Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 22 October 2024
Sec. Cognition

Proactive control for conflict resolution is intact in subclinical obsessive-compulsive individuals

  • 1Padua Neuroscience Center, University of Padua, Padua, Italy
  • 2Department of Neuroscience, University of Padua, Padua, Italy
  • 3Department of General Psychology, University of Padua, Padua, Italy
  • 4Department of Developmental Psychology and Socialization, University of Padua, Padua, Italy

Background: Obsessive-compulsive (OC) traits (i.e., tendency to implement stereotyped behaviors to avoid negative consequences) are transversally observed in psychiatric disorders largely differing in terms of clinical manifestations and etiopathogenesis. Interestingly, OC traits were also extensively found in the prodromal phases of the full-blown psychopathology and in healthy relatives of affected individuals. Moreover, OC traits were found to be associated—and possibly underlain by—cognitive control impairments. Nonetheless, the role of such interplay in the onset of OC disorders is yet to be understood. We hypothesized that OC traits are associated with abnormalities in proactively implement cognitive control for solving conflict.

Methods: We administered healthy individuals (n = 104) with the perifoveal spatial Stroop task to measure their ability of solving conflict in a proactive fashion, and with Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory (OCI) to stratify population according to the severity of OC traits.

Results: Analysis of response times by means of Linear Mixed-effect models revealed that proactive control performance was not associated with and the severity of OC traits. Furthermore, an equivalence test (Two One-Sided Test) revealed that the association between OCI scores and task performance was equivalent to zero.

Conclusion: These results suggest that the interplay between OC traits and proactive control abnormalities might not contribute to the development of OC-related disorders. Therefore, the role of other cognitive endophenotypes should be scrutinized for exploiting alternative prevention and intervention strategies.

Introduction

Obsessive-compulsive (OC) traits are broadly described as the tendency to implement stereotyped and repetitive behaviors and thoughts lacking any adaptive functions to avoid perceived negative consequences, accompanied by a subjective feeling of urgency (Figee et al., 2013; Fineberg et al., 2013; Luigjes et al., 2019). Notably, obsessive-compulsive (OC) symptoms and their cognitive and behavioral correlates are trans-diagnostically observed in several mental disorders, fitting in clinical pictures largely differing in terms of their nature, clinical manifestations and underlying causes (Fineberg et al., 2016; Yilmaz et al., 2020; Mandelli et al., 2020; Monaco et al., 2020; Peterson et al., 2022; Zadegan et al., 2023). Interestingly, subclinical OC traits are also extensively present in the healthy population with a non-negligible prevalence and incidence (Fullana et al., 2010; Chamberlain et al., 2018; Tiego et al., 2019). Thus, they negatively impact affected populations—either clinical or subclinical—in terms of quality of life and social functioning, entailing huge economic costs.

Given the ubiquitous nature of OC traits, they might play a key role in the etiopathogenesis of several OC-related mental disorders, possibly contributing to their onset and chronicity (Fontenelle et al., 2020; Snorrason et al., 2021). Indeed, OC traits might represent an important risk factor for the development of OC-related manifestations and disorders, especially when high-risk individuals encounter environmental stressors (Destrée et al., 2023). For instance, during COVID-19 pandemic a widespread and aspecific increase in OC behaviors has been observed, even in healthy individuals (Albertella et al., 2021; Dong et al., 2020; Elhai et al., 2020; Ornell et al., 2021; Tanir et al., 2020). Despite rigorous efforts for investigating cognitive and neurobiological processes underlying the onset and maintenance of OC traits have been made, evidence is still mixed and often inconclusive (for reviews, see Ferreira et al., 2020; Hezel and McNally, 2016).

From the cognitive viewpoint, individuals with OC traits were found to share alterations in functions falling under the cognitive control domain, broadly defined as the ability to coordinate information processing and actions complying with behavioral goals (Botvinick et al., 2001; Cohen, 2017). Specifically, cognitive control sub-functions thought to play a role in OC disorders include behavioral and cognitive inhibition, cognitive flexibility, conflict resolution and decision making. Indeed, the neuropsychological profile of patients with OC disorders was found to be predominantly characterized by impairments in cognitive flexibility, interference resolution, inhibitory control and decision-making (Ferreira et al., 2020; Hezel and McNally, 2016; Nisticò et al., 2021; Norman et al., 2019; Perera et al., 2023; Snyder et al., 2015; Shin et al., 2014). Specifically, moderate impairments of conflict resolution (i.e., ability of processing task-relevant information when perceiving a stimulus and/or selecting a response, simultaneously ignoring interfering task-irrelevant information and/or inhibiting prepotent responses; Nee et al., 2007, Tafuro et al., 2019), along with neurofunctional alterations of brain areas involved in conflict detection and resolution, have been consistently reported (see Ahmari and Rauch, 2022; Martínez-Esparza et al., 2021, for an overview). Notably, similar impairments have been observed both in healthy relatives of patients with OC disorder and in patients during the premorbid phase (Bora, 2020; Cavedini et al., 2010; Chamberlain et al., 2007). Additionally, cognitive control deficits have been also found in other compulsive-impulsive populations, like those affected by social network addiction (Müller et al., 2021), internet addiction (Ioannidis et al., 2019), internet gaming addiction (Yao et al., 2022; Zhou et al., 2012), binge eating (Córdova et al., 2017), and compulsive shopping (Lindheimer et al., 2020). Interestingly, cognitive control impairments are hypothesized to be trans-diagnostically impaired in several mental disorders (McTeague et al., 2016).

Therefore, deficits in cognitive control might not just be one of the features associated with OC traits. Rather, they might be at the core of several OC phenomena typically falling under other domains (e.g., subjective experience, mental representations and, eventually, behavior).

Concerning cognitive control, the dual mechanism of control (DMC) hypothesis (Braver, 2012) distinguishes between two forms of control: proactive and reactive. Specifically, proactive control is a tonic process underlying the ability to actively sustain task-relevant representations in working memory before the occurrence of conflicting information, biasing perception and action accordingly. This control mode is implemented in an anticipatory fashion to prioritize the task-relevant information over the task-irrelevant one. That is, by representing the abstract structure of the task (e.g., overall proportion of congruent stimuli), we can funnel in advance our cognitive resources and, eventually, adjust our behavior accordingly (i.e., I expect congruent stimuli to occur with a specific probability, therefore I would adjust cognitive resources and behavior according to that specific probability of encountering conflicting information). In contrast, reactive control is a phasic process underlying the ability to transiently retrieve and reactivate task-relevant representations only after conflict has been detected. That is, if specific conflict-eliciting events occur with different structured probabilities, each event-specific representation and, eventually, behavior would be affected accordingly. Nonetheless, if different probabilities are associated with specific events, the behavior can be adjusted only after stimulus has occurred. In other words, it is a late correction mechanism triggered by conflict detection. Evidence suggests that these two mechanisms work independently and simultaneously (Mäki-Marttunen et al., 2019) and are underlain by dissociable neurofunctional substrates (Braver et al., 2007; Burgess and Braver, 2010; Cohen and Cavanagh, 2011; Cavanagh and Frank, 2014; De Pisapia and Braver, 2006; West and Bailey, 2012, but see Viviani et al., 2023c).

Given the literature about typical OC phenomena (e.g., pathological doubt and uncertainty; Chiang and Purdon, 2023; Lunn et al., 2023; Pinciotti et al., 2021; Vazard, 2021; Williams and Levinson, 2021), some OC manifestations might arise from abnormalities in judging when, how and to what extent implementing control processes or actions for obtaining a specific outcome (i.e., proactive control). In other words, the pathological uncertainty observed in OCD might derive from deficits in accurately representing the structure of the environment and/or in using representations to implement adaptive behaviors accordingly. In turn, this might reflect an impairment in forming, adapting and/or maintaining relevant representations according to environmental demands. This might lead to questioning one’s own representations (reflected in pathological doubt and uncertainty), eventually hindering the proper implementation of goal-directed processes and behaviors. These hypotheses are supported by recent Bayesian models describing OC phenomena as abnormalities in representing, correcting and adaptively using action-outcome state transitions (see Fradkin et al., 2020a, 2020b; Sharp et al., 2021, for a detailed description). Nonetheless, these studies investigated hypotheses by means of in-silico simulations, and empirical confirmation is still lacking or controversial (e.g., Fruehauf et al., 2021).

