Hidalgo et al., 2019). Collectively, these factors may lessen the impact of mixed exercise interventions on working memory in comparison to more targeted programs such as dance or aerobic exercise, emphasizing the importance of structured and purposefully designed physical activity regimens for promoting optimal cognitive benefits in young populations.

These findings bear considerable practical significance for educational programs, public health strategies, and clinical interventions aimed at enhancing cognitive development in children and adolescents. By highlighting the relative effectiveness of distinct exercise modalities—particularly dance and aerobic exercise—this study provides an evidence-based framework to inform physical education curricula, after-school activity programs, and structured exercise interventions. Encouraging youth to engage in targeted exercise routines aligned with these proven modalities may not only enhance working memory functions but also cultivate enduring physical activity habits, thereby fostering both cognitive and physical health benefits over the long term (Wang et al., 2025). Moreover, these insights can help policymakers and practitioners allocate resources efficiently, designing accessible and scalable exercise interventions that align with the developmental needs of diverse populations. Through careful integration of scientifically grounded exercise programs into educational and community settings, stakeholders can bolster working memory development and ultimately support the broader academic and psychosocial well-being of children and adolescents.

This study presents several noteworthy strengths and limitations. First, one of its key strengths lies in the application of a network meta-analysis framework, which allowed for simultaneous comparisons among multiple exercise modalities using both direct and indirect evidence. This comprehensive approach enhances the robustness of the findings by integrating data across a diverse range of randomized controlled trials. Second, the large aggregated sample size of the included studies, encompassing varied geographic locations and participant characteristics, helps bolster the external validity and generalizability of the results. Nevertheless, a few limitations warrant consideration. First, heterogeneity in intervention protocols—for instance, disparities in session duration, frequency, and intensity within the same exercise category—may have influenced effect estimates and constrained the capacity to draw definitive conclusions regarding optimal parameters. Second, variations in outcome measures and assessments of working memory accuracy and reaction time across different trials may introduce measurement bias and limit the direct comparability of results. Finally, although the risk of bias was carefully appraised, potential biases within the primary studies, such as incomplete blinding or selective reporting, cannot be entirely ruled out. These factors should be taken into account when interpreting the findings and underscore the need for more standardized and methodologically rigorous research to elucidate the full impact of distinct exercise interventions on working memory in children and adolescents.

5 Conclusion

This network meta-analysis of 33 RCTs with 20,697 healthy children and adolescents reveals that distinct exercise modalities yield different cognitive benefits. Dance is most effective for enhancing working memory accuracy, whereas aerobic exercise confers the greatest improvement in reaction time. Other interventions, including high-intensity interval training and sports games, demonstrate intermediate benefits, whereas mixed exercise shows comparatively limited effects. These findings underscore the importance of aligning exercise modalities with specific cognitive targets when developing educational and clinical interventions. Tailored exercise programs that match the modality to a particular working memory outcome may optimize cognitive gains and foster long-term healthy habits. Future research should refine intervention protocols, investigate underlying mechanisms, and explore applicability across diverse pediatric populations to promote evidence-based strategies for enhancing working memory and broader cognitive development.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Author contributions

XG: Data curation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. JW: Supervision, Writing – review & editing. JL: Formal analysis, Methodology, Software, Writing – review & editing. TX: Writing – review & editing, Investigation, Methodology, Software. LZ: Methodology, Software, Writing – review & editing, Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1373824/full#supplementary-material

References

Alesi, M., Bianco, A., Luppina, G., Palma, A., and Pepi, A. (2016). Improving Children’s coordinative skills and executive functions: the effects of a football exercise program. Percept. Mot. Skills 122, 27–46. doi: 10.1177/0031512515627527

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, M. M., Lind, R. R., Geertsen, S. S., Ritz, C., Lundbye-Jensen, J., and Wienecke, J. (2016). Motor-enriched learning activities can improve mathematical performance in preadolescent children. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:645. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00645

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Burzynska, A. Z., Finc, K., Taylor, B. K., Knecht, A. M., and Kramer, A. F. (2017a). The dancing brain: structural and functional signatures of expert dance training. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:566. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00566

