Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Psychol., 01 October 2024
Sec. Developmental Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Music and Pro-sociality View all articles

Revisiting the musical reminiscence bump: insights from neurocognitive and social brain development in adolescence

  • 1Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, United States
  • 2Department of Music, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, United States

Music listening is enjoyed across the lifespan and around the world. This has spurred many theories on the evolutionary purpose of music. The Music for Social Bonding hypothesis posits that the human capacity to make music evolved for the purpose of creating and preserving relationships between one another. Considering different time periods of music use across the lifespan, adolescence is especially a period of social reorientation away from family towards peers, characterized by new social bonds and increased prosocial behavior. This shift is accompanied by notable structural and functional changes in brain networks supporting reward processing and prosocial behavior. Reviewing the extant literature on developmental cognitive neuroscience and adolescent music use, we propose that neurocognitive changes in the reward system make adolescence an ideal developmental time window for investigating interactions between prosocial behavior and reward processing, as adolescence constitutes a time of relative increase in music reward valuation. Testing this hypothesis may clarify our understanding of developmental trajectories in music reward valuation, and offer insights into why music from adults’ adolescence holds a great deal of personal significance.

Introduction

Despite no obvious evolutionary advantage, music is enjoyed by listeners across the lifespan and around the globe (Dubé and Le Bel, 2003). Adult listeners show increased preference for and personal memories associated with music from their adolescence and young adulthood (the “reminiscence bump” effect; Krumhansl and Zupnick, 2013; Krumhansl, 2017; Jakubowski et al., 2020). Why we enjoy listening to music (Loui et al., 2017; Mas-Herrero et al., 2013; Salimpoor et al., 2013) and show this reminiscence bump effect (Lamont and Loveday, 2020; Peck and Grealey, 2020) has been the topic of much research. Neuroscientific studies have generally converged on the idea that music listening involves interactions between the reward system, including the medial prefrontal cortex and striatum (similar to primary rewards such as food; Mas-Herrero et al., 2021), and the with the auditory cortex (Quinci et al., 2022; Salimpoor et al., 2013). The Music for Social Bonding Hypothesis argues that human capacity to make music evolved for the purpose of creating and preserving relationships between one another (Savage et al., 2020). In this view, the engagement of the reward system through the enjoyment of music (Quinci et al., 2022; Salimpoor et al., 2013) leads to positive emotional affiliations with those with whom music is shared.

Social bonds play an important role in human development across cultures and can even support healthy aging (Antonucci et al., 2019). Adolescence, which can be defined as the time encompassing the onset of puberty until independence is achieved (Casey et al., 2015), is a period of “social reorientation” away from caregivers (Faust et al., 2020) and towards peers (Cosme et al., 2022; Crone and Dahl, 2012; Larson, 2001; Nelson et al., 2005). This shift in the importance of peer relationships intensifies adolescents’ social sensitivity, or the attention, salience and emotional response directed towards processing information related to social evaluations and social standing (Somerville, 2013). At the same time, adolescents report listening to music in social contexts more often than older age groups (Bonneville-Roussy et al., 2013) and even establish friendships according to musical preferences (Franken et al., 2017; Kistler et al., 2010; Miranda, 2013; North and Hargreaves, 1999; Selfhout et al., 2009; ter Bogt et al., 2017). Here, we review key neurocognitive developmental changes in the social brain, specifically in the reward system, during adolescence, and interpret them in light of the existing literature on adolescent music usage and consumption. Together, we argue that these suggest the existence of a developmental increase in music reward valuation during adolescence, which can provide unique insight into the relationship between music and memory across the lifespan.

Neurocognitive development of social evaluation engages reward processing in adolescence

The reorientation of adolescents’ social world is accompanied by ongoing changes in social cognition. Fundamental skills which support mentalizing, or understanding the mental states of others, are apparent as early as two years old (for a review, see Carlson et al., 2013). Popularly employed methods used to study mentalizing typically show ceiling-level performance at around five years old (Wellman et al., 2001), suggesting that this ability is readily developed by the end of childhood. However, more complex mentalizing tasks which require individuals to take others’ perspectives while inhibiting their own (i.e., perspective-taking) indicate otherwise. These studies show that adolescents make more errors compared to adults (Dumontheil et al., 2010; Symeonidou et al., 2016), with older adolescents (17 year-olds) outperforming younger adolescents (12- and 15-year-olds; Humphrey and Dumontheil, 2016). These findings suggest that higher-level mentalizing skills improve throughout adolescence until reaching adultlike abilities.

Studies of brain structure and function during adolescence complement this interpretation. Neuroimaging studies have identified a network of regions that support mentalizing (known as the “social brain”), which includes the superior temporal sulcus, temporoparietal junction, and medial prefrontal cortex (Amodio and Frith, 2006; Lieberman et al., 2019; Rilling and Sanfey, 2011; Saxe et al., 2009). This network is active in youth while listening to descriptions of others’ mental states (Gweon et al., 2012; Mukerji et al., 2019; Saxe et al., 2009), watching cartoons (Brunet et al., 2000; Gallagher et al., 2000; Sebastian et al., 2012), and even in the mere presence of peers (van Hoorn et al., 2016). These regions undergo notable structural changes throughout adolescence, with decreases in cortical thickness and gray matter volume in the superior temporal sulcus, temporoparietal junction, and dorsal medial prefrontal cortex (Mills et al., 2014). Adolescents also show increased activation of the medial prefrontal cortex compared to adults during mentalizing tasks (Blakemore et al., 2007; Gunther Moor et al., 2012; Sebastian et al., 2012), which some interpret as adolescents employing different mentalizing strategies compared to adults (Blakemore, 2008).

Such developments in social cognition and the social brain come at a time of increased prosocial behavior, or actions which serve the goal of helping others. Numerous longitudinal studies show a relative increase in such behavior throughout adolescence (Crocetti et al., 2016; te Brinke et al., 2023; Van der Graaff et al., 2018). Importantly, the development of mentalizing abilities is linked to adolescent prosocial behavior (Blankenstein et al., 2020; Hall et al., 2021). For instance, perspective taking performance in adolescence is positively correlated with self-reported prosocial behavior (Tamnes et al., 2018), more giving behavior (van de Groep et al., 2020), and higher levels of trust and reciprocity in group cooperation tasks (Fett et al., 2014). As a result, adolescents’ increased engagement in prosocial behavior may be due, in part, by their improved ability to understand the wants and needs of others.