Thus, the present study is aimed at investigating how OC traits are associated with alterations of proactive control when implemented for conflict resolution. Specifically, we hypothesized that OC traits might be associated with impairments in representing the probabilistic context reflecting the environmental requirements for proactive control. Notably, a deep understanding of how cognitive control impairments and OC phenomena are related to each other could be relevant for developing and testing innovative prevention and/or intervention strategies, eventually improving the quality of life of OC populations.

To this aim, we stratified our sample according to the severity of their OC traits. Afterwards, we administered the participants with the Stroop task (MacLeod, 1991; Stroop, 1935) to assess conflict resolution. Specifically, we used the perifoveal spatial Stroop task developed by Viviani et al. (2023a). Notably, the spatial Stroop tasks have been already used successfully to detect the neurofunctional substrates of proactive control (Tafuro et al., 2019; Visalli et al., 2023; Viviani et al., 2023a, 2023b). Importantly, the demands for proactive control were systematically manipulated by changing the probabilistic context (for further details, see Methods section). Consequently, we expected the Stroop effect to vary consistently with proactive control requirements. That is, in contexts characterized by higher control demands, more cognitive resources are recruited and, therefore, the Stroop effect should be less pronounced, compared to contexts characterized by lower control demands.

Additionally, we used a novel approach that allowed us to further manipulate proactive control on a trial-by-trial basis (see Viviani et al., 2023a, 2023b, 2023c). Interestingly, this approach was proven to be effective in overcoming design-level limitations in simultaneously manipulating different variables and in minimizing the effects of low-level confounds (see Viviani et al., 2023a, 2023c for a detailed discussion).

Finally, concerning specific hypotheses, we expect this LWPC-dependent modulation of the Stroop effect to be reduced in participants with more severe OC traits, given the hypothesized impairment in proactive control.

Materials and methods

Participants

Participants were recruited among psychology students at the University of Padova (Padova, Italy), through advertisements on social media and through snowball sampling. Both Italian and English-speaking participants were included. A total of 242 participants completed the screening phase. During this phase, participants were administered online versions of the Obsessive Compulsive Inventory (OCI) and the Depression-Anxiety-Stress Scale (DASS-21, detailed below) to measure the severity of obsessive compulsive traits and possible comorbid symptoms (i.e., depression, anxiety, stress). Participants were required to subscribe the informed consent to participate in the study, which fulfills the ethics standards of the 2013 Declaration of Helsinki for human studies. Subsequently, we selected participants according to their OCI scores, following a stratified sampling procedure. Specifically, we divided the OCI scores into equal-width intervals and we selected participants in order to obtain homogeneous subsamples—in terms of size and variance—for each layer while sampling the full continuum of compulsive traits.

After screening, a total of 104 participants (70 females, 33 males, 1 other; mean age = 26.1 years, SD = 6.6; 83 right-handed, 73 Italian native speakers) met inclusion criteria (i.e., no current or past history of psychiatric or neurological disorders, no recent assumption of alcohol or drugs possibly affecting cognitive functions) and were selected to perform the task. Descriptive statistics about the distribution of OCI and DASS-21 scores are reported in Table 1. At the moment of data collection, the project had been already approved by the Ethical Committee for the Psychological Research of the University of Padova (approved protocol reference number: 4631) and completed a behavioral task online between April 2022 and October 2022.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory (OCI) and the Depression-Anxiety-Stress Scale (DASS-21) scores.

To determine the minimum sample size, we performed an a priori power analysis in G*Power (Erdfelder et al., 1996) for the interaction of main theoretical interest regarding the LWPC-dependent proactive control modulation of the Stroop effect, with a 2 (Congruency: Congruent vs. Incongruent) × 3 (block-level LWPC: 30, 50, and 70%) repeated measure ANOVA, assuming an alpha of 0.05, a power of 0.80 and correlation between repeated measures of 0.75. The power analysis revealed that a sample size of 82 was enough to detect a significant interaction with a SESOI of Cohen’s d = 0.2. We also performed a similar power analysis using GLIMMPSE (Kreidler et al., 2013), which allowed us to compute the statistical power for a linear mixed-effect (LMM) model more similar to that we used in our main analysis, that is, one with the same Congruency and LWPC factors, but also including a categorical covariate (low- vs. high-OCI) to model the OCI modulation of the Congruency by LWPC interaction; This analysis also revealed a required sample size of 82. It should also be noted that LMMs tend to provide higher power than other more standard statistical approaches (de Melo et al., 2022; Yang et al., 2014).

Another a priori power analysis was conducted for the OCI modulation of the participants’ proactive control and interference resolution abilities, which is assessed by the correlation between participants’ OCI scores and their Congruency and LWPC by Congruency effects estimated by the LMM, with a smallest effect size of interest (SESOI) of r = 0.31623, corresponding to 10% of explained variance, assuming an alpha of 0.05 and a power of 0.80. This analysis revealed that 58 participants were needed. Finally, an a priori power analysis for the equivalence test (Two One-Sided Test, TOST; Lakens, 2017; Lakens et al., 2020; Meyners, 2012; testing for the practical absence of a significant correlation between participants’ OCI and their proactive control ability, see below), with the same assumed alpha and power levels, revealed a minimum sample size of 83 participants.

Of note, we decided to recruit a larger sample than that indicated by these power analyses, so as to be able to estimate the effects of interest with greater precision and detect them with greater statistical power. Indeed, our final sample size of 104 participants allowed us to find a significant LWPC by Congruency effect, a significant correlation between participants’ OCI scores and their cognitive control abilities, and a significant TOST for this correlation, with the above-mentioned smallest effect sizes of interest, with a power >0.89, 0.95, and 0.90, respectively.

Materials and procedure

Obsessive compulsive inventory (OCI)

The Obsessive Compulsive Inventory (OCI, Foa et al., 1998) is a multidimensional self-report questionnaire developed to quantify obsessive-compulsive symptoms. It consists of 42 items divided into seven subscales, specifically: (1) Washing (eight items, e.g., “I think contact with bodily secretions [perspiration, saliva, blood, urine, etc.] may contaminate my clothes or somehow harm me.”); (2) Checking (nine items, e.g., “I repeatedly check doors, windows, drawers etc.”), (3) Doubting (three items, e.g., “I ask people to repeat things to me several times, even though I understood them the first time.”), (4) Ordering (five items, e.g., “I need things to be arranged in a particular order.”), (5) Obsessing (eight items, e.g., “I have thoughts that I might want to harm myself or others.”), (6) Hoarding (three items, “I collect things I do not need.”), (7) Mental neutralizing (six items, “I need to pray to cancel bad thoughts or feelings.”). Each item is rated according to a 5-point Likert scale (0 = not at all; 4 = extremely). The participant is asked to answer according to the distress experienced during the past month with reference to the described phenomenon. The OCI scale and its subscales have overall good reliability (Cronbach’s α; OCI tot: 0.86–0.95, OCI scales: >0.70), good test–retest reliability (0.68 > rt-t > 0.90) and good convergent and divergent validity (Foa et al., 1998). The questionnaire was administered in English (Foa et al., 1998) to non-Italian native speakers and in Italian (Sica et al., 2009) to Italian native speakers.

