Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 08 October 2024
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Cognitive Benefits of Technologies Applied to Learning in Education View all 17 articles

The effect of augmented reality storybooks on the story comprehension and retelling of preschool children

  • Department of Preschool Education, Faculty of Education, Akdeniz University, Antalya, Türkiye

This study aimed to compare the retelling and story comprehension performance of two groups of preschool children—an experimental and a control group—who experienced printed and augmented reality storybooks. The participant group consisted of 90 participants, with 45 in the experimental group (22 girls, 23 boys) and 45 in the control group (21 girls, 24 boys). The average age of the children was 54.2 months. In the study, the researcher evaluated children’s story-retelling performance using the rubric and used a Story Comprehension Test to measure their story comprehension performance. The researchers utilized the ROAR application to incorporate augmented reality content into the books. In the pre-test phase, the teachers read the designated texts to the children in the experimental and control groups. After the eight-week break, the control group experienced the same texts with printed books, while the experimental group experienced them with augmented reality support. Each child participated individually in the reading process with the teacher. In the study, teachers asked the children to retell the story and asked the questions from the Story Comprehension Test at the end of each book reading section for pre-test and post-test measurements. The pre-test results revealed no significant difference in the two groups’ story-retelling and Story Comprehension Test scores. The post-test results indicated a significant difference in the story-retelling performance and Story Comprehension Test scores between the experimental and control groups, favoring the experimental group. Based on these findings, the augmented reality content can potentially enhance children’s retelling and story comprehension performances.

1 Introduction

The preschool period is a critical phase during which the foundations of children’s cognitive, emotional, social, and language development are established. The educational materials and activities provided to children during this period significantly influenced their future learning and life skills. Among these materials, picture storybooks hold a special place. Reading picture storybooks can contribute to children’s acquisition of knowledge, learning to communicate, forming their cultural identities, enhancing their imagination, and fulfilling their desire for discovery (Carrión Candel et al., 2020; Takacs and Bus, 2018; Yamaji et al., 2020). Additionally, it is known that picture storybooks are beneficial for developing children’s current literacy skills (Fears and Lockman, 2020; Hu and Commeyras, 2008; Wang, 2022) and language skills (Grolig et al., 2019). Story reading provides a meaningful context for children to acquire skills such as listening comprehension, retelling, following plot structures, and inferential thinking (Collins, 2016; Florit et al., 2011; Morrow, 1985; Strasser and Río, 2014). By contributing to language development, reading stories enables children to learn new words and grasp story elements (Breit-Smith et al., 2017; Han and Neuharth-Pritchett, 2015). Furthermore, it has been determined that reading storybooks contributes to language skills and academic achievement from the preschool period to university (Mol and Bus, 2011). Therefore, it is worth considering how children’s interaction with picture storybooks can be supported.

With technological advancements, storybooks have started to be presented in different formats. As technology-enhanced books become prevalent, they have gained increased attention from researchers. Indeed, numerous studies have been conducted on topics such as electronic books (Lauricella et al., 2014), interactive books (Lim et al., 2021; Zipke, 2017), and multimedia book applications (Zhou and Yadav, 2017; Tsou et al., 2006). These books, used today, have also transformed children’s early literacy experiences compared to previous generations. Children nowadays have the opportunity to experience both printed and digital books (Kucirkova, 2016). Each type of book has its distinctive features. Printed storybooks typically consist of static illustrations alongside the text, whereas digital storybooks offer multimedia features that enhance the story’s content and allow children to listen to the narrative (Merjovaara et al., 2020; O'Byrne et al., 2018). Augmented reality technology, which has become increasingly prevalent in recent years, combines printed and digital books’ features, enabling innovative storybooks to be created (Danaei et al., 2020; Şimşek and Direkci, 2023).

According to Azuma (1997), augmented reality is the innovative integration of real-world and virtual elements, enabling seamless interaction between the two realms. This technology presents a format where natural environment images are used as a background, and digital content (such as animation, 3D content, etc.) is overlaid on top, enabling user interaction (Billinghurst et al., 2001). In this regard, it bridges the real and virtual worlds through computer screens or mobile applications. The relevant literature has listed numerous benefits of augmented reality applications. These studies have shown that augmented reality applications reduce cognitive load (Alvarez-Marin and Velazquez-Iturbide, 2021; Cheng, 2015; Keller et al., 2021; Ibili and Billinghurst, 2019; Wilson, 2015; Wen, 2021), facilitates comprehension of abstract concepts (Yoon et al., 2017), enhance learning performance (Chang et al., 2015; Chen, 2019; Chin et al., 2021; Ferrer-Torregrosa et al., 2015), increase learning motivation (Amores-Valencia et al., 2022; Chiang et al., 2014; Di Serio et al., 2013; Ferrer-Torregrosa et al., 2015; Khan et al., 2019), and support academic achievement (Barreira et al., 2012; Duenser, 2008; Ibáñez et al., 2020; Ömurtak and Zeybek, 2022). Furthermore, augmented reality applications are enjoyable (Wojciechowski and Cellary, 2013) and engaging (Delello, 2014; Fidan and Tuncel, 2019; Perez-Lopez and Contero, 2013; Tomi and Rambli, 2013), as well as contributing to the development of positive attitudes toward the subject among students (Şahin and Yılmaz, 2020; Akçayır et al., 2016).

One of the applications of augmented reality technology is in storybooks. Augmented reality is often used in storybooks to enhance immersion through animations and virtual pop-ups (Kljun et al., 2019). Augmented reality storybooks incorporate characters and objects in 2D or 3D animations, accompanied by diverse technical effects such as videos, sounds, and interactive functions like touch, zoom, and rotate (Alhumaidan et al., 2018). This gives readers a reading experience that dynamically interacts with virtual content in a real-world setting (Cheng and Tsai, 2014; Green et al., 2019). According to Santos et al. (2016), when children engage with augmented reality storybooks, they can visualize information within a context-rich environment. This immersive experience enables them to forge genuine connections between educational content and the real world. Previous research has reported a range of advantages of augmented reality storybooks, including their ability to be engaging (Soon et al., 2022), enhance attention (Aydoğdu, 2022), contribute to learning (Duenser et al., 2012), improve empathy skills (Gil et al., 2014), enable interactive storytelling (Zhou et al., 2004), increase reading motivation (Roumba and Nicolaidou, 2022), and foster reading habits (Anuardi et al., 2022). One of the benefits mentioned in the literature is that the multisensory stimuli provided by augmented reality contribute to understanding the story (Ertem, 2009; Kao et al., 2016; Pan et al., 2021).