Engaging in prosocial behavior elicits activation of regions within the reward system, which consists of the ventral tegmental area, ventral striatum, and ventral medial prefrontal cortex (Decety et al., 2004; Rilling and Sanfey, 2011; Telzer, 2016; Telzer et al., 2010; van de Groep et al., 2020; Westhoff et al., 2021; Zaki and Mitchell, 2013). This network—and ventral striatum in particular—plays an important role in the anticipation of and reactivity to a wide array of rewarding experiences (Pagnoni et al., 2002; Schultz et al., 1997), including monetary (Galvan et al., 2006; Van Leijenhorst et al., 2010) and social rewards (such as smiling faces; Somerville et al., 2011, and peer evaluation; Cheng et al., 2020; Guyer et al., 2009; Jones et al., 2014; Sherman et al., 2016). This system also shows protracted development into late adolescence (Dahl, 2004; Padmanabhan and Luna, 2014; Spear, 2000; Wahlstrom et al., 2010). Compared to children and adults, adolescents show heightened activity of the ventral striatum, a region longitudinally linked to greater self-reports of reactivity to rewards (Urošević et al., 2012), during the receipt of a reward compared to children and adults (Braams et al., 2015; Cohen et al., 2010; Ernst et al., 2005; Galvan et al., 2006; Schreuders et al., 2018; Silverman et al., 2015; Van Leijenhorst et al., 2010). This has led some to hypothesize that adolescents spend more time with peers due to a relative increase in the value of social interactions during this time (Foulkes and Blakemore, 2016). Indeed, adolescents show greater activation in the ventral striatum compared to children (6–12 year olds) and adults (18–29) in response to positive (happy facial expressions) compared to neutral (calm facial expressions) social cues (Somerville et al., 2011).

Some evidence also suggests that the value of non-social rewards increase in the presence of peers during adolescence (Chein et al., 2011; Powers et al., 2022; Weigard et al., 2014). For instance, the ventral striatum and temporoparietal junction show greater activation while adolescents make decisions while being told they were being observed by peers compared to parents (van Hoorn et al., 2018). The ventral striatum also responds to rewards received by others (Braams and Crone, 2017), and in adults this activity scales with how close individuals feel with the recipient (Hackel et al., 2017; Morelli et al., 2015). The magnitude of this vicarious response is positively correlated with everyday prosocial behavior (Morelli et al., 2018). The engagement of the ventral striatum during prosocial behavior may thus motivate adolescents to partake in such behavior (Crone et al., 2022). This interpretation is consistent with results that show adolescents typically act prosocially towards their friends (Berndt and Hoyle, 1985; Güroğlu et al., 2014; Padilla-Walker et al., 2018). In sum, improvements in mentalizing and relative increases in reactivity to rewarding experience make adolescence a uniquely suited time window for investigating interactions between prosocial behavior and reward processing.

Music reward valuation

Advances in understanding the brain structures underlying pleasurable music listening experiences show that music engages the reward system similar to other primary rewards (Mas-Herrero et al., 2021). Broadly speaking, enjoyable music listening experiences typically involve interactions between these structures (including the ventral striatum) and auditory and frontal cortices (Mas-Herrero et al., 2021; Salimpoor et al., 2011; Salimpoor et al., 2013; Belden et al., 2023). Activity of this network scales with preference listeners have with music (Salimpoor et al., 2013), and evidence for the causality of this network transcranial magnetic stimulation studies (Mas-Herrero et al., 2018). As a result, how music acquires reward value has been the focus of much research. One line of work has specifically investigated how music acquires intrinsic reward value (i.e., the enjoyment of music in and of itself). The Predictive Coding of Music Model (Vuust et al., 2022) argues that listening to music involves an ongoing process of generating predictions about upcoming musical elements, with the goal of reducing mismatches between listeners’ predictions and experience of the music (i.e., prediction errors). Rewarding feelings that accompany the experience of musical prediction errors thus serve as the basis for the updating of listeners’ musical predictions.

To date, there have been few cross-sectional age comparisons of the experience of music-related reward. Belden et al. (2023) demonstrated age-related differences in functional connectivity during music listening. In this study, both younger (18–23 years old) and older adults (54–89 years old) completed a music listening fMRI task during which they listened to self- and researcher-selected music. During this task, the younger adult cohort exhibited greater functional connectivity between the medial prefrontal cortex and auditory cortex compared to the older adults (Belden et al., 2023). These results mirror age-related declines in self-reported music enjoyment across adulthood (Belfi et al., 2022). Only two studies, to our knowledge, have investigated functional differences in brain activity in populations younger than young adults (Berns et al., 2010; Fasano et al., 2022). Berns et al. (2010) exposed adolescent participants (ages 12–17 years) to 15 s music clips, after which they rated each on liking and familiarity. Activity in the striatum was correlated with participants’ liking ratings. Similarly, Fasano et al. (2022) identified a network involving the medial orbitofrontal cortex and ventromedial prefrontal cortex, which reliably subserved music listening in preadolescents (ages 10–11 years). Importantly, these studies show that music listening engages frontostriatal circuitry similar to adults. While comparisons across ages in this experience remain to be tested, these studies suggest that musical reward may also elicit a relative increase in the engagement of this circuitry during adolescence.

Responses to the Barcelona Music Reward Questionnaire (Mas-Herrero et al., 2013), a self-report measure of individual differences in sensitivity to musical reward, show that sharing music with others is another source musical enjoyment across cultures (the “Social Reward” dimension; Mannino et al., 2024; Wang et al., 2023). While Belfi and Loui (2020) found that scores on this measure remain constant across adulthood, adolescents’ sensitivity to social information (Guyer et al., 2009; Somerville, 2013) offers a theoretical basis as to why this construct may show age-related differences earlier in development. Indeed, musical training between the ages of 10–14 years old is linked to greater sensitivity to this source of musical pleasure (Lippolis et al., 2023). Berns et al. (2010) allowed participants to update their liking ratings after being presented with “popularity scores” of each music clip. Participants were likely to update their liking ratings in accordance with these popularity metrics (e.g., popular songs were liked more; Berns et al., 2010), providing evidence of peer influence on adolescents’ subjective music enjoyment, even when they are not physically present. A recent study has also demonstrated that merely telling adult participants that others were listening to the same music at the same time boosted the subjective pleasure participants felt listening to that music (Curzel et al., 2024). Together, these studies suggest that sharing music with others may be a salient source of musical reward for adolescents and that doing so may increase overall pleasure during music listening.

Extant work on adolescent music usage

Reports of adolescent music usage are broadly consistent with the idea that listeners are particularly responsive to both the implicit and social rewards of music during this time. Cross-sectional studies show the importance attributed to music is higher in adolescent listeners (ages 13–18 years old) compared to older age groups (Bonneville-Roussy et al., 2013; Lamont and Hargreaves, 2019). Further, music listening nearly doubles for 11–14 year-olds listeners compared to those 8–10 years old, and this engagement continues to increase as listeners reach 18 years of age (Roberts and Henriksen, 2004). On the other hand, a large-scale study on music making in teenagers in the UK and Germany showed that about 50% of all students drop out of music lessons and other musical activities by the time they turn 17 years old, with most students quitting between the ages of 15 and 17 (Ruth and Müllensiefen, 2021). Thus, music listening appears to increase during adolescence, even while active music making decreases. This may be indicative of a general increase in media consumption during this time window; however, adolescents prioritize music listening over other forms of entertainment, such as reading a book, going shopping, or playing computer games (North et al., 2000). This suggests that adolescents’ affinity for music is likely due to affordances of music listening not offered by other forms of media.

