Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 26 August 2024
Sec. Health Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic The Interplay of Stress, Health, and Well-being: Unraveling the Psychological and Physiological Processes - Volume II View all 12 articles

The relationship between risk perceptions and negative emotions in the COVID-19: a meta-analysis

Ruihua Zhou
Ruihua Zhou1*Kan Shi
Kan Shi1*Xudong SongXudong Song2Shuqi LiShuqi Li1Wei ZhouWei Zhou1
  • 1Department of Psychology, School of Education, Wenzhou University, Wenzhou, China
  • 2School of Public Administration and Policy, Renmin University of China, Beijing, China

Background: COVID-19, as a significant public health issue, has had a major impact on the mental health of people worldwide. Research shows a significant positive correlation between individuals’ risk perception levels and negative emotions during the outbreak of COVID-19. However, some studies also suggest that the relationship between the two is not significant. Therefore, we will conduct a meta-analysis to explore the relationship between risk perception and negative emotions from cultural, temporal, and individual psychological perspectives.

Methods: Searches were conducted in the Web of Science, Pub Med, Google Scholar, PsycINFO, Scopus, and China National Knowledge Infrastructure databases, focusing on publications from January 2020 onwards, specifically targeting studies examining the relationship between risk perception and negative emotion during COVID-19.

Results: A total of 58 papers with 85 effect sizes were meta-analyzed using Comprehensive Meta-Analysis 3.0 software, with a combined sample of 83,948 individuals. Risk perception of COVID-19 showed a moderate positive correlation with negative emotions (r = 0.211, 95%CI [0.18, 0.24]). There was no moderating effect of tight-loose cultures on the relationship between risk perception of COVID-19 and negative emotions. However, the epidemic period, gender ratio, and measurement methods did have moderating effects on the relationship between risk perception of COVID-19 and negative emotions.

Conclusion: In future research, we can further develop theories related to the risk perception of COVID-19 and negative emotions, and based on these, formulate interventions to promote people’s mental health.

Introduction

The outbreak of COVID-19 has caused profound harm to various parts of the world in the past 4 years, with significant negative impacts on physical and mental health. The WHO (2005) classified the coronavirus as an “international public health emergency” that poses a threat to human lives worldwide. The WHO still considers COVID-19 a public health emergency of international concern 3 years later (WHO, 2005). At the same time, due to the recurrent nature of the outbreak, the uncertainty of its spread and the strong transmissibility after mutation have led to a decrease in people’s psychological wellbeing (Talevi et al., 2020). The existing studies have found that in the context of the COVID-19 outbreak, negative emotions such as anxiety, depression, and sensitivity to social risks in the Chinese population increased, while life satisfaction and positive emotions decreased (Li et al., 2020; Rubin and Wessely, 2020).

Negative emotions refer to subjective experiences of unpleasantness and distress in response to adverse events in life. These emotions mainly include depression and anxiety, but also encompass feelings such as anger, fear, and discouragement (Parkitny and McAuley, 2010; Watson et al., 1988). COVID-19 has led to challenges in regulating these negative emotions, resulting in clinical symptoms like depression and anxiety (Wang C. et al., 2020). Thus, this study focuses on three types of emotions: (1) General negative emotions: characterized by unpleasant and distressing subjective experiences, including various emotions such as anxiety, depression, anger, and discouragement. (2) Anxiety: A tense emotional state experienced when individuals perceive potential dangers or threats in their current situation (Spielberger and Reheiser, 2009). (3) Depression: prolonged emotional states characterized by sadness and fear (Jackson, 2008).

Risk is defined by the magnitude of event probabilities and their consequences (Freudenburg, 1988). In the field of psychological research on risk, there is a primary focus on risk perception. Risk perception refers to an individual’s awareness and understanding of various objective risks in the external environment, emphasizing subjective feelings in this process (Slovic, 1987). Following Slovic and Peters (2006) definition, we define the risk perception of COVID-19 as an individual’s subjective evaluation and response to the potential risks and consequences associated with COVID-19-related information and situations.

Numerous studies have demonstrated a positive correlation between risk perceptions and negative emotions (Hogarth et al., 2011; Johnson and Tversky, 1983; Kopetz, 2017; Sjöberg, 2007; Yuen and Lee, 2003). This conclusion has also been confirmed in the context of COVID-19 (Barattucci et al., 2020). Research indicates that risk perception of COVID-19 is associated with higher levels of negative emotions (Han Q. et al., 2021). According to cognitive appraisal theory, individual emotions are determined by their appraisal of events. When assessing threats with high uncertainty, such as COVID-19, individuals are more likely to experience anxiety (Epstein, 1994; Lazarus, 1968; Lazarus, 1991). A study conducted during COVID-19 highlighted that worry can activate a dysfunctional overestimation of threats, leading to psychological distress. However, it can also activate functional appraisals, enhancing self-efficacy and thereby reducing distress (Diotaiuti et al., 2023). The transactional stress theory suggests that when individuals perceive an unavoidable health risk, they are more likely to use emotion-focused coping strategies (Lazarus and Folkman, 1984). In such cases, the relationship between risk perception of COVID-19 and negative emotions might be stronger (Chen et al., 2022). The social amplification framework hypothesizes that network communication will exaggerate the risks of COVID-19, causing people to focus more on negative information, thereby increasing negative emotions and risk perception (Ng et al., 2018). The behavioral immune system (BIS) theory suggests that when individuals face pathogen threats, the BIS triggers negative emotions such as disgust and anxiety, and enhances disease perception to avoid infection (Li et al., 2020; Makhanova and Shepherd, 2020; Neuberg et al., 2011). Furthermore, infectious diseases (such as COVID-19) have had a significant impact on human genetic evolution, making people more likely to overestimate related risks and experience negative emotions.

However, existing studies suggest that there may be moderating variables between risk perception and negative emotions. Tight-loose cultures, as a new cultural dimension, have their roots in ancient history and philosophy, and were first studied anthropologically by distinguishing between “tight” and “loose” traditional societies (Pelto, 1968). Tight cultures have high social norm strength and low tolerance for deviant behavior, while loose cultures are the opposite (Gelfand et al., 2011). A study highlighted that during the threat of COVID-19, governments in tightened cultural regions implemented stricter intervention policies to curb the spread of the disease, thereby protecting the population from the threat (Gelfand et al., 2011; Gelfand et al., 2021). Moreover, Dong et al. (2021) conducted a cross-sectional study that demonstrated cultural tightness–looseness as a moderating variable, relieving the positive correlation between risk perception and depression and anxiety. In the context of COVID-19, research on gender differences in risk perception has yielded various results (Jin et al., 2020; Dryhurst et al., 2022). Historical experiences suggest that the risks associated with COVID-19 may have a greater impact on females (for example, immune function in pregnant females is suppressed, leading to more severe viral damage to the body), resulting in higher risk perception among females and triggering more intense negative emotions (Ackerman et al., 2018). Additionally, some scholars argue that due to the high heterogeneity in the conceptualization of risk perception, research findings vary (Vieira et al., 2022). Currently, there are mainly two types of risk perception models. The first is from the Health Belief Model (HBM), which includes Perceived Severity (an individual’s awareness of the consequences of diseases), Perceived Susceptibility (an individual’s assessment of the likelihood of contracting a disease), and Perceived Vulnerability (an individual’s belief in their susceptibility to infection and consequent harm) (Brewer et al., 2007; Champion and Skinner, 2008). The second is based on Slovic’s paradigm, which primarily involves the dimensions of Familiarity and Controllability (Gan and Fu, 2022). Current research rarely discusses the distinctions between these two types of risk perception measurement models in the context of COVID-19, and it is still unknown whether they influence the relationship with negative emotions. Finally, research has shown that the relationship between COVID-19 risk perception and depression is influenced by the pandemic period (Liu et al., 2024). Dyer and Kolic (2020) found through semantic network analysis that the phenomenon of psychophysical numbing under COVID-19 is increasingly evident, demonstrating that negative emotions gradually decline over the duration of the pandemic, leading to a numbed mindset. It is uncertain whether this change affects the relationship between risk perception and negative emotions. Given that humanity may need to coexist with COVID-19 for an extended period, it is crucial to study the dynamic relationship between COVID-19 risk perception and negative emotions over different time periods.