Depression-anxiety-stress scale (DASS-21)

The Depression-Anxiety-Stress Scale (DASS-21; Antony et al., 1998; Henry and Crawford, 2005) is a short 21-item form of the original DASS scale (Lovibond and Lovibond, 1995). It is designed at measuring the severity of anxious-depressive and stress-related symptoms, either in clinical or in nonclinical samples. Specifically, we administered the DASS-21 to control for confounding effects of depression, anxiety and/or stress on possible OCI effects. The DASS-21 includes three subscales (seven item each), specifically: (1) Depression (dysphoria, hopelessness, devaluation of life, self-deprecation, lack of interest/involvement, anhedonia and inertia, e.g., “I felt that I had nothing to look forward to”); (2) Anxiety (autonomic arousal, skeletal muscle effects, situational anxiety, and subjective experience of anxious affect; e.g., “I felt I was close to panic”) and (3) Stress (chronic non-specific arousal; e.g., “I found myself getting agitated”). The participant is required to rate how much the statement applies to himself/herself referring to the past week. The answer is given through a 4-point Likert scale (0 = did not apply to me at all; 3 = applied to me very much or most of the time). Both total and partial scores have good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α: 0.88 for the Depression scale; 0.82 for the Anxiety scale; 0.90 for the Stress scale, and 0.93 for the total score), and good factorial solution (eigenvalues 9.07, 2.89, and 1.23, respectively, accounting for 67% of the variance). The questionnaire was administered in English and in Italian (Bottesi et al., 2015) to Italian native speakers.

Perifoveal spatial Stroop task

After screening, the selected participants were administered online with an adapted version of the perifoveal spatial Stroop task (Viviani et al., 2023a). The probabilistic context was manipulated as illustrated below. The experiment was programmed using Psytoolkit (Stoet, 2010, 2017). Participants were given a link and were asked to complete the task on their own, in a quiet and silent environment.

The task was executed in full-screen mode. Stimuli were presented on a 1,024 × 768 pixels screen colored with a light sky-blue background (RGB: 189, 215, 238). At the beginning of each trial, a fixation stimulus—consisting of a partial black square outline (94 × 94 pixels) enclosing a black fixation cross (30 × 30 pixels)—was presented. Previously, participants had been instructed to fixate the cross and to keep their gaze at that screen position. After 500 ms, a target stimulus consisting of a thick arrow pointing to one of four possible directions (upper-left, upper-right, lower-left or lower-right) appeared in one of the four positions outlined by the elements characterizing the previously-described fixation stimulus (i.e., upper-left, upper-right, lower-left, or lower-right corner). Notably, the arrow direction could be consistent (i.e., congruent) or inconsistent (i.e., incongruent) with the position (i.e., the corner) at which it appeared. Participants were instructed to respond according to the direction pointed by the arrow, independently from its position on the screen. Therefore, the task required the participant to focus on the direction (task-relevant feature), while ignoring the position (task-irrelevant feature) of the stimulus to respond correctly. The target lasted until the participant’s response, up to a maximum of 2,000 ms. Participants were instructed to provide their response by pressing one of four keys (D, C, K or M) with their fingers (left middle, left index, right middle or right index finger, respectively) on their computer keyboard. Specifically, the spatial arrangement of the keys reflected the spatial arrangement of both task-relevant (direction) and task-irrelevant (position) features of the stimuli. This choice was made to ensure a complete Stroop effect, including response conflict, by establishing a dimensional overlap between all the task dimensions (i.e., relevant/irrelevant stimulus features and response dimensions), eventually creating a type-eight ensemble (Kornblum, 1992; Viviani et al., 2023b). Participants were instructed to position their fingers on the corresponding buttons before starting each block and to hold them in place until the end of the block. Thereafter, a blank screen with a light sky-blue background was presented for 500 ms. Subsequently, a new trial started.

Intrinsic requirements for proactive control (Gonthier et al., 2016) were operationalized and manipulated by systematically varying the proportion of congruent trials in a list-wide fashion (list-wide proportion congruency; LWPC; see Figure 1). In other words, we changed the probabilistic context, eventually creating different levels of proactive control demands (Bugg and Hutchison, 2013; Gonthier et al., 2016; Visalli et al., 2023). Indeed, when a block is characterized by a higher proportion of congruent trials (high LWPC), conflict occurs less frequently, therefore the demand for implementing proactive control is lower. In contrast, when the proportion of congruent trials is lower (low LWPC), conflict occurs (and must be solved) more frequently. In other words, control demands are higher, thus more cognitive resources are typically funneled for implementing proactive control (Bugg and Hutchison, 2013; Gonthier et al., 2016; Visalli et al., 2023).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental paradigm illustrating a congruent (A) and incongruent (B) trial, respectively. Participants were asked to answer according to the direction of the arrow and to ignore its position within the square surrounding the fixation cross. The response was given by pressing one of four keys (D, C, K, M) on a canonical computer keyboard, which were spatially arranged to overlap with the four stimulus directions and positions. Specifically, participants were asked to respond with the index (C, M) or middle (D, K) finger of the left (D, C) or right (K, M) hand according to the arrow direction. (C) Experimental manipulation. When the proportion of congruent trials is lower (low LWPC), the elicited control is higher and the Stroop effect (difference in terms of response times between congruent and incongruent trials, gray dashed line) is less pronounced. Vice-versa, when the proportion of congruent trials is higher (high LWPC), the elicited control is lower and the Stroop effect is more pronounced. Con, Congruent (blue solid line); Inc, Incongruent (orange solid line); LWPC, list-wide proportion congruency; RT, response times.

Consequently, the Stroop effect is expected to be reduced in contexts where LWPC is lower (higher control demands) compared to contexts in which the latter is higher (lower control demands). Specifically, we assumed that in blocks characterized by a higher probability of incurring in incongruent trials (low LWPC), cognitive control implemented in an anticipatory fashion (i.e., proactive control) should be facilitated. On the other hand, when the list-wide probability of incurring in incongruent trials is low (high LWPC), it would be inconvenient for the system to implement proactive control, as the latter is a really onerous process in terms of cognitive resources. Therefore, the implementation of proactive control in this case would be attenuated or hindered. This approach was proven to be more effective for manipulating requirements proactive control than traditional ones e.g., (see Braem et al., 2019; Visalli et al., 2023; Viviani et al., 2023c).

Concerning the trial-list design, the task was divided into five or six blocks per condition (block-level LWPC: 70, 50, 30%), for a total of 680 trials (17 40-trial blocks). List-wide probabilities were presented in a balanced order. The order of trials was pseudorandomized within each block using the Mix software (van Casteren and Davis, 2006) for avoiding first-order priming effects. A total of 1,000 trial-lists were generated. Then, trial-level LWPC and probability of response given a stimulus (PRS) were computed for each trial-list by applying the Hierarchical Gaussian Filter (for a detailed description, see Mathys et al., 2011; Visalli et al., 2023). In short, the HGF allows to update the expected probability of the occurrence of an event, that is, the probability of target congruency for trial-level LPWC (hereafter, continuous LWPC, cLWPC) according to the past history of their specific occurrences, in a trial-wise fashion. Notably, since participants were blind to the probabilistic structure of the task, we assumed that the estimated probability of congruency (PC) evolved within the block and substantially differed between the initial and the final trials of the same block, regardless of its discrete PC value. Therefore, the HGF allowed us to create a continuous PC, an index representing the trial-wise probability of target congruency. A graphical representation of the HGF estimation is shown in Figure 2.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Depiction of the experimental manipulation of the list-wide proportion of congruency (LWPC), both before (orange solid line, block-wise) and after (blue line, trial-wise) applying the Hierarchical Gaussian Filter (Mathys et al., 2011). P(C), proportion of congruency.

Moreover, we used the HGF to compute trial-wise probabilities of PRS and of the stimulus location and the response, which were included as predictors of non-interest in the statistical model (see Statistical analyses). Specifically, we computed PRS to account for the effect of contingency on performance, as previous studies demonstrated that PRS partially overlaps with the effect of interest, eventually masking it (see” contingency hypothesis,” Schmidt, 2019; Schmidt and Besner, 2008). To this end, the trial-list with the lowest correlation between trial-level LWPC and PRS was used for all the participants. In the selected trial-list, the collinearity between trial-level LWPC and PRS variables was r = 0.12 (corresponding to 1.5% of shared variance).

Precise and simple instructions were provided before the beginning of the task. After instructions were given, a training block started (block-level LWPC = 50%) and participants received feedback about the correctness and the speed of their responses. Practice trials stopped when the participant reached an accuracy of 75% within 16 trials (after at least six trials). The task included 680 trials, interleaved by a 60-s break after every 170 trials, for a total of four breaks.