A unique feature of music is its ability to create interpersonal synchrony, or time-locked coordinated movement (Trainor and Cirelli, 2015). Such synchrony to music signals group membership (Tarr et al., 2014; Hove and Risen, 2009) and motivates prosocial behavior (Wiltermuth and Heath, 2009; Stupacher et al., 2017; Cirelli et al., 2014), highlighting the social nature of music. At the same time, adolescents report listening to music in more social contexts compared to older individuals (Bonneville-Roussy et al., 2013) and form peer groups and close affiliations around shared musical preferences (Bakagiannis and Tarrant, 2006; Franken et al., 2017; Miranda and Claes, 2009; Selfhout et al., 2009). Further, adolescents attribute the ability to use music to curate their identities, which likely serve as the basis for relationships created through shared musical preferences, as one reason why music holds such great importance during this time (North et al., 2000). Such results suggest that increases in social sensitivity during adolescence may account for the importance placed on music during this time. Consistent with studies that directly manipulate interpersonal synchrony (Wiltermuth and Heath, 2009; Stupacher et al., 2017; Cirelli et al., 2014), affiliations based solely on musical preferences elicit prosocial behavior in adolescents (Bakagiannis and Tarrant, 2006; North et al., 2004).

Adolescent music usage, paired with ongoing neurocognitive development of brain systems for reward and social processes, make this an insightful window for understanding the role of rewarding music listening experiences in the Music for Social Bonding hypothesis. We propose that heightened reactivity to both intrinsic and social rewards of music listening support adolescent prosocial behavior towards those they share music with. Specifically, adolescents may initially be motivated to listen to music with others due to the boost that social music listening offers to musical pleasure (Curzel et al., 2024), and adolescents’ increased reactivity to rewarding stimuli (Galvan et al., 2006; Van Leijenhorst et al., 2010; Galván and McGlennen, 2013) especially among peers (Chein et al., 2011; Weigard et al., 2014). According to the Music for Social Bonding Hypothesis (Savage et al., 2020), such boosts in musical pleasure shared with individuals may, in return, contribute to strong social bonds built around music during adolescence. The shift towards social music listening during adolescence may thus be supported by the ongoing development of reward processing during this time period. These affiliations established through music may then be the basis for the experience of vicarious rewards which motivate prosocial behavior. Similar mechanisms likely support prosocial behavior elicited between individuals with shared musical preferences (Bakagiannis and Tarrant, 2006; North et al., 2004). The social inferences that individuals may make about each other (e.g., what kind of clothes someone might wear or activities they might enjoy), based solely upon others’ musical preferences, may be used as information to cue individuals to form new relationships (North and Hargreaves, 1999; Rentfrow and Gosling, 2006).

Discussion: evaluating accounts of the reminiscence bump effect

A core tenet of our proposal is that adolescence may be a time during which individuals show a socially-boosted increase in the rewards of music listening. While this offers implications for how we might understand music and prosocial behavior, it may also shed light on other music-related phenomena. One such instance is the “reminiscence bump” effect, which is a robust finding in autobiographical memory, or recollections of one’s own experiences (Koppel and Berntsen, 2015; Rubin et al., 1986). This describes adults’ general tendency to recall the most autobiographical memories from when they were between the ages of 10–30 years old (Koppel and Berntsen, 2015; Rubin et al., 1986). This effect has been illustrated in response to a number of cues, such as words (Crovitz and Schiffman, 1974; Rubin and Schulkind, 1997), pictures (Willander and Larsson, 2006), and music (Jakubowski et al., 2020; Krumhansl, 2017). Studies have shown that the timing of this reminiscence bump effect differs between these cues, suggesting contributions from different mechanisms supporting this effect across modalities (Koppel and Berntsen, 2015).

In the realm of music, the reminiscence bump effect specifically describes a relative increase in the amount of personally significant memories associated with or the amount of spontaneous autobiographical memories evoked by music from listeners’ adolescence and early adulthood (Jakubowski et al., 2020; Krumhansl, 2017; Krumhansl and Zupnick, 2013; Platz et al., 2015). Studies which illustrate the reminiscence bump in music typically use popular music cues and calculate “song-specific age” of each cue (Jakubowski et al., 2020; Krumhansl, 2017; year of music popularity minus participants’ birth year) as a proxy for developmental exposure. For example, Jakubowski et al. (2020) found that participants across adulthood (ages 18–82 years old) reported having the most memories associated with music with a song-specific age between 10–19 years (with a peak around 14 years). A notable departure of this approach compared to other methods which characterize this effect is that it emphasizes the timing of listeners’ developmental exposure to popular music cues and not necessarily the timing of the evoked memories. Though this presents a confound in comparing the music-related bump to the general reminiscence bump, it points to developmental processes within the musical reminiscence bump time period giving rise to this effect. However, using song-specific age as a proxy for developmental exposure to music can be limited by the fact that listeners may not be first exposed to popular music at the time it was popularized. Here, we argue that investigations of music reward valuation across adolescence may provide additional evidence to link ongoing developmental processes to the musical reminiscence bump effect with higher specificity and experimental control.

Two theoretical accounts which emphasize ongoing developmental processes during the reminiscence bump time period exist to explain the origin of this effect (for a review of these accounts, see Munawar et al., 2018). Some have theorized that the ongoing importance of self and group identity construction during the reminiscence bump window is the origin of the reminiscence bump effect (i.e., the “Identity Formation” account; Conway, 2005; Munawar et al., 2018; Peck and Grealey, 2020). This account argues that experiences acquired during this period are integrated into an individual’s lifelong narratives and are thus often recalled later in life as an understanding or explanation of one’s identity (Conway, 2005). As a result, memories from the reminiscence bump time period remain durable and easily recalled across the lifespan. Given the emphasis some adolescents place on music listening for their identity (Franken et al., 2017; Kistler et al., 2010; Miranda and Claes, 2009; North and Hargreaves, 1999; Selfhout et al., 2009; ter Bogt et al., 2017), this account fits nicely with why individuals may experience a reminiscence bump in response to music. A similar explanation for this effect which emphasizes ongoing developmental processes is the Cognitive Abilities or Enhanced Encoding account (Janssen and Murre, 2008; Janssen et al., 2007). This account argues that individuals have an increased ability to encode memories during this time due to improvements in cognitive and neural functions which may cause the bump.

Other accounts of the reminiscence bump effect emphasize the nature of the experiences during the reminiscence bump time period and subsequent effects on memory retrieval. The Cultural Lifescript account, for instance, argues that the reminiscence bump time window is made up of stereotypical episodes consisting of multiple culturally significant events in a specific order (e.g., start high school, go to prom, graduate, go to college, etc.). While these lifescripts may vary cross-culturally, individuals’ awareness of this timeline results in events occurring within the reminiscence bump time period as both personally and culturally significant (Berntsen and Rubin, 2002). As a result, these memories are both strongly encoded and subsequently recalled often throughout the lifespan, resulting in their lifelong durability. Similarly, the Cognitive account states that increased exposure to novel experiences during this time period, such as one’s first date or first time driving, leads to increased lifelong memory retention of these events. Because these are first-time experiences, these memories serve as reference points for similar events later in life (Pillemer, 2001), and are thus not only retained because they are originally novel experiences, but also because individuals continue to recall them as they get older. Together, these four accounts are not mutually exclusive, but they do place different emphases on memory encoding and retrieval which are all likely at play in producing the musical reminiscence bump. Given methods used to elicit this effect emphasize likely developmental exposure to music cues, we focus our argument on the links between the proposed increase in music reward valuation during adolescence and accounts which emphasize ongoing developmental processes, i.e., the Cognitive Abilities account.

