Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 23 August 2024
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Physical Activity Applied to Learning and Psycho-social Variables in Young People View all 4 articles

Effects of cognitively engaging physical activity interventions on executive function in children and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis

Fan Mao,Fan Mao1,2Fang HuangFang Huang3Shan ZhaoShan Zhao1Qun Fang,
Qun Fang1,2*
  • 1School of Physical Education, Qingdao University, Qingdao, China
  • 2Research Center for Youth Football, Qingdao University, Qingdao, China
  • 3Department of Neurology, Louisiana State University Health Shreveport, Shreveport, LA, United States

Background: This systematic review and meta-analysis aim to evaluate the effects of cognitively engaging Physical Activity (PA) interventions on Executive Function (EF) in children and adolescents. It examines how different intervention modalities, durations, frequencies, and session lengths influence these effects.

Methods: We followed the PRISMA guidelines and searched PubMed, SPORTDiscus, Embase, and Web of Science for relevant studies. Studies were included if they were Randomized Controlled Trials (RCTs) focusing on PA with cognitive elements targeting EF in healthy children and adolescents. Data were extracted and effect sizes computed using Standardized Mean Differences (SMDs).

Results: From an initial 1,635 articles, 23 studies with 2,857 participants were included. The overall effect of cognitively engaging PA on EF was significant (SMD = 0.32, 95% CI 0.14–0.51), with notable improvements in inhibitory control (SMD = 0.35) and working memory (SMD = 0.34). High heterogeneity was observed (I2 = 91.1%). Moderator analyses revealed that interventions lasting more than 6 weeks, with sessions over 20 min and conducted more than twice a week, were particularly effective.

Conclusion: Cognitively engaging PA interventions positively impact EF in children and adolescents, particularly in inhibitory control. Effective interventions are characterized by longer duration, higher frequency, and extended session lengths. These findings underscore the importance of integrating cognitive challenges within PA programs to enhance EF, warranting future research and practical applications in educational and developmental settings.

1 Introduction

Executive Function (EF) refers to a series of interrelated higher-order cognitive processes responsible for cognitive regulation and the adaptive control of goal-directed behavior (Diamond, 2013). This cognitive process is particularly triggered in situations that require concentrated attention, present challenges, and involve complexity (Diamond and Ling, 2016). The utilization of EFs prompts individuals to shift cognitive strategies, inhibit impulsive responses, and engage in planning and action (Diamond, 2012; Diamond and Ling, 2020). Core EFs comprise inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility, and working memory (Diamond, 2015; Miyake et al., 2000), which has been proved a predictor of school readiness and academic achievement for children and adolescents (Blair and Diamond, 2008; Roebers et al., 2014). Furthermore, related studies have found EF to be negatively correlated with a broad range of school-related behavioral issues (Espy et al., 2011; Friedman et al., 2007). Espy et al. (2011) established the Problem Behavior-Executive Control Model in which executive control is negatively correlated to hyperactive behaviors (r = −0.49, p < 0.05), attention problems (r = −0.55, p < 0.05), and disinhibition behaviors (r = −0.48, p < 0.05). Friedman et al. (2007) examined correlations between behavioral problems and EFs across ages. The patterns of correlations remained consistent over years, with statistically significant correlations between attention problems and EFs including inhibition (r = −0.41), updating (r = −0.24), and shifting (r = −0.15). Given the correlations between EF and educational outcomes as well as daily behavior, the question about how to enhance cognitive performance in children and adolescents becomes increasingly important.

EF development involves a complex interplay of genetic, environmental, and experiential factors (Diamond, 2013). One such factor that has gained an increasing attention in recent years is Physical Activity (PA) and its potential to enhance EF (Hillman et al., 2008). PA interventions have been proposed as a promising avenue to EF promotion for children and adolescents due to the overlapping neural networks and physiological mechanisms involved in both PA and EF (Best, 2010). A meta-analysis investigated the effects of different types of PA on cognitive function. The synthesized findings indicated that aerobic exercise, motor skill training, and cognitively engaging PA have positive effects on cognitive performance (Vazou et al., 2019). However, not all forms of PA interventions induce comparable improvement in cognitive performance. According to the research by Diamond and Ling (2020), interventions that focus solely on increasing energy expenditure without considering cognitive demands have relatively modest effects on EF. Therefore, interventions involving PA should adopt a comprehensive approach to student development, targeting multiple objectives such as physical health and psychological well-being (Best, 2010; Tomporowski et al., 2011).

Empirical evidence has shown that cognitively engaging PA interventions, which combine physical exertion with cognitive challenges, may be particularly effective in enhancing EF in youth (Best et al., 2011). Cognitively engaging PA interventions involve activities that require both physical effort and cognitive engagement, such as aerobic exercise combined with tasks demanding attention, memory, or problem-solving (Hillman et al., 2009; Schmidt et al., 2020). This approach highlights a shift from “simple movement to thinking moves” (Diamond, 2015). According to this view, PA programs not only focus on health-related outcomes and energy expenditure, but also consider promoting both physical and cognitive development (Pesce, 2012; Pesce et al., 2018).

Applications of neuroimaging technique facilitated investigating exercise-induced cognitive promotion at the neural level. A recent study via functional near-infrared spectroscopy identified enhanced inhibitory control after acute physical activity (Ludyga et al., 2024). Specifically, faster reaction and higher accuracy in the Stroop task were associated with decreased lateral oxygenation difference in the cognitive trials, suggesting an increased neural efficiency in the left prefrontal cortex of the participants (N = 29, age = 11.1 years). Another study employing electroencephalography examined brain activation changes after a 20-week exercise program for children with overweight and obesity (Mora-Gonzalez et al., 2024). Significant changes were identified when performing the assigned cognitive tasks, suggesting exercise-induced modulations in brain activity which facilitated working memory and inhibitory control of the children.