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Burzynska, A. Z., Jiao, Y., Knecht, A. M., Fanning, J., Awick, E. A., Chen, T., et al. (2017b). White matter integrity declined over 6-months, but dance intervention improved integrity of the fornix of older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:59. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00059

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaimani, A., Higgins, J. P., Mavridis, D., Spyridonos, P., and Salanti, G. (2013). Graphical tools for network meta-analysis in STATA. PLoS One 8:e76654. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0076654

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Contreras-Osorio, F., Guzmán-Guzmán, I. P., Cerda-Vega, E., Chirosa-Ríos, L., Ramírez-Campillo, R., and Campos-Jara, C. (2022). Effects of the type of sports practice on the executive functions of schoolchildren. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:886. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19073886

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Costigan, S. A., Eather, N., Plotnikoff, R. C., Hillman, C. H., and Lubans, D. R. (2016). High-intensity interval training for cognitive and mental health in adolescents. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 48, 1985–1993. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000993

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cowan, N. (2022). Working memory development: a 50-year assessment of research and underlying theories. Cognition 224:105075. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105075

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

De Bruijn, A. G. M., van der Fels, I. M. J., Renken, R. J., Königs, M., Meijer, A., Oosterlaan, J., et al. (2021). Differential effects of long-term aerobic versus cognitively-engaging physical activity on children's visuospatial working memory related brain activation: a cluster RCT. Brain Cogn. 155:105812. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2021.105812

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

de Greeff, J. W., Bosker, R. J., Oosterlaan, J., Visscher, C., and Hartman, E. (2018). Effects of physical activity on executive functions, attention and academic performance in preadolescent children: a meta-analysis. J. Sci. Med. Sport 21, 501–507. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2017.09.595

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Egger, F., Benzing, V., Conzelmann, A., and Schmidt, M. (2019). Boost your brain, while having a break! The effects of long-term cognitively engaging physical activity breaks on children's executive functions and academic achievement. PLoS One 14:e0212482. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0212482

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gentile, A., Boca, S., Şahin, F. N., Güler, Ö., Pajaujiene, S., Indriuniene, V., et al. (2020). The effect of an enriched sport program on Children's executive functions: the ESA program. Front. Psychol. 11:657. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00657

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hatch, L. M., Dring, K. J., Williams, R. A., Sunderland, C., Nevill, M. E., and Cooper, S. B. (2021). Effect of differing durations of high-intensity intermittent activity on cognitive function in adolescents. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:1594. doi: 10.3390/ijerph182111594

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hessl, D., Schweitzer, J. B., Nguyen, D. V., McLennan, Y. A., Johnston, C., Shickman, R., et al. (2019). Cognitive training for children and adolescents with fragile X syndrome: a randomized controlled trial of Cogmed. J. Neurodev. Disord. 11:4. doi: 10.1186/s11689-019-9264-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, J. P., and Green, S. (2008). Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley-Blackwell.

Google Scholar

Hutton, B., Salanti, G., Caldwell, D. M., Chaimani, A., Schmid, C. H., Cameron, C., et al. (2015). The PRISMA extension statement for reporting of systematic reviews incorporating network meta-analyses of health care interventions: checklist and explanations. Ann. Intern. Med. 162, 777–784. doi: 10.7326/M14-2385

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeon, Y. K., and Ha, C. H. (2017). The effect of exercise intensity on brain derived neurotrophic factor and memory in adolescents. Environ. Health Prev. Med. 22:27. doi: 10.1186/s12199-017-0643-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kamijo, K., Pontifex, M. B., O'Leary, K. C., Scudder, M. R., Wu, C. T., Castelli, D. M., et al. (2011). The effects of an afterschool physical activity program on working memory in preadolescent children. Dev. Sci. 14, 1046–1058. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01054.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kao, S. C., Baumgartner, N., Noh, K., Wang, C. H., and Schmitt, S. (2023). Acute effects of intense interval versus aerobic exercise on children's behavioral and neuroelectric measures of inhibitory control. J. Sci. Med. Sport 26, 316–321. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2023.05.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kenney, W. L., Kenney, W. L., and Kenney, W. L. (2012). Physiology of sport and exercise W/web study guide. 5th Edn. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Google Scholar