A consistent finding within the music-related reminiscence bump literature is that this effect is often associated with reports of stronger emotional responses (Schulkind et al., 1999; Platz et al., 2015) and greater preference for music from this time window (Holbrook and Schindler, 1989; Janssen et al., 2007; Kathios et al., 2023; Krumhansl, 2017). For instance, Janssen et al. (2007) instructed participants to report their favorite movies, books, and music records, as well as when they first encountered and the last time they had seen, read, or listened to each. Of these mediums, the amount of participants’ favorite records encountered during the reminiscence bump (ages 11–25) showed the biggest peak. This common finding has led some to hypothesize that increased preference for and emotional reactions to music from the reminiscence bump time period is due to the developmentally-specific associations listeners have for this music (Lamont and Loveday, 2020). However, heightened responsivity to the rewards of music listening during the reminiscence bump time period may also account for this lifelong preference. In fact, such increased responses to musical reward during this time period might be causally involved in the reminiscence bump effect.

Rewarding experiences generally enhance memory of associated information (Bialleck et al., 2011; Mather and Schoeke, 2011), perhaps more so in adolescence (Davidow et al., 2016; Nussenbaum and Hartley, 2021; Rosenbaum et al., 2022). Consistent with the Cognitive Abilities or Enhanced Encoding accounts of the reminiscence bump effect (Janssen and Murre, 2008; Janssen et al., 2007), heightened reactivity to musical reward may optimize the effect of musical reward on memory during this time. Only a handful of studies have investigated the relationship between reward and memory in music (Cardona et al., 2020; Curzel et al., 2024; Ferreri et al., 2021; Ferreri and Rodriguez-Fornells, 2017). These studies show that pleasure experienced during music listening is positively associated with memory outcomes, such as remembering the music itself (Ferreri et al., 2021; Ferreri and Rodriguez-Fornells, 2017; Curzel et al., 2024) and recalling words encoded during music listening (Cardona et al., 2020). Interestingly, in three of these studies (Ferreri and Rodriguez-Fornells, 2017; Ferreri et al., 2021; Cardona et al., 2020), this effect was strongest for those who reported high sensitivity to musical reward. These individual differences results are consistent with our proposition that developmental changes in music reward valuation within an individual (specifically during adolescence) may result in improved associated memory encoding during this time window. Such findings highlight the relevance of our proposal that adolescents may be a period of heightened music reward valuation to memory.

Conversely, our proposal may imply that adolescents who are uninterested in music may not show a musical reminiscence bump effect later in life. However, it is important to note that music-evoked autobiographical memories tend to be social in nature (Jakubowski and Ghosh, 2021; Janata et al., 2007). This, paired with the increase in social music listening in adolescence (Bonneville-Roussy et al., 2013), suggests that these individuals may still show a bump as adults due to incidental music exposure in social contexts. Though adolescents report a notable degree of dropout of music lessons (Ruth and Müllensiefen, 2021), it may be the case that music listening (but not recurrent music lessons) elicits specific episodic music-evoked autobiographical memories which develop alongside the development of theory of mind (Kristen-Antonow, 2019) and reward processing in adolescence.

Conclusion

Adolescence is a time of intense changes, both in the social environment and in the brain. It stands to reason that music use during this time period may undergo developmentally significant changes as well. The engagement of neural systems for processing rewards during music listening is hypothesized to play a critical role relating enjoyable music listening experiences to prosocial outcomes (Savage et al., 2020). Extant studies suggest that the importance of music in adolescence is driven by a combination of intrinsic enjoyment and extrinsic social motivations. We propose that developmental changes in the reward system and the social brain may support heightened brain and behavioral responsivity to musical reward during adolescence. Specifically, we argue that greater sensitivity to the intrinsic and social rewards of music listening may motivate adolescents to share music with others, thus facilitating greater social bonds and prosocial behavior between those who share music with one another. Finally, we highlight how investigating this hypothesis may also provide insight into why music from listeners’ adolescence is preferred and elicits more autobiographical memories compared to music from outside this time period. Though current research on the developmental trajectory of music reward valuation across adolescence is limited, inquiries in this area hold relevance beyond reward processing to both prosocial behavior and lifelong memories.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

RK: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Validation, Funding acquisition, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. NK: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. PL: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing. JD: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This research was funded by a Northeastern University Center for Cognitive and Brain Health seed award to NK, PL, and JD and a Northeastern University Department of Psychology undergraduate research award to RK and JD.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Amodio, D. M., and Frith, C. D. (2006). Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 268–277. doi: 10.1038/nrn1884

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Antonucci, T. C., Ajrouch, K. J., Webster, N. J., and Zahodne, L. B. (2019). Social Relations Across the Life Span: Scientific Advances, Emerging Issues, and Future Challenges. Annu Rev Psychol. 1, 313–336. doi: 10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121318-085212

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakagiannis, S., and Tarrant, M. (2006). Can music bring people together? Effects of shared musical preference on intergroup bias in adolescence. Scand. J. Psychol. 47, 129–136. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2006.00500.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Belden, A., Quinci, M. A., Geddes, M., Donovan, N. J., Hanser, S. B., and Loui, P. (2023). Functional Organization of Auditory and Reward Systems in aging. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 35, 1570–1592. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_02028

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Belfi, A. M., and Loui, P. (2020). Musical anhedonia and rewards of music listening: current advances and a proposed model. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1464, 99–114. doi: 10.1111/nyas.14241

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Belfi, A. M., Moreno, G. L., Gugliano, M., and Neill, C. (2022). Musical reward across the lifespan. Aging Ment. Health 26, 932–939. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2021.1871881

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Berndt, T. J., and Hoyle, S. G. (1985). Stability and change in childhood and adolescent friendships. Dev. Psychol. 21, 1007–1015. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.21.6.1007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Berns, G. S., Capra, C. M., Moore, S., and Noussair, C. (2010). Neural mechanisms of the influence of popularity on adolescent ratings of music. NeuroImage 49, 2687–2696. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.070

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Berntsen, D., and Rubin, D. C. (2002). Emotionally charged autobiographical memories across the life span: The recall of happy, sad, traumatic and involuntary memories. Psychology and Aging. 17, 636–652. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.4.636

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialleck, K. A., Schaal, H.-P., Kranz, T. A., Fell, J., Elger, C. E., and Axmacher, N. (2011). Ventromedial prefrontal cortex activation is associated with memory formation for predictable rewards. PLoS One 6:e16695. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0016695

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blakemore, S.-J. (2008). The social brain in adolescence. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 267–277. doi: 10.1038/nrn2353

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blakemore, S.-J., den Ouden, H., Choudhury, S., and Frith, C. (2007). Adolescent development of the neural circuitry for thinking about intentions. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2, 130–139. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blankenstein, N. E., Telzer, E. H., Do, K. T., van Duijvenvoorde, A. C. K., and Crone, E. A. (2020). Behavioral and neural pathways supporting the development of prosocial and risk-taking behavior across adolescence. Child Dev. 91, e665–e681. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13292

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonneville-Roussy, A., Rentfrow, P. J., Xu, M. K., and Potter, J. (2013). Music through the ages: trends in musical engagement and preferences from adolescence through middle adulthood. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 105, 703–717. doi: 10.1037/a0033770