Despite increasing body of literature investigating the effects of cognitively engaging PA interventions on EF in children and adolescents, the findings of the existing studies are inconsistent. Some studies have reported significant improvements in EF following participation in cognitively engaging PA interventions (Benzing et al., 2016; Egger et al., 2019; Schmidt et al., 2015), while others reported non-significant results (Bedard et al., 2021; Mavilidi et al., 2023; Meijer et al., 2020), with some even reporting negative effects (Egger et al., 2018; van der Fels et al., 2020). These discrepancies may be attributed to methodological variations across studies, including differences in intervention protocols, duration, frequency of exercise, outcome measures, and participant characteristics. A systematic review analyzed the effects of cognitive engaging PA on EF in children, incorporating findings from 11 articles. The results indicated a moderate effect size of cognitive engaging PA on EF, particularly in the domains of working memory and cognitive flexibility (Song et al., 2022). However, the limited number of studies included in this review may lead to potential misinterpretation of the results. Additionally, the lack of research focusing on adolescent populations suggests potential limitations in the generalizability of the findings. Moreover, the underlying mechanisms through which cognitively engaging PA influences EF are still not fully understood, warranting further investigation.

The aim of this paper is to provide a comprehensive meta-analysis regarding the specific effects of cognitively engaging PA interventions on EF in children and adolescents. Moderator analyses are conducted to understand the potential moderating effects of intervention modalities, duration, exercise frequency, and outcome measures on the EFs. By synthesizing data from diverse studies and quantitatively analyzing effect sizes, a systematic review and meta-analysis can offer valuable insights into the magnitude and consistency of the effects of cognitively engaging PA interventions on EF, thus informing future research directions and practical applications in educational settings.

2 Methods

This meta-analysis was performed following the guidelines of Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) statement (Moher et al., 2015) and Cochrane Collaboration handbook (Cumpston et al., 2019).

2.1 Search strategy

The electronic databases PubMed, SPORTDiscus, Embase, and Web of Science were searched for original research published in peer-reviewed journals by March 2024. According to the main purpose of the current review, which aims to examine influence of cognitively engaging PA intervention on cognitive performance of children and adolescents, the keywords for literature search involve four categories including cognitive, physical activity, executive function, and population. Specifically, combinations of the four categories of MeSH terms were “cognitively engaging OR cognitive engagement OR cognitive demand OR cognitively challenging OR cognitive challenge” AND “physical activity OR exercise OR exergaming” AND “executive function OR inhibition OR working memory OR updating OR cognitive flexibility OR shifting” AND “adolescent OR child OR teen OR youth.”

2.2 Eligibility criteria

Inclusion criteria included publication in peer review journals in the English language. The language was restricted to English. Further requirements on the eligible criteria followed the guide of Population, Intervention, Comparison, Outcomes, and Study (PICOS). First, participants of the included studies should be healthy children and adolescents. Second, intervention was designed PA with cognitive elements. Third, comparisons were made between intervention and control groups at different time points. Fourth, outcomes measured EF such as cognitive flexibility, working memory, inhibitory control, etc. Fifth, the included studies must be Randomized Control Trials (RCTs).

Studies were excluded for any of the following reasons: (1) review, book chapter, commentaries, or proceedings; (2) articles in which data could not be obtained or extracted for estimating effect size even after contacting the authors; (3) non-target population; (4) studies only included an experimental group, lacking both intervention and control group comparisons.

The initial search was screened by an overall examination on the title to remove duplicates and irrelevant articles. The second phase of screening was conducted by analysis on the abstracts. For the articles that passed the first two phases, a full-text evaluation was performed to determine the included studies of the current review. Two authors (FM and QF) independently worked on the literature screening and selection. Any disagreement on the eligibility of an article was resolved by discussing with other authors to reach a consensus.

2.3 Data extraction and synthesis

The data of publication year, participant description, study design, instrument, EF variables, settings and interventions of eligible studies were extracted and summarized in Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of the included studies.

The quantitative data were synthesized according to the categories of cognitive functions. Effect sizes which assessed the same cognitive domain were combined to compute an overall effect. Multiple effects in the same study were addressed by the following steps. First, if multiple results were reported by one cognitive assessment, the result of the more cognitive demanding condition was extracted (Álvarez-Bueno et al., 2017; Xue et al., 2019). Second, for the tests which reported both accuracy and time, accuracy was selected as a representation of cognitive performance. Therefore, in the study which applied Flanker task to assess inhibitory control, accuracy of incongruent trials would be extracted in data synthesis.

2.4 Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was conducted in Comprehensive Meta-Analysis 3.3 (BioStat Inc., Englewood, NJ, United States). Considering the different outcomes and units of cognitive measures used in the studies, Standardized Mean Differences (SMD) of pre-post intervention were calculated and given weight by its inverse variance. The magnitude of the effect sizes was assessed using Cohen’s d values, with SMD values of <0.2, 0.2 ≤ SMD < 0.5, and 0.5 ≤ SMD < 0.8 indicating small, moderate, and large effect sizes, respectively (Cumpston et al., 2019). Quantitative pooled analysis based on a fixed-effects model, and a random-effects model if heterogeneity exists. To assess the degree of statistical heterogeneity in the meta-analysis, we employed the I2 statistic and p-value. Specifically, I2 values of 25, 50, and 75% were used to indicate small, moderate, and large levels of heterogeneity, respectively (Egger et al., 1997). Egger’s regression test was performed to assess publication bias in the reviewed literature. A two-tailed test with p-value less than 0.05 was considered significant publication bias.

Subgroup analyses based on three core EFs domains (cognitive flexibility, inhibitory control and working memory) were conducted after the overall meta-analysis. Intervention modality (Fundamental movement skills-FMS, PA games and exergaming), duration (>6 weeks vs. ≤6 weeks), session length (>20 min vs. ≤20 min), and frequency of intervention (>2 times/week. vs. ≤2 times/week) were examined by subgroup analyses and moderator analysis.

2.5 Risk of bias assessment

Two authors (FM and QF) independently assessed the risk of bias of included studies following the PEDro scale (Maher et al., 2008). The summary of the quality assessment was presented in Table 2. Any disagreements were discussed with a third author (SZ) until a consensus was achieved.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Quality assessment of included studies.

3 Results

The initial search retrieved 1,635 peer-reviewed articles. After removing duplications and reviewing the titles, 1,104 articles were eligible for further screening. Through a careful reading of the abstracts, 76 articles were eligible for thorough examination. Finally, 23 studies were included in the meta-analysis. Among the initial pool of 76 articles, 53 were excluded because they had no control group, lacked randomization, no RCT or cluster RCT design, lacked available data or had unqualified samples. Figure 1 displays the flow of study selection.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flowchart of literature search and study selection.

3.1 Study characteristics

Overall, 23 studies were included in the meta-analysis and the study characteristics are summarized in Table 1. Nine studies were RCTs and 14 studies were cluster RCTs. The total sample included 2,857 children and adolescents aged from 4 to 16 years. There were 23 studies investigating the effects of cognitively engaging PA on core EFs, of which, 13 studies examined all three core features, three examined 2 features, and 7 examined a single feature. Regarding the exercise intervention protocols, there were 22 studies with sessions over 20 min, 14 studies with more than two sessions per week and 19 studies with durations over 6 weeks. The major confounding factors considered in the studies included intervention modality, duration, frequency, and session length.

3.2 Effects of cognitive engagement PA on EFs

As illustrated by Figure 2, the pooled SMD of over EF was 0.32 (95% CI 0.14 to 0.51, p = 0.001), with large heterogeneity (I2 = 91.1%, p < 0.001). Regarding core EFs, a total of 23 studies were included in the analysis. Thirteen of these studies evaluated three core features, three assessed 2 features, and 7 examined a single feature. The SMD was 0.26 (95% CI −0.11 to 0.62) for cognitive flexibility, with large heterogeneity (I2 = 89.1%, p < 0.001); 0.35 (95% CI 0.06 to 0.65) for inhibitory control, with large heterogeneity (I2 = 94.1%, p < 0.001); and 0.34 (95% CI 0.02 to 0.67) for working memory, with large heterogeneity (I2 = 86.1%, p < 0.001). The high heterogeneity for core EFs reflects the importance of taking various moderator factors into consideration when analyzing the effects of cognitively engaging PA.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Forest plot for meta-analysis regarding the effect of cognitively engaging PA interventions on different EF domains.

3.3 Moderator analysis

Table 3 summarizes the results from the moderator analysis and subgroup analyses for potential factors on EFs. In the subgroup analysis, the effect of cognitively engaging PA on EFs was significantly moderated by intervention modality, frequency, duration, and session length. Firstly, a total of 52 studies evaluated the effects of different modalities of PA interventions on EFs. Among them, 24 studies investigated the intervention effects of FMS, 20 studies assessed the impact of PA games interventions, and 8 studies examined the effects of exergaming intervention modalities. The SMD for modality of FMS was 0.49 (95% CI 0.22 to 0.77) indicating large heterogeneity (I2 = 94.3%, p < 0.001), whereas the SMD for PA games was 0.20 (95% CI −0.10 to 0.49), with large heterogeneity (I2 = 86.1%, p < 0.001), and 0.13 (95% CI −0.36 to 0.61) for exergaming, with moderate heterogeneity (I2 = 67.3%, p = 0.003). With respect to the exercise duration, 19 studies assessed intervention periods longer than 6 weeks, while 33 studies examined intervention durations of 6 weeks or less. The SMD for duration >6 weeks was 0.38 (95% CI 0.15 to 0.62), with large heterogeneity (I2 = 91.0%, p < 0.001), whereas the SMD for ≤6 weeks was 0.22 (95% CI-0.08 to 0.52), with large heterogeneity (I2 = 91.4%, p < 0.001). With regard to the intervention frequency, 14 studies evaluated intervention frequencies of more than twice a week, while 38 studies examined exercise frequencies of 2 weeks or less. The SMD for frequency > 2 times/week was 0.89 (95% CI 0.53 to 1.26), with large heterogeneity (I2 = 94.4%, p < 0.001), and the SMD for ≤2 times/week was 0.12 (95% CI −0.09 to 0.34), with large heterogeneity (I2 = 89.1%, p < 0.001). With respect to the session length, 22 studies assessed session length longer than 20 min, while 30 studies examined session length of 20 min or less. The SMD for session length > 20 min was 0.37 (95% CI 0.10 to 0.65), with large heterogeneity (I2 = 91.1%, p < 0.001), whereas the SMD for ≤20 min was 0.28 (95% CI 0.04 to 0.52), with large heterogeneity (I2 = 91.0%, p < 0.001).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Moderator analysis of cognitively engaging PA and EFs.

3.4 Methodological quality assessment

The quality of the included studies is presented in Table 2. In general, those studies were scored as fairly high quality, with a mean quality score of 7.6, because they were (cluster) RCTs. Moreover, over half of the studies (18 out of 23) were school-based intervention, including active class, curricular PA, and extracurricular PA.

3.5 Publication bias

The funnel plot is presented in Figure 3. Egger’s test was used to assess publication bias (t = 1.67, df = 50, p = 0.10). The results indicated no evidence of publication bias for all included studies (Egger et al., 1997).

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Funnel plot no publication bias cognitively engaging PA and EFs.

4 Discussion

4.1 Main study findings

The current review included 23 RCT studies which investigated the effects of cognitively challenging PA interventions on core EFs (cognitive flexibility, inhibitory control and working memory) in children and adolescents. Meta-analysis indicated a small but significant positive benefits of cognitive engagement PA on overall core EFs, particularly on inhibitory control. Moreover, our analysis identified several key moderating variables that influence the relationship between cognitive engagement PA and EFs, including intervention modality, duration, frequency, and session length.

The positive impact of cognitively engaging PA on overall EFs highlights the importance of integrating cognitive challenges within physical activities to enhance cognitive outcomes. The pooled SMD for overall EF improvement was 0.32, which, despite being modest, underscores a meaningful advancement in the cognitive capabilities of children and adolescents engaging in such interventions. Notably, the significant improvement in inhibitory control (SMD = 0.35) aligns with the theory that PA demanding cognitive engagement can strengthen brain regions responsible for self-regulation and attentional control (Best, 2010).

4.2 Analysis of moderating variables between cognitive engagement PA and EFs

The modality of PA intervention significantly influences its effectiveness on EFs. Various studies have shown that interventions combining physical and cognitive tasks, such as FMS and PA games, yield more significant improvements in EFs compared to traditional PA. Best (2010) highlighted that aerobic exercises integrated with cognitive challenges, like sports requiring strategic planning or movement routines involving memorization, show greater enhancements in EFs. This is corroborated by Ferreira Vorkapic et al. (2021), who emphasized that activities requiring real-time decision-making and problem-solving, such as cognitive games and complex sports drills, are particularly effective. For instance, exergaming, which involves active video games that require physical movement and cognitive processing, has been shown to improve inhibitory control and cognitive flexibility (Anzeneder et al., 2023). Furthermore, a review by Tomporowski et al. (2015) found that PA interventions with cognitive engagement had positive effects on EFs, especially in tasks demanding higher-order cognitive functions. This indicates that the inclusion of cognitive demands in PA is a critical factor in enhancing EFs in children and adolescents. Therefore, it is essential for PA interventions to incorporate complex cognitive elements to maximize their impact on EFs.

The duration and frequency of PA interventions are also crucial moderators of their impact on EFs. Long-term interventions tend to produce more substantial improvements in cognitive functions compared to short-term interventions. For example, a study by Hillman et al. (2014) demonstrated that a 9-month intervention significantly enhanced EFs, particularly in areas of working memory and cognitive flexibility. In contrast, shorter interventions, typically lasting less than 6 weeks, often show mixed results due to insufficient exposure (de Greeff et al., 2018). This is consistent with findings from Pesce et al. (2016), who reported that sustained cognitively engaging PA interventions over several months resulted in more pronounced cognitive benefits. The current meta-analysis supports these findings, indicating that interventions lasting more than 6 weeks generally have a more significant impact on cognitive flexibility and working memory. Additionally, the frequency of interventions plays a vital role; those conducted more than twice a week tend to yield better outcomes. Ludyga et al. (2018) suggested that regular engagement in cognitively demanding PA facilitates sustained cognitive engagement and neural adaptations. Frequent and prolonged exposure to such activities likely promotes neuroplasticity and cognitive development, which are essential for improving EFs (Kvalø et al., 2017). Therefore, to achieve significant cognitive benefits, PA interventions should be designed to last longer and be administered more frequently.

The length of each PA session is another important factor affecting its efficacy. Sessions longer than 20 min have been shown to be more beneficial for cognitive outcomes compared to shorter sessions. Wang et al. (2023) demonstrated that extended PA sessions provide sufficient time for both physical exertion and cognitive engagement, which are necessary for stimulating cognitive improvements. Longer sessions allow for more complex cognitive tasks and greater mental effort, leading to better EF outcomes (Davis et al., 2011; Kvalø et al., 2017). Additionally, longer sessions may enhance the cardiovascular and metabolic responses needed for cognitive benefits, as suggested by de Greeff et al. (2016). This is further supported by a study conducted by Gallotta et al. (2012), which found that longer PA sessions resulted in significant improvements in cognitive performance due to sustained cognitive engagement. The current meta-analysis corroborates these findings, showing that PA sessions exceeding 20 min are associated with significant improvements in inhibitory control and working memory. Therefore, to maximize cognitive benefits, it is crucial for PA sessions to be designed with sufficient length to ensure comprehensive cognitive and physical engagement.

4.3 Potential mechanisms of cognitive engagement PA and EF

Cognitive promotion associated with the cognitively engaging PA interventions implies potential changes in the central nervous system. Neuroimaging evidence has shown that cognitive activities during exercise stimulate blood flow and activation in prefrontal regions, thus resulting in better cognitive performance than simple aerobic exercises (Hillman et al., 2009; Liu et al., 2023). Cognitively challenging PA also increases the release of the brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) which plays an essential role in enhancing synaptic plasticity and improving cognitive performance (Voss et al., 2011; Winter et al., 2007). Schmidt et al. (2015) found that children who participated in cognitively engaging PA exhibited greater increases in BDNF and corresponding improvements in EF tasks compared to those involved in standard aerobic exercises.

In addition to the explanations at the neural level, researchers proposed potential mechanisms from psychological perspectives. Cognitively engaging PA interventions may enhance EF by improving self-regulation and motivational states. Engaging in activities that require constant cognitive input, such as strategy-based games or problem-solving tasks, helps children and adolescents practice self-control, attention regulation, and goal-directed behavior (Kolovelonis and Goudas, 2022). These activities often require participants to navigate complex rules, make quick decisions, and adjust strategies in real-time, which mirrors the demands of EF. Consequently, regular participation in such activities can translate into better EF by continuously challenging and refining these cognitive processes. Moreover, the integration of cognitive and physical elements in PA interventions can enhance motivation and enjoyment, which are critical factors for sustained engagement and practice. Studies have shown that when children find activities enjoyable and intrinsically rewarding, they are more likely to persist, thereby gaining more practice and potential benefits for EF (Kolovelonis et al., 2023; Tomporowski et al., 2008). This increased engagement not only promotes physical fitness but also provides repeated opportunities to exercise cognitive skills in a dynamic and interactive context, leading to cumulative benefits for EF.