When reviewing the relevant literature, studies have been conducted on the effects of augmented reality storybooks on comprehension at different educational levels. Duenser (2008) has found through these studies that augmented reality storybooks offer a favorable learning environment that effectively supports readers with lower abilities. Moreover, in many studies, it has been found that students who read augmented reality storybooks have higher comprehension performance compared to groups reading printed books (Çetinkaya Özdemir and Akyol, 2021; Ebadi and Ashrafabadi, 2022; Şimşek and Direkci, 2023). Additionally, students who experienced augmented reality storybooks demonstrated better performance in retelling (Danaei et al., 2020), answering inferential questions (Liu et al., 2024), and learning retention (Bursalı and Yılmaz, 2019). However, Şimşek and Direkci's (2023) study found no significant difference in basic comprehension levels between augmented reality intervention and traditional reading. Similarly, Tobar-Munoz et al. (2017) did not detect a significant difference in total scores between groups in their study. Overall, when examining the studies in the relevant literature, it can be said that augmented reality storybooks have the potential to support comprehension.

In the conducted studies, comprehension processes have generally been evaluated through tests (Bursalı and Yılmaz, 2019; Çetinkaya Özdemir and Akyol, 2021; Ebadi and Ashrafabadi, 2022; Şimşek and Direkci, 2023; Tobar-Munoz et al., 2017) and retelling (Çetin and Ulusoy, 2023). Limited studies combine retelling and testing (Danaei et al., 2020; Liu et al., 2024). Therefore, this study used both assessment methods to yield more in-depth results. Furthermore, when examining studies conducted on augmented reality and language instruction in the literature, it can be observed that there is a scarcity of augmented reality studies with preschool children (Cai et al., 2022; Parmaxi and Demetriou, 2020). There is evidence of a connection between preschool children’s listening comprehension skills and elementary school children’s reading comprehension skills (Kendeou et al., 2007; Kendeou et al., 2009). Indeed, several studies have found that understanding narratives in preschool years predicts later reading comprehension (Kim, 2016; Language and Reading Research Consortium and Chiu, 2018). Therefore, the impact of augmented reality storybooks, which have clear findings supporting reading comprehension in elementary and later grades, on preschool children is an essential research topic.

1.1 Current study

This study examined and compared the retelling and story comprehension performance of preschool children who engage with printed and augmented reality storybooks. The following research questions were addressed in the study:

RQ1: What was the difference in retelling performance between children participating in the traditional storybook narration activity and those participating in the augmented reality storybook narration activity?

RQ2: What was the difference in story comprehension performance between children participating in the traditional storybook narration activity and those participating in the augmented reality storybook narration activity?

2 Methodology

2.1 Participants

This study was conducted in preschools located in Antalya, Türkiye. The participant group consisted of 90 participants, with 45 in the experimental group (22 girls, 23 boys) and 45 in the control group (21 girls, 24 boys). The average age of the children was 54.2 months. The children were randomly assigned to either the experimental group, where they experienced augmented reality storybooks under the teacher’s guidance, or the control group, where the teacher individually read to each participant. The participants were enrolled in four classrooms, and the teachers were included in the research process to facilitate the children’s comfortable expression. This choice was made to ensure that the children could express themselves freely. Moreover, preliminary interviews indicated that teachers and children were accustomed to using technological applications in the classroom.

2.2 The augmented reality storybook and application

This research aimed to provide children with an engaging reading experience by transforming three selected picture books into an augmented reality-supported storybook format. In line with this objective, the opinions of the teachers in the classrooms where the application was implemented were obtained to determine the suitability of the books titled “Aç Tırtıl (The Very Hungry Caterpillar),” “Kafası Karışık Bukalemun (The Mixed-Up Chameleon)” and “Huysuz Uğurböceği (The Grouchy Ladybug)” for children and the research. After identifying the books, a digital content scan was conducted to convert the books into an augmented reality-supported storybook format. Various video contents were identified, and necessary permissions were obtained to adjust the videos. The augmented reality content to be integrated into the reading text should contribute to understanding and consistency with the text. Thus, both the texts and visuals had been prepared in a manner consistent with the narrative flow. Consequently, each page in the printed book aligned with the pages of the augmented reality storybook. The differences were achieved by adding animations and voiceovers on certain pages. To highlight these pages, a small tablet icon had been placed on them. As a result, when teachers read the book with children, the children also heard the same sentences that were voiced using the augmented reality content. In this study, the ROAR application had been selected as the augmented reality tool. The ROAR application allows for content creation via the website “https://theroar.io/.” Through this website, the visuals in the book had been matched with the digital content developed by the researcher. The ROAR application was chosen due to its utility for integrating digital content into printed text and its ability to facilitate the process for teachers during implementation. After the content matching was completed, the associated digital content was triggered when the ROAR application was opened on a tablet and pointed at the visual in the printed book. Consequently, the augmented reality content was activated solely on the designated visual. Data Collection Tools.

In the study, the researcher evaluated children’s story-retelling performance using the rubric suggested by Cruz de Quiros et al. (2012). This rubric included indicators such as “Setting When and Where, Characters, Event/Plot, Problem, and Solution.” Each indicator was assessed on a scale of 3 points, resulting in a score range of 0–15 for each story. This rubric was selected to evaluate children’s storytelling performance more systematically in this study. In addition to assessing children’s retelling performance, a Story Comprehension Test was used to measure their story comprehension performance. This test consists of nine open-ended questions prepared based on the three picture books. The researcher developed the questions. A draft scoring rubric was prepared to ensure a reliable evaluation of the open-ended questions. The draft rubric scoring key was presented for evaluation by the four teachers participating in the study. As a result of the assessment, the questions prepared by the researcher, specifically the 3rd question (“What happened after the caterpillar became enormous?”) and the 4th question (“What would happen when the chameleon got cold?”), were revised as follows. Thus, a consensus was reached, and the final version of the rubric scoring key was established. According to the final scoring rubric, incorrect answers in the open-ended questions were evaluated as 0 points, partially correct answers as 1 point, and correct answers as 2 points. Additionally, two teachers participated in the evaluation process alongside the researcher, and the agreement between the results was assessed. Here are some sample questions from the Story Comprehension Test:

1. Name four fruits that the caterpillar ate.

2. How did the caterpillar’s stomachache go away?

3. What happened after the caterpillar became enormous and fat?

4. What would happen when the chameleon got cold and hungry?

5. What would the chameleon do when it was hungry?

6. Name four animals that the chameleon wanted to resemble.

7. Name four animals that the grumpy ladybug encountered.

8. What did the wet, hungry, and tiredladybug eat for dinner? Who did it eat with?

9. What were the fireflies doing when the ladybugs fell asleep?

2.3 Procedures

This study is a pre-test, post-test, and control group design conducted as a quasi-experimental study. Before the implementation, ethical approval was obtained from the Ethics Committee of Akdeniz University, and permission to conduct the study was obtained from the Antalya Provincial Directorate of National Education. Subsequently, meetings were held with the teachers working in the implementation schools to provide information about the study. The implementation phase began once the teachers fully understood the principles of the augmented reality storybook. The researcher presented in the classroom to provide technical support and oversee the implementation process. In the initial phase of the study, teachers read three printed texts to children in both the experimental and control groups. After each book was read, the children were asked to retell the story. Subsequently, questions from the Story Comprehension Test were posed to the children. The teacher recorded the children’s storytelling performances and their responses to the comprehension test during both the pre-test and post-test. These processes were conducted in the classroom environment where the books were read. The pre-test phase was concluded after the children answered the questions, and the teacher recorded responses. An eight-week break was provided with the assumption that the content of the books could be remembered. During this period, the teachers did not intervene in the classrooms regarding this research. However, 1 week before the post-test phase with the experimental group, the teachers read the “Minik Tohum (The Tiny Seed)” book, which was not included in the assessment process, together with the children as an augmented reality-enriched reading experience. This practice was conducted to familiarize the children with augmented reality storybooks and make them feel more comfortable during the reading process in the post-test application. Tablet computers were used during the implementation process, and the researcher brought these devices to the classroom with the necessary applications installed. After an 8-week interval, the teacher read the same printed books to the control group. Meanwhile, the experimental group experienced the augmented reality storybooks. During this process, the teacher read the printed book to the children and used the tablet on the visuals that required activation of the augmented reality content. The augmented reality application activated an animation on the visual, which was accompanied by a narration of the story. As a result, the teacher remained more passive in situations where the augmented reality application was used. Visuals related to this application are presented in Figure 1. Each child participated individually in the reading process with the teacher. After the reading process, each child retold the story and answered the questions from the Story Comprehension Test. The children were informed that there were no right or wrong answers to the questions and that it was not an exam to encourage them to think freely. After collecting the post-test data, the analysis phase was initiated.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Augmented reality storybook application view.

2.4 Data analysis

Normality tests were initially performed to determine the analysis to be conducted in the study. Due to the normal distribution of the data, an independent samples t-test was used to examine possible differences between the experimental and control groups in the analysis of pre-test results. Similarly, a paired samples t-test was conducted to determine within-group differences in the analysis of post-test results, considering the normal distribution of the data. Furthermore, independent samples t-tests were utilized to examine potential variances between the experimental and control groups. Cohen’s d was employed to determine the effect size when a statistically significant difference was found. Kendall’s W Concordance coefficient was calculated to evaluate the agreement between the two teachers and the researcher involved in the Story Comprehension Test evaluation process. The results indicated a concordance coefficient of 0.964 for the pre-test and 0.973 for the post-test.

3 Results

The study’s data analysis commenced with a normality test. It was determined that the retelling and Story Comprehension Test scores for both the experimental and control groups followed a normal distribution. Subsequently, the analysis of the data obtained from the pre-tests revealed no significant difference in the retelling and Story Comprehension Test scores between the children in the two groups within a 95% confidence interval. The means, standard deviations, and results of the independent samples t-test for the pre-test processes are presented in Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Mean, standard deviation and results of the independent samples t-test for the pre-test.

Upon reviewing Table 1, it is evident that there was no notable difference in the story-retelling performance in the experimental and control groups (t = 1.340, p > 0.05). Similar results were obtained for the Story Comprehension Test scores between the groups (t = 1.392, p > 0.05). Therefore, it can be concluded that there is no significant difference in the comprehension and retelling performance of the children who participated in the study without any intervention. No intervention was conducted for the control group during the post-test phase of the study. However, it was found that the retelling score of the control group increased from 22.115 to 23.622. Nevertheless, no significant distinction was observed between the pre-test and post-test scores of the control group (t = −1.843, p > 0.05). Similarly, the Story Comprehension Test scores of the control group increased from 11.177 to 11.888. However, there was no significant difference between the scores (t = −1.907, p > 0.05). In contrast, significant differences were discovered in the pre-test and post-test scores of the experimental group, indicating notable variations in retelling performance (t = −13.984, p < 0.05) and Story Comprehension Test (t = −10.715, p < 0.05). Furthermore, the disparity between the post-test scores of the experimental and control groups was assessed. The means, standard deviations, and results of the independent samples t-test for the post-test are presented in Table 2.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mean, standard deviation and results of the independent samples t-test for the post-test.

Based on the findings in Table 2, a significant difference was evident between the retelling performance of the story in the experimental and control groups (t = 5.701, p < 0.05). Similar results were found for the Story Comprehension Test scores indicating a significant difference between the two groups (t = 6.075, p < 0.05). The findings demonstrated that the augmented reality intervention better supported the understanding of the story compared to traditional narration. Additionally, the retelling performance of the children also improved with the augmented reality intervention. The effect size analyses indicated a substantial effect size for both the retelling and Story Comprehension Test scores, highlighting the substantial difference between the two groups (Cohen, 1988).

Given the information provided, the research questions can be addressed and answered in the following manner:

RQ1: The retelling performance of children who participated in the augmented reality storybook narration activity is better than that of children who participated in the traditional story narration activity.

