![Man ultramarathon runner in the mountains he trains at sunset](https://d2csxpduxe849s.cloudfront.net/media/E32629C6-9347-4F84-81FEAEF7BFA342B3/0B4B1380-42EB-4FD5-9D7E2DBC603E79F8/webimage-C4875379-1478-416F-B03DF68FE3D8DBB5.png)
94% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.
Find out more
ORIGINAL RESEARCH article
Front. Psychol. , 12 February 2025
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology
Volume 15 - 2024 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1441927
Introduction: Research to date has focused largely on the consequences of delayed parenting. However, little is known about the reasons and relevant mechanisms that are involved in people’s decisions to delay parenthood. The aim of our study was to verify how anxiety about the future relates to the motives for deferred parenthood and how this relationship is mediated by generational time perspective and life satisfaction.
Methods: A total of 203 Polish adults between 18 and 40 years of age participated in the study. All of them were of reproductive age but did not yet have children. Respondents completed the following questionnaires: The Dark Future Scale, The Multidimensional Scale of Motives for Postponing Parenthood, The Generational Time Perspective Questionnaire, and The Riverside Life Satisfaction Scale.
Results: The results obtained in the study show that future anxiety correlates significantly and positively with all six motives for postponing parenthood, postponed parenthood overall, and generational affectivity. It is also associated with the generational cognitive perspective at the level of tendency, and negatively with life satisfaction. According to our findings, in all seven models, at least one of the factors mediated the relationship between future anxiety and motives for postponing parenthood/its total score.
Conclusion: The current study advances the understanding of how the subjective future time perspective is related to delayed parenthood through generational concern and reduced life satisfaction. Our findings may indicate that despite the importance of sociodemographic variables in postponing parenthood (e.g., lack of housing, financial instability, acquiring knowledge, education, etc.), variables related to personality and time perspective play a very important role in postponing the decision to have a child.
Postponed parenthood consists of delaying the first childbearing toward later ages, into the mid or even late 30s (Sobotka, 2010; Waldenström, 2016). This growing phenomenon (Schlesinger and Schlesinger, 1989) from the early 1960s (Wilkie, 1981) is one of the most striking trends in contemporary reproductiveness (Skirbekk, 2022). According to different analyses, delaying parenthood characterizes high-income Western and East Asian countries (Sobotka, 2010; Thompson and Lee, 2011; Zacchini et al., 2022), middle-income developing societies of Latin America (Díaz, 2021; Díaz and Abufhele, 2023), and various groups of immigrants (Dupray and Pailhé, 2017).
Qualitative and quantitative findings indicate that many young people wait longer to have children because of multifaceted motives (Thompson and Lee, 2011; Zabak et al., 2023). Among numerous and nuanced factors accounting for delayed parenthood, scientists mention precarious social-economic conditions (e.g., lacking a partner, financial instability) (Martin, 2020), the need for maturity, personal growth, and autonomy (Boivin et al., 2009; Tydén et al., 2006), educational achievements, aspirations of self-realization (Díaz and Abufhele, 2023; Zabak et al., 2023), career advancement and women’s participation in the workplace (Zabak et al., 2023), technological advancements in assisted reproduction (e.g., egg freezing, donor insemination, in vitro fertilization) (Bellieni, 2012; NeJaime, 2017), a lack of fertility knowledge (Adachi et al., 2020; Okine et al., 2023), lifestyle goals (Datta et al., 2023), incompatibility of social roles (Datta et al., 2023; Nisén et al., 2022), and natural and anthropogenic causes of climate change (Schneider-Mayerson and Leog, 2020).
Besides the social and material preconditions of late parenthood, researchers also suggest the importance of individual and psychological determinants (Benzies et al., 2006; Bodin et al., 2021; Dion, 1995; Thompson and Lee, 2011). A factor that intuitively seems particularly salient in the context of parenting, although rarely discussed, is fear of the future, which can lead to indecision about starting a family. When people tend to anticipate negative outcomes and think about the future with worry (Carelli et al., 2011), they may consider undertaking more avoidant decision-making styles (Stolarski et al., 2018) and postpone being a parent. There are studies, especially those carried out among people with specific disabilities, that clearly document the link between anxiety and postponed parenthood. Such people often face fear for their own and their children’s future. For example, research conducted among visually impaired women revealed that they try to deal with their shortcomings to reduce their level of child-related anxiety (Commodari et al., 2022). In addition, potential parents with disabilities experience higher levels of anxiety while thinking of not being able to fulfill their parental responsibilities (Panuccio et al., 2020).
Less is known about relevant mechanisms that are involved in people’s decisions to delay parenthood. It seems that the generational time perspective, expressing interest in the life of future generations (Timoszyk-Tomczak and Próchniak, 2024), and life satisfaction, which is associated with a global cognitive assessment of one’s past life, might be such mechanisms. Therefore, the aim of this study was to explore the mediating role of generational time perspective and life satisfaction on the relationship between fear of the future and motives for postponing parenthood.
Future time perspective, expressed through aspirations, expectations, fears, projects, and hopes, is considered an important psychological variable that facilitates imagining possible scenarios (Jannini et al., 2022), influencing people’s behavior (Coudin and Lima, 2011) and the choices they make (Tucholska et al., 2022). A review of the literature shows that reproductive decision-making, while it brings contentment and joy (Clarke et al., 2020), is often related to feelings of personal uncertainty (Reuter, 2019), conflicting emotions and attitudes about the transition to parenthood, and fear of taking an action that might be regretted (Lazarus et al., 2022).
Future anxiety is one of the basic elements of a negative future time perspective and refers to concern about unfavorable changes that may occur in the future (Zaleski, 1996). It has been found that future anxiety correlates positively with moving to independent adulthood (Koprowicz and Gumowska, 2022) and fear of childbirth (Zdybek and Haimann, 2017). This may be because anxious people make predictions of possible outcomes in a more negative than positive way (Sebaie et al., 2024).
Future anxiety, typically operationalized as a state of insecurity (Hwayan, 2020), a sense of uncertainty (Zaleski, 1996), and the fear of the unknown (Regnoli et al., 2024), reflects possible or anticipated adverse or dangerous events (Zaleski et al., 2019). People with anxious attitudes toward what is to come experience higher fear of forthcoming and distant threats, and lower levels of hope (Jannini et al., 2022). Likewise, the shift of childbearing to a later age is often accompanied by insecurities and uncertainties in life (Bodin et al., 2021). A lack of firm economic stability (Zabak et al., 2023), feeling of unpreparedness for parenthood (Avignon et al., 2023), and fear of childbirth (Avignon et al., 2023) contribute to postponing parenthood. It has been found that couples postponing their decision to have their first child are more concerned about the possible negative effects of parenthood on their freedom, career, and lifestyle than non-delayers (Dion, 1995). Climate change concerns are also reported by younger generations in the context of their reproductive plans and decisions (Schneider-Mayerson and Leog, 2020).
There is some empirical evidence that future anxiety and motives for deferred parenthood share common features. For example, Szcześniak et al. (2024) have noticed a positive association between anxiety and six motives for postponing the decision of parenthood (uncertainty about one’s skills in the context of parenthood, self-focus, perception of parenthood as a burden, fear of changes that the presence of the child may cause, financial concern, worries about the child’s future) and the overall score of postponing parenthood. This pattern of results was found both in Polish and American samples (Szcześniak et al., under review). Building upon the literature and previous empirical studies, we assumed that:
H1: Future anxiety is positively related to motives for postponing the start of childbearing and overall delayed parenthood.
The generational time perspective is understood as a cognitive-affective representation of the future relating to the life of a generation of people in the future, which the current generation of people will not live to (see Timoszyk-Tomczak and Próchniak, 2024). Previous research shows that fear of the future correlates with generational anxiety. Both constructs predict a negative future, but fear of the future is primarily cognitive in nature. It is associated with thoughts, fantasies, and images of what may happen, and only based on these images does anxiety appear (Zaleski, 1996). However, generational anxiety includes various difficult emotions regarding the future of subsequent generations in addition to anxiety, as well as anger and shame in connection with current events (Timoszyk-Tomczak and Próchniak, 2024).
Fear of the future is related to the personal future and to what extent personal plans can be implemented in a changing and hostile world (Zaleski, 1996). In contrast, generational anxiety goes beyond the personal future and reaches the future of the next generations. It may involve thinking in terms of the possibility of children and grandchildren, and people who will appear later, realizing their own plans. The future is the part of time that allows people to set long- and short-term goals and predict changes in their own capabilities, as well as their personal and broader social situations. It allows for locating goals in the near and distant future, which has motivational and behavioral consequences in the present (Lens et al., 2012).
The future can arouse hope and be a space for development (Zimbardo and Boyd, 2008), but it can also raise fears. Both aspects are not mutually exclusive (Zaleski, 1996). Recent years full of social and ecological disasters and experiences related to the pandemic may intensify these experiences (Pihkala, 2020). From a broader theoretical and empirical perspective, the predicted relationship between anxiety about future and generational anxiety can be based on the concept of micro and macro worries (Boehnke et al., 1998). The concept describes the structure of worries, indicating two aspects: the object of worry (the self or the in-group as examples of micro worries; the wider society and world as examples of macro worries) and the domain of worry, which relates to the area of life it concerns (e.g., environment, social relationships, achievement, economics). The authors of the concept emphasize that this structure refers to the cognitive dimension of worries (that includes an object and a domain of life) and to the affective dimension (which embraces differences in expectations related to the object and the domain of worries). In relation to the variables of interest to us, anxiety about the future refers to oneself as an object without reference to the area of life (Zaleski et al., 2019), while generational anxiety from the perspective of the self concerns specific areas of life, environmental changes, and social relations. Both constructs examine the same area, i.e., worries, of which anxiety is an important component. Given the existing evidence showing a link between future anxiety and generational affectivity, we hypothesized that:
H2: Future anxiety is positively related to generational time perspective in its two dimensions (affective and cognitive).
Life satisfaction is most commonly referred to as an individual evaluation of the quality of life based on one’s own perspective (Diener et al., 2013) and personal criteria (Chen et al., 2020). It is a stable indicator of subjective well-being (Shek and Li, 2016) and an important buffer against the negative effects of numerous stressors (Proctor et al., 2009). Theories of subjective well-being suggest that people derive life satisfaction from the objective conditions of their lives (e.g., contextual circumstances) and a variety of personality characteristics (Baird, 2010; Steckermeier, 2021). These theories may be complemented by the dual-pathway framework by Cunningham et al. (2015), according to which, time perspective (in our case, future anxiety) may directly affect life satisfaction.
There is an empirical consensus that anxiety, which is future-oriented (Eysenck et al., 2006), and life satisfaction are moderately negatively related (Headey et al., 1993). A growing body of research indicates that there is a consistent association between future anxiety and different aspects of well-being. For example, temporal negative affect (Chen et al., 2020; Mooney et al., 2017) is negatively correlated with life satisfaction. This means that people who view the future in an aversive way and have negative expectations (Rönnlund et al., 2021) tend to report lower levels of life satisfaction. Paolini et al. (2006) observe that unpleasant thoughts about the future significantly impede life satisfaction. Future-negative and future-confusional perspectives generate negative emotions and reduce psychological resources (Hao et al., 2024), leading to lower well-being.
Although the transition to parenthood is a normative change in people’s lives, it constitutes a source of stress and crises for couples, as well (Cowan and Cowan, 1995). Szcześniak et al. (under review) confirm that trait anxiety correlates moderately and negatively with resilience that, in turn, empowers childless individuals to grow in the face of adversity. Since resilience is considered a process of adaptation (Tamarit et al., 2023) and is positively associated with different domains of satisfaction, based on theoretical premises and previous findings, we considered that:
H3: Future anxiety is negatively associated with life satisfaction.
In addition to the direct relationship, there is likely an indirect effect of future anxiety on delaying parenthood through generational time perspective and life satisfaction. Although there are no studies that clearly refer to the mediation process of both phenomena, except one study by Szcześniak et al. (2024), where the relationship between anxiety and postponed parenthood was mediated by maturity, there is some evidence that time perspective and life satisfaction may play such a role.
Starting with the generational time perspective, the basis for choosing this type of time as a mediator is that even if people generally do not focus much of their attention on the human beings who will come after them, they think about the future of their potential children and/or grandchildren, especially in the context of worrying about the world. Nowadays, there are more and more reasons for concern about climate change, and the concept of environmental anxiety or eco-anxiety appears in the literature. Research shows that anxiety about climate change can cause consequences at the psychological or spiritual level and lead to existential questions about existence and its meaning (Pihkala, 2020).
The rationale for selecting life satisfaction as a mediator is that this variable has been reported to have a mediating effect in the context of adverse life experiences. For example, Baruffol et al. (1995) noticed that life satisfaction explains why some people adapt to challenging or adverse situations without more frustration, whereas others do not. In another study (Luo et al., 2024), life satisfaction played a mediating role in the relationship between emerging adulthood characteristics and anxiety.
Taken together, the generational time perspective may coexist with lower subjective life satisfaction and, thus, increase doubts related to starting a family or planning children. This is because the anticipated changes do not provide a sense of stability, and the lack of stability in the context of various life challenges results in reduced subjective well-being (Szymańska, 2021). This, in turn, may lead to postponing the decision to have the first child. This is consistent with the perspective of Bradley and Corwyn (2004, p. 385), who imply that “life satisfaction reflects both the extent to which basic needs are met and the extent to which a variety of other goals are viewed as attainable.”
Moreover, a review of the literature on eco-anxiety shows that its severity may be associated with impaired functioning, symptoms of depression, anxiety, post-traumatic stress disorder, stress and insomnia, lower self-esteem of mental health, as well as reluctance to have children (Boluda-Verdú et al., 2022). All the above-mentioned constructs reflect reduced well-being, which is associated with lower levels of life satisfaction. Recent research also shows a sense of hopelessness and fear for the future of humanity among children and adolescents around the world (Hickman et al., 2021; Marks et al., 2021). Previous findings on the relationship between worries and mental health demonstrate that micro worries are strongly associated with poor mental health, whereas macro worries are unrelated to mental health or have little relationship with positive well-being (Boehnke et al., 1998). This suggests that the vision and expectations for the future influence responses about the impact of the climate on their current, hypothetical, and expected children. Planning to have children is also associated with increased anxiety about the future, although this relationship is complex and requires additional research (Schneider-Mayerson and Leog, 2020). Considering the previous studies, we proposed the following hypothesis:
H4: Generational time perspective and life satisfaction act as mediators in the relationship between future anxiety and dimensions of postponed parenthood/its overall score.
The hypothesized parallel model is presented in Figure 1.
Figure 1. Hypothesized parallel model of the role of generational affectivity, cognitive perspective, and life satisfaction in the relationship between future time perspective and postponed parenthood.
A total of 203 Polish adults participated in the study, 74.9% of whom were women (25.1% men). The age range of the sample was 18–40 years (M = 22.75; SD = 4.18). With respect to place of residence, 37.4% of the respondents were from cities between 150,000 and 500,000 inhabitants; 18.7% were from cities over 500,000 or from villages; 13.4% were from cities between 50,000 and 150,000 inhabitants; and 11.8% were from towns up to 50,000 inhabitants. Three-quarