Skip to main content

DATA REPORT article

Front. Psychol.
Sec. Psychology of Language
Volume 15 - 2024 | doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1474494
This article is part of the Research Topic Community Series: Spanish Psycholinguistics - Volume II View all 4 articles

Theme Identifiability Indices in Spanish for a set of 70 Ad Hoc Categorical Lists

Provisionally accepted
  • University of La Laguna, San Cristóbal de La Laguna, Spain

The final, formatted version of the article will be published soon.

    The field of false memories has been widely studied in cognitive psychology through the DRM paradigm (Deese, 1959;Roediger & McDermott, 1995), an experimental task used to induce false memories from materials that are conceptually and semantically related. This paradigm involves presenting lists of words that are semantically related to a non-presented critical word, with a subsequent memory test showing high levels of false recall and false recognition of that non-studied critical word. For example, after studying a list containing words, such as "butter", "food" and "sandwich", it is likely that in a subsequent free recall or recognition test, the word "bread" will be mistakenly identified as studied.False memory is generated, in part, by the relationship between the list words and the critical word (Gallo, 2010;Roediger & Gallo, 2016). This phenomenon has been explained by two theories: the fuzzy-trace theory (FTT) (Brainerd & Reyna, 1998), and the activationmonitoring framework (AMF) (Roediger et al., 2001). Both theories agree that, understanding the production of false memories requires considering two complementary processes: an error inflation process, identified as gist encoding in FTT and as activation in AMF; and an error editing process, identified as monitoring in AMF and as recollection rejection in FTT (Brainerd & Reyna, 2002).According to FTT (Brainerd & Reyna, 1998), "gist" refers to the general theme extracted from studied material. When a list of words related to a non-presented critical word is studied, both literal and semantic information are encoded. In a subsequent memory test, these literal and semantic memory traces operate simultaneously, providing information about the items. The retrieval of semantic information may lead to considering the critical word as having been studied due to its similarity to the presented words. However, the retrieval of literal information about the studied associates can counteract this effect by providing evidence that the critical word was not studied, a process known as recollection rejection (Brainerd & Reyna, 2002).According to the AMF (Roediger et al., 2001), two processes work together to produce false memory: activation and monitoring. Studying a list of words can trigger activation that spreads through the lexical-semantic system, creating implicit associations between interconnected words. This activation is moderated by a subsequent monitoring process that helps distinguishing between correct recall (studied words) and false memories (non-studied words).Even when considering different perspectives, both FTT and AMF agree that presenting a list of associates words activates a non-presented critical word, leading to error inflation. If the error inflation process is not accompanied by its corresponding error editing process, or if this process fails, false recall or false recognition may occur (Arndt & Gould, 2006). Therefore, studying the strategies used to avoid false memories is crucial for understanding the mechanisms underlying their formation.Theme identifiability of a list is one of the essential factors involved in this error editing process (Carneiro et al., 2009). In their normative study in Portuguese, they provided theme identifiability norms for 40 DRM associative lists selected from Albuquerque's (2005) study. Participants were presented with the lists and asked to generate a word that best described the general theme of each list. To study the effect of this factor on the production of false memories, they selected those lists with the highest and lowest levels of theme identifiability. The results showed that lists with high identifiability of the critical word as the theme produced lower levels of false recall and false recognition compared to lists where the critical word was not as easily identifiable. According to Carneiro et al. (2012), this outcome was attributed to an error editing strategy called "Identify to reject," which involves several stages: detecting that all the words in a list are related to a common theme, identifying the word that best describes the theme but is not present in the list, keeping it in mind to avoid recalling it in the future, and consequently, reducing false memories. This pattern has been observed in other studies using lists with an associative structure (Beato et al., 2023;Carneiro & Fernandez, 2013;Carneiro et al., 2012). Additionally, there are other normative studies that provide theme identifiability indices for associative lists in Spanish (Beato & Cadavid, 2016) and in English (Neuschatz et al., 2003).Most studies on false memories and error editing mechanisms using the DRM paradigm employ lists with an associative structure. However, ad hoc categorical relationships are less explored in the literature. Ad hoc categories are spontaneously constructed to achieve a specific goal in a given context, and their elements can come from different taxonomic categories (e.g., "Things that can fall on your head") (Barsalou, 1983). Both common and ad hoc categories can lead to similar memory distortions. While false memories are typically more pronounced for common categories, they are still robust for ad hoc categories (Soro et al., 2017).The mechanisms for avoiding memory distortions in such lists are not well understood. Ad hoc categories provide a valuable tool for studying situated concept representations, which are characterized by their flexibility and dynamism (Barsalou, 2005). Their use allows researchers to explore how individuals organize and retrieve information when categories are not predefined but emerge from context. This is particularly relevant for understanding how memory adapts to new information and situations, adding depth to theoretical debates on flexible concept representation.In the study with associative lists by Carneiro et al. (2009), the percentages of identification for critical words ranged from 1% to 77%. However, Soro et al. (2017) indicate that, unlike associative lists, in ad hoc