RQ2: The story comprehension performance of children who participated in the augmented reality storybook narration activity is better than that of children who participated in the traditional story narration activity.

4 Discussion

This study delved into the differences in comprehension levels between the control group, which was exposed to printed books with traditional storytelling, and the experimental group, which engaged in augmented reality storybook narration. The children were tasked with retelling the stories they heard and answering the questions in the Story Comprehension Test to gauge this difference. In the pre-test phase, there was no discernible difference between the two groups regarding retelling and Story Comprehension Test scores. However, the final test results unveiled a compelling revelation-augmented reality storybook narration that significantly bolstered comprehension and retelling, surpassing the effectiveness of traditional storytelling using printed books.

According to the research findings, children who participated in augmented reality storybook narration performed better in retelling the story than in traditional storytelling activities. Augmented reality studies conducted with preschool children demonstrate that this technology is well-liked by children (Rambli et al., 2013), provides an enjoyable learning environment for children (Albayrak and Yılmaz, 2022), enhances children’s motivation (Aydoğdu, 2022), and captures children’s interest (Bülbül and Özdinç, 2022). Therefore, augmented reality storybooks may have also captured children’s interest and contributed to their comprehension. It can be argued that this situation supports children’s performance in retelling the story. Another factor that may support children’s performance is the multimedia features within the augmented reality storybooks. Elements such as the tone of voice of narrators that can convey emotions and moods, music, and sound effects may capture the readers’ attention and immerse them in the story, potentially enhancing their imagination as if they were the narrative’s main characters. Additionally, animations can better depict characters’ facial expressions and gestures than static images (Danaei et al., 2020). These features provide augmented reality with a rich and vibrant reading experience (ChanLin, 2018). Furthermore, augmented reality support enhances students’ learning experiences by providing interactivity and visual representation (Bujak et al., 2013; Teng et al., 2018). Consequently, with their existing features, augmented reality storybooks can support children’s storytelling performance. Indeed, previous studies in the literature also support these results. In their study, Liu et al. (2024) compared comprehension levels between two groups. One group read an augmented reality storybook, while the other read a printed picture book. The results showed that participants were particularly successful in the narrative structures related to the setting and plot during the retelling process.

Furthermore, using augmented reality, storybooks helped participants better understand the characters and their emotional changes (Liu et al., 2024). In contrast to this study, Danaei et al. (2020) found no difference in the setting between groups in their research. However, when looking at the total scores, it was determined that augmented reality storybooks improved retelling performance (Danaei et al., 2020). Another research conducted by Çetin and Ulusoy (2023) with third-grade students also supports these results. Upon reviewing the literature, it can be observed that the participants in the relevant studies were elementary and post-elementary school students. Therefore, the results regarding how augmented reality storybooks affect the retelling performance of preschool children are essential for the relevant literature.

According to the research results, children who participated in augmented reality storybook narration activities exhibited better performance in story comprehension than in traditional storytelling activities. According to the study by Takacs and Bus (2016), children exposed to an animated book understand the story better than those who view static images. In this regard, the ability of augmented reality to integrate verbal and non-verbal information and provide dynamic interaction may assist children in comprehending the text (Alsowat, 2016; Dunleavy et al., 2009). Additionally, augmented reality storybooks can offer children a sense of presence and an immersive environment through audiovisual content (Ebadi and Ashrafabadi, 2022). Consequently, it can be concluded that augmented reality storybooks support children’s story comprehension performance. Studies in the literature indicate that augmented reality storybooks enhance users’ comprehension performance. Yılmaz et al. (2017) determined that children who interacted with these books displayed strong story comprehension in their study with five- and six-year-olds. The majority of other studies conducted on this topic involve students in elementary and higher grades, and these studies also provide evidence that augmented reality storybooks support comprehension (Bursalı and Yılmaz, 2019; Çetinkaya Özdemir and Akyol, 2021; Ebadi and Ashrafabadi, 2022). Some of these studies show that augmented reality interventions do not create a significant difference compared to printed books in simple comprehension-based questions (Danaei et al., 2020; Şimşek and Direkci, 2023). However, users perform better in answering implicit questions (Danaei et al., 2020; Liu et al., 2024; Şimşek and Direkci, 2023) and higher-level questions related to reorganization, evaluation, and appreciation compared to printed books (Şimşek and Direkci, 2023). Augmented reality storybooks can support users’ comprehension performance with their ability to appeal to different senses and make the story more tangible. For example, children who interacted with an augmented reality storybook were more likely to provide correct answers to the question, “What were the fireflies doing when the ladybugs fell asleep?” The reason behind this could be that the visuals supported by sound and video make it easier to follow the flow of the story.

This study, along with various studies in the literature, provides evidence that augmented reality storybooks support users’ comprehension performance. Enriching the learning experience using multimedia content, such as text-to-speech, video, and interactive elements, can be a reason for this observed difference (Billinghurst and Duenser, 2012; Yuen et al., 2011). Indeed, incorporating information through multiple channels, encompassing both auditory and visual modalities, has contributed to more effective learning and better retention than processing information through a single channel (Bus et al., 2015). Thus, multimedia content can reduce cognitive load and enhance the story’s comprehensibility (Kao et al., 2016). The relevant literature also supports the idea that multimedia content can enhance preschool children’s comprehension (Altun, 2024) and retelling performance (Crawshaw et al., 2020; Diehm et al., 2020). However, the integrated multimedia content must be consistent with the text. Multimedia content that is not aligned with the story may distract children’s attention from the narrative and increase cognitive load, thereby reducing children’s comprehension performance (Chang et al., 2011; Takacs and Bus, 2016; Takacs et al., 2015).

5 Conclusion, limitations and recommendations

This study compared the comprehension levels of preschool children who experienced printed and augmented reality storybooks. In this context, children’s books titled “Aç Tırtıl (The Very Hungry Caterpillar),” “Kafası Karışık Bukalemun (The Mixed-Up Chameleon)” and “Huysuz Uğurböceği (The Grouchy Ladybug)” were enriched with augmented reality content. Subsequently, teachers read the books in printed and augmented reality-supported formats and engage in activities. In order to compare the groups, children retold the stories and answered the questions from the Story Comprehension Test. The results indicated that children who listened to augmented reality storybooks demonstrated higher comprehension and retelling performance than children who listened to printed storybooks. The consistently high scores in both the test and retelling provide in-depth evidence supporting the effectiveness of augmented reality storybooks in enhancing comprehension. Additionally, the limited data available in the literature regarding preschool children makes the findings of this study noteworthy.