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Braams, B. R., Duijvenvoorde, A. C. K. van, Peper, J. S., and Crone, E. A. (2015). Longitudinal changes in adolescent risk-taking: A comprehensive study of neural responses to rewards, pubertal development, and risk-taking behavior. J Neurosci, 35, 7226–7238. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4764-14.2015

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Braams, B. R., and Crone, E. A. (2017). Peers and parents: a comparison between neural activation when winning for friends and mothers in adolescence. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 417–426. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw136

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brunet, E., Sarfati, Y., Hardy-Baylé, M. C., and Decety, J. (2000). A PET investigation of the attribution of intentions with a nonverbal task. NeuroImage 11, 157–166. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1999.0525

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardona, G., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., Nye, H., Rifà-Ros, X., and Ferreri, L. (2020). The impact of musical pleasure and musical hedonia on verbal episodic memory. Sci. Rep. 10:16113. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-72772-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlson, S. M., Koenig, M. A., and Harms, M. B. (2013). Theory of mind. WIREs. Cogn. Sci. 4, 391–402. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1232

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Casey, B., Galván, A., and Somerville, L. H. (2015). Beyond simple models of adolescence to an integrated circuit-based account: a commentary. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 128–130. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.12.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chein, J., Albert, D., O’Brien, L., Uckert, K., and Steinberg, L. (2011). Peers increase adolescent risk taking by enhancing activity in the brain’s reward circuitry. Dev. Sci. 14, F1–F10. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01035.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, T. W., Vijayakumar, N., Flournoy, J. C., Op de Macks, Z., Peake, S. J., Flannery, J. E., et al. (2020). Feeling left out or just surprised? Neural correlates of social exclusion and overinclusion in adolescence. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 20, 340–355. doi: 10.3758/s13415-020-00772-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cirelli, L. K., Einarson, K. M., and Trainor, L. J. (2014). Interpersonal synchrony increases prosocial behavior in infants. Dev. Sci. 17, 1003–1011. doi: 10.1111/desc.12193

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. R., Asarnow, R. F., Sabb, F. W., Bilder, R. M., Bookheimer, S. Y., Knowlton, B. J., et al. (2010). A unique adolescent response to reward prediction errors. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 669–671. doi: 10.1038/nn.2558

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Conway, M. A. (2005). Memory and the self. J. Mem. Lang. 53, 594–628. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2005.08.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cosme, D., Flournoy, J. C., Livingston, J. L., Lieberman, M. D., Dapretto, M., and Pfeifer, J. H. (2022). Testing the adolescent social reorientation model during self and other evaluation using hierarchical growth curve modeling with parcellated fMRI data. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 54:101089. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2022.101089

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Crocetti, E., Moscatelli, S., Van der Graaff, J., Rubini, M., Meeus, W., and Branje, S. (2016). The interplay of self-certainty and prosocial development in the transition from late adolescence to emerging adulthood. Eur. J. Personal. 30, 594–607. doi: 10.1002/per.2084

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Crone, E. A., and Dahl, R. E. (2012). Understanding adolescence as a period of social–affective engagement and goal flexibility. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 636–650. doi: 10.1038/nrn3313

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Crone, E. A., Sweijen, S. W., te Brinke, L. W., and van de Groep, S. (2022). “Chapter six—pathways for engaging in prosocial behavior in adolescence” in Advances in child development and behavior. ed. J. J. Lockman, vol. 63 (JAI), 149–190.

Google Scholar

Crovitz, H. F., and Schiffman, H. (1974). Frequency of episodic memories as a function of their age. Bull. Psychon. Soc. 4, 517–518. doi: 10.3758/BF03334277

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Curzel, F., Osiurak, F., Trân, E., Tillmann, B., Ripollés, P., and Ferreri, L. (2024). Enhancing musical pleasure through shared musical experience. iScience 27:109964. doi: 10.1016/j.isci.2024.109964

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dahl, R. E. (2004). Adolescent brain development: a period of vulnerabilities and opportunities. Keynote address. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1021, 1–22. doi: 10.1196/annals.1308.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Davidow, J. Y., Foerde, K., Galván, A., and Shohamy, D. (2016). An upside to reward sensitivity: the Hippocampus supports enhanced reinforcement learning in adolescence. Neuron 92, 93–99. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.08.031

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Decety, J., Jackson, P. L., Sommerville, J. A., Chaminade, T., and Meltzoff, A. N. (2004). The neural bases of cooperation and competition: an fMRI investigation. NeuroImage 23, 744–751. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.05.025

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dubé, L., and Le Bel, J. (2003). The content and structure of laypeople’s concept of pleasure. Cognit. Emot. 17, 263–295. doi: 10.1080/02699930302295

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dumontheil, I., Apperly, I. A., and Blakemore, S.-J. (2010). Online usage of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence. Dev. Sci. 13, 331–338. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00888.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ernst, M., Nelson, E. E., Jazbec, S., McClure, E. B., Monk, C. S., Leibenluft, E., et al. (2005). Amygdala and nucleus accumbens in responses to receipt and omission of gains in adults and adolescents. NeuroImage 25, 1279–1291. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.038

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fasano, M. C., Cabral, J., Stevner, A., Vuust, P., Cantou, P., Brattico, E., et al. (2022). The early adolescent brain on music: analysis of functional dynamics reveals engagement of orbitofrontal cortex reward system. Hum. Brain Mapp. 44, 429–446. doi: 10.1002/hbm.26060

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Faust, K. M., Carouso-Peck, S., Elson, M. R., and Goldstein, M. H. (2020). The origins of social knowledge in Altricial species. Ann. Rev. Dev. Psychol. 2, 225–246. doi: 10.1146/annurev-devpsych-051820-121446

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferreri, L., Mas-Herrero, E., Cardona, G., Zatorre, R. J., Antonijoan, R. M., Valle, M., et al. (2021). Dopamine modulations of reward-driven music memory consolidation. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1502, 85–98. doi: 10.1111/nyas.14656

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferreri, L., and Rodriguez-Fornells, A. (2017). Music-related reward responses predict episodic memory performance. Exp. Brain Res. 235, 3721–3731. doi: 10.1007/s00221-017-5095-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fett, A.-K. J., Shergill, S. S., Gromann, P. M., Dumontheil, I., Blakemore, S.-J., Yakub, F., et al. (2014). Trust and social reciprocity in adolescence – a matter of perspective-taking. J. Adolesc. 37, 175–184. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2013.11.011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Foulkes, L., and Blakemore, S.-J. (2016). Is there heightened sensitivity to social reward in adolescence? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 40, 81–85. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2016.06.016

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Franken, A., Keijsers, L., Dijkstra, J. K., and ter Bogt, T. (2017). Music preferences, friendship, and externalizing behavior in early adolescence: a SIENA examination of the music marker theory using the SNARE study. J. Youth Adolesc. 46, 1839–1850. doi: 10.1007/s10964-017-0633-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallagher, H. L., Happé, F., Brunswick, N., Fletcher, P. C., Frith, U., and Frith, C. D. (2000). Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of “theory of mind” in verbal and nonverbal tasks. Neuropsychologia 38, 11–21. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(99)00053-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Galvan, A., Hare, T. A., Parra, C. E., Penn, J., Voss, H., Glover, G., et al. (2006). Earlier development of the Accumbens relative to orbitofrontal cortex might underlie risk-taking behavior in adolescents. J. Neurosci. 26, 6885–6892. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1062-06.2006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Galván, A., and McGlennen, K. M. (2013). Enhanced striatal sensitivity to aversive reinforcement in adolescents versus adults. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 284–296. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00326