categorical structured lists, theme identification typically refers to identifying the category label rather than the critical word itself. They used two criteria: exact identifiability, where participants identified the original theme of the lists (e.g., "Materials that cover the ground" for the category "Things that can be walked upon"), and comprehensive identifiability, where participants identified a theme that could include the critical word (e.g., a label that includes the critical word "grass" for "Things that can be walked upon"). Both criteria are important for describing our findings.To our knowledge, no previous studies have addressed false memories or theme identification with ad hoc categories in Spanish. Therefore, the aim of this research was to obtain theme identifiability indices for 70 lists that maintained ad hoc categorical relationships with a non-presented critical word using the DRM paradigm. These lists were created based on a normative study of ad hoc categories conducted in Spanish, which, to our knowledge, is the first of its kind in this language. Additionally, this research aims to lay the groundwork for studying the underlying mechanisms of error editing processes in lists with ad hoc categorical relationships in Spanish.In future research, these data may help in understanding the role of theme identifiability in the formation of false memories. Moreover, having these indices will enable more accurate predictions and better control over the experimental materials. A total of 188 students from the Psychology degree program at the University of La Laguna participated. All participants were native Spanish speakers (146 women, 42 men; mean age= 20.14, SD = 2.94). The material consisted of 70 ad hoc categorical lists, each containing 10 words. Both the critical words and their corresponding associates were extracted from a normative study conducted to collect data on ad hoc categories in Spanish (Alonso et al., in preparation;Benítez, Alonso, Fernandez, & Díez, 2022). Ad hoc categories were selected from various normative studies in English and Portuguese and then translated into Spanish (Barsalou, 1982(Barsalou, , 1983(Barsalou, , 1985;;Hough & Pierce, 1989;Soro & Ferreira, 2017;Vallée-Tourangeau et al., 1998;van Overschelde et al., 2004). The general procedure used in the Spanish normative study was similar to that used by Battig & Montague (1969), with the exception that in the present study both the presentation of the material and the collection for responses were done by computer (see van Overschelde et al., 2004). The participants were instructed to generate as many exemplars as possible for each category within one minute. This study, currently in preparation, will provide indices of frequency, rank, and lexical availability for the exemplars of each category.The lists were constructed based on the frequencies obtained from the normative study. Critical words were selected as those with the highest frequency within each category, while their associates were the next most frequent words. Care was taken to ensure that the critical words did not appear in more than one list and that no associate was repeated across lists. The selected critical words were primarily nouns (with only 4 being verbs and 1 adjective), ranging from 1 to 5 syllables, with a mean frequency of occurrence in Spanish of 60.86 per million (Alonso et al., 2011).The 70 lists were divided into 5 blocks: 4 blocks containing 15 lists each and 1 block with 10 lists. For the theme identification test, a booklet was prepared with several pages. The first page collected participant information (name, age, gender, and degree). The second page included practice examples to familiarize participants with the task. The remaining pages were dedicated to the experimental lists. Each page displayed the list number and had three blank spaces for participants to write the word or words they believed identified the theme of the list (up to three), along with a Likert scale to indicate their confidence in whether the word given in the first position represented the list's theme. Procedure Data collection took place in November 2023 during group sessions, each consisting of approximately 35 participants and lasting around 30 minutes. Each group studied 15 lists, except for one group that studied only 10 lists. The order of list presentation within each group was randomly determined.Participants began by completing the demographic information on the first page of the booklet and then received instructions similar to those used in previous studies (Carneiro et al., 2009;Neuschatz et al., 2003). They were shown a series of lists in a PowerPoint presentation, with one word displayed every 2 seconds. Before the start of the experimental session, and to ensure participants fully understood the task, two practice trials were conducted. These trials involved the same task as the main session: following the presentation of each list, participants had 50 seconds to generate up to three words that they believed best described the theme of the list. They also provided a confidence judgment on how certain they were that the first word given represented the list's theme, using a Likert scale ranging from 1 ("not very confident") to 5 ("very confident").Before each list, a message appeared on the screen indicating the list number to be presented. The process of presenting the list and identifying the theme was repeated until the experimental session was complete. The spreadsheet file accompanying this report (Theme_Identifiability_Ad_hoc_Spanish.xls) consists of five sheets. The first sheet, "Theme identifiability", contains the raw data for each participant. It includes the 70 critical words and their respective lists of 10 ad hoc associates. The first column shows the participant number, the second column identifies the list, the third column specifies the type of relationship of the lists (ad hoc), and the fourth column indicates the type of word (studied vs. critical). The fifth column contains the words themselves, while the sixth column provides the English translations of the critical and studied words. Adjacent columns include all responses provided by participants in the first, second, and third positions, as well as the confidence judgements related to the first response. Additionally, intrusions are noted-i.e., words from a study list that a participant mistakenly identified as the theme of that list, and therefore are not considered valid responses.The second sheet, "First