Knatauskaitė, J., Pukėnas, K., Trinkūnienė, L., and Budde, H. (2021). A randomized controlled trial on the influence of two types of exercise training vs control on visuospatial processing and mathematical skills: the role of cortisol. Physiol. Behav. 229:113213. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.113213

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koutsandréou, F., Wegner, M., Niemann, C., and Budde, H. (2016). Effects of motor versus cardiovascular exercise training on Children's working memory. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 48, 1144–1152. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000869

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kvalø, S. E., Bru, E., Brønnick, K., and Dyrstad, S. M. (2017). Does increased physical activity in school affect children's executive function and aerobic fitness? Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 27, 1833–1841. doi: 10.1111/sms.12856

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Latino, F., Cataldi, S., and Fischetti, F. (2021). Effects of a coordinative ability training program on Adolescents' cognitive functioning. Front. Psychol. 12:620440. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.620440

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leahy, A. A., Michels, M. F. I., Eather, N., Hillman, C. H., Shigeta, T. T., Lubans, D. R., et al. (2020). Feasibility of test administration and preliminary findings for cognitive control in the burn 2 learn pilot randomised controlled trial. J. Sports Sci. 38, 1708–1716. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2020.1756673

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leong, I. T., Moghadam, S., and Hashim, H. A. (2015). Aggregated effects of combining daily milk consumption and aerobic exercise on short-term memory and sustained attention among female students. Percept. Mot. Skills 120, 57–66. doi: 10.2466/22.06.PMS.120v11x3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leucht, S., Chaimani, A., Cipriani, A. S., Davis, J. M., Furukawa, T. A., and Salanti, G. (2016). Network meta-analyses should be the highest level of evidence in treatment guidelines. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 266, 477–480. doi: 10.1007/s00406-016-0715-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liang, X., Li, R., Wong, S. H. S., Sum, R. K. W., and Sit, C. H. P. (2021). The impact of exercise interventions concerning executive functions of children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactive disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 18:68. doi: 10.1186/s12966-021-01135-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lind, R. R., Geertsen, S. S., Ørntoft, C., Madsen, M., Larsen, M. N., Dvorak, J., et al. (2018). Improved cognitive performance in preadolescent Danish children after the school-based physical activity programme "FIFA 11 for health" for Europe - a cluster-randomised controlled trial. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 18, 130–139. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2017.1394369

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lubans, D. R., Smith, J. J., Eather, N., Leahy, A. A., Morgan, P. J., Lonsdale, C., et al. (2020). Time-efficient intervention to improve older adolescents' cardiorespiratory fitness: findings from the 'Burn 2 Learn' cluster randomised controlled trial. Br. J. Sports Med. 55, 751–758. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2020-103277

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ludyga, S., Gerber, M., Kamijo, K., Brand, S., and Pühse, U. (2018). The effects of a school-based exercise program on neurophysiological indices of working memory operations in adolescents. J. Sci. Med. Sport 21, 833–838. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2018.01.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mansfield, L., Kay, T., Meads, C., Grigsby-Duffy, L., Lane, J., John, A., et al. (2018). Sport and dance interventions for healthy young people (15-24 years) to promote subjective well-being: a systematic review. BMJ Open 8:e020959. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2017-020959

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mavilidi, M. F., Drew, R., Morgan, P. J., Lubans, D. R., Schmidt, M., and Riley, N. (2020). Effects of different types of classroom physical activity breaks on children's on-task behaviour, academic achievement and cognition. Acta Paediatr. 109, 158–165. doi: 10.1111/apa.14892

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

May, T., Chan, E. S., Lindor, E., McGinley, J., Skouteris, H., Austin, D., et al. (2021). Physical, cognitive, psychological and social effects of dance in children with disabilities: systematic review and meta-analysis. Disabil. Rehabil. 43, 13–26. doi: 10.1080/09638288.2019.1615139

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Meijer, A., Königs, M., Pouwels, P. J. W., Smith, J., Visscher, C., Bosker, R. J., et al. (2022). Effects of aerobic versus cognitively demanding exercise interventions on brain structure and function in healthy children-results from a cluster randomized controlled trial. Psychophysiology 59:e14034. doi: 10.1111/psyp.14034