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunther Moor, B., Op de Macks, Z. A., Güroğlu, B., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., Van der Molen, M. W., and Crone, E. A. (2012). Neurodevelopmental changes of reading the mind in the eyes. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7, 44–52. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Güroğlu, B., van den Bos, W., and Crone, E. A. (2014). Sharing and giving across adolescence: an experimental study examining the development of prosocial behavior. Front. Psychol. 5:291. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00291

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Guyer, A. E., McClure-Tone, E. B., Shiffrin, N. D., Pine, D. S., and Nelson, E. E. (2009). Probing the neural correlates of anticipated peer evaluation in adolescence. Child Dev. 80, 1000–1015. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01313.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gweon, H., Dodell-Feder, D., Bedny, M., and Saxe, R. (2012). Theory of mind performance in children correlates with functional specialization of a brain region for thinking about thoughts. Child Dev. 83, 1853–1868. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01829.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hackel, L., Zaki, J., and Van Bavel, J. (2017). Social identity shapes social valuation: evidence from prosocial behavior and vicarious reward. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 1219–1228. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx045

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, H., Millear, P., Summers, M., and Isbel, B. (2021). Longitudinal research on perspective taking in adolescence: a systematic review. Adolesc. Res. Rev. 6, 125–150. doi: 10.1007/s40894-021-00150-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Holbrook, M. B., and Schindler, R. M. (1989). Some exploratory findings on the development of musical tastes. J. Consum. Res. 16, 119–124. doi: 10.1086/209200

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hove, M. J., and Risen, J. L. (2009). It’s all in the timing: interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Soc. Cogn. 27, 949–960. doi: 10.1521/soco.2009.27.6.949

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Humphrey, G., and Dumontheil, I. (2016). Development of risk-taking, perspective-taking, and inhibitory control during adolescence. Dev. Neuropsychol. 41, 59–76. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2016.1161764

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jakubowski, K., Eerola, T., Tillmann, B., Perrin, F., and Heine, L. (2020). A Cross-sectional study of reminiscence bumps for music-related memories in adulthood. Music Sci. 3:205920432096505. doi: 10.1177/2059204320965058

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jakubowski, K., and Ghosh, A. (2021). Music-evoked autobiographical memories in everyday life. Psychol. Music 49, 649–666. doi: 10.1177/0305735619888803

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Janata, P., Tomic, S. T., and Rakowski, S. K. (2007). Characterisation of music-evoked autobiographical memories. Memory 15, 845–860. doi: 10.1080/09658210701734593

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Janssen, S. M. J., Chessa, A. G., and Murre, J. M. J. (2007). Temporal distribution of favourite books, movies, and records: differential encoding and re-sampling. Memory 15, 755–767. doi: 10.1080/09658210701539646

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Janssen, S. M. J., and Murre, J. M. J. (2008). Reminiscence bump in autobiographical memory: unexplained by novelty, emotionality, valence, or importance of personal events. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 61, 1847–1860. doi: 10.1080/17470210701774242

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, R. M., Somerville, L. H., Li, J., Ruberry, E. J., Powers, A., Mehta, N., et al. (2014). Adolescent-specific patterns of behavior and neural activity during social reinforcement learning. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 14, 683–697. doi: 10.3758/s13415-014-0257-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kathios, N., Bloom, P. A., Singh, A., Bartlett, E., Algharazi, S., Siegelman, M., et al. (2023). Considering both experimenter-manipulated and self-reported familiarity in music-evoked autobiographical memories. Psy. Ar. Xiv. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/dpj6g

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kistler, M., Rodgers, K. B., Power, T., Austin, E. W., and Hill, L. G. (2010). Adolescents and music media: toward an involvement-mediational model of consumption and self-concept. J. Res. Adolesc. 20, 616–630. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00651.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koppel, J., and Berntsen, D. (2015). The peaks of life: the differential temporal locations of the reminiscence bump across disparate cueing methods. J. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn. 4, 66–80. doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2014.11.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kristen-Antonow, S. (2019). The role of ToM in creating a reminiscence bump for MEAMs from adolescence. Psychol. Music 47, 51–68. doi: 10.1177/0305735617735374

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Krumhansl, C. L. (2017). Listening niches across a century of popular music. Front. Psychol. 8:431. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00431

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Krumhansl, C. L., and Zupnick, J. A. (2013). Cascading reminiscence bumps in popular music. Psychol. Sci. 24, 2057–2068. doi: 10.1177/0956797613486486

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamont, A., and Hargreaves, D. J. (2019). Musical preference and social identity in adolescence. In (eds.) K. S. McFerran, P. Derrington, and S. Saarikallio. 109–118. Music and adolescents, An Oxford University press handbook. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Lamont, A., and Loveday, C. (2020). A new framework for understanding memories and preference for music. Music Sci. 3:205920432094831. doi: 10.1177/2059204320948315

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Larson, R. W. (2001). How U.S. children and adolescents spend time: what it does (and doesn’t) tell us about their development. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 10, 160–164. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00139

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lieberman, M. D., Straccia, M. A., Meyer, M. L., Du, M., and Tan, K. M. (2019). Social, self, (situational), and affective processes in medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC): causal, multivariate, and reverse inference evidence. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 99, 311–328. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.12.021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lippolis, M., Carraturo, G., Ferreri, L., Vuust, P., Müllensiefen, D., Matarelli, B., et al. (2023). Sensitivity to social reward in music behavior changes after music training in preadolescence. Adv. Cogn. Psychol. 19, 106–125. doi: 10.5709/acp-0409-z

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Loui, P., Patterson, S., Sachs, M. E., Leung, Y., Zeng, T., and Przysinda, E. (2017). White matter correlates of musical anhedonia: implications for evolution of music. Front. Psychol. 8:1664. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01664

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mannino, A., Di Maso, M., Viganò, M., Corbo, M., and Sozzi, M. (2024). Italian validation of the Barcelona music reward questionnaire. Music. Percept. 41, 307–319. doi: 10.1525/mp.2024.41.4.307

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mas-Herrero, E., Dagher, A., Farrés-Franch, M., and Zatorre, R. J. (2021). Unraveling the temporal dynamics of reward signals in music-induced pleasure with TMS. J. Neurosci. 41, 3889–3899. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0727-20.2020

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mas-Herrero, E., Dagher, A., and Zatorre, R. J. (2018). Modulating musical reward sensitivity up and down with transcranial magnetic stimulation. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2, 27–32. doi: 10.1038/s41562-017-0241-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mas-Herrero, E., Marco-Pallares, J., Lorenzo-Seva, U., Zatorre, R. J., and Rodriguez-Fornells, A. (2013). Individual differences in music reward experiences. Music. Percept. 31, 118–138. doi: 10.1525/mp.2013.31.2.118

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mather, M., and Schoeke, A. (2011). Positive outcomes enhance incidental learning for both younger and older adults. Front. Neurosci. 5:129. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2011.00129