Statistical analyses

Inverse-transformed response times (iRTs: −1,000/RT) were analyzed by means of linear mixed-effect models (LMMs) in R1 using the lme4 library (Bates et al., 2015). Specifically, we computed inverted RTs to account for the positive skewness of the RTs distribution (Ambrosini et al., 2019; Brysbaert and Stevens, 2018; Viviani et al., 2023a). As common practice in the literature, RTs in the first trial at the beginning of each block, as well as the RTs in error trials (i.e., incorrect key pressing according to the presented target) and in post-error trials, as well as trials with missed responses, were excluded from the analyses (10% of excluded trials).

Concerning the LMMs, initially we detailed a full LMM accounting for all experimental effects and possible low-level confounds known to affect trial-wise iRTs variability (Baayen and Milin, 2010; Visalli et al., 2023).

Specifically, we included in the fixed part: (1) congruency (two-level factor, congruent and incongruent), (2) the cLWPC and (3) the OCI z-scores (zOCI) as continuous predictors, as well as their three-level interaction (Congruency × cLWPC × OCI z-scores and related low-order interactions). Moreover, as low-level confounding variables the fixed part included: (4) the rank-order of each trial (Trial) and (5) the iRT of the preceding trial (pre_iRT) to account for time-dependent (e.g., learning and fatigue) effects; (6) the horizontal and vertical coding of the arrow direction (hDIR and vDIR, respectively) to account for potential effects related to the response hand and/or finger, respectively; (7) the horizontal and vertical coding of the arrow location (hPOS and vPOS, respectively) to account for potential effects related to left/right and/or upper/lower visual field differences, respectively; finally (8) trial-level contingency (cPRS), (9) probability of the stimulus location (cPL), and (10) probability of the response (cPR) were included to account for potential effects of perceptual and/or motor confounding probabilistic contexts.

The random part included: participant-specific correlated random intercepts and slopes for the Congruency × cLWPC interaction and related low-order interactions and main effects. Therefore, the final model was specified as the following Wilkinson-notation formula (Equation 1):

i R T ~ Trial + p r e i R T + cPRS + c P L + c P R + hPOS + vPOS + hDIR + vDIR + Congruency × cLWPC × zOCI + Congruency × cLWPC | Participant     (1)

After fitting the LMM model, we inspected its residuals to assess whether there was evidence of stress in the model fit and, in that case, the model was refitted after excluding trials characterized by absolute standardized residuals exceeding 2.5 SD (Baayen and Milin, 2010).

Finally, in case of null results, we tested for the absence of a significant OCI modulation of participants’ cognitive control abilities (i.e., the correlation between participants’ OCI scores and both their general interference resolution abilities and their LWPC-dependent proactive control abilities, as assessed, respectively, by their random slopes for the Congruency and the LWPC by Congruency effect, both estimated by the LMM). To this aim, we performed TOST equivalence tests (Lakens, 2017; Lakens et al., 2020; Meyners, 2012). Specifically, the TOST compares the observed effect with the smallest effect of interest (SESOI) examining whether the former falls within a region around 0 delimited by a upper and a lower bound—defined according to the SESOI—describing two extreme regions containing 90% of observations if alpha is set to 0.05. If the observed effect falls in the area around 0, the alternative hypothesis (in this case, the absence of effect) is confirmed. The TOST was performed using the Excel spreadsheet developed by Lakens (2017; available at the following link: https://daniellakens.blogspot.com/2016/12/tost-equivalence-testing-r-package.html).

Results

The accuracy was high (M = 0.94; SD = 0.07), especially for congruent (M = 0.98; SD = 0.05) but also for incongruent (M = 0.90; SD = 0.10) trials. Inverted response times (iRTs) were consistent with the literature about interference resolution tasks (overall: M = −1.96, SD = 0.30; congruent: M = −2.89, SD = 0.32; incongruent: M = −1.72, SD = 0.32). Moreover, differences between congruent and incongruent trials (i.e., Stroop effect)—both in accuracy (M = 0.78, SD = 0.09, t = 9.17; p < 0.0001) and in iRTs (M = 0.47, SD = 0.11, t = 42.48; p < 0.0001)—were statistically significant. For our analyses, we focused on iRTs (see also Visalli et al., 2023).

Linear mixed-effect model (LMM)

As described in the method section, we report the results of the trimmed LMM model that was refitted after excluding observations with absolute standardized residuals exceeding 2.5 SD (2.06% of trials), which achieved a sufficient closeness to normality of residuals. For a summary of LMM results, see Table 2.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Parameters of the main effects and interaction for the predictors of specified model (see Equation 1).

The hypothesized two-way interaction between congruency and proactive control (Congruency × cLWPC) was significant. In other words, environmental requirements for proactive control modulated the Stroop effect in the expected direction (i.e., less marked difference in iRTs between congruent and incongruent trials in case of low cLWPC and vice versa for high cLWPC, see Figure 3).

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Effect of the interaction between congruency and trial-wise list-wide proportion congruency (Congruency × cLWPC). Con, Congruent trials (blue solid line); Inc, Incongruent trials (orange solid line). iRT, inverted response times. Shaded error bars indicate standard errors of estimated marginal means.

Nonetheless, the effect of total OCI scores, as well as their two- and three-way interactions with the experimentally-manipulated variables, were not significant.

Equivalence test (TOST)

The TOST resulted to be statistically significant for the correlations between participants’ OCI scores and both their general interference resolution abilities, as assessed by their random slopes for the Congruency effect estimated by the LMM (r = 0.137, CIlow = −0.025, CIhigh = 0.293, p = 0.042) and their LWPC-dependent proactive control abilities, as assessed by their random slopes for the LWPC by Congruency effect estimated by the LMM (r = 0.093, CIlow = −0.070, CIhigh = 0.252, p = 0.015). In other words, this analysis revealed that the observed correlations between participants’ OCI scores and both their Congruency and their LWPC by Congruency effects were statistically equivalent to 0. Similar TOST analyses were performed on the Spearman’s correlation coefficients, to control for potential violations of the statistical assumptions required for parametric tests, confirming the results observed for the Pearson’s correlations (Congruency: r = 0.089, CIlow = −0.075, CIhigh = 0.247, p = 0.013; LWPC by Congruency: r = 0.059, CIlow = −0.207, CIhigh = 0.218, p = 0.006). Finally, since the effect of OCI on performance was statistically equivalent to zero, we did not control for possible confounding effects of DASS-21 factors.

Discussion

Our study was aimed at investigating if and how subclinical OC traits were related to specific alterations in proactive control. To this end, we sampled the full continuum of OC traits in the general population and assessed their ability to solve conflict in a proactive fashion. Specifically, the administered Stroop task implicitly induced the participant to represent the probabilistic structure of the environment—in terms of congruency—to solve conflict and respond optimally. Given the cognitive and behavioral phenomena associated with OC traits, we expected individuals with more severe (although sub-clinical) OC traits to show alterations in forming or maintaining and, eventually, in adaptively using those task-relevant representations to solve conflict. Therefore, we hypothesized that an increased severity of OC traits could have been associated with a reduced modulation of performance according to probabilistically-manipulated requirements for proactive control (LWPC). In operational terms, this could be translated into a less marked difference in terms of Stroop effects between low and high LWPC trials.

Contrary to our hypothesis, we found that OC traits were not significantly associated with behavioral performance. Then, we tested for the absence of such effects and, indeed, we found that they were significantly equivalent to zero. Therefore, we can conclude that OC traits are not associated with the implementation of proactive control to solve conflict, at least not in the context of the Stroop task used here on a non-clinical sample.