Therefore, the purpose of this meta-analysis and systematic review is to explore the relationship between risk perception and negative emotions during COVID-19 and to investigate four potential moderating variables: cultural tightness–looseness, gender ratio, epidemic period, and risk perception measurement models.

Methodology

This article has been reported by the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) checklist.

Data sources

All literature data are from the following databases: Web of Science, Pub Med, PsycINFO, Scopus and China National Knowledge Infrastructure. We used “risk perception COVID-19” AND “Negative affect” OR “Anxiety” OR “Depression” OR “Negative emotion” as the keywords to search in the PubMed dataset and used “TS = (risk perception COVID-19) AND ((TS = (Negative emotion)) OR (TS = (Anxiety)) OR (TS = (depression)))” in the Web of science dataset. We retrieved articles spanning from June to September 2022, totaling 1976 documents.

Inclusion criteria and exclusion criteria

1. This study includes correlation coefficients and other metrics that can be converted into Fisher’s Z-values for meta-analysis, such as regression coefficients and chi-square values. Data that cannot be transformed in this manner will be excluded. (2) Negative emotions include “negative emotions,” “anxiety,” or “depression.” Considering that negative emotion measurements specific to events such as COVID-19 may temporarily exaggerate due to situational influences (Davis et al., 2011), we have chosen to exclude such specific measures (e.g., the COVID-19 Anxiety Scale: How anxious are you when discussing COVID-19?). (3) Participants in this study include all individuals affected by COVID-19, excluding those with mental disorders or severe illnesses, as well as COVID-19 patients, healthcare workers, or individuals with special experiences related to COVID-19. (4) This study will include empirical research but will not include review studies such as meta-analyses, scoping reviews, or systematic reviews. (5) The study must be published after January 2020, that is, after the outbreak of COVID-19. (6) Risk perception measurement methods include perceived severity, perceived possibility, perceived vulnerability, and the Slovic paradigm. Methods not falling into these categories are defined as “others.”

Literature coding and quality evaluation

Each study was reviewed and independently coded by two psychology professionals who underwent a one-month training program and used a standardized template (Supplementary Table S2) for data extraction. Differences in coding were resolved through consultation, with the final coding determined by the corresponding author. The final encoding consistency is 100%. The coding content includes author information, publication year, male ratio, age, country, tight-loose cultures, epidemic period, and risk perception measurement method. For the coding of tightness culture, Fischer and Karl (2022) were adopted, combined with Gelfand et al. (2011, 2021)‘s measurements of national tightness. For countries or regions not included in the study, we did not perform coding. Due to standardization and centralization, A score of 0 represents the average level of tight-loose culture across countries. Scores above 0 indicate a tight culture, characterized by stronger social norms and lower tolerance for deviant behavior, while those below 0 were coded as loose cultures. Based on the Health Belief Model, risk perception is encoded as “perceived likelihood,” “perceived severity,” “perceived vulnerability” or a combination of these three measurement methods (Brewer et al., 2007; Champion and Skinner, 2008). According to Slovic’s psychological measurement model, familiarity and controllability are defined as the “Slovic paradigm.” If the measurement methods do not belong to the above, they are coded as “the others.” For effect size, linear regression β-values are converted into correlation coefficients using the formula of Peterson and Brown (2005). In this study, we used the Joanna Briggs Institute (JBI) critical appraisal checklist to assess the quality of cross-sectional studies (Munn et al., 2020). Studies with scores below 50% were excluded to ensure the reliability of the included research and the credibility of the results.

Meta-analytic process

In the study, Comprehensive Meta-Analysis 3.0 software was used for the main effect test and the adjustment effect test. We conducted subgroup analysis on negative emotions, a method that divides a specific variable into different groups and examines differences between these groups to determine whether there are variations between different dependent variable indicators. If there are differences, they will be analyzed separately later. When there are multiple similar effect size indicators in a single study, the average effect size of multiple correlation coefficients is used. We will further explore the potential impact of non-independent effects on the results through sensitivity analysis. Specifically, we will exclude studies with non-independent effects to examine changes in the overall effect size.

Due to differences in research characteristics such as risk perception measurement methods and types of negative emotions, a random effects model was used to aggregate effect sizes. The model uses a weighted average to estimate the overall effect size, considering not only the within-study variance but also the between-study variance. On the tests of heterogeneity, the variability of the effect size in the main research was evaluated by the significance of Q-test results, I2, Tau2. An I2 > 75% indicates high heterogeneity, suggesting the appropriateness of the random effects model (Higgins et al., 2003).

Publication bias was tested qualitatively using funnel plot distribution of effect sizes. Quantitatively, Egger’s regression coefficient and Begg rank correlation test, with significant results indicating potential bias (Begg and Mazumdar, 1994; Sterne and Egger, 2001). Additionally, Orwin’s fail-safe N test and Classic Fail-safe N tests were used to determine how many unpublished studies are needed to make the overall effect size trivial or the p-value non-significant. Finally, the trim and fill method was used to correct for publication bias by adjusting for missing studies.

We employed subgroup analysis for categorical moderating variables, ensuring that at least three effect sizes were included at each level (Song et al., 2014). To test the effects of continuous moderating variables, this study employed a random effects model meta-regression.

Results

Literature inclusion and quality evaluation

According to the search strategy (Supplementary Table S1), a total of 58 studies were included in this meta-analysis, comprising 85 effect sizes and 83,948 subjects. This includes doctoral and master’s theses, with 12 studies in Chinese and 46 in English. Negative emotions include 19 effect size (N = 23,722), anxiety includes 41 effect size (N = 54,215), and depression includes 25 effect size (N = 40,592). Detailed data are shown in Supplementary Table S2. The present meta-analysis contained a total of 58 primary studies, 41 of which were from tight cultures. For data encoding, Cohen’s Kappa (k) scored by two raters is 0.89–0.94. Of the quality assessments, all the studies scored above 50% (Supplementary Table S3). The specific process is shown in Figure 1.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Process of selecting and screening articles.