This study has several limitations. Firstly, the participants were children between the ages of four and five. Thus, the results are limited to this age range. However, future studies could involve children in different age groups within the preschool period, enabling broader interpretations regarding the impact of augmented reality storybooks on preschool children’s comprehension and retelling performance.

Additionally, the data in this study is limited to 90 children. While studies testing new technological devices and applications generally have smaller sample sizes, a more significant number of studies are needed for the generalizability of the data. The current study’s lack of a follow-up assessment limits the research. Therefore, there is an urgent need for future studies should include follow-up assessments that could provide insights into the lasting effects of augmented reality interventions. The researcher did not use multimedia content on every page in the augmented reality storybooks created in this study. This decision was made to maintain the narrative of a storybook and avoid creating an environment solely focused on watching videos. The integration of multimedia content into the books was done in consultation with teachers and placed in locations deemed appropriate by the researcher. As there are currently no established design principles for augmented reality storybooks regarding factors such as appropriate duration and frequency of multimedia experiences in the literature, future studies should focus on developing design principles that cater to different age groups and align with the characteristics of different texts. Furthermore, this study used videos as the augmented reality content. Subsequent studies could explore the role of interactive options or 3D visuals in children’s comprehension and retelling performance.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the author, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Akdeniz University Rectorate Social and Human Sciences Scientific Research and Publication Ethics Committee. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

EEŞ: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author declares that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Akçayır, M., Akçayır, G., Pektaş, H. M., and Ocak, M. A. (2016). Augmented reality in science laboratories: the effects of augmented reality on university students' laboratory skills and attitudes toward science laboratories. Comput. Hum. Behav. 57, 334–342. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.12.054

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Albayrak, S., and Yılmaz, R. M. (2022). An investigation of pre-school children's interactions with augmented reality applications. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Interact. 38, 165–184. doi: 10.1080/10447318.2021.1926761

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Alhumaidan, H., Lo, K., and Selby, A. (2018). Co-designing with children a collaborative augmented reality book based on a primary school textbook. Int. J. Child Comput. Interact. 15, 24–36. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcci.2017.11.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Alsowat, H. H. (2016). Breaking down the classroom walls: augmented reality effect on EFL reading comprehension, self-efficacy, autonomy and attitudes. Stud. English Lang. Teach. 5, 1–23. doi: 10.22158/selt.v5n1p1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Altun, D. (2024). The effects of e-stories on preschoolers' narrative comprehension, retelling and reading attitudes among poor and good comprehenders. J. Early Child. Lit. 24, 471–504. doi: 10.1177/14687984221079010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Alvarez-Marin, A., and Velazquez-Iturbide, J. (2021). Augmented reality and engineering education: a systematic review. Trans. Learn. Technol. 14, 817–831. doi: 10.1109/TLT.2022.3144356

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Amores-Valencia, A., Burgos, D., and Branch-Bedoya, J. W. (2022). Influence of motivation and academic performance in the use of augmented reality in education. A systematic review. Front. Psychol. 13:1011409. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1011409

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Anuardi, M. A. K., Mustapha, S., and Mohammed, M. N. (2022). Towards increase reading habit for preschool children through interactive augmented reality storybook. 2022 IEEE 12th symposium on computer applications & industrial electronics (ISCAIE) (pp. 252–257). IEEE.

Google Scholar

Aydoğdu, F. (2022). Augmented reality for preschool children: an experience with educational contents. Br. J. Educ. Technol. 53, 326–348. doi: 10.1111/bjet.13168

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Azuma, R. (1997). A survey of augmented reality. Presence Teleop. Virt. 6, 355–385. doi: 10.1162/pres.1997.6.4.355

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barreira, J., Bessa, M., Pereira, L. C., Adão, T., Peres, E., and Magalhães, L. (2012). MOW: augmented reality game to learn words in different languages: case study: learning English names of animals in elementary school. In 7th Iberian conference on information systems and technologies (CISTI 2012) (pp. 1–6). IEEE.

Google Scholar

Billinghurst, M., and Duenser, A. (2012). Augmented reality in the classroom. Computer 45, 56–63. doi: 10.1109/MC.2012.111

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Billinghurst, M., Kato, H., and Poupyrev, I. (2001). Collaboration with tangible augmented reality interfaces. In Proceedings of the HCI 2001, 797–803.

Google Scholar

Breit-Smith, A., van Kleeck, A., Prendeville, J. A., and Pan, W. (2017). Preschool children's exposure to story grammar elements during parent-child book reading. J. Res. Read. 40, 345–364. doi: 10.1111/1467-9817.12071

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bujak, K. R., Radu, I., Catrambone, R., MacIntyre, B., Zheng, R., and Golubski, G. (2013). A psychological perspective on augmented reality in the mathematics classroom. Comput. Educ. 68, 536–544. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.02.017

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bülbül, H., and Özdinç, F. (2022). How real is augmented reality in preschool? Examination of young children's AR experiences. Kuramsal Eğitimbilim 15, 884–906. doi: 10.30831/akukeg.1098113

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bursalı, H., and Yılmaz, R. M. (2019). Effect of augmented reality applications on secondary school students' reading comprehension and learning permanency. Comput. Hum. Behav. 95, 126–135. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2019.01.035

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bus, A. G., Takacs, Z. K., and Kegel, C. A. T. (2015). Affordances and limitations of electronic storybooks for young children's emergent literacy. Dev. Rev. 35, 79–97. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2014.12.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cai, Y., Pan, Z., and Liu, M. (2022). Augmented reality technology in language learning: a meta‐analysis. J. Comput. Assist. Learn. 38, 929–945. doi: 10.1111/jcal.12661

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrión Candel, E., Pérez Agustín, M., Matilde Barrios Vicente, I., and Aparicio Molina, Y. (2020). Benefits of introducing picture books through ICTs in bilingual settings: an example with cuadernia and press here by herve tullet. ARTSEDUCA 27, 144–158. doi: 10.6035/Artseduca.2020.27.11

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Çetin, H., and Ulusoy, M. (2023). The effect of augmented reality-based reading environments on retelling skills: formative experiment. Educ. Inf. Technol. 28, 5849–5873. doi: 10.1007/s10639-022-11415-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Çetinkaya Özdemir, E., and Akyol, H. (2021). Effect of augmented reality-based reading activities on some reading variables and participation in class. Int. J. Progress. Educ. 17, 135–154. doi: 10.29329/ijpe.2021.366.9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, Y. L., Hou, H. T., Pan, C. Y., Sung, Y. T., and Chang, K. E. (2015). Apply an augmented reality in a mobile guidance to increase sense of place for heritage places. J. Educ. Technol. Soc. 18, 166–178. doi: 10.2307/jeductechsoci.18.2.166

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, C. C., Lei, H., and Tseng, J. S. (2011). Media presentation mode, English listening comprehension and cognitive load in ubiquitous learning environments: modality effect or redundancy effect? Australas. J. Educ. Technol. 27, 633–654. doi: 10.14742/ajet.942

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

ChanLin, L. J. (2018). Bridging children’s reading with an augmented reality story library. Libri 68, 219–229. doi: 10.1515/libri-2018-0017

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Y. C. (2019). Effect of mobile augmented reality on learning performance, motivation, and math anxiety in a math course. J. Educ. Comput. Res. 57, 1695–1722. doi: 10.1177/0735633119854036

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, K. H. (2015). Reading an augmented reality book: an exploration of learners' cognitive load, motivation, and attitudes. Australas. J. Educ. Technol. 33, 53–69. doi: 10.14742/ajet.2820

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, K. H., and Tsai, C. C. (2014). Children and parents' reading of an augmented reality picture book: analyses of behavioral patterns and cognitive attainment. Comput. Educ. 72, 302–312. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.12.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiang, T. H., Yang, S. J., and Hwang, G. J. (2014). An augmented reality-based mobile learning system to improve students' learning achievements and motivations in natural science inquiry activities. J. Educ. Technol. Soc. 17, 352–365. doi: 10.2307/jeductechsoci.17.4.352

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chin, K. Y., Kao, Y. C., and Wang, C. S. (2021). Effects of augmented reality technology in a mobile touring system on university students' learning performance and interest. Australas. J. Educ. Technol. 37, 27–42. doi: 10.14742/ajet.5841

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd Edn: United States: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Collins, M. F. (2016). Supporting inferential thinking in preschoolers: effects of discussion on children's story comprehension. Early Educ. Dev. 27, 932–956. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2016.1170523

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawshaw, C. E., Kern, F., Mertens, U., and Rohlfing, K. J. (2020). Children's narrative elaboration after reading a storybook versus viewing a video. Front. Psychol. 11:569891. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.569891

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cruz de Quiros, A. M., Lara‐Alecio, R., Tong, F., and Irby, B. J. (2012). The effect of a structured story reading intervention, story retelling and higher order thinking for English language and literacy acquisition. Journal of Research in Reading, 35, 87–113. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2010.01472.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Danaei, D., Jamali, H. R., Mansourian, Y., and Rastegarpour, H. (2020). Comparing reading comprehension between children reading augmented reality and print storybooks. Comput. Educ. 153:103900. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2020.103900

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Delello, J. A. (2014). Insights from pre-service teachers using science-based augmented reality. J. Comput. Educ. 1, 295–311. doi: 10.1007/s40692-014-0021-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Serio, Á., Ibáñez, M. B., and Kloos, C. D. (2013). Impact of an augmented reality system on students' motivation for a visual art course. Comput. Educ. 68, 586–596. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2012.03.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Diehm, E. A., Wood, C., Puhlman, J., and Callendar, M. (2020). Young children's narrative retell in response to static and animated stories. Eur. J. Disord. Commun. 55, 359–372. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12523

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Duenser, A. (2008). Supporting low ability readers with interactive augmented reality. Annu. Rev. Cyberther. Telemed. 6, 39–46.

Google Scholar

Duenser, A., Walker, L., Horner, H., and Bentall, D. (2012). Creating interactive physics education books with augmented reality. In Proceedings of the 24th Australian computer-human interaction conference, 107–114.

Google Scholar

Dunleavy, M., Dede, C., and Mitchell, R. (2009). Affordances and limitations of immersive participatory augmented reality simulations for teaching and learning. J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 18, 7–22. doi: 10.1007/s10956-008-9119-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebadi, S., and Ashrafabadi, F. (2022). An exploration into the impact of augmented reality on EFL learners' reading comprehension. Educ. Inf. Technol. 27, 9745–9765. doi: 10.1007/s10639-022-11021-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ertem, I. S. (2009). Investigating the effects of electronic CD-ROM storybooks and traditional print storybooks on reading comprehension of fourth grade struggling readers [Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation]: University of Florida.

Google Scholar

Fears, N. E., and Lockman, J. J. (2020). Case- and form-sensitive letter frequencies in children's picture books. Early Child. Res. Q. 53, 370–378. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2020.05.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrer-Torregrosa, J., Torralba, J., Jimenez, M. A., Garcia, S., and Barcia, J. M. (2015). ARBOOK: development and assessment of a tool based on augmented reality for anatomy. J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 24, 119–124. doi: 10.1007/s10956-014-9526-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fidan, M., and Tuncel, M. (2019). Integrating augmented reality into problem based learning: the effects on learning achievement and attitude in physics education. Comput. Educ. 142:103635. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2019.103635

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Florit, E., Roch, M., and Levorato, M. C. (2011). Listening text comprehension of explicit and implicit information in preschoolers: the role of verbal and inferential skills. Discourse Process. 48, 119–138. doi: 10.1080/0163853X.2010.494244

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gil, K., Rhim, J., Ha, T., Doh, Y. Y., and Woo, W. (2014). AR petite theater: augmented reality storybook for supporting children's empathy behavior. In 2014 IEEE international symposium on mixed and augmented reality-media, art, social science, humanities and design (ISMAR-MASH'D) (pp. 13–20). IEEE.

Google Scholar

Green, M., McNair, L., Pierce, C., and Harvey, C. (2019). An investigation of augmented reality picture books: meaningful experiences or missed opportunities? J. Educ. Multimedia Hypermedia 28, 357–380.