4.4 Implications for educational practices and interventions

Given the findings from this meta-analysis, several practical implications emerge for integrating cognitively engaging PA into educational settings. Notably, recent studies underscore the importance of considering the intensity, duration, and frequency of PA to maximize its benefits. Research has consistently shown that moderate to vigorous intensity PA, particularly when sustained over longer periods, yields substantial improvements in EFs such as working memory, cognitive flexibility, and inhibitory control (Morales et al., 2024; Wen et al., 2018). For instance, a meta-analysis by Vazou et al. (2019) highlighted that aerobic exercises, motor skill training, and cognitively engaging activities all positively impact cognitive performance in young individuals.

Furthermore, the type of PA intervention is critical. High-Intensity Interval Training (HIIT), for example, has demonstrated considerable promise due to its combination of short bursts of intense exercise with brief recovery periods. This format not only enhances physical fitness but also challenges cognitive processes, thereby improving EFs (Gilson et al., 2023; Leahy et al., 2020). Similarly, cooperative and playful activities, which integrate social interaction and cognitive engagement, can significantly enhance motivation and enjoyment, leading to sustained participation and greater cognitive benefits (Wang et al., 2024; Wei et al., 2024). Such interventions are particularly effective in educational settings, where the dual goals of promoting physical health and cognitive development can be seamlessly integrated into the curriculum.

Educators and families can leverage these insights to design PA interventions that are both engaging and beneficial. For instance, incorporating game-based activities that require strategic thinking and problem-solving can make physical education classes more enjoyable and cognitively stimulating (Vita-Barrull et al., 2023). These activities could range from team sports that necessitate real-time decision-making to classroom exercises that combine physical movement with academic content. The key is to ensure that the PA is not merely about physical exertion but also includes elements that challenge students’ cognitive abilities, thereby promoting holistic development (Schmidt et al., 2015). The integration of cognitively engaging PA within the school day holds significant potential for enhancing academic performance and cognitive development. By focusing on the intensity, duration, frequency, and type of exercise, educators can design interventions that provide comprehensive benefits to students. This approach not only supports physical health but also fosters essential cognitive skills, preparing students for both academic success and lifelong well-being.

4.5 Strengths and limitations

One of the advantages of this meta-analysis is the stringent inclusion criteria, which focus exclusively on RCT while excluding observational or longitudinal studies, as RCTs are considered the gold standard for intervention studies. This rigorous approach ensures the validity of the study results, enhancing the reliability of causal inferences. Additionally, the use of SMD to quantify effect sizes allows for the comparison of outcomes across different measures, providing a more consistent and interpretable analysis. Another strength is the significant effect observed in the moderator analysis conducted in this study. By examining variables such as intervention modality, duration, frequency, and session length, this meta-analysis identifies key factors that influence the effectiveness of cognitively engaging PA interventions.

A limitation of this review is the high heterogeneity in the participants of the included studies. The research works involve preschool children (Gao et al., 2019; Giordano and Alesi, 2022; Mavilidi et al., 2023; Schmidt et al., 2020; Vazou and Mavilidi, 2021), school-aged children (Crova et al., 2014; Egger et al., 2019; Flynn and Richert, 2017; Kolovelonis et al., 2023), and adolescents (Benzing et al., 2016; Robinson et al., 2022; Schmidt et al., 2015). Cognitive development is age-and sex-specific, suggesting that the effect of physical exercise may be different by age and sex. Given that both factors were not considered in data synthesis and analysis, it is important to interpret the results with cautions.

5 Conclusion

This systematic review and meta-analysis reveal that cognitively engaging PA interventions have a small yet significant positive impact on EFs in children and adolescents, particularly on inhibitory control. The analysis identified key moderating variables, such as intervention modality, duration, frequency, and session length, which influence the effectiveness of these interventions. Despite substantial heterogeneity among the included studies, the findings suggest that longer and more frequent interventions yield greater cognitive benefits. Understanding the underlying mechanisms of these cognitive benefits remains crucial, warranting further explorations through neuroimaging and neurophysiological research. Overall, incorporating cognitive elements into PA programs shows promise for enhancing EFs, providing valuable insights for future educational and clinical applications. On the other hand, substantial variability in age and sex of the participants highlights the need for practitioners and researchers to interpret the results with cautions.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

FM: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Validation. FH: Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. SZ: Conceptualization, Data curation, Writing – original draft. QF: Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all the participants involved in the studies included in this systematic review.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Álvarez-Bueno, C., Pesce, C., Cavero-Redondo, I., Sánchez-López, M., Martínez-Hortelano, J. A., and Martínez-Vizcaíno, V. (2017). The effect of physical activity interventions on Children's cognition and metacognition: a systematic review and Meta-analysis. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 56, 729–738. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2017.06.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Anzeneder, S., Zehnder, C., Martin-Niedecken, A. L., Schmidt, M., and Benzing, V. (2023). Acute exercise and children's cognitive functioning: what is the optimal dose of cognitive challenge? Psychol. Sport Exerc. 66:102404. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2023.102404

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bedard, C., Bremer, E., Graham, J. D., Chirico, D., and Cairney, J. (2021). Examining the effects of acute cognitively engaging physical activity on cognition in children. Front. Psychol. 12:653133. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.653133

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Benzing, V., Heinks, T., Eggenberger, N., and Schmidt, M. (2016). Acute cognitively engaging Exergame-based physical activity enhances executive functions in adolescents. PLoS One 11:e0167501. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0167501

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Best, J. R. (2010). Effects of physical activity on Children's executive function: contributions of experimental research on aerobic exercise. Dev. Rev. 30, 331–351. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2010.08.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., and Naglieri, J. A. (2011). Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative National Sample. Learn. Individ. Differ. 21, 327–336. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2011.01.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blair, C., and Diamond, A. (2008). Biological processes in prevention and intervention: the promotion of self-regulation as a means of preventing school failure. Dev. Psychopathol. 20, 899–911. doi: 10.1017/s0954579408000436