Our results suggest that, contrary to the literature positioning cognitive control abnormalities at the core of compulsive disorders (Nisticò et al., 2021; Norman et al., 2019; Perera et al., 2023; Snyder et al., 2015; Shin et al., 2014), OC phenomena—at least at the subclinical level—are not related to abnormalities in proactive control processes implemented for solving conflict. Specifically, clinical and experimental evidence suggests that OC manifestations might emerge from executive dysfunctions (i.e., conflict monitoring, attention, response inhibition); this hypothesis is further supported by neuroimaging evidence about abnormalities in frontal and fronto-striatal circuits underlying executive processes for OC populations compared to healthy controls (see Nisticò et al., 2021; Norman et al., 2019; Perera et al., 2023; Snyder et al., 2015; Shin et al., 2014 for reviews). Such inconsistencies between our findings and previous literature may have several explanations, whose nature might concern either the real underlying mechanisms of compulsive disorders or our methodological choices.

Concerning the former possibility (i.e., underlying mechanisms of compulsive disorders), from our results we must conclude that impairments in the implementation of proactive control for conflict resolution do not underlie subclinical OC traits. This is in line with studies which did not find any deficits in conflict resolution and cognitive control functions for compulsive populations, although evidence is mixed, and that is the reason why we wanted to test this relationship (for reviews, see Ferreira et al., 2020; Hezel and McNally, 2016). Importantly, assuming that OC traits are not underlain by proactive control deficits has important implications for developing new preventive strategies. For instance, clinical and experimental research should focus on potentiating other cognitive functions in subclinical populations, which might possibly play a more prominent role in the onset and maintenance of the OC-related disorder.

Moreover, we cannot exclude that impairments in other control processes or modes might underpin the development of compulsive disorders. For instance, reactive rather than proactive control processes might be impaired in OC populations. That is, OC manifestations might arise from abnormalities in properly implementing reactive control to solve conflict. Indeed, this might be in line with some clinical observations: for instance, individuals with OC traits may detect conflict or “disturbing” information in contexts where it is not even there (e.g., sensory phenomena like not-just-right experiences and incompleteness; Coles and Ravid, 2016; Ferrão et al., 2012; Horncastle et al., 2022; Melli et al., 2020). Therefore, given the outlined clinical phenomena, it would be worth investigating the relationship between OC traits and possible impairments in reactive control mechanisms in future research.

Furthermore, we might hypothesize that other processes falling into the cognitive control domain could be at the basis of OC traits and disorders. For instance, evidence about a bias and overreliance toward habitual at the expenses of goal-directed behavior has been extensively found (Banca et al., 2015; Gillan et al., 2016; Gillan and Robbins, 2014; Robbins et al., 2012; Voon et al., 2015). Indeed, habits share with compulsions the rigidity of the underlying action-outcome associations and the automaticity characterizing their learning and implementation. Therefore, an imbalance between the two processes might lead to issues in encoding the task structure (Seow et al., 2021) and, eventually, to massively implement stereotyped and repetitive actions. Nonetheless, experimental paradigms developed to investigate model-based and model-free control (e.g., sequential decision-making tasks) often make use of rewards, posing problems in dissociating ‘hot’ (reward-based) from ‘cold’ decision-making (Dayan and Berridge, 2014; Voon et al., 2017).

Nevertheless, given the diverse clinical pictures and phenomena characterizing compulsive disorders, alterations in several cognitive control mechanisms and even in specific sub-processes (e.g., detecting versus solving conflict) could be responsible for their onset and maintenance. Future studies should focus on thoroughly investigating cognitive control processes in populations with OC traits, possibly administering a complete battery of cognitive control tasks and trying to spot the behavioral endophenotype(s) associated with OC traits in a transdiagnostic fashion.

Regarding the methodological choices which might have affected our results, we must pinpoint several aspects eventually limiting their generalizability.

Firstly, participants recruited in the present study post-screening were not clinically diagnosed with an OC-related disorder. On one hand, this choice is preferable to control for possible clinical-related confounding variables (e.g., effects of medication, years from diagnosis, age of onset, etc.) and to investigate possible prodromic factors predisposing to the development of different OC-related disorders. On the other hand, our approach prevented us from investigating cognitive control abnormalities and/or clinical phenomena that need to exceed a certain threshold in terms of severity to be experimentally observable. Moreover, the sampling procedure adopted here might prevent the inclusion of specific categories of individuals, therefore reducing the generalizability of our results.

Secondly, the experimental procedure (both screening and experimental phases) were performed online and autonomously by participants. This procedure allowed us to reach a broader and somehow heterogeneous population, as well as to control for the experimenter’s effect. Nonetheless, this experimental approach severely limited our control over experimental conditions and possible confounds.

Thirdly, the investigation of the association of interest was limited to a very specific cognitive control process (i.e., conflict resolution) implemented in a very peculiar mode (i.e., forming, maintaining and proactively using behaviorally-relevant representations of the probabilistic structure of the environment). Although this methodological approach is optimal to increase task specificity for selectively assessing the process(es) of interest, it drastically reduces task sensitivity. In other words, other processes falling into the cognitive control domain (e.g., decision-making) and/or other control modes (e.g., reactive control) might be involved in the development and maintenance of compulsive disorders. Albeit our experimental procedure was not suited to investigate these working hypotheses, our work might inspire future studies conceived to more specifically test them.

Lastly, specific moral emotions (e.g., fear of guilt) are known to play a pivotal role in OC-related manifestations (Mancini and Gangemi, 2004). Indeed, the presence of stimuli eliciting obsessive concerns might heavily impact on cognitive processes and, importantly, on the ability to solve conflict (Mancini and Gangemi, 2004, 2018). Therefore, the choice to elicit conflict by means of emotionally neutral stimuli prevented us from investigating the potential influence of such secondary moral emotions on the ability to solve conflict. In other words, we cannot exclude that deficits in conflict resolution in OC populations may only arise following the exposure to stimuli evoking moral emotions related to the specific obsessive concern. Importantly, this might explain our findings about the absence of effects. Therefore, future studies should also address the influence of specific moral emotions, which might represent a precondition to observe abnormalities in conflict resolution for OC individuals.

Conclusion

We found that OC traits were not associated with alterations of proactive control for conflict resolution. Several scientific explanations—also derived from specific methodological choices—might be at the basis of these results. Future research should address these issues by developing tailored methodologies accordingly. Firstly, assuming the presence of a to-be-reached severity threshold to observe an effect, a similar experimental procedure could be performed with clinical populations, controlling for the well-known clinically-relevant confounding variables. Additionally, the task should be adapted to control for possible ceiling effects and to increase the interindividual variability in cognitive performance. Lastly, other cognitive control processes and/or modes should be investigated and addressed, as OC traits have been consistently found, in the literature, to be associated with several executive deficits transcending the investigated probabilistic processes underlying the implementation of proactive control.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Ethics Committee for the Psychological Research of the University of Padova (approved protocol reference number: 4631). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

SF: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AVi: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. GV: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. EA: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AVa: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was partially supported by the “Department of excellence 2018-2022” initiative of the Italian Ministry of University and Research (MIUR) awarded to the Department of Neuroscience, University of Padova. SF’s PhD fellowship was supported by the Cassa di Risparmio di Padova e Rovigo (CARIPARO) Foundation. AV is partially supported by a grant from the Italian Ministry of Health (project code: GR-2018-12367927—FINAGE). Open Access funding provided by Università degli Studi di Padova | University of Padua, Open Science Committee.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

References

Ahmari, S. E., and Rauch, S. L. (2022). The prefrontal cortex and OCD. Neuropsychopharmacology 47, 211–224. doi: 10.1038/s41386-021-01130-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Albertella, L., Rotaru, K., Christensen, E., Lowe, A., Brierley, M.-E., Richardson, K., et al. (2021). The influence of trait compulsivity and impulsivity on addictive and compulsive behaviors during COVID-19. Front. Psych. 12:634583. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.634583

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ambrosini, E., Arbula, S., Rossato, C., Pacella, V., and Vallesi, A. (2019). Neuro-cognitive architecture of executive functions: a latent variable analysis. Cortex 119, 441–456. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.07.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Antony, M. M., Bieling, P. J., Cox, B. J., Enns, M. W., and Swinson, R. P. (1998). Psychometric properties of the 42-item and 21-item versions of the depression anxiety stress scales in clinical groups and a community sample. Psychol. Assess. 10, 176–181. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.10.2.176