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Meijer, A., Königs, M., van der Fels, I. M. J., Visscher, C., Bosker, R. J., Hartman, E., et al. (2020). The effects of aerobic versus cognitively demanding exercise interventions on executive functioning in school-aged children: a cluster-randomized controlled trial. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 43, 1–13. doi: 10.1123/jsep.2020-0034

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mezcua-Hidalgo, A., Ruiz-Ariza, A., Suárez-Manzano, S., and Martínez-López, E. J. (2019). 48-hour effects of monitored cooperative high-intensity interval training on adolescent cognitive functioning. Percept. Mot. Skills 126, 202–222. doi: 10.1177/0031512518825197

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mora-Gonzalez, J., Esteban-Cornejo, I., Solis-Urra, P., Rodriguez-Ayllon, M., Cadenas-Sanchez, C., Hillman, C. H., et al. (2024). The effects of an exercise intervention on neuroelectric activity and executive function in children with overweight/obesity: the ActiveBrains randomized controlled trial. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 34:e14486. doi: 10.1111/sms.14486

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Page, M. J., McKenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., et al. (2021). The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 372:n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, S. K., and Jee, Y. S. (2022). Effects of rhythm step training on physical and cognitive functions in adolescents: a prospective randomized controlled trial. Healthcare 10:712. doi: 10.3390/healthcare10040712

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pickersgill, J. W., Turco, C. V., Ramdeo, K., Rehsi, R. S., Foglia, S. D., and Nelson, A. J. (2022). The combined influences of exercise, diet and sleep on neuroplasticity. Front. Psychol. 13:831819. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.831819

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, K. J., Lubans, D. R., Mavilidi, M. F., Hillman, C. H., Benzing, V., Valkenborghs, S. R., et al. (2022). Effects of classroom-based resistance training with and without cognitive training on Adolescents' cognitive function, on-task behavior, and muscular fitness. Front. Psychol. 13:811534. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.811534

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, M., Jäger, K., Egger, F., Roebers, C. M., and Conzelmann, A. (2015). Cognitively engaging chronic physical activity, but not aerobic exercise, affects executive functions in primary school children: a group-randomized controlled trial. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 37, 575–591. doi: 10.1123/jsep.2015-0069

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

St Laurent, C. W., Burkart, S., and Alhassan, S. (2019). Feasibility, acceptability, and preliminary efficacy of a recess-based fitness intervention in elementary school children. Int. J. Exerc. Sci. 12, 1225–1243. doi: 10.70252/VOII8947

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sterne, J. A. C., Savović, J., Page, M. J., Elbers, R. G., Blencowe, N. S., Boutron, I., et al. (2019). RoB 2: a revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ 366:l4898. doi: 10.1136/bmj.l4898

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tocci, N., Scibinetti, P., Mazzoli, E., Mavilidi, M. F., Masci, I., Schmidt, M., et al. (2022). Giving ideas some legs or legs some ideas? Children's motor creativity is enhanced by physical activity enrichment: direct and mediated paths. Front. Psychol. 13:806065. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.806065

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Torbeyns, T., de Geus, B., Bailey, S., Decroix, L., Van Cutsem, J., De Pauw, K., et al. (2017). Bike desks in the classroom: energy expenditure, physical health, cognitive performance, brain functioning, and academic performance. J. Phys. Act. Health 14, 429–439. doi: 10.1123/jpah.2016-0224

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tottori, N., Morita, N., Ueta, K., and Fujita, S. (2019). Effects of high intensity interval training on executive function in children aged 8-12 years. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 16:127. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16214127

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van den Berg, V., Saliasi, E., de Groot, R. H. M., Chinapaw, M. J. M., and Singh, A. S. (2019). Improving cognitive performance of 9-12 years old children: just dance? A randomized controlled trial. Front. Psychol. 10:174. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00174

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

VANR, S. M., Tempest, G. D., Piccirilli, A., Ma, Q., and Reiss, A. L. (2023). Aerobic exercise, cognitive performance, and brain activity in adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 55, 1445–1455. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000003159

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Veldman, S. L. C., Jones, R. A., Stanley, R. M., Cliff, D. P., Vella, S. A., Howard, S. J., et al. (2020). Promoting physical activity and executive functions among children: a cluster randomized controlled trial of an after-school program in Australia. J. Phys. Act. Health 17, 940–946. doi: 10.1123/jpah.2019-0381

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Walsh, E. I., Smith, L., Northey, J., Rattray, B., and Cherbuin, N. (2020). Towards an understanding of the physical activity-BDNF-cognition triumvirate: a review of associations and dosage. Ageing Res. Rev. 60:101044. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2020.101044

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J., Yang, Y., Wu, J., Xie, T., Guo, X., Yuan, X., et al. (2025). Association between moderate-to-vigorous physical activity trajectories and academic achievement in Chinese primary school children: a 3-year longitudinal study. BMC Public Health 25:341. doi: 10.1186/s12889-025-21487-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, H., Yang, Y., Xu, J., Niu, L., Zhang, Y., Shi, J., et al. (2023). Meta-analysis on the effects of moderate-intensity exercise intervention on executive functioning in children. PLoS One 18:e0279846. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0279846

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wassenaar, T. M., Wheatley, C. M., Beale, N., Nichols, T., Salvan, P., Meaney, A., et al. (2021). The effect of a one-year vigorous physical activity intervention on fitness, cognitive performance and mental health in young adolescents: the fit to study cluster randomised controlled trial. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 18:47. doi: 10.1186/s12966-021-01113-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, X., Zhao, H., Song, Y., Cai, H., Zhao, W., Tang, J., et al. (2024). Molecular mechanisms underlying the neural correlates of working memory. BMC Biol. 22:238. doi: 10.1186/s12915-024-02039-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Xue, Y., Yang, Y., and Huang, T. (2019). Effects of chronic exercise interventions on executive function among children and adolescents: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Br. J. Sports Med. 53, 1397–1404. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2018-099825

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuan, S., Lin, L., Liu, L., Zhang, X., and Gu, Q. (2025). A comparison of the acute effects of high intensity interval training and moderate intensity continuous training on working memory and emotional state in adolescent women with subthreshold depression. Front. Public Health 13:1505959. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2025.1505959

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, M., Garnier, H., Qian, G., and Li, S. (2023). Effect of 11 weeks of physical exercise on physical fitness and executive functions in children. Children (Basel, Switzerland) 10:485. doi: 10.3390/children10030485

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Y., Ji, W., Jiang, F., Wu, F., Li, G., Hu, Y., et al. (2023). Associations among body mass index, working memory performance, gray matter volume, and brain activation in healthy children. Cereb. Cortex 33, 6335–6344. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhac507

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, Y., Sun, F., Chiu, M. M., and Siu, A. Y. (2021). Effects of high-intensity interval exercise and moderate-intensity continuous exercise on executive function of healthy young males. Physiol. Behav. 239:113505. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2021.113505

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zinelabidine, K., Elghoul, Y., Jouira, G., and Sahli, S. (2022). The effect of an 8-week aerobic dance program on executive function in children. Percept. Mot. Skills 129, 153–175. doi: 10.1177/00315125211058001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: children, adolescents, exercise, working memory, network meta-analysis, randomized controlled trials

Citation: Guo X, Wang J, Liang J, Xie T and Zhang L (2025) Effects of different long-term exercise interventions on working memory in children and adolescents: a network meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 16:1373824. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1373824

Received: 12 March 2024; Accepted: 19 March 2025;
Published: 10 April 2025.

Edited by:

Pedro Alexandre Duarte-Mendes, Polytechnic Institute of Castelo Branco, Portugal

Reviewed by:

Miguel Jacinto, Polytechnic Institute of Leiria, Portugal
Fernanda M. Silva, University of Coimbra, Portugal

Copyright © 2025 Guo, Wang, Liang, Xie and Zhang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lin Zhang, emwxNzM2MTA2NzQzMkAxNjMuY29t

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Research integrity at Frontiers

94% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good

Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.


Find out more