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mills, K. L., Lalonde, F., Clasen, L. S., Giedd, J. N., and Blakemore, S.-J. (2014). Developmental changes in the structure of the social brain in late childhood and adolescence. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 123–131. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss113

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Miranda, D. (2013). The role of music in adolescent development: much more than the same old song. Int. J. Adolesc. Youth 18, 5–22. doi: 10.1080/02673843.2011.650182

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Miranda, D., and Claes, M. (2009). Music listening, coping, peer affiliation and depression in adolescence. Psychol. Music 37, 215–233. doi: 10.1177/0305735608097245

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morelli, S. A., Lee, I. A., Arnn, M. E., and Zaki, J. (2015). Emotional and instrumental support provision interact to predict well-being. Emotion 15, 484–493. doi: 10.1037/emo0000084

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morelli, S. A., Leong, Y. C., Carlson, R. W., Kullar, M., and Zaki, J. (2018). Neural detection of socially valued community members. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 115, 8149–8154. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1712811115

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mukerji, C. E., Lincoln, S. H., Dodell-Feder, D., Nelson, C. A., and Hooker, C. I. (2019). Neural correlates of theory-of-mind are associated with variation in children’s everyday social cognition. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 14, 579–589. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsz040

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Munawar, K., Kuhn, S. K., and Haque, S. (2018). Understanding the reminiscence bump: a systematic review. PLoS One 13:e0208595. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0208595

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, E. E., Leibenluft, E., McClure, E. B., and Pine, D. S. (2005). The social re-orientation of adolescence: a neuroscience perspective on the process and its relation to psychopathology. Psychol. Med. 35, 163–174. doi: 10.1017/s0033291704003915

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

North, A. C., and Hargreaves, D. J. (1999). Music and adolescent identity. Music. Educ. Res. 1, 75–92. doi: 10.1080/1461380990010107

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

North, A. C., Hargreaves, D. J., and O’Neill, S. A. (2000). The importance of music to adolescents. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 70, 255–272. doi: 10.1348/000709900158083

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

North, A. C., Tarrant, M., and Hargreaves, D. J. (2004). The effects of music on helping behavior: a field study. Environ. Behav. 36, 266–275. doi: 10.1177/0013916503256263

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nussenbaum, K., and Hartley, C. A. (2021). Developmental change in prefrontal cortex recruitment supports the emergence of value-guided memory. eLife 10:e69796. doi: 10.7554/eLife.69796

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Padilla-Walker, L. M., Carlo, G., and Memmott-Elison, M. K. (2018). Longitudinal change in adolescents’ prosocial behavior toward strangers, friends, and family. J. Res. Adolesc. 28, 698–710. doi: 10.1111/jora.12362

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Padmanabhan, A., and Luna, B. (2014). Developmental imaging genetics: linking dopamine function to adolescent behavior. Brain Cogn. 89, 27–38. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2013.09.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pagnoni, G., Zink, C. F., Montague, P. R., and Berns, G. S. (2002). Activity in human ventral striatum locked to errors of reward prediction. Nat. Neurosci, 5, 97–98. doi: 10.1038/nn802

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Peck, L. S. L., and Grealey, P. (2020). Autobiographical significance of meaningful musical experiences: reflections on youth and identity. Music Science 3:2059204320974221. doi: 10.1177/2059204320974221

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pillemer, D. B. (2001). Momentous events and the life story. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 5, 123–134. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.5.2.123

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Platz, F., Kopiez, R., Hasselhorn, J., and Wolf, A. (2015). The impact of song-specific age and affective qualities of popular songs on music-evoked autobiographical memories (MEAMs). Music. Sci. 19, 327–349. doi: 10.1177/1029864915597567

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Powers, K. E., Schaefer, L., Figner, B., and Somerville, L. H. (2022). Effects of peer observation on risky decision-making in adolescence: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 148, 783–812. doi: 10.1037/bul0000382

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Quinci, M. A., Belden, A., Goutama, V., Gong, D., Hanser, S., Donovan, N. J., et al. (2022). Longitudinal changes in auditory and reward systems following receptive music-based intervention in older adults. Sci. Rep. 12:11517. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-15687-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rentfrow, P. J., and Gosling, S. D. (2006). Message in a ballad: the role of music preferences in interpersonal perception. Psychol. Sci. 17, 236–242. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01691.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rilling, J. K., and Sanfey, A. G. (2011). The neuroscience of social decision-making. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62, 23–48. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131647

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, D. F., and Henriksen, L. (2004). “Adolescents and media” in Handbook of adolescent psychology (eds.) M. L. Richard and S. Laurence. (John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.), 487–521.

Google Scholar

Rosenbaum, G. M., Grassie, H. L., and Hartley, C. A. (2022). Valence biases in reinforcement learning shift across adolescence and modulate subsequent memory. eLife 11:e64620. doi: 10.7554/eLife.64620

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubin, D. C., and Schulkind, M. D. (1997). Distribution of important and word-cued autobiographical memories in 20-, 35-, and 70-year-old adults. Psychol. Aging 12, 524–535. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.12.3.524

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubin, D. C., Wetzler, S. E., and Nebes, R. D. (1986). Autobiographical memory across the lifespan (D. C. Rubin, Ed.; 1stst ed., pp. 202–222). Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Ruth, N., and Müllensiefen, D. (2021). Survival of musical activities. When do young people stop making music? PLoS One 16:e0259105. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0259105

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Salimpoor, V. N., van den Bosch, I., Kovacevic, N., McIntosh, A. R., Dagher, A., and Zatorre, R. J. (2013). Interactions between the nucleus accumbens and auditory cortices predict music reward value. Science (New York, N.Y.) 340, 216–219. doi: 10.1126/science.1231059

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Salimpoor, V. N., Benovoy, M., Larcher, K., Dagher, A., and Zatorre, R. J. (2011). Anatomically distinct dopamine release during anticipation and experience of peak emotion to music. Nat. Neurosci, 14, 257–262. doi: 10.1038/nn.2726

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Savage, P., Loui, P., Tarr, B., Schachner, A., Glowacki, L., Mithen, S., et al. (2020). Music as a coevolved system for social bondin. Behav. Brain Sci. 44:e59. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X20000333

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Saxe, R. R., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Scholz, J., and Pelphrey, K. A. (2009). Brain regions for perceiving and reasoning about other people in school-aged children. Child Dev. 80, 1197–1209. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01325.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schreuders, E., Braams, B. R., Blankenstein, N. E., Peper, J. S., Güroğlu, B., and Crone, E. A. (2018). Contributions of reward sensitivity to ventral striatum activity across adolescence and early adulthood. Child Dev. 89, 797–810. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13056

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulkind, M. D., Hennis, L. K., and Rubin, D. C. (1999). Music, emotion, and autobiographical memory: They’re playing your song. Mem. Cogn. 27, 948–955. doi: 10.3758/BF03201225

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, W., Dayan, P., and Montague, P. R. (1997). A Neural Substrate of Prediction and Reward. Science, 275, 1593–1599. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5306.1593