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baayen, R. H., and Milin, P. (2010). Analyzing reaction times. Int. J. Psychol. Res. 3, 12–28. doi: 10.21500/20112084.807

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Banca, P., Voon, V., Vestergaard, M. D., Philipiak, G., Almeida, I., Pocinho, F., et al. (2015). Imbalance in habitual versus goal directed neural systems during symptom provocation in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain 138, 798–811. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu379

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., and Walker, S. (2015). Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bora, E. (2020). Meta-analysis of neurocognitive deficits in unaffected relatives of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD): comparison with healthy controls and patients with OCD. Psychol. Med. 50, 1257–1266. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720001634

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bottesi, G., Ghisi, M., Altoè, G., Conforti, E., Melli, G., and Sica, C. (2015). The Italian version of the depression anxiety stress scales-21: factor structure and psychometric properties on community and clinical samples. Compr. Psychiatry 60, 170–181. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2015.04.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol. Rev. 108, 624–652. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Braem, S., Bugg, J. M., Schmidt, J. R., Crump, M. J., Weissman, D. H., Notebaert, W., et al. (2019). Measuring adaptive control in conflict tasks. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 769–783. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.07.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Braver, T. S. (2012). The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual mechanisms framework. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 106–113. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Braver, T. S., Gray, J. R., and Burgess, G. C. (2007). “Explaining the many varieties of working memory variation: dual mechanisms of cognitive control” in Variation in working memory. eds. A. R. A. Conway, C. Jarrold, M. J. Kane, A. Miyake, and J. N. Towse, vol. 75 (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 106.

Google Scholar

Brysbaert, M., and Stevens, M. (2018). Power analysis and effect size in mixed effects models: a tutorial. J. Cogn. 1:9. doi: 10.5334/joc.10

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bugg, J. M., and Hutchison, K. A. (2013). Converging evidence for control of color–word Stroop interference at the item level. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 39, 433–449. doi: 10.1037/a0029145

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgess, G. C., and Braver, T. S. (2010). Neural mechanisms of interference control in working memory: effects of interference expectancy and fluid intelligence. PLoS One 5:e12861. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012861

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavanagh, J. F., and Frank, M. J. (2014). Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 414–421. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavedini, P., Zorzi, C., Piccinni, M., Cavallini, M. C., and Bellodi, L. (2010). Executive dysfunctions in obsessive-compulsive patients and unaffected relatives: searching for a new intermediate phenotype. Biol. Psychiatry 67, 1178–1184. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.02.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chamberlain, S. R., Fineberg, N. A., Menzies, L. A., Blackwell, A. D., Bullmore, E. T., Robbins, T. W., et al. (2007). Impaired cognitive flexibility and motor inhibition in unaffected first-degree relatives of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 164, 335–338. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.2.335

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chamberlain, S. R., Stochl, J., Redden, S. A., and Grant, J. E. (2018). Latent traits of impulsivity and compulsivity: toward dimensional psychiatry. Psychol. Med. 48, 810–821. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717002185

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiang, B., and Purdon, C. (2023). A study of doubt in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 80:101753. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2022.101753

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. D. (2017). “Cognitive control: core constructs and current considerations” in The Wiley handbook of cognitive control. ed. T. Egner (Hoboken, New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.), 1–28.

Google Scholar

Cohen, M. X., and Cavanagh, J. F. (2011). Single-trial regression elucidates the role of prefrontal theta oscillations in response conflict. Front. Psychol. 2:30. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00030

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Coles, M. E., and Ravid, A. (2016). Clinical presentation of not-just right experiences (NJREs) in individuals with OCD: characteristics and response to treatment. Behav. Res. Ther. 87, 182–187. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2016.09.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Córdova, M. E., Schiavon, C. C., Busnello, F. M., and Reppold, C. T. (2017). Nutritional and neuropsychological profile of the executive functions on binge eating disorder in obese adults. Nutr. Hosp. 34, 1448–1454. doi: 10.20960/nh.1151

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dayan, P., and Berridge, K. C. (2014). Model-based and model-free Pavlovian reward learning: revaluation, revision, and revelation. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 14, 473–492. doi: 10.3758/s13415-014-0277-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

de Melo, M. B., Daldegan-Bueno, D., Menezes Oliveira, M. G., and de Souza, A. L. (2022). Beyond ANOVA and MANOVA for repeated measures: advantages of generalized estimated equations and generalized linear mixed models and its use in neuroscience research. Eur. J. Neurosci. 56, 6089–6098. doi: 10.1111/ejn.15858

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

De Pisapia, N., and Braver, T. S. (2006). A model of dual control mechanisms through anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex interactions. Neurocomputing 69, 1322–1326. doi: 10.1016/j.neucom.2005.12.100

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Destrée, L., Albertella, L., Jobson, L., McGorry, P., Chanen, A., Ratheesh, A., et al. (2023). The association between stressful experiences and OCD symptoms in young adults at transdiagnostic risk. J. Affect. Disord. 328, 128–134. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2023.02.059

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dong, H., Yang, F., Lu, X., and Hao, W. (2020). Internet addiction and related psychological factors among children and adolescents in China during the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) epidemic. Front. Psych. 11:00751. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00751

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Elhai, J. D., Yang, H., McKay, D., and Asmundson, G. J. G. (2020). COVID-19 anxiety symptoms associated with problematic smartphone use severity in Chinese adults. J. Affect. Disord. 274, 576–582. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.05.080

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Erdfelder, E., Faul, F., and Buchner, A. (1996). GPOWER: a general power analysis program. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 28, 1–11. doi: 10.3758/BF03203630

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrão, Y. A., Shavitt, R. G., Prado, H., Fontenelle, L. F., Malavazzi, D. M., de Mathis, M. A., et al. (2012). Sensory phenomena associated with repetitive behaviors in obsessive-compulsive disorder: an exploratory study of 1001 patients. Psychiatry Res. 197, 253–258. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.09.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferreira, S., Pêgo, J. M., and Morgado, P. (2020). A systematic review of behavioral, physiological, and neurobiological cognitive regulation alterations in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain Sci. 10:797. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10110797

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Figee, M., Wielaard, I., Mazaheri, A., and Denys, D. (2013). Neurosurgical targets for compulsivity: what can we learn from acquired brain lesions? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 328–339. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fineberg, N. A., Hengartner, M. P., Bergbaum, C. E., Gale, T. M., Gamma, A., Ajdacic-Gross, V., et al. (2013). A prospective population-based cohort study of the prevalence, incidence and impact of obsessive-compulsive symptomatology. Int. J. Psychiatry Clin. Pract. 17, 170–178. doi: 10.3109/13651501.2012.755206

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fineberg, N. A., Menchon, J. M., Zohar, J., and Veltman, D. J. (2016). Compulsivity—a new trans-diagnostic research domain for the roadmap for mental health research in Europe (ROAMER) and research domain criteria (RDoC) initiatives. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 26, 797–799. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2016.04.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Foa, E. B., Kozak, M. J., Salkovskis, P. M., Coles, M. E., and Amir, N. (1998). The validation of a new obsessive–compulsive disorder scale: the obsessive–compulsive inventory. Psychol. Assess. 10, 206–214. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.10.3.206

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fontenelle, L. F., Oldenhof, E., Eduarda Moreira-de-Oliveira, M., Abramowitz, J. S., Antony, M. M., Cath, D., et al. (2020). A transdiagnostic perspective of constructs underlying obsessive-compulsive and related disorders: an international Delphi consensus study. Austral. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 54, 719–731. doi: 10.1177/0004867420912327

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fradkin, I., Adams, R. A., Parr, T., Roiser, J. P., and Huppert, J. D. (2020a). Searching for an anchor in an unpredictable world: a computational model of obsessive compulsive disorder. Psychol. Rev. 127, 672–699. doi: 10.1037/rev0000188

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fradkin, I., Ludwig, C., Eldar, E., and Huppert, J. D. (2020b). Doubting what you already know: uncertainty regarding state transitions is associated with obsessive compulsive symptoms. PLoS Comput. Biol. 16:e1007634. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1007634