Google Scholar

Grolig, L., Cohrdes, C., Tiffin-Richards, S. P., and Schroeder, S. (2019). Effects of preschoolers' storybook exposure and literacy environments on lower level and higher level language skills. Read. Writ. 32, 1061–1084. doi: 10.1007/s11145-018-9901-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, J., and Neuharth-Pritchett, S. (2015). Meaning-related and print-related interactions between preschoolers and parents during shared book reading and their associations with emergent literacy skills. J. Res. Child. Educ. 29, 528–550. doi: 10.1080/02568543.2015.1073819

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, R., and Commeyras, M. (2008). A case study: Emergent biliteracy in English and Chinese of a 5-year-old Chinese child with wordless picture books. Read. Psychol. 29:1–30. doi: 10.1080/02702710701260581

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ibáñez, M. B., Uriarte Portillo, A., Zatarain Cabada, R., and Barrón, M. L. (2020). Impact of augmented reality technology on academic achievement and motivation of students from public and private Mexican schools. A case study in a middle-school geometry course. Comput. Educ. 145:103734. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2019.103734

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ibili, E., and Billinghurst, M. (2019). Assessing the relationship between cognitive load and the usability of a mobile augmented reality tutorial system: a study of gender effects. Int. J. Assess. Tools Educ. 6, 378–395. doi: 10.21449/ijate.594749

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kao, G. Y. M., Tsai, C. C., Liu, C. Y., and Yang, C. H. (2016). The effects of high/low interactive electronic storybooks on elementary school students' reading motivation, story comprehension and chromatics concepts. Comput. Educ. 100, 56–70. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2016.04.013

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keller, S., Rumann, S., and Habig, S. (2021). Cognitive load implications for augmented reality supported chemistry learning. Information 12, 96–115. doi: 10.3390/info12030096

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kendeou, P., Van Den Broek, P., White, M. J., and Lynch, J. (2007). “Comprehension in preschool and early elementary children: skill development and strategy interventions” in Reading comprehension strategies: Theories, interventions, and technologies. ed. D. S. McNamara (United States: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 27–45.

Google Scholar

Kendeou, P., Van den Broek, P., White, M. J., and Lynch, J. S. (2009). Predicting reading comprehension in early elementary school: the independent contributions of oral language and decoding skills. J. Educ. Psychol. 101, 765–778. doi: 10.1037/a0015956

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Khan, T., Johnston, K., and Ophoff, J. (2019). The impact of an augmented reality application on learning motivation of students. Adv. Hum. Comput. Interact. 2019, 1–14. doi: 10.1155/2019/7208494

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, Y. S. G. (2016). Direct and mediated effects of language and cognitive skills on comprehension of oral narrative texts (listening comprehension) for children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 141, 101–120. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.08.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kljun, M., Pucihar, K. Č., Alexander, J., Weerasinghe, M., Campos, C., Ducasse, J., et al. (2019). Augmentation not duplication: considerations for the design of digitally-augmented comic books. In Proceedings of the 2019 CHI conference on human factors in computing systems (pp. 1–12).

Google Scholar

Kucirkova, N. (2016). Personalisation: a theoretical possibility to reinvigorate children's interest in storybook reading and facilitate greater book diversity. Contemp. Issues Early Child. 17, 304–316. doi: 10.1177/1463949116660950

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Language and Reading Research ConsortiumChiu, Y. D. (2018). The simple view of reading across development: prediction of grade 3 reading comprehension from prekindergarten skills. Remedial Spec. Educ. 39, 289–303. doi: 10.1177/0741932518762055

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lauricella, A. R., Barr, R., and Calvert, S. L. (2014). Parent–child interactions during traditional and computer storybook reading for children’s comprehension: implications for electronic storybook design. Int. J. Child Comput. Interact. 2, 17–25. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcci.2014.07.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lim, J., Whitehead, G. E., and Choi, Y. (2021). Interactive e-book reading vs. paper-based reading: comparing the effects of different mediums on middle school students' reading comprehension. System 97:102434. doi: 10.1016/j.system.2020.102434

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, S., Sui, Y., You, Z., Shi, J., Wang, Z., and Zhong, C. (2024). Reading better with AR or print picture books? A quasi-experiment on primary school students' reading comprehension, story retelling and reading motivation. Educ. Inf. Technol. 29, 11625–11644. doi: 10.1007/s10639-023-12231-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Merjovaara, O., Nousiainen, T., Turja, L., and Isotalo, S. (2020). Digital stories with children: examining digital storytelling as a pedagogical process in ECEC. J. Early Childhood Educ. Res. 9, 99–123.

Google Scholar

Mol, S. E., and Bus, A. G. (2011). To read or not to read: a meta-analysis of print exposure from infancy to early adulthood. Psychol. Bull. 137, 267–296. doi: 10.1037/a0021890

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morrow, L. M. (1985). Retelling stories: a strategy for improving young children's comprehension, concept of story structure, and oral language complexity. Elem. Sch. J. 85, 647–661. doi: 10.1086/461427

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Byrne, W. I., Stone, R., White, M., and White, M. (2018). Digital storytelling in early childhood: student illustrations shaping social interactions. Front. Psychol. 9:1800. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01800

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ömurtak, E., and Zeybek, G. (2022). The effect of augmented reality applications in biology lesson on academic achievement and motivation. J. Educ. Environ. Sci. Health 8, 55–74. doi: 10.21891/jeseh.1059283

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pan, Z., López, M., Li, C., and Liu, M. (2021). Introducing augmented reality in early childhood literacy learning. Res. Learn. Technol. 29, 1–21. doi: 10.25304/rlt.v29.2539

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Parmaxi, A., and Demetriou, A. A. (2020). Augmented reality in language learning: a state-of-the-art review of 2014–2019. J. Comput. Assist. Learn. 36, 861–875. doi: 10.1111/jcal.12486

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Perez-Lopez, D., and Contero, M. (2013). Delivering educational multimedia contents through an augmented reality application: a case study on its impact on knowledge acquisition and retention. Turkish Online J. Educ. Technol. 12, 19–28.