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bulten, R., Bedard, C., Graham, J. D., and Cairney, J. (2022). Effect of cognitively engaging physical activity on executive functions in children. Front. Psychol. 13:841192. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.841192

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chou, C.-C., Chen, K.-C., Huang, M.-Y., Tu, H.-Y., and Huang, C.-J. (2020). Can movement games enhance executive function in overweight children? A randomized controlled trial. J. Teach. Phys. Educ. 39, 527–535. doi: 10.1123/jtpe.2019-0165

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Crova, C., Struzzolino, I., Marchetti, R., Masci, I., Vannozzi, G., Forte, R., et al. (2014). Cognitively challenging physical activity benefits executive function in overweight children. J. Sports Sci. 32, 201–211. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2013.828849

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cumpston, M., Li, T., Page, M. J., Chandler, J., Welch, V. A., Higgins, J. P., et al. (2019). Updated guidance for trusted systematic reviews: a new edition of the Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 10:Ed000142. doi: 10.1002/14651858.Ed000142

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, C. L., Tomporowski, P. D., McDowell, J. E., Austin, B. P., Miller, P. H., Yanasak, N. E., et al. (2011). Exercise improves executive function and achievement and alters brain activation in overweight children: a randomized, controlled trial. Health Psychol. 30, 91–98. doi: 10.1037/a0021766

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

de Greeff, J. W., Bosker, R. J., Oosterlaan, J., Visscher, C., and Hartman, E. (2018). Effects of physical activity on executive functions, attention and academic performance in preadolescent children: a meta-analysis. J. Sci. Med. Sport 21, 501–507. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2017.09.595

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

de Greeff, J. W., Hartman, E., Mullender-Wijnsma, M. J., Bosker, R. J., Doolaard, S., and Visscher, C. (2016). Long-term effects of physically active academic lessons on physical fitness and executive functions in primary school children. Health Educ. Res. 31, 185–194. doi: 10.1093/her/cyv102

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2012). Activities and programs that improve Children's executive functions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 21, 335–341. doi: 10.1177/0963721412453722

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2015). Effects of physical exercise on executive functions: going beyond simply moving to moving with thought. Ann Sports Med Res 2:1011

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Diamond, A., and Ling, D. S. (2016). Conclusions about interventions, programs, and approaches for improving executive functions that appear justified and those that, despite much hype, do not. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 34–48. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.11.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A., and Ling, D. (2020). Review of the evidence on, and fundamental questions about, efforts to improve executive functions, including working memory. New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press, 143–431.

Google Scholar

Egger, F., Benzing, V., Conzelmann, A., and Schmidt, M. (2019). Boost your brain, while having a break! The effects of long-term cognitively engaging physical activity breaks on children's executive functions and academic achievement. PLoS One 14:e0212482. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0212482

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Egger, F., Conzelmann, A., and Schmidt, M. (2018). The effect of acute cognitively engaging physical activity breaks on children's executive functions: too much of a good thing? Psychol. Sport Exerc. 36, 178–186. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.02.014

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Egger, M., Davey Smith, G., Schneider, M., and Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ 315, 629–634. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Espy, K. A., Sheffield, T. D., Wiebe, S. A., Clark, C. A., and Moehr, M. J. (2011). Executive control and dimensions of problem behaviors in preschool children. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 52, 33–46. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02265.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferreira Vorkapic, C., Alves, H., Araujo, L., Joaquim Borba-Pinheiro, C., Coelho, R., Fonseca, E., et al. (2021). Does physical activity improve cognition and academic performance in children? A systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Neuropsychobiology 80, 454–482. doi: 10.1159/000514682

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Flynn, R., and Richert, R. (2017). Cognitive, not physical, engagement in video gaming influences executive functioning. J. Cogn. Dev. 19, 1–20. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2017.1419246

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedman, N. P., Haberstick, B. C., Willcutt, E. G., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., Corley, R. P., et al. (2007). Greater attention problems during childhood predict poorer executive functioning in late adolescence. Psychol. Sci. 18, 893–900. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01997.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallotta, M. C., Guidetti, L., Franciosi, E., Emerenziani, G. P., Bonavolontà, V., and Baldari, C. (2012). Effects of varying type of exertion on children's attention capacity. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 44, 550–555. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e3182305552

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, Z., Lee, J. E., Zeng, N., Pope, Z. C., Zhang, Y., and Li, X. (2019). Home-based exergaming on Preschoolers' energy expenditure, cardiovascular fitness, body mass index and cognitive flexibility: a randomized controlled trial. J. Clin. Med. 8:745. doi: 10.3390/jcm8101745

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilson, N. D., Andersson, D., Papinczak, Z. E., Rutherford, Z., John, J., Coombes, J. S., et al. (2023). High intensity and sprint interval training, and work-related cognitive function in adults: a systematic review. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 33, 814–833. doi: 10.1111/sms.14349

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Giordano, G., and Alesi, M. (2022). Does physical activity improve inhibition in kindergarteners? A Pilot Study. Percept Mot Skills 129, 1001–1013. doi: 10.1177/00315125221109216

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hillman, C. H., Erickson, K. I., and Kramer, A. F. (2008). Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 58–65. doi: 10.1038/nrn2298

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hillman, C. H., Pontifex, M. B., Castelli, D. M., Khan, N. A., Raine, L. B., Scudder, M. R., et al. (2014). Effects of the FITKids randomized controlled trial on executive control and brain function. Pediatrics 134, e1063–e1071. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-3219

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hillman, C. H., Pontifex, M. B., Raine, L. B., Castelli, D. M., Hall, E. E., and Kramer, A. F. (2009). The effect of acute treadmill walking on cognitive control and academic achievement in preadolescent children. Neuroscience 159, 1044–1054. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.01.057