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fruehauf, L. M., Fair, J. E., Liebel, S. W., Bjornn, D., and Larson, M. J. (2021). Cognitive control in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD): proactive control adjustments or consistent performance? Psychiatry Res. 298:113809. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2021.113809

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fullana, M. A., Vilagut, G., Rojas-Farreras, S., Mataix-Cols, D., de Graaf, R., Demyttenaere, K., et al. (2010). Obsessive–compulsive symptom dimensions in the general population: results from an epidemiological study in six European countries. J. Affect. Disord. 124, 291–299. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.11.020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillan, C. M., Kosinski, M., Whelan, R., Phelps, E. A., and Daw, N. D. (2016). Characterizing a psychiatric symptom dimension related to deficits in goal-directed control. eLife 5:e11305. doi: 10.7554/eLife.11305

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillan, C. M., and Robbins, T. W. (2014). Goal-directed learning and obsessive–compulsive disorder. Philos. Trans. Roy. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 369:20130475. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0475

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonthier, C., Braver, T. S., and Bugg, J. M. (2016). Dissociating proactive and reactive control in the Stroop task. Mem. Cogn. 44, 778–788. doi: 10.3758/s13421-016-0591-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Henry, J. D., and Crawford, J. R. (2005). The short-form version of the depression anxiety stress scales (DASS-21): construct validity and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 44, 227–239. doi: 10.1348/014466505X29657

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Horncastle, T., Ludlow, A. K., and Gutierrez, R. (2022). Not just right experiences and incompleteness as a predictor of OC symptoms in clinical and community samples: a meta-analysis. J. Obsessive Compulsive Related Disord. 35:100762. doi: 10.1016/j.jocrd.2022.100762

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hezel, D. M., and McNally, R. J. (2016). A theoretical review of cognitive biases and deficits in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Biological Psychology, 121:221–232.

Google Scholar

Ioannidis, K., Hook, R., Goudriaan, A. E., Vlies, S., Fineberg, N. A., Grant, J. E., et al. (2019). Cognitive deficits in problematic internet use: meta-analysis of 40 studies. Br. J. Psychiatry 215, 639–646. doi: 10.1192/bjp.2019.3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kornblum, S. (1992). “Dimensional overlap and dimensional relevance in stimulus-response and stimulus-stimulus compatibility” in Portions of this paper were presented at the annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society (New Orleans, LA: North-Holland).

Google Scholar

Kreidler, S. M., Muller, K. E., Grunwald, G. K., Ringham, B. M., Coker-Dukowitz, Z. T., Sakhadeo, U. R., et al. (2013). GLIMMPSE: online power computation for linear models with and without a baseline covariate. J. Stat. Softw. 54:i10. doi: 10.18637/jss.v054.i10

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lakens, D. (2017). Equivalence tests: a practical primer for t tests, correlations, and meta-analyses. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 8, 355–362. doi: 10.1177/1948550617697177

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lakens, D., McLatchie, N., Isager, P. M., Scheel, A. M., and Dienes, Z. (2020). Improving inferences about null effects with Bayes factors and equivalence tests. J. Gerontol. Ser. B 75, 45–57. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gby065

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindheimer, N., Nicolai, J., and Moshagen, M. (2020). General rather than specific: cognitive deficits in suppressing task irrelevant stimuli are associated with buying-shopping disorder. PLoS One 15:e0237093. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0237093

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovibond, P. F., and Lovibond, S. H. (1995). Self-report scales (DASS) for the differentiation and measurement of depression, anxiety, and stress. Behav. Res. Ther. 33, 43.

Google Scholar

Luigjes, J., Lorenzetti, V., de Haan, S., Youssef, G. J., Murawski, C., Sjoerds, Z., et al. (2019). Defining compulsive behavior. Neuropsychol. Rev. 29, 4–13. doi: 10.1007/s11065-019-09404-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lunn, J., Greene, D., Callaghan, T., and Egan, S. J. (2023). Associations between perfectionism and symptoms of anxiety, obsessive-compulsive disorder and depression in young people: a meta-analysis. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 52, 460–487. doi: 10.1080/16506073.2023.2211736

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychol. Bull. 109, 163–203. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mäki-Marttunen, V., Hagen, T., and Espeseth, T. (2019). Proactive and reactive modes of cognitive control can operate independently and simultaneously. Acta Psychol. 199:102891. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2019.102891

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mancini, F., and Gangemi, A. (2004). Fear of guilt from behaving irresponsibly in obsessive–compulsive disorder. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 35, 109–120. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2004.04.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mancini, F., and Gangemi, A. (2018). “Cognitive processes in obsessive-compulsive disorder” in The obsessive mind (Abingdon, Oxfordshire: Routledge), 73–92.

Google Scholar

Mandelli, L., Draghetti, S., Albert, U., De Ronchi, D., and Atti, A. R. (2020). Rates of comorbid obsessive-compulsive disorder in eating disorders: a meta-analysis of the literature. J. Affect. Disord. 277, 927–939. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.09.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martínez-Esparza, I. C., Olivares-Olivares, P. J., Rosa-Alcázar, Á., Rosa-Alcázar, A. I., and Storch, E. A. (2021). Executive functioning and clinical variables in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain Sci. 11:267. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11020267

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathys, C., Daunizeau, J., Friston, K. J., and Stephan, K. E. (2011). A Bayesian foundation for individual learning under uncertainty. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:39. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00039

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McTeague, L. M., Goodkind, M. S., and Etkin, A. (2016). Transdiagnostic impairment of cognitive control in mental illness. J. Psychiatr. Res. 83, 37–46. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.08.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Melli, G., Moulding, R., Puccetti, C., Pinto, A., Caccico, L., Drabik, M. J., et al. (2020). Assessing beliefs about the consequences of not just right experiences: psychometric properties of the not just right experience-sensitivity scale (NJRE-SS). Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 27, 847–857. doi: 10.1002/cpp.2468

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyners, M. (2012). Equivalence tests–a review. Food Qual. Prefer. 26, 231–245. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2012.05.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Monaco, M. R. L., Di Stasio, E., Zuccala, G., Petracca, M., Genovese, D., Fusco, D., et al. (2020). Prevalence of obsessive-compulsive symptoms in elderly Parkinson disease patients: a case-control study. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 28, 167–175. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2019.08.022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, S. M., Wegmann, E., García Arias, M., Bernabéu Brotóns, E., Marchena Giráldez, C., and Brand, M. (2021). Deficits in executive functions but not in decision making under risk in individuals with problematic social-network use. Compr. Psychiatry 106:152228. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2021.152228

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nee, D. E., Wager, T. D., and Jonides, J. (2007). Interference resolution: insights from a meta-analysis of neuroimaging tasks. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 7, 1–17. doi: 10.3758/CABN.7.1.1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nisticò, V., De Angelis, A., Erro, R., Demartini, B., and Ricciardi, L. (2021). Obsessive-compulsive disorder and decision making under ambiguity: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Brain Sci. 11:143. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11020143

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Norman, L. J., Taylor, S. F., Liu, Y., Radua, J., Chye, Y., De Wit, S. J., et al. (2019). Error processing and inhibitory control in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a meta-analysis using statistical parametric maps. Biol. Psychiatry 85, 713–725. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.11.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ornell, F., Braga, D. T., Bavaresco, D. V., Francke, I. D., Scherer, J. N., von Diemen, L., et al. (2021). Obsessive-compulsive disorder reinforcement during the COVID-19 pandemic. Trends Psychiatry Psychother. 43, 81–84. doi: 10.47626/2237-6089-2020-0054

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Perera, M. P. N., Mallawaarachchi, S., Bailey, N. W., Murphy, O. W., and Fitzgerald, P. B. (2023). Obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD) is associated with increased engagement of frontal brain regions across multiple event-related potentials. Psychol. Med. 53, 7287–7299. doi: 10.1017/S0033291723000843

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, B. S., Pine, D. S., Cohen, P., and Brook, J. S. (2022). “Prospective, longitudinal study of tic, obsessive-compulsive, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorders in an epidemiological sample” in Obsessive-compulsive disorder and Tourette's syndrome (Abingdon, Oxfordshire: Routledge), 107–117.