word", summarizes the total count of responses given in the first position for each list. It includes all the words generated by participants as the theme in the first position, associated with their respective critical word and list number. Additionally, it provides the English translation of the critical word, the total number of participants who responded to each list, the number and percentage of participants who identified a word as the theme, and the mean confidence judgement for each first-word response. Intrusions are also noted, including quantity and incorrectly identified words.The third and fourth sheets, "Second word" and "Third word", respectively, are dedicated to responses given in the second and third positions. The layout is identical to the previous sheets, but it does not include the column for mean confidence judgement. The fifth sheet, "Summary," presents the final summary, showing the most frequently identified theme for the first, second, and third positions for each list. Responses recorded in the database were maintained in their original format, with corrections made only for spelling errors. Singular/plural and masculine/feminine forms of words were counted separately.Table 1, available as supplementary material, shows the 70 critical words with their corresponding list identifier and English translation, the number of participants who responded to each list, the number of different themes given as the first response, the percentage of participants who indicated the critical word (comprehensive identification) as the theme of the list in the first position, and the mean confidence rating for the critical word as the first response. Additionally, it provides the percentages of participants who identified the ad hoc category label (exact identification) in the first position, as well as the mean confidence rating for these first-position responses.Theme identifiability is a crucial factor in the error editing process and thus influences the formation of false memories (Carneiro et al., 2009;Soro et al., 2017). However, the study of false memories in ad hoc categorical lists and the factors contributing to their formation remain unexplored in Spanish. Therefore, this study aimed to provide theme identifiability indices for 70 ad hoc lists within the framework of the DRM paradigm in Spanish.When examining identifiability levels using the same approach as in studies with associative lists (Carneiro et al., 2009), where the critical word is considered as the theme, the levels of identifiability are relatively low. Regarding comprehensive identifiability, the critical word was identified as a theme in the first position in only 10 out of the 70 lists, with identification percentages ranging from 2.1% to 17.2%. On average, the critical word was identified as the theme 0.98% (SD=3.11) of the time in the first position across all 70 lists. When considering only where the critical word was identified, this average increased to 6.88% (SD=5.39), with a mean confidence rating of 3.57 (SD=1.29). Given the ad hoc category exemplars can come from different categories, their membership is not immediately apparent without context, making the activation of the critical word challenging.In contrast, exact identifiability showed higher levels of identification. The theme of 59 lists (ranging from 2.1% to 87.5%) was identified in the first position, demonstrating a significant increase in identification compared to when only the critical word was considered. Participants often generated words that, while not precisely matching the category labels, were related to them, suggesting some thematic processing even if not explicitly expressed. On average, the theme was identified 22.22% (SD=23.75) of the time in the first position across all 70 lists. When considering only the lists where the theme was identified, this average rose to 26.36% (SD=23.67), with a mean confidence rating of 3.98 (SD=0.70) for the first response.According to Carneiro et al. (2009), in associative lists, the level of identification of the critical word as the theme is inversely related to the occurrence of false memories. Identifying the critical word as the theme triggers an error editing process that mitigates false memories in subsequent memory tests. However, this assumption may not hold true for categorical lists, particularly ad hoc categorical lists, where theme identifiability might not exert the same effect on the error editing process. Soro et al. (2017) suggested that for false memories to occur with ad hoc categories, a positive relationship with theme identifiability might be necessary due to the inherent variability among exemplars. Their study found no correlation between false recognition and theme identifiability. Instead, false recognition was influenced more by individual factors such as experience or creative thinking. Some participants generated more associations between exemplars, leading to an increased likelihood of errors in a subsequent recognition test.Our results suggest that context plays a fundamental role in theme identification within ad hoc categories, particularly concerning comprehensive identifiability. Therefore, considering the findings of Soro et al. (2017), the participant's ability to integrate exemplars into an appropriate context may significantly influence the activation of critical words. This aligns with the notion that memory is a dynamic process shaped by contextual factors (Barsalou, 2005). The present study provides valuable data on theme identifiability for a wide range of ad hoc categorical lists, underscoring the need for a different approach when studying error editing processes in this context. Our findings reveal a crucial aspect of ad hoc categories: while traditional associative lists tend to exhibit reduced false memories due to clearer theme identifiability, the inherent variability and contextual emergence of ad hoc categories introduce complexities in memory retrieval that merit further investigation. This area represents a promising opportunity for advancing our understanding of false memories.

    Keywords: Theme identifiability, ad hoc categorical relationships, false memories, DRM paradigm, error editing processes

    Received: 01 Aug 2024; Accepted: 04 Nov 2024.

    Copyright: © 2024 Benítez and Alonso. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

    * Correspondence: Verónica Benítez, University of La Laguna, San Cristóbal de La Laguna, Spain

    Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.