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jäger, K., Schmidt, M., Conzelmann, A., and Roebers, C. M. (2015). The effects of qualitatively different acute physical activity interventions in real-world settings on executive functions in preadolescent children. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 9, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2015.05.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kolovelonis, A., and Goudas, M. (2022). Exploring the effects of three different types of cognitively challenging physical activity games on students’ executive functions and situational interest in physical education. Cogent Educ. 9:2148448. doi: 10.1080/2331186X.2022.2148448

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kolovelonis, A., and Goudas, M. (2023). The effects of cognitively challenging physical activity games versus health-related fitness activities on Students' executive functions and situational interest in physical education: a group-randomized controlled trial. Eur J Investig Health Psychol Educ 13, 796–809. doi: 10.3390/ejihpe13050060

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kolovelonis, A., Papastergiou, M., Samara, E., and Goudas, M. (2023). Acute effects of exergaming on Students' executive functions and situational interest in elementary physical education. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 20:902. doi: 10.3390/ijerph20031902

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kvalø, S. E., Bru, E., Brønnick, K., and Dyrstad, S. M. (2017). Does increased physical activity in school affect children's executive function and aerobic fitness? Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 27, 1833–1841. doi: 10.1111/sms.12856

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Layne, T., Yli-Piipari, S., and Knox, T. (2020). Physical activity break program to improve elementary students’ executive function and mathematics performance. Education 49, 583–591. doi: 10.1080/03004279.2020.1746820

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leahy, A. A., Mavilidi, M. F., Smith, J. J., Hillman, C. H., Eather, N., Barker, D., et al. (2020). Review of high-intensity interval training for cognitive and mental health in youth. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 52, 2224–2234. doi: 10.1249/mss.0000000000002359

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, J., Min, L., Liu, R., Zhang, X., Wu, M., Di, Q., et al. (2023). The effect of exercise on cerebral blood flow and executive function among young adults: a double-blinded randomized controlled trial. Sci. Rep. 13:8269. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-33063-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ludyga, S., Gerber, M., Kamijo, K., Brand, S., and Pühse, U. (2018). The effects of a school-based exercise program on neurophysiological indices of working memory operations in adolescents. J. Sci. Med. Sport 21, 833–838. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2018.01.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ludyga, S., Gerber, M., Schwarz, A., Greco, A., Müller, T., Pühse, U., et al. (2024). Effects of cognitive and physical load of acute exercise on inhibitory control and prefrontal cortex hemodynamics in children. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 56, 1328–1336. doi: 10.1249/mss.0000000000003410

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Maher, C. G., Moseley, A. M., Sherrington, C., Elkins, M. R., and Herbert, R. D. (2008). A description of the trials, reviews, and practice guidelines indexed in the PEDro database. Phys. Ther. 88, 1068–1077. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20080002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mavilidi, M. F., Pesce, C., Mazzoli, E., Bennett, S., Paas, F., Okely, A. D., et al. (2023). Effects of cognitively engaging physical activity on preschool Children's cognitive outcomes. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 94, 839–852. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2022.2059435

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Meijer, A., Königs, M., Pouwels, P. J. W., Smith, J., Visscher, C., Bosker, R. J., et al. (2022). Effects of aerobic versus cognitively demanding exercise interventions on brain structure and function in healthy children-results from a cluster randomized controlled trial. Psychophysiology 59:e14034. doi: 10.1111/psyp.14034

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Meijer, A., Königs, M., van der Fels, I. M. J., Visscher, C., Bosker, R. J., Hartman, E., et al. (2020). The effects of aerobic versus cognitively demanding exercise interventions on executive functioning in school-aged children: a cluster-randomized controlled trial. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 43, 1–13. doi: 10.1123/jsep.2020-0034

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex "frontal lobe" tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moher, D., Shamseer, L., Clarke, M., Ghersi, D., Liberati, A., Petticrew, M., et al. (2015). Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement. Syst. Rev. 4:1. doi: 10.1186/2046-4053-4-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mora-Gonzalez, J., Esteban-Cornejo, I., Solis-Urra, P., Rodriguez-Ayllon, M., Cadenas-Sanchez, C., Hillman, C. H., et al. (2024). The effects of an exercise intervention on neuroelectric activity and executive function in children with overweight/obesity: the ActiveBrains randomized controlled trial. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 34:e14486. doi: 10.1111/sms.14486

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morales, J. S., Alberquilla del Río, E., Valenzuela, P. L., and Martínez-de-Quel, Ó. (2024). Physical activity and cognitive performance in early childhood: a systematic review and Meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Sports Med. 54, 1835–1850. doi: 10.1007/s40279-024-02020-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Oppici, L., Rudd, J. R., Buszard, T., and Spittle, S. (2020). Efficacy of a 7-week dance (RCT) PE curriculum with different teaching pedagogies and levels of cognitive challenge to improve working memory capacity and motor competence in 8–10 years old children. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 50:101675. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2020.101675

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pesce, C. (2012). Shifting the focus from quantitative to qualitative exercise characteristics in exercise and cognition research. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 34, 766–786. doi: 10.1123/jsep.34.6.766

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pesce, C., Faigenbaum, A., Goudas, M., and Tomporowski, P. (2018). Coupling our plough of thoughtful moving to the star of children's right to play: from neuroscience to multisectoral promotion. London: Routledge. 247–274.

Google Scholar

Pesce, C., Masci, I., Marchetti, R., Vazou, S., Sääkslahti, A., and Tomporowski, P. D. (2016). Deliberate play and preparation jointly benefit motor and cognitive development: mediated and moderated effects. Front. Psychol. 7:349. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00349

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, K. J., Lubans, D. R., Mavilidi, M. F., Hillman, C. H., Benzing, V., Valkenborghs, S. R., et al. (2022). Effects of classroom-based resistance training with and without cognitive training on Adolescents' cognitive function, on-task behavior, and muscular fitness. Front. Psychol. 13:811534. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.811534

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roebers, C. M., Röthlisberger, M., Neuenschwander, R., Cimeli, P., Michel, E., and Jäger, K. (2014). The relation between cognitive and motor performance and their relevance for children's transition to school: a latent variable approach. Hum. Mov. Sci. 33, 284–297. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2013.08.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, M., Jäger, K., Egger, F., Roebers, C. M., and Conzelmann, A. (2015). Cognitively engaging chronic physical activity, but not aerobic exercise, affects executive functions in primary school children: a group-randomized controlled trial. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 37, 575–591. doi: 10.1123/jsep.2015-0069

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, M., Mavilidi, M. F., Singh, A., and Englert, C. (2020). Combining physical and cognitive training to improve kindergarten children’s executive functions: a cluster randomized controlled trial. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 63:101908. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2020.101908

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, W., Feng, L., Wang, J., Ma, F., Chen, J., Qu, S., et al. (2022). Play smart, be smart? Effect of cognitively engaging physical activity interventions on executive function among children 4~12 years old: a systematic review and Meta-analysis. Brain Sci. 12:762. doi: 10.3390/brainsci12060762

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomporowski, P. D., Davis, C. L., Miller, P. H., and Naglieri, J. A. (2008). Exercise and Children's intelligence, cognition, and academic achievement. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 20, 111–131. doi: 10.1007/s10648-007-9057-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomporowski, P. D., Lambourne, K., and Okumura, M. S. (2011). Physical activity interventions and children's mental function: an introduction and overview. Prev. Med. 52, S3–S9. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2011.01.028

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomporowski, P. D., McCullick, B., Pendleton, D. M., and Pesce, C. (2015). Exercise and children's cognition: the role of exercise characteristics and a place for metacognition. J. Sport Health Sci. 4, 47–55. doi: 10.1016/j.jshs.2014.09.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Fels, I. M. J., Smith, J., Roel, J., Königs, M., Oosterlaan, J., Visscher, C., et al. (2020). The acute effects of two types of physical activity in physical education on response inhibition and lapses of attention in children aged 8-10 years a cluster randomized controlled trial. Int. J. Sport Psychol. 51, 158–182. doi: 10.7352/IJSP.2020.51.158

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Niet, A. G., Smith, J., Oosterlaan, J., Scherder, E. J., Hartman, E., and Visscher, C. (2016). Effects of a cognitively demanding aerobic intervention during recess on Children's physical fitness and executive functioning. Pediatr. Exerc. Sci. 28, 64–70. doi: 10.1123/pes.2015-0084

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vazou, S., and Mavilidi, M. F. (2021). Cognitively engaging physical activity for targeting motor, cognitive, social, and emotional skills in the preschool classroom: the move for thought preK-K program. Front. Psychol. 12:729272. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.729272

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vazou, S., Pesce, C., Lakes, K., and Smiley-Oyen, A. (2019). More than one road leads to Rome: a narrative review and meta-analysis of physical activity intervention effects on cognition in youth. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 17, 153–178. doi: 10.1080/1612197x.2016.1223423

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vita-Barrull, N., Estrada-Plana, V., March-Llanes, J., Guzmán, N., Fernández-Muñoz, C., Ayesa, R., et al. (2023). Board game-based intervention to improve executive functions and academic skills in rural schools: a randomized controlled trial. Trends Neurosci Educ 33:100216. doi: 10.1016/j.tine.2023.100216

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Voss, M. W., Nagamatsu, L. S., Liu-Ambrose, T., and Kramer, A. F. (2011). Exercise, brain, and cognition across the life span. J. Appl. Physiol. (1985) 111, 1505–1513. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00210.2011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J., Yang, Y., Li, L., Yang, X., Guo, X., Yuan, X., et al. (2024). Comparative efficacy of physical activity types on executive functions in children and adolescents: a network meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J. Sci. Med. Sport 27, 187–196. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2023.11.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J., Zhao, X., Bi, Y., Jiang, S., Sun, Y., Lang, J., et al. (2023). Executive function elevated by long term high-intensity physical activity and the regulation role of beta-band activity in human frontal region. Cogn. Neurodyn. 17, 1463–1472. doi: 10.1007/s11571-022-09905-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wei, Y., Wang, L., Tang, Y., Deng, H., Su, J., and Li, Q. (2024). Enhancing young children's executive function through physical activities: a three-level meta-analysis. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 26:100592. doi: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2024.100592

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wen, X., Zhang, Y., Gao, Z., Zhao, W., Jie, J., and Bao, L. (2018). Effect of Mini-trampoline physical activity on executive functions in preschool children. Biomed. Res. Int. 2018, 2712803–2712807. doi: 10.1155/2018/2712803

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Winter, B., Breitenstein, C., Mooren, F. C., Voelker, K., Fobker, M., Lechtermann, A., et al. (2007). High impact running improves learning. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 87, 597–609. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2006.11.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Xue, Y., Yang, Y., and Huang, T. (2019). Effects of chronic exercise interventions on executive function among children and adolescents: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Br. J. Sports Med. 53, 1397–1404. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2018-099825

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: physical activity, cognitive challenge, cognitive function, children, adolescent

Citation: Mao F, Huang F, Zhao S and Fang Q (2024) Effects of cognitively engaging physical activity interventions on executive function in children and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 15:1454447. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1454447

Received: 25 June 2024; Accepted: 14 August 2024;
Published: 23 August 2024.

Edited by:

Alberto Ruiz-Ariza, University of Jaén, Spain

Reviewed by:

Sara Suarez-Manzano, University of Jaén, Spain
Nancy Getchell, University of Delaware, United States

Copyright © 2024 Mao, Huang, Zhao and Fang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Qun Fang, 2020910112@qdu.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.