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sebastian, C. L., Fontaine, N. M. G., Bird, G., Blakemore, S.-J., De Brito, S. A., McCrory, E. J. P., et al. (2012). Neural processing associated with cognitive and affective theory of mind in adolescents and adults. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7, 53–63. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr023

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Selfhout, M. H. W., Branje, S. J. T., ter Bogt, T. F. M., and Meeus, W. H. J. (2009). The role of music preferences in early adolescents’ friendship formation and stability. J. Adolesc. 32, 95–107. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2007.11.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherman, L. E., Payton, A. A., Hernandez, L. M., Greenfield, P. M., and Dapretto, M. (2016). The Power of the like in adolescence: effects of peer influence on neural and behavioral responses to social media. Psychol. Sci. 27, 1027–1035. doi: 10.1177/0956797616645673

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Silverman, M. H., Jedd, K., and Luciana, M. (2015). Neural networks involved in adolescent reward processing: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. NeuroImage 122, 427–439. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.083

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Somerville, L. H. (2013). Special issue on the teenage brain: sensitivity to social evaluation. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 22, 121–127. doi: 10.1177/0963721413476512

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Somerville, L. H., Hare, T., and Casey, B. J. (2011). Frontostriatal maturation predicts cognitive control failure to appetitive cues in adolescents. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 2123–2134. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21572

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spear, L. P. (2000). The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 24, 417–463. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(00)00014-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stupacher, J., Maes, P.-J., Witte, M., and Wood, G. (2017). Music strengthens prosocial effects of interpersonal synchronization – if you move in time with the beat. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 72, 39–44. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2017.04.007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Symeonidou, I., Dumontheil, I., Chow, W.-Y., and Breheny, R. (2016). Development of online use of theory of mind during adolescence: an eye-tracking study. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 149, 81–97. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.11.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tamnes, C. K., Overbye, K., Ferschmann, L., Fjell, A. M., Walhovd, K. B., Blakemore, S.-J., et al. (2018). Social perspective taking is associated with self-reported prosocial behavior and regional cortical thickness across adolescence. Dev. Psychol. 54, 1745–1757. doi: 10.1037/dev0000541

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tarr, B., Launay, J., and Dunbar, R. I. M. (2014). Music and social bonding: “self-other” merging and neurohormonal mechanisms. Front. Psychol. 5:1096. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01096

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

te Brinke, L. W., van de Groep, S., van der Cruijsen, R., and Crone, E. A. (2023). Variability and change in adolescents’ prosocial behavior across multiple time scales. J. Res. Adolesc. 33, 575–590. doi: 10.1111/jora.12827

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Telzer, E. H. (2016). Dopaminergic reward sensitivity can promote adolescent health: a new perspective on the mechanism of ventral striatum activation. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 57–67. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.10.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Telzer, E. H., Masten, C. L., Berkman, E. T., Lieberman, M. D., and Fuligni, A. J. (2010). Gaining while giving: an fMRI study of the rewards of family assistance among White and Latino youth. Soc. Neurosci. 5, 508–518. doi: 10.1080/17470911003687913

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

ter Bogt, T. F. M., Vieno, A., Doornwaard, S. M., Pastore, M., and van den Eijnden, R. J. J. M. (2017). “You’re not alone”: music as a source of consolation among adolescents and young adults. Psychol. Music 45, 155–171. doi: 10.1177/0305735616650029

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Trainor, L. J., and Cirelli, L. (2015). Rhythm and interpersonal synchrony in early social development. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1337, 45–52. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12649

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Urošević, S., Collins, P., Muetzel, R., Lim, K., and Luciana, M. (2012). Longitudinal changes in behavioral approach system sensitivity and brain structures involved in reward processing during adolescence. Dev. Psychol. 48, 1488–1500. doi: 10.1037/a0027502

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van de Groep, S., Meuwese, R., Zanolie, K., Güroğlu, B., and Crone, E. A. (2020). Developmental changes and individual differences in trust and reciprocity in adolescence. J. Res. Adolesc. 30, 192–208. doi: 10.1111/jora.12459

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van der Graaff, J., Carlo, G., Crocetti, E., Koot, H. M., and Branje, S. (2018). Prosocial behavior in adolescence: gender differences in development and links with empathy. J. Youth Adolesc. 47, 1086–1099. doi: 10.1007/s10964-017-0786-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van Hoorn, J., McCormick, E. M., Rogers, C. R., Ivory, S. L., and Telzer, E. H. (2018). Differential effects of parent and peer presence on neural correlates of risk taking in adolescence. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 13, 945–955. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsy071

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Hoorn, J., Van Dijk, E., Güroğlu, B., and Crone, E. A. (2016). Neural correlates of prosocial peer influence on public goods game donations during adolescence. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 923–933. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Leijenhorst, L., Zanolie, K., Van Meel, C. S., Westenberg, P. M., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., and Crone, E. A. (2010). What motivates the adolescent? Brain regions mediating reward sensitivity across adolescence. Cereb. Cortex 20, 61–69. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp078

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vuust, P., Heggli, O. A., Friston, K. J., and Kringelbach, M. L. (2022). Music in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 23, 287–305. doi: 10.1038/s41583-022-00578-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wahlstrom, D., Collins, P., White, T., and Luciana, M. (2010). Developmental changes in dopamine neurotransmission in adolescence: behavioral implications and issues in assessment. Brain Cogn. 72, 146–159. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J., Xu, M., Jin, Z., Xia, L., Lian, Q., Huyang, S., et al. (2023). The Chinese version of the Barcelona music reward questionnaire (BMRQ): associations with personality traits and gender. Music. Sci. 27, 218–232. doi: 10.1177/10298649211034547

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Weigard, A., Chein, J., Albert, D., Smith, A., and Steinberg, L. (2014). Effects of anonymous peer observation on adolescents’ preference for immediate rewards. Dev. Sci. 17, 71–78. doi: 10.1111/desc.12099

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., and Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. Child Dev. 72, 655–684. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00304

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Westhoff, B., Blankenstein, N. E., Schreuders, E., Crone, E. A., and van Duijvenvoorde, A. C. K. (2021). Increased ventromedial prefrontal cortex activity in adolescence benefits prosocial reinforcement learning. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 52:101018. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2021.101018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Willander, J., and Larsson, M. (2006). Smell your way back to childhood: autobiographical odor memory. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 13, 240–244. doi: 10.3758/BF03193837

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiltermuth, S. S., and Heath, C. (2009). Synchrony and cooperation. Psychol. Sci. 20, 1–5. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02253.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaki, J., and Mitchell, J. P. (2013). Intuitive Prosociality. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 22, 466–470. doi: 10.1177/0963721413492764

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: music, social, reward, adolescence, development, emotion

Citation: Kudaravalli R, Kathios N, Loui P and Davidow JY (2024) Revisiting the musical reminiscence bump: insights from neurocognitive and social brain development in adolescence. Front. Psychol. 15:1472767. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1472767

Received: 02 August 2024; Accepted: 16 September 2024;
Published: 01 October 2024.

Edited by:

Kumiko Toyoshima, Osaka Shoin Women’s University, Japan

Reviewed by:

Eckart Altenmüller, Hanover University of Music Drama and Media, Germany

Copyright © 2024 Kudaravalli, Kathios, Loui and Davidow. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Juliet Y. Davidow, j.davidow@northeastern.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.