Google Scholar

Rambli, D. R. A., Matcha, W., and Sulaiman, S. (2013). Fun learning with AR alphabet book for preschool children. Proc. Comput. Sci. 25, 211–219. doi: 10.1016/j.procs.2013.11.026

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roumba, E., and Nicolaidou, I. (2022). Augmented reality books: motivation, attitudes, and behaviors of young readers. Int. J. Interact. Mob. Technol. 16, 59–73. doi: 10.3991/ijim.v16i16.31741

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Şahin, D., and Yılmaz, R. M. (2020). The effect of augmented reality technology on middle school students' achievements and attitudes towards science education. Comput. Educ. 144:103710. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2019.103710

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Santos, M. E. C., Lübke, A. I. W., Taketomi, T., Yamamoto, G., Rodrigo, M. M. T., Sandor, C., et al. (2016). Augmented reality as multimedia: the case for situated vocabulary learning. Res. Pract. Technol. Enhanc. Learn. 11:4. doi: 10.1186/s41039-016-0028-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Şimşek, B., and Direkci, B. (2023). The effects of augmented reality storybooks on student's reading comprehension. Br. J. Educ. Technol. 54, 754–772. doi: 10.1111/bjet.13293

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Soon, C. T., Neo, H. F., and Teo, C. C. (2022). Reading augmented reality story book in enhancing learning perceptions. J. Logist. Inf. Serv. Sci. 9, 105–118. doi: 10.33168/LISS.2022.0408

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Strasser, K., and Río, F. D. (2014). The role of comprehension monitoring, theory of mind, and vocabulary depth in predicting story comprehension and recall of kindergarten children. Read. Res. Q. 49, 169–187. doi: 10.1002/rrq.68

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Takacs, Z. K., and Bus, A. G. (2016). Benefits of motion in animated storybooks for Children's visual attention and story comprehension. An eye-tracking study. Front. Psychol. 7:1591. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01591

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Takacs, Z. K., and Bus, A. G. (2018). How pictures in picture storybooks support young children's story comprehension: an eye-tracking experiment. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 174, 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.04.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Takacs, Z. K., Swart, E. K., and Bus, A. G. (2015). Benefits and pitfalls of multimedia and interactive features in technology-enhanced storybooks: a meta-analysis. Rev. Educ. Res. 85, 698–739. doi: 10.3102/0034654314566989

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Teng, C. H., Chen, J. Y., and Chen, Z. H. (2018). Impact of augmented reality on programming language learning: efficiency and perception. J. Educ. Comput. Res. 56, 254–271. doi: 10.1177/0735633117706109

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tobar-Munoz, H., Baldiris, S., and Fabregat, R. (2017). Augmented reality game-based learning: enriching students' experience during reading comprehension activities. J. Educ. Comput. Res. 55, 901–936. doi: 10.1177/0735633116689789

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomi, A. B., and Rambli, D. R. A. (2013). An interactive mobile augmented reality magical playbook: learning number with the thirsty crow. Proc. Comput. Sci. 25, 123–130. doi: 10.1016/j.procs.2013.11.015

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsou, W., Wang, W., and Tzeng, Y. (2006). Applying a multimedia storytelling website in foreign language learning. Comput. Educ. 47, 17–28. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2004.08.013

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, R. (2022). Application of augmented reality technology in children's picture books based on educational psychology. Front. Psychol. 13:782958. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.782958

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wen, Y. (2021). Augmented reality enhanced cognitive engagement: designing classroom-based collaborative learning activities for young language learners. Educ. Technol. Res. Dev. 69, 843–860. doi: 10.1007/s11423-020-09893-z

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, L. E. (2015). The utilization of technology-mediated evaluation tools to analyze learning efficiency through cognitive load and performance. [dissertation/ doctorate]. Pennsylvania: Indiana University of Pennsylvania.

Google Scholar

Wojciechowski, R., and Cellary, W. (2013). Evaluation of learners' attitude toward learning in ARIES augmented reality environments. Comput. Educ. 68, 570–585. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.02.014

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamaji, N., Sawaguchi, M., and Ota, E. (2020). Talking with children about cancer: a content analysis of text in children's picture books. Health 12, 750–763. doi: 10.4236/health.2020.127055

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yılmaz, R. M., Küçük, S., and Göktaş, Y. (2017). Are augmented reality picture books magic or real for preschool children aged five to six? Br. J. Educ. Technol. 48, 824–841. doi: 10.1111/bjet.12452

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoon, S., Anderson, E., Lin, J., and Elinich, K. (2017). How augmented reality enables conceptual understanding of challenging science content. J. Educ. Technol. Soc. 20:156. doi: 10.2307/jeductechsoci.20.1.156

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuen, S., Yaoyuneyong, G., and Johnson, E. (2011). Augmented reality: an overview and five directions for AR in education. J. Educ. Technol. Dev. Exchange 4, 119–140. doi: 10.18785/jetde.0401.10

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, Z., Cheok, A. D., Pan, J., and Li, Y. (2004). An interactive 3D exploration narrative interface for storytelling. In Proceedings of the 2004 conference on interaction design and children: building a community (pp. 155–156).

Google Scholar

Zhou, N., and Yadav, A. (2017). Effects of multimedia story reading and questioning on preschoolers' vocabulary learning, story comprehension and reading engagement. Educ. Technol. Res. Dev. 65, 1523–1545. doi: 10.1007/s11423-017-9533-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zipke, M. (2017). Preschoolers explore interactive storybook apps: the effect on word recognition and story comprehension. Educ. Inf. Technol. 22, 1695–1712. doi: 10.1007/s10639-016-9513-x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: augmented reality, story-retelling, story comprehension, preschool children, retelling

Citation: Şimşek EE (2024) The effect of augmented reality storybooks on the story comprehension and retelling of preschool children. Front. Psychol. 15:1459264. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1459264

Received: 03 July 2024; Accepted: 27 September 2024;
Published: 08 October 2024.

Edited by:

Rafael Crismán Crismán Pérez, University of Cádiz, Spain

Reviewed by:

Kostas Karpouzis, Panteion University, Greece
Kirstie Hartwell, The University of Manchester, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2024 Şimşek. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Emine Ela Şimşek, ZWxha29rN0BnbWFpbC5jb20=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.