Google Scholar

Pinciotti, C. M., Riemann, B. C., and Abramowitz, J. S. (2021). Intolerance of uncertainty and obsessive-compulsive disorder dimensions. J. Anxiety Disord. 81:102417. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2021.102417

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Robbins, T. W., Gillan, C. M., Smith, D. G., de Wit, S., and Ersche, K. D. (2012). Neurocognitive endophenotypes of impulsivity and compulsivity: towards dimensional psychiatry. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 81–91. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, J. R. (2019). Evidence against conflict monitoring and adaptation: an updated review. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 26, 753–771. doi: 10.3758/s13423-018-1520-z

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, J. R., and Besner, D. (2008). The Stroop effect: why proportion congruent has nothing to do with congruency and everything to do with contingency. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 34, 514–523.A28. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.3.514

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Seow, T. X., Benoit, E., Dempsey, C., Jennings, M., Maxwell, A., O'Connell, R., et al. (2021). Model-based planning deficits in compulsivity are linked to faulty neural representations of task structure. J. Neurosci. 41, 6539–6550. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0031-21.2021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharp, P. B., Dolan, R. J., and Eldar, E. (2021). Disrupted state transition learning as a computational marker of compulsivity. Psychol. Med., 53, 2095–2105. doi: 10.1017/S0033291721003846

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shin, N. Y., Lee, T. Y., Kim, E., and Kwon, J. S. (2014). Cognitive functioning in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 44, 1121–1130. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713001803

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sica, C., Ghisi, M., Altoè, G., Chiri, L. R., Franceschini, S., Coradeschi, D., et al. (2009). The Italian version of the obsessive compulsive inventory: its psychometric properties on community and clinical samples. J. Anxiety Disord. 23, 204–211. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.07.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Snorrason, I., Beard, C., Peckham, A. D., and Björgvinsson, T. (2021). Transdiagnostic dimensions in obsessive-compulsive and related disorders: associations with internalizing and externalizing symptoms. Psychol. Med. 51, 1657–1665. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720000380

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Snyder, H. R., Kaiser, R. H., Warren, S. L., and Heller, W. (2015). Obsessive-compulsive disorder is associated with broad impairments in executive function: a meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 3, 301–330. doi: 10.1177/2167702614534210

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stoet, G. (2010). PsyToolkit: a software package for programming psychological experiments using Linux. Behav. Res. Methods 42, 1096–1104. doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.4.1096

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stoet, G. (2017). PsyToolkit: a novel web-based method for running online questionnaires and reaction-time experiments. Teach. Psychol. 44, 24–31. doi: 10.1177/0098628316677643

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 18, 643–662. doi: 10.1037/h0054651

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tafuro, A., Ambrosini, E., Puccioni, O., and Vallesi, A. (2019). Brain oscillations in cognitive control: a cross-sectional study with a spatial Stroop task. Neuropsychologia 133:107190. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.107190

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanir, Y., Karayagmurlu, A., Kaya, İ., Kaynar, T. B., Türkmen, G., Dambasan, B. N., et al. (2020). Exacerbation of obsessive compulsive disorder symptoms in children and adolescents during COVID-19 pandemic. Psychiatry Res. 293:113363. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113363

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tiego, J., Oostermeijer, S., Prochazkova, L., Parkes, L., Dawson, A., Youssef, G., et al. (2019). Overlapping dimensional phenotypes of impulsivity and compulsivity explain co-occurrence of addictive and related behaviors. CNS Spectr. 24, 426–440. doi: 10.1017/S1092852918001244

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van Casteren, M., and Davis, M. H. (2006). Mix, a program for pseudorandomization. Behav. Res. Methods 38, 584–589. doi: 10.3758/BF03193889

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vazard, J. (2021). (Un) reasonable doubt as affective experience: obsessive–compulsive disorder, epistemic anxiety and the feeling of uncertainty. Synthese 198, 6917–6934. doi: 10.1007/s11229-019-02497-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Visalli, A., Ambrosini, E., Viviani, G., Sambataro, F., Tenconi, E., and Vallesi, A. (2023). On the relationship between emotions and cognitive control: evidence from an observational study on emotional priming Stroop task. PLoS One 18:e0294957. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0294957

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Viviani, G., Visalli, A., Finos, L., Vallesi, A., and Ambrosini, E. (2023a). A comparison between different variants of the spatial Stroop task: the influence of analytic flexibility on Stroop effect estimates and reliability. Behav. Res. Methods, 56, 934–951. doi: 10.3758/s13428-023-02091-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Viviani, G., Visalli, A., Montefinese, M., Vallesi, A., and Ambrosini, E. (2023b). The Stroop legacy: a cautionary tale on methodological issues and a proposed spatial solution. Behav. Res. Methods, 56, 4758–4785. doi: 10.3758/s13428-023-02215-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Viviani, G., Visalli, A., Montefinese, M., Vallesi, A., and Ambrosini, E. (2023c). Tango of control: the interplay between proactive and reactive control. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 153, 1644–1670. doi: 10.1037/xge0001585

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Voon, V., Derbyshire, K., Rück, C., Irvine, M. A., Worbe, Y., Enander, J., et al. (2015). Disorders of compulsivity: a common bias towards learning habits. Mol. Psychiatry 20, 345–352. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.44

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Voon, V., Reiter, A., Sebold, M., and Groman, S. (2017). Model-based control in dimensional psychiatry. Biol. Psychiatry 82, 391–400. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.04.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

West, R., and Bailey, K. (2012). ERP correlates of dual mechanisms of control in the counting Stroop task. Psychophysiology 49, 1309–1318. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01464.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, B. M., and Levinson, C. A. (2021). Intolerance of uncertainty and maladaptive perfectionism as maintenance factors for eating disorders and obsessive-compulsive disorder symptoms. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 29, 101–111. doi: 10.1002/erv.2807

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, J., Zaitlen, N. A., Goddard, M. E., Visscher, P. M., and Price, A. L. (2014). Advantages and pitfalls in the application of mixed-model association methods. Nat. Genet. 46, 100–106. doi: 10.1038/ng.2876

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yao, Y., Zhang, J., Fang, X., Liu, L., and Potenza, M. N. (2022). Reward-related decision-making deficits in internet gaming disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Addiction 117, 19–32. doi: 10.1111/add.15518

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yilmaz, Z., Halvorsen, M., Bryois, J., Yu, D., Thornton, L. M., Zerwas, S., et al. (2020). Examination of the shared genetic basis of anorexia nervosa and obsessive–compulsive disorder. Mol. Psychiatry 25, 2036–2046. doi: 10.1038/s41380-018-0115-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zadegan, S. A., Kupcha, L., Patino, J., Rocha, N. P., Teixeira, A. L., and Stimming, E. F. (2023). Obsessive-compulsive and perseverative behaviors in Huntington’s disease. Behav. Brain Res. 458:114767. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2023.114767

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, Z., Yuan, G., and Yao, J. (2012). Cognitive biases toward internet game-related pictures and executive deficits in individuals with an internet game addiction. PLoS One 7:e48961. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048961

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: obsessive-compulsive traits, proactive control, transdiagnostic, risk factors, conflict resolution, Stroop

Citation: Fornaro S, Visalli A, Viviani G, Ambrosini E and Vallesi A (2024) Proactive control for conflict resolution is intact in subclinical obsessive-compulsive individuals. Front. Psychol. 15:1490147. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1490147

Received: 02 September 2024; Accepted: 08 October 2024;
Published: 22 October 2024.

Edited by:

Simone Migliore, University of L’Aquila, Italy

Reviewed by:

Sabrina Maffi, IRCCS Casa Sollievo della Sofferenza Hospital, Italy
Francesca Proietti, Campus Bio-Medico University, Italy

Copyright © 2024 Fornaro, Visalli, Viviani, Ambrosini and Vallesi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Silvia Fornaro, silvia.fornaro.1@phd.unipd.it; Antonino Vallesi, antonino.vallesi@unipd.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.