Heterogeneity

The analysis results show that the overall Q-value is 2190.04, the I2-value is 96.16% and more than 75%, indicating a high level of heterogeneity in the results. Therefore, a random-effects model is appropriate (Higgins et al., 2003). And further explore other moderating variables that lead to heterogeneity of research results (Table 1).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Heterogeneity test between risk perception of COVID-19 and negative emotion.

Overall analyses

For the 58 papers analyzed, according to the analysis (as shown in Table 2), the overall effect size between risk perceptions and negative emotions of COVID-19 was r = 0.211 (K = 85) with 95%CI [0.18, 0.24] and no 0 in the interval, showing a moderate correlation, and the correlation coefficients for each sub-indicator of negative emotion, anxiety, and depression were 0.17 (K = 19, 95%CI [0.08, 0.25]), 0.24 (K = 41, 95%CI [0.21, 0.27]), 0.20 (K = 25, 95%CI [0.16, 0.24]). The data analysis conducted using subgroup analyses highlighted no significant differences between the groups regarding negative emotions (Qb = 3.78, p = 0.156), so the analysis of negative affect in the latter part was only for the overall indicators.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Overall effect size between risk perception of COVID-19 and negative emotion.

Sensitivity analysis

We performed the following sensitivity analyses to ensure the stability of our results. Firstly, we compared the overall effect sizes from the random effects model (r = 0.211) and the fixed effects model (r = 0.18). Secondly, we excluded studies using the average of multiple effect sizes (4 effect sizes were computed using average effect values, constituting 4.7% of the total studies). After exclusion, the overall effect size was 0.213, and the heterogeneity test showed that I2 = 96.19%. Thirdly, using leave-one-out analysis, the results showed that the effect size r-value fluctuated between 0.206 and 0.216. The above sensitivity analysis results indicate that the overall results are relatively stable.

Moderating effect

Firstly, for continuous moderating variables, random effects meta-regression was used, and the results showed that gender ratio moderated risk perceptions and negative emotions for COVID-19. The regression coefficient of the male ratio on the effect size was significant β = 0.46 (this indicates that the change of one male ratio unit leads to 0.46 times change of effect size), 95% CI [0.27, 0.65], p < 0.001, R2 = 29%. Meanwhile, for the subgroup with k (this implies the existence of a pairwise relationship of correlation coefficients) <3. For example, since there were only two sets of correlation coefficients in 2020.09, they were not included in the subgroup analysis. Finally, for categorical moderating variables, subgroup analysis was taken, and the results showed (Table 3) that.

1. There was no moderating effect of tight-loose cultures on risk perceptions and negative emotions of COVID-19, QB = 0.07, p = 0.785. For the insignificant moderating effect of tight-loose cultures, we conducted a further analysis by splitting tight culture and loose culture into two subgroups and observing the correlations under the corresponding cultures (Table 4). Excluding studies lacking corresponding cultural codes, we still did not find significant differences in the association between risk perceptions and negative emotions under tight-loose cultures. However, some numerical trends were found, for example, the association between risk perception and depression was slightly stronger in the tight culture (r = 0.221) than in the loose culture (r = 0.188), while the opposite was true for negative emotions (in the tight culture r = 0.159, in loose culture r = 0.212). The anxiety group showed a similar effect size (in tight culture r = 0.258, in loose culture r = 0.244). We also used the continuous variable of cultural tightness for supplementary meta-regression analysis. The regression coefficient was not significant β = −0.08, 95%CI [−0.17, 0.004], p > 0.05, R2 = 1% (Table 5).

2. Risk perception measures moderated risk perception and negative emotions of COVID-19, QB = 18.22, p = 0.011, where the combination based on perceived severity and perceived possibility showed the strongest positive correlation (r = 0.28) between risk perceptions and negative emotions of COVID-19 (when other tools are excluded), while the combination based on Slovic’s familiarity, controllability showed the weakest positive correlation (r = 0.12).

3. The epidemic period moderated risk perceptions of COVID-19 and negative emotions, QB = 15.66, p = 0.016. Risk perception was most strongly positively associated (r = 0.47) with negative emotions in the time period 2021 and beyond, and least positively associated (r = 0.11) in the time period May 2020. In addition, we found an approximate inverted U curve of the effect sizes between January 2020 and May 2020 (although numerically this appears to be very weak), as shown in Figure 2. In addition, we also conducted a meta-regression analysis to test the moderating effect by using the epidemic period as a continuous variable. Specifically, we used the months since the outbreak of the pandemic as a continuous numerical variable (for example, January 2020 was 1, and February 2021 was 14). The regression coefficient was significant β = 0.02, 95%CI [0.01, 0.03], p < 0.01, R2 = 2% (Table 5).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Moderating effect between risk perception of COVID-19 and negative emotion.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Analysis of moderating effects of outcome variables.

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Results of meta regression.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Epidemic period curve. The vertical axis represents the effect size, and the horizontal axis represents the epidemic period. Numbers 1–6 represent January to June 2020, while 7 represents 2021 and later.

Publication bias

When there is a possibility of publication bias, effect sizes show an asymmetric distribution on the funnel plot (Borenstein et al., 2009). From the funnel chart observation (as shown in Figure 3), the effect sizes are mostly concentrated in the upper part of the funnel, and the distribution is uniform and symmetrical. The intercept of the Egger linear regression is 2.01, 95%CI [− 0.12, 4.15], and the p-value is 0.064. The p-value of the Begg test is 0.093. The results are not significant. The trim and fill test found 17 missing effect sizes on the left side of the funnel plot. These effect sizes were included in the analysis to get a new weighted effect size r = 0.161. The difference from the observed effect size is 23.7%, which belongs to the moderate cut-off value in publication bias (Chang et al., 2022). The classic fail-safe number is 4145, which is >5 K + 10 standard. The value of the classic fail-safe number indicates that at least 4,145 studies are needed to make the results insignificant. Orwin’s fail-safe N-value was 70, this means that when 70 articles with a correlation of 0.00 are included, the effect size will be lower than 0.1. Based on the above results, there is a low possibility of publication bias in this study. To further verify, we also used P-curve analysis, cumulative meta-analysis, and contour-enhanced funnel plot methods. The results also showed that there was a low probability of publication bias in the study (Supplementary Table S4).

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Funnel plot.

Discussion

The relationship between risk perceptions and negative emotions of COVID-19

So far, this paper has examined the relationship between risk perceptions of COVID-19 and negative emotions through a meta-analysis, which includes 58 studies. The results reveal a moderate positive correlation (r = 0.211), with all three types of negative emotions strongly associated with risk perceptions. This aligns with the majority of research findings in the context of COVID-19 (Zhao et al., 2021), indicating that individuals with a heightened risk perception of COVID-19 also experience elevated levels of negative emotions. The moderate effect size indicated by the study may also be due to the numerous online measures that improved people’s mental health during COVID-19 (D’Oliveira et al., 2022). Furthermore, the lack of significant differences in the three types of negative emotions may be attributed to the simultaneous occurrence of depression and anxiety, which often coexist with considerable overlap in both psychological and clinical domains (Kessler et al., 2015). In terms of effect sizes, depression exhibits the lowest effect size, while anxiety shows the highest. This could possibly be attributed to a heightened awareness of unknown risks, such as anxiety, prompting individuals to promptly identify and avoid potential environmental cues associated with the virus. Another plausible explanation could be that, for an extended period, people have not felt helpless in the face of the virus spread. In summary, the results of the study indicate that the connection between risk perception and negative emotions remains significant in the context of COVID-19. The risk perception theories developed in previous pandemics are still applicable to the current situation of COVID-19.

Tight-loose cultures

We found that the moderating effect of tight-loose culture on the risk perception of COVID-19 and negative emotions is not statistically significant. The reason for this may lie in the uneven distribution of tightness–looseness culture in the current sample (Tight culture = 55, Loose culture = 21). Numerous studies suggest that the moderating effect of tightness–looseness culture during the pandemic is significant (Tu et al., 2023). If the sample sizes from different cultures could be balanced, we speculate that a significant moderating effect of tightness–looseness culture might be observed. Another possible reason is that the studies included in this research focus on the early stages of COVID-19 or within the first 6 months after the outbreak. During this period, governments in regions with different cultures almost universally implemented various preventive measures, such as quarantine, social distancing, and the cancelation of large events (Rodríguez et al., 2022). This uniformity in response measures makes it difficult to reflect cultural differences in looseness–tightness.

Gender ratio

Meta-regression analysis showed that a higher male ratio was associated with a stronger link between risk perceptions of COVID-19 and negative emotions. This finding contradicts the common observation that women are more prone to negative emotions and higher risk assessments (Campbell, 2013). One possible explanation is that men have higher mobility during the pandemic, which increases their exposure to the virus and subsequently elevates their levels of negative emotions (Semenova et al., 2021). Furthermore, men, as primary protectors and providers, are more likely to work outside. This increases their risk exposure and anxiety levels. Economic pressures and concerns about family stability may also contribute to higher levels of anxiety in men. Conversely, women’s heightened sensitivity to risk leads them to adopt more proactive measures to mitigate risks, potentially reducing the link between risk perceptions and negative emotions (Rana et al., 2021).

Measurement method

The meta-analysis results indicated that, excluding other measures, the combination of perceived severity and possibility was the strongest predictor of the relationship between COVID-19 risk perceptions and negative emotions, while Slovic’s measurement paradigm was the weakest. One possible explanation is that familiarity and controllability measures lead people to judge risk more subjectively, whereas perceived possibility, severity, and vulnerability focus on objective, observable attributes of risk (Capone et al., 2021). Strong preventive measures and information about the virus and health behaviors at the pandemic’s outset enhanced people’s subjective motivation in risk perception, reducing the association with negative emotions. However, infection and mortality rates continued to rise during the pandemic’s initial phase (Baud et al., 2020). Therefore, objective epidemic risk still threatened people’s health security, and objective measures of perception remained strongly associated with negative emotions.

Epidemic period

Meta-analysis showed that the correlation between risk perceptions and negative affect increased until March 2020, decreased after the WHO declared COVID-19 a global outbreak on March 11, and spiked again in June 2020, peaking in 2021. Studies have found that risk perceptions and anxiety levels were higher during the pandemic’s initial period, declining sharply over time (Savadori and Lauriola, 2022; Wang Z. et al., 2021; Qiao et al., 2023). According to the diffusion of innovation theory (Rogers et al., 2014), it takes time for people to absorb COVID-19 risk information, and as they receive more, their risk assessment increases. Early in the pandemic, limited information weakened people’s sense of control and heightened their sense of threat. Emotional information spreads quickly via social media, strengthening the link between risk perceptions and negative emotions (Brady et al., 2017).

In the later stages of the pandemic, meta-analytic results partially validate the phenomenon of psychological numbness. The high cost of maintaining intense negative emotions over time prompts people to start avoiding these emotions (Li et al., 2021). This may explain the gradual decrease in effect size. Risk perceptions were more strongly associated with negative emotions before the pandemic than after. This shift could be due to increased information availability, governmental control, and psychological adaptation (Tedaldi et al., 2022). The effect size spiked in June 2020 and June 2021, likely related to the coronavirus mutations. As people received more information about new variants, uncertainty rose, temporarily increasing the correlation between risk perceptions and negative emotions. Therefore, the effect values varied significantly across different periods, reflecting the epidemic’s impact on psychological states.

Limitations

However, this study also has some shortcomings and perspectives: (1) As negative emotions contain many connotations and few studies have further sorted them out, the current study only includes the common indicators of mental health: negative emotions, depression, and anxiety. (2) It is necessary to discuss whether the participants included in the current study can adequately and effectively reveal the relationship between risk perception of COVID-19 and negative emotions. Studies have shown that more than one-third of healthcare workers experienced varying degrees of anxiety and depression during COVID-19 (Amin et al., 2020). Due to the widespread impact of COVID-19, it is necessary to include a broader range of populations in future studies. Additionally, it is important to explore whether the research results change over the duration of. For instance, people may gradually lower their risk assessment of the pandemic, which necessitates including more literature in future studies for further examination. (3) The moderating effect was not significant because the study of tight-loose cultures is still in its early stages, and each country experiences dynamic changes in its degree of tightness and looseness. This indicates the need for longitudinal studies on tight-loose cultures over time. For the epidemic period, our method of dividing by month needs improvement. Future research should use more refined coding or other methods. Regarding risk perception measurement, while mainstream models were used, other studies suggest that risk perceptions should include more questions and employ more accurate methods. Future research should integrate newly developed risk perception of COVID-19 measurements. (4) This study focused on the relationship between risk perceptions of COVID-19 and negative emotions. Future research should compare these results with other epidemic periods to determine if the findings are specific to COVID-19 and to establish a comprehensive theoretical framework for risk perception under different epidemics. (5) Finally, the choice of research methods may introduce potential biases. Compared to the traditional bivariate meta-analysis used in this study, multilevel meta-analysis has been widely adopted in recent years due to its ability to better control for biases. Future research could consider employing more advanced statistical methods to reduce bias.

Conclusion

In the research, we discuss the relationship between risk perceptions and negative emotions. Cognitive theory and evolutionary psychology theory have also appropriately predicted their results, which not only expands psychological research in the context of COVID-19, but also provides an integrated model for risk perception for future research. In a large number of COVID-19 research, this study used the meta-analysis method for the first time to explore the relationship between risk perceptions of COVID-19 and negative emotions. Firstly, it reconfirms the association between risk perceptions of COVID-19 and negative emotions. In addition, it also enriched the research on risk perception during COVID-19, and explored the influence of cultural factors, demographic factors, psychometrics and psychological factors. Thus, we provide empirical support for the field of mental health of the general population with a targeted and scientific basis and lay the foundation for future risk perception research and mental health intervention development.

Data availability statement

The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Author contributions

RZ: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Resources, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft. KS: Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing. XS: Data curation, Software, Validation, Writing – review & editing. SL: Conceptualization, Data curation, Resources, Software, Writing – review & editing. WZ: Investigation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This study was supported by the Zhejiang Province Philosophy and Social Sciences Emerging (Cross disciplinary) Major Project “Research on Public Risk Perception, Behavior Rules and Countermeasures under Major Public Health Emergencies”, an emerging (crossover) major project of Philosophy and Social Sciences in Zhejiang Province in 2021 (21XXJC04ZD).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1453111/full#supplementary-material

References

Ackerman, J. M., Hill, S. E., and Murray, D. R. (2018). The behavioral immune system: current concerns and future directions. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 12:e12371. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12371

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Amin, F., Sharif, S., Saeed, R., Durrani, N., and Jilani, D. (2020). COVID-19 pandemic-knowledge, perception, anxiety and depression among frontline doctors of Pakistan. BMC Psychiatr. 20, 1–9. doi: 10.1186/s12888-020-02864-x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baud, D., Qi, X., Nielsen-Saines, K., Musso, D., Pomar, L., and Favre, G. (2020). Real estimates of mortality following COVID-19 infection. Lancet Infect. Dis. 20:773. doi: 10.1016/S1473-3099(20)30195-X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Begg, C. B., and Mazumdar, M. (1994). Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias. Biometrics 50:1088. doi: 10.2307/2533446

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., and Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Chapter 20. Metaregression. Introduction to meta-analysis, 187–204.

Google Scholar

Brady, W. J., Wills, J. A., Jost, J. T., Tucker, J. A., and Van Bavel, J. J. (2017). Emotion shapes the diffusion of moralized content in social networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 114, 7313–7318. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1618923114

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brewer, N. T., Chapman, G. B., Gibbons, F. X., Gerrard, M., McCaul, K. D., and Weinstein, N. D. (2007). Meta-analysis of the relationship between risk perception and health behavior: the example of vaccination. Health Psychol. 26, 136–145. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.26.2.136

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell, A. (2013). The evolutionary psychology of women's aggression. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B. Biol. Sci. 368:20130078. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0078

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Capone, V., Donizzetti, A. R., and Park, M. S. A. (2021). Validation and psychometric evaluation of the COVID-19 risk perception scale (CoRP): a new brief scale to measure individuals’ risk perception. Int. J. Ment. Heal. Addict. 27, 1–14. doi: 10.1007/s11469-021-00660-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Champion, V. L., and Skinner, C. S. (2008). The health belief model. Health Behav. Health Educ. Theor. Res. Prac. 4, 45–65.

Google Scholar

Chang, H. Y., Binali, T., Liang, J. C., Chiou, G. L., Cheng, K. H., Lee, S. W. Y., et al. (2022). Ten years of augmented reality in education: a meta-analysis of (quasi-) experimental studies to investigate the impact. Comput. Educ. 191:104641. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2022.104641

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, C., Guan, Z., Sun, L., Zhou, T., and Guan, R. (2022). COVID-19 exposure, pandemic-related appraisals, coping strategies, and psychological symptoms among the frontline medical staff and gender differences in coping processes. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 36, 111–120. doi: 10.1002/acp.3902

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, J. I., Gross, J. J., and Ochsner, K. N. (2011). Psychological distance and emotional experience: what you see is what you get. Emotion 11, 438–444. doi: 10.1037/a0021783

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dong, D., Chen, Z., Zong, M., Zhang, P., Gu, W., Feng, Y., et al. (2021). What protects us against the COVID-19 threat? Cultural tightness matters. BMC Pub. Health 21, 1–11. doi: 10.1186/s12889-021-12161-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Diotaiuti, P., Valente, G., Mancone, S., Corrado, S., Bellizzi, F., Falese, L., et al. (2023). Effects of cognitive appraisals on perceived self-efficacy and distress during the COVID-19 lockdown: an empirical analysis based on structural equation modeling. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 20:5294. doi: 10.3390/ijerph20075294

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dryhurst, S., Schneider, C. R., Kerr, J., Freeman, A. L., Recchia, G., Van Der Bles, A. M., et al. (2022). Risk perceptions of COVID-19 around the world. J. Risk Res. 23, 994–1006. doi: 10.1080/13669877.2020.1758193

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dyer, J., and Kolic, B. (2020). Public risk perception and emotion on twitter during the Covid-19 pandemic. Appl. Netw. Sci. 5, 99–32. doi: 10.1007/s41109-020-00334-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Oliveira, A., De Souza, L. C., Langiano, E., Falese, L., Diotaiuti, P., Vilarino, G. T., et al. (2022). Home physical exercise protocol for older adults, applied remotely during the COVID-19 pandemic: protocol for randomized and controlled trial. Front. Psychol. 13:828495. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.828495

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. Am. Psychol. 49, 709–724. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.49.8.709

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, R., and Karl, J. A. (2022). Predicting behavioral intentions to prevent or mitigate COVID-19: a cross-cultural meta-analysis of attitudes, norms, and perceived behavioral control effects. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 13, 264–276. doi: 10.1177/19485506211019844

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Freudenburg, W. R. (1988). Perceived risk, real risk: social science and the art of probabilistic risk assessment. Science 242, 44–49. doi: 10.1126/science.3175635

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gelfand, M. J., Jackson, J. C., Pan, X., Nau, D., Pieper, D., Denison, E., et al. (2021). The relationship between cultural tightness–looseness and COVID-19 cases and deaths: a global analysis. Lancet Planet. Health 5, e135–e144. doi: 10.1016/S2542-5196(20)30301-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gelfand, M. J., Raver, J. L., Nishii, L., Leslie, L. M., Lun, J., Lim, B. C., et al. (2011). Differences between tight and loose cultures: a 33-nation study. Science 332, 1100–1104. doi: 10.1126/science.1197754

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, Q., Zheng, B., Agostini, M., Bélanger, J. J., Gützkow, B., and Kreienkamp, J. (2021). Associations of risk perception of COVID-19 with emotion and mental health during the pandemic. J. Affect. Disord. 284, 247–255. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2021.01.049

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, J. P., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., and Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ 327, 557–560. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hogarth, R. M., Portell, M., Cuxart, A., and Kolev, G. I. (2011). Emotion and reason in everyday risk perception. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 24, 202–222. doi: 10.1002/bdm.689

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, S. W. (2008). “A history of melancholia and depression” in History of psychiatry and medical psychology: with an epilogue on psychiatry and the mind-body relation. ed. M. B. Relation (Boston, MA: Springer US), 443–460.

Google Scholar

Jin, J. M., Bai, P., He, W., Wu, F., Liu, X. F., Han, D. M., et al. (2020). Gender differences in patients with COVID-19: focus on severity and mortality. Front. Public Health 8:152. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2020.00152

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, E. J., and Tversky, A. (1983). Affect, generalization, and the perception of risk. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 45, 20–31. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.45.1.20

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, R. C., Sampson, N. A., Berglund, P., Gruber, M. J., Al-Hamzawi, A., Andrade, L., et al. (2015). Anxious and non-anxious major depressive disorder in the World Health Organization world mental health surveys. Epidemiol. Psychiatr. Sci. 24, 210–226. doi: 10.1017/S2045796015000189

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kopetz, C. E. (2017). “Risk and self-defeating behaviors as goal pursuit rather than regulatory failure” in The motivation-cognition interface (London: Routledge), 106–130.

Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S. (1968). Emotions and adaptation: conceptual and empirical relations. In Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.

Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S., and Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. Cham: Springer publishing company.

Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Li, S., Wang, Y., Xue, J., Zhao, N., and Zhu, T. (2020). The impact of COVID-19 epidemic declaration on psychological consequences: a study on active Weibo users. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:2032. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17062032

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Y., Luan, S., Li, Y., and Hertwig, R. (2021). Changing emotions in the COVID-19 pandemic: a four-wave longitudinal study in the United States and China. Soc. Sci. Med. 285:114222. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2021.114222

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, D., Li, J., Wang, Q., Wang, H., Liu, X., Zhai, M., et al. (2024). Effects of COVID-19 infection risk perception on depressive symptoms among pregnant women in different periods of the COVID-19 pandemic in China: a mediation model. Int. J. Gynecol. Obstet. 166, 819–827. doi: 10.1002/ijgo.15416

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Makhanova, A., and Shepherd, M. A. (2020). Behavioral immune system linked to responses to the threat of COVID-19. Personal. Individ. Differ. 167:110221. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110221

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Munn, Z., Barker, T. H., Moola, S., Tufanaru, C., Stern, C., McArthur, A., et al. (2020). Methodological quality of case series studies: an introduction to the JBI critical appraisal tool. JBI Evid. Synth. 18, 2127–2133. doi: 10.11124/JBISRIR-D-19-00099

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Neuberg, S. L., Kenrick, D. T., and Schaller, M. (2011). Human threat management systems: self-protection and disease avoidance. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 1042–1051. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.08.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ng, Y. J., Yang, Z. J., and Vishwanath, A. (2018). To fear or not to fear? Applying the social amplification of risk framework on two environmental health risks in Singapore. Journal of Risk Research, 21, 1487–1501. doi: 10.1080/13669877.2017.1313762

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Parkitny, L., and McAuley, J. (2010). The depression anxiety stress scale (DASS). J. Physiother. 56:204. doi: 10.1016/S1836-9553(10)70030-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pelto, P. J. (1968). The differences between" tight" and" loose" societies. Transaction 5, 37–40. doi: 10.1007/BF03180447

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, R. A., and Brown, S. P. (2005). On the use of beta coefficients in meta-analysis. J. Appl. Psychol. 90, 175–181. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.90.1.175

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Qiao, S., Li, Z., Liang, C., Li, X., and Rudisill, C. (2023). Three dimensions of COVID-19 risk perceptions and their socioeconomic correlates in the United States: a social media analysis. Risk Anal. 43, 1174–1186. doi: 10.1111/risa.13993

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rana, I. A., Bhatti, S. S., Aslam, A. B., Jamshed, A., Ahmad, J., and Shah, A. A. (2021). COVID-19 risk perception and coping mechanisms: does gender make a difference? Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduc. 55:102096. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdrr.2021.102096

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, E. M., Singhal, A., and Quinlan, M. M. (2014). “Diffusion of innovations” in An integrated approach to communication theory and research (London: Routledge), 432–448.

Google Scholar

Rubin, G. J., and Wessely, S. (2020). The psychological effects of quarantining a city. BMJ 368:m313. doi: 10.1136/bmj.m313

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Savadori, L., and Lauriola, M. (2022). Risk perceptions and COVID-19 protective behaviors: a two-wave longitudinal study of epidemic and post-epidemic periods. Soc. Sci. Med. 301:114949. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2022.114949

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Semenova, O., Apalkova, J., and Butovskaya, M. (2021). Sex differences in spatial activity and anxiety levels in the COVID-19 pandemic from evolutionary perspective. Sustain. For. 13:1110. doi: 10.3390/su13031110

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sjöberg, L. (2007). Emotions and risk perception. Risk Manage. 9, 223–237. doi: 10.1057/palgrave.rm.8250038

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Slovic, P. (1987). Perception of risk. Science 236, 280–285. doi: 10.1126/science.3563507

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Slovic, P., and Peters, E. (2006). Risk perception and affect. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 15, 322–325. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00461.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, H., Zmyslinski-Seelig, A., Kim, J., Drent, A., Victor, A., Omori, K., et al. (2014). Does Facebook make you lonely?: a meta analysis. Comput. Hum. Behav. 36, 446–452. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.04.011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D., and Reheiser, E. C. (2009). Assessment of emotions: anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity. Appl. Psychol. Health Well Being 1, 271–302. doi: 10.1111/j.1758-0854.2009.01017.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sterne, J. A., and Egger, M. (2001). Funnel plots for detecting bias in meta-analysis: guidelines on choice of axis. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 54, 1046–1055. doi: 10.1016/S0895-4356(01)00377-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Talevi, D., Socci, V., Carai, M., Carnaghi, G., Faleri, S., Trebbi, E., et al. (2020). Mental health outcomes of the COVID-19 pandemic. Riv. Psichiatr. 55, 137–144. doi: 10.1708/3382.33569

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tedaldi, E., Orabona, N., Hovnanyan, A., Rubaltelli, E., and Scrimin, S. (2022). Trends in state anxiety during the full lockdown in Italy: the role played by COVID-19 risk perception and trait emotional intelligence. Trauma Care 2, 418–426. doi: 10.3390/traumacare2030034

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tu, K., Chen, S., and Mesler, R. M. (2023). Policy stringency and the spread of COVID-19: the moderating role of culture and its implications on first responses. Health Policy 137:104896. doi: 10.1016/j.healthpol.2023.104896

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vieira, K. M., Potrich, A. C. G., Bressan, A. A., Klein, L. L., Pereira, B. A. D., and Pinto, N. G. M. (2022). A pandemic risk perception scale. Risk Anal. 42, 69–84. doi: 10.1111/risa.13802

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, C., Pan, R., Wan, X., Tan, Y., Xu, L., Ho, C. S., et al. (2020). Immediate psychological responses and associated factors during the initial stage of the 2019 coronavirus disease (COVID-19) epidemic among the general population in China. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:1729. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17051729

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Z., Luo, S., Xu, J., Wang, Y., Yun, H., Zhao, Z., et al. (2021). Well-being reduces COVID-19 anxiety: a three-wave longitudinal study in China. J. Happiness Stud. 22, 3593–3610. doi: 10.1007/s10902-021-00385-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

WHO . (2005). Statement on the fourteenth meeting of the international health regulations emergency committee regarding the coronavirus disease (COVID-19) pandemic. Available at: https://www.who.int/news/item/30-01-2023-statement-on-the-fourteenth-meeting-of-the-international-health-regulations-(2005)-emergency-committee-regarding-the-coronavirus-disease-(covid-19)-pandemic (Accessed January 30, 2023).

Google Scholar

Yuen, K. S., and Lee, T. M. (2003). Could mood state affect risk-taking decisions? J. Affect. Disord. 75, 11–18. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(02)00022-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Further reading

Alan, H., Eskin Bacaksiz, F., Tiryaki Sen, H., Taskiran Eskici, G., Gumus, E., and Harmanci Seren, A. K. (2021). " I'm a hero, but…": An evaluation of depression, anxiety, and stress levels of frontline healthcare professionals during COVID-19 pandemic in Turkey. Perspect. Psychiatr. Care 57:12666. doi: 10.1111/ppc.12666

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

An, L. C., Ma, Z. Y., and Xiao, P. (2022). The influence of resilience and risk perception on college students' anxiety under major public health emergencies. Heilongjiang Res. Higher Educ. 40, 138–143.

Google Scholar

Barattucci, M., Chirico, A., Kuvačić, G., and De Giorgio, A. (2020). Rethinking the role of affect in risk judgment: what we have learned from COVID-19 during the first week of quarantine in Italy. Front. Psychol. 11:554561. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.554561

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, B., Sun, J., and Feng, Y. (2020). How have COVID-19 isolation policies affected young people’s mental health?–evidence from Chinese college students. Front. Psychol. 11:1529. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01529

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, (2021). Impact of public risk perception on emotional health in the context of COVID-19. Master's thesis. Chengdu: Southwest University of Finance and Economics.

Google Scholar

Commodari, E., and La Rosa, V. L. (2020). Adolescents in quarantine during COVID-19 pandemic in Italy: perceived health risk, beliefs, psychological experiences and expectations for the future. Front. Psychol. 11:559951. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.559951

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Feng, Y., Zong, M., Yang, Z., Gu, W., Dong, D., and Qiao, Z. (2020). When altruists cannot help: the influence of altruism on the mental health of university students during the COVID-19 pandemic. Glob. Health 16, 1–8. doi: 10.1186/s12992-020-00587-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fu, H., and Wang, B. (2022). The effect of risk perception on anxiety in emerging adulthood under the local outbreak of COVID-19: a conditional process analysis. Front. Psychol. 13:759510. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.759510

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gan, Y., and Fu, Q. (2022). Risk perception and coping response to COVID-19 mediated by positive and negative emotions: a study on Chinese college students. PLoS One 17:e0262161. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0262161

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gan, Y., Zhang, J., and Quan, Z. (2021). Public perception of risk and coping response to COVID-19 in China: the moderating role of negative emotion. J. Psychol. Afr. 31, 117–123. doi: 10.1080/14330237.2021.1903167

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Germani, A., Buratta, L., Delvecchio, E., Gizzi, G., and Mazzeschi, C. (2020). Anxiety severity, perceived risk of COVID-19 and individual functioning in emerging adults facing the pandemic. Front. Psychol. 11:567505. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.567505

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haliwa, I., Spalding, R., Smith, K., Chappell, A., and Strough, J. (2022). Risk and protective factors for college students’ psychological health during the COVID-19 pandemic. J. Am. Coll. Heal. 70, 2257–2261. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2020.1863413

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, W., Wang, Q., Chen, H. L., and Jiang, Y. H. (2020). The relationship between public risk perception and unintentional/intentional rumor spreading behavior in the COVID-19: the mediation role of negative emotions. Chin. J. Clin. Psych. 28, 734–738. doi: 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2020.04.017

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, Y., Shu, L., Zhang, H., Wang, C., Yu, C., and Cui, G. (2022). Perceived epidemic risk and depression symptoms during the COVID-19 pandemic: the mediating role of security and the moderation role of perceived discrimination. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:7054. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19127054

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hubbard, G., den Daas, C., Johnston, M., and Dixon, D. (2021). Sociodemographic and psychological risk factors for anxiety and depression: findings from the Covid-19 health and adherence research in Scotland on mental health (CHARIS-MH) cross-sectional survey. Int. J. Behav. Med. 28, 788–800. doi: 10.1007/s12529-021-09967-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaspal, R., and Breakwell, G. M. (2022). Socio-economic inequalities in social network, loneliness and mental health during the COVID-19 pandemic. Int. J. Soc. Psychiatr. 68, 155–165. doi: 10.1177/0020764020976694

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, H., and Ma, Y. Y. (2022). The relationship between public information concern and avoidance attitude during the epidemic of novel coronavirus pneumonia. Med. Soc. 35, 103–107. doi: 10.13723/j.yxysh.2022.03.021

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, Y. C., Wu, W. L., and Lee, C. K. (2021). How COVID-19 triggers our herding behavior? Risk perception, state anxiety, and trust. Front. Public Health 9:587439. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2021.587439

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, J. B., Yang, A., Dou, K., Wang, L. X., Zhang, M. C., and Lin, X. Q. (2020). Chinese public’s knowledge, perceived severity, and perceived controllability of COVID-19 and their associations with emotional and behavioural reactions, social participation, and precautionary behaviour: a national survey. BMC Public Health 20, 1–14. doi: 10.1186/s12889-020-09695-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Q., Luo, R., Zhang, X., Meng, G., Dai, B., and Liu, X. (2021). Intolerance of COVID-19-related uncertainty and negative emotions among Chinese adolescents: a moderated mediation model of risk perception, social exclusion and perceived efficacy. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:2864. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18062864

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, X., and Lyu, H. (2021). Epidemic risk perception, perceived stress, and mental health during COVID-19 pandemic: a moderated mediating model. Front. Psychol. 11:563741. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.563741

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, L., Xie, J., Li, K., and Ji, S. (2020). Exploring how media influence preventive behavior and excessive preventive intention during the COVID-19 pandemic in China. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:7990. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17217990

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, M., Zhang, H., and Huang, H. (2020). Media exposure to COVID-19 information, risk perception, social and geographical proximity, and self-rated anxiety in China. BMC Public Health 20, 1–8. doi: 10.1186/s12889-020-09761-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Luo, R., Li, Q., Meng, G., Zheng, Y., Hu, K., Zhang, X., et al. (2022). The association between intolerance of uncertainty and internet addiction during the second wave of the coronavirus disease 2019 pandemic: a multiple mediation model considering depression and risk perception. PsyCh J. 11, 383–391. doi: 10.1002/pchj.545

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Malesza, M., and Kaczmarek, M. C. (2021). Predictors of anxiety during the COVID-19 pandemic in Poland. Personal. Individ. Differ. 170:110419. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110419

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Olagoke, A. A., Olagoke, O. O., and Hughes, A. M. (2020). Exposure to coronavirus news on mainstream media: the role of risk perceptions and depression. Br. J. Health Psychol. 25, 865–874. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12427

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Padmanabhanunni, A., and Pretorius, T. (2022). Behaviour is the key in a pandemic: the direct and indirect effects of COVID-19-related variables on psychological wellbeing. Psychol. Rep. 125, 2456–2469. doi: 10.1177/00332941211025269

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Peleg, S., Nudelman, G., and Shiloh, S. (2022). COVID-19 state anxiety of older adults: effects of defensive information processes. Anxiety Stress Coping 35, 111–123. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2021.1956479

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pramukti, I., Strong, C., Sitthimongkol, Y., Setiawan, A., Pandin, M. G. R., Yen, C. F., et al. (2020). Anxiety and suicidal thoughts during the COVID-19 pandemic: cross-country comparative study among Indonesian, Taiwanese, and Thai university students. J. Med. Internet Res. 22:e24487. doi: 10.2196/24487

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Riesel, A., Härpfer, K., Kathmann, N., and Klawohn, J. (2021). In the face of potential harm: the predictive validity of neural correlates of performance monitoring for perceived risk, stress, and internalizing psychopathology during the COVID-19 pandemic. Biolo. Psychiatr. Global Open Sci. 1, 300–309. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsgos.2021.08.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez, D. L., Lopes, D., and Balzarini, R. N. (2022). Having a prevention regulatory focus longitudinally predicted distress and health-protective behaviours during the COVID-19 pandemic. Stress. Health 38, 767–775. doi: 10.1002/smi.3132

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosi, A., Van Vugt, F. T., Lecce, S., Ceccato, I., Vallarino, M., Rapisarda, F., et al. (2021). Risk perception in a real-world situation (COVID-19): how it changes from 18 to 87 years old. Front. Psychol. 12:646558. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.646558

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubaltelli, E., Tedaldi, E., Orabona, N., and Scrimin, S. (2020). Environmental and psychological variables influencing reactions to the COVID-19 outbreak. Br. J. Health Psychol. 25, 1020–1038. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12473

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Salazar, A., Palomo-Osuna, J., de Sola, H., Moral-Munoz, J. A., Dueñas, M., and Failde, I. (2021). Psychological impact of the lockdown due to the COVID-19 pandemic in university workers: factors related to stress, anxiety, and depression. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:4367. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18084367

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shi, K., Zhou, H. M., Jiao, S. M., Guo, H. D., and Dong, Y. (2022). Research on the mechanism of the impact of COVID-19 information on People's risk cognition and coping behavior. Manag. Rev. 34:217. doi: 10.14120/j.cnki.cn11-5057/f.2022.08.022

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shi, Z., Qin, Y., Liu, Y., Tian, Y., Li, X., Hu, W., et al. (2021). Anxiety and depression levels of the general population during the rapid progressing stage in the coronavirus disease 2019 outbreak: a cross-sectional online investigation in China. BMJ Open 11:e050084. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2021-050084

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sica, C., Caudek, C., Cerea, S., Colpizzi, I., Caruso, M., Giulini, P., et al. (2021). Health anxiety predicts the perceived dangerousness of covid-19 over and above intrusive illness-related thoughts, contamination symptoms, and state and trait negative affect. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:1933. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18041933

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tagini, S., Brugnera, A., Ferrucci, R., Mazzocco, K., Pievani, L., Priori, A., et al. (2021). Attachment, personality and locus of control: psychological determinants of risk perception and preventive behaviors for COVID-19. Front. Psychol. 12:634012. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.634012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Torrente, F., Yoris, A., Low, D., Lopez, P., Bekinschtein, P., Vázquez, G. H., et al. (2022). Psychological symptoms, mental fatigue and behavioural adherence after 72 continuous days of strict lockdown during the COVID-19 pandemic in Argentina. BJPsych Open 8:e10. doi: 10.1192/bjo.2021.1065

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Q., Chen, H. L., Hu, W., and Qi, Y. J. (2021). The relationship between risk perception and rumor spreading behavior during COVID-19 pneumonia: sequence mediating roles of anxiety and information gathering addiction. Chin. J. Clin. Psychol. 29, 794–798. doi: 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2021.04.026

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., He, Y., Sheng, Z., and Yao, X. (2022). When does safety climate help? A multilevel study of COVID-19 risky decision making and safety performance in the context of business reopening. J. Bus. Psychol. 37, 1313–1327. doi: 10.1007/s10869-022-09805-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y. X., and Li, X. B. (2020). Study on the relationship between time attitude, anxiety and risk perception of community residents during the COVID-19. Commun. Psychol. Res. 10, 72–86.

Google Scholar

Wu, L., Li, X., and Lyu, H. (2021). The relationship between the duration of attention to pandemic news and depression during the outbreak of coronavirus disease 2019: the roles of risk perception and future time perspective. Front. Psychol. 12:564284. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.564284

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Xin, T. T., Li, X. J., and Ding, W. Y. (2023). The relationship between adolescent risk perception and emotions during the COVID-19: a short-term longitudinal study. Curr. Psychol. 42, 20091–20100. doi: 10.1007/s12144-022-02860-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, S. H. (2022). The influence of People's risk cognition familiarity on positive coping style during COVID-19-- moderation effects of negative emotion and mediation of organizational defamation. J. WenZhou Univ. Nat. Sci. Edn. 43, 34–42. doi: 10.3875/j.issn.1674-3563.2022.01.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, C., and Yan, W. (2023). The relationship between information overload and state of anxiety in the period of regular epidemic prevention and control in China: a moderated multiple mediation model. Curr. Psychol. 42, 21842–21859. doi: 10.1007/s12144-022-03289-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, K. (2022). The impact of novel coronavirus cognition on depression in the elderly during the normalization period of epidemic prevention and control: the chain mediation effects of discrimination perception and risk perception. Chin. J. Health Psychol. 30, 1496–1501. doi: 10.13342/j.cnki.cjhp.2022.10.011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ye, Y., Zhao, Q. G., and Li, X. (2021). Research on the relationship between risk cognition, coping tendency and negative emotion of college students during the COVID-19. Chin. Primary Health Care 35, 74–77. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1001-568X.2021.02.0022

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yue, C., Liu, C., Wang, J., Zhang, M., Wu, H., Li, C., et al. (2021). Association between social support and anxiety among pregnant women in the third trimester during the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) epidemic in Qingdao, China: the mediating effect of risk perception. Int. J. Soc. Psychiatr. 67, 120–127. doi: 10.1177/0020764020941567

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, N. (2022). Risk perception, mental health distress, and flourishing during the COVID-19 pandemic in China: the role of positive and negative affect. Curr. Psychol. 42, 1–9. doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-02624-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, J., Ye, B., and Ma, T. (2021). Positive information of COVID-19 and anxiety: a moderated mediation model of risk perception and intolerance of uncertainty. Front. Psych. 12:715929. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.715929

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zou, R., and Zhou, X. (2020). A study on the influential factors of anxiety of ordinary people under the novel coronavirus pneumonia epidemic situation. Chin. J. Appl. Psychol. 26, 99–107.

Google Scholar

Zhu, Z. Y., Zhang, Y., Gan, H., Tao, S. M., and Tao, F. B. (2022). A study on the relationship between depression and anxiety symptoms and risk perception of COVID-19 in college students. Chin. J. Health Educ. 38, 145–149. doi: 10.16168/j.cnki.issn.1002-9982.2022.02.010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gu, J., He, R., Wu, X., Tao, J., Ye, W., and Wu, C. (2022). Analyzing risk communication, trust, risk perception, negative emotions, and behavioral coping strategies during the COVID-19 pandemic in China using a structural equation model. Front. Public Health 10:843787. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2022.843787

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, M. F., Mahendran, R., and Yu, J. (2021). Associations between fear of COVID-19, affective symptoms and risk perception among community-dwelling older adults during a COVID-19 lockdown. Front. Psychol. 12:638831. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.638831

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: COVID-19, risk perception, negative emotions, tight and loose culture, meta-analysis

Citation: Zhou R, Shi K, Song X, Li S and Zhou W (2024) The relationship between risk perceptions and negative emotions in the COVID-19: a meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 15:1453111. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1453111

Received: 22 June 2024; Accepted: 13 August 2024;
Published: 26 August 2024.

Edited by:

Edgar Galindo, University of Evora, Portugal

Reviewed by:

Yeji Yang, Korea University, Republic of Korea
Stefania Mancone, University of Cassino, Italy

Copyright © 2024 Zhou, Shi, Song, Li and Zhou. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ruihua Zhou, m13816172091@126.com; Kan Shi, shik@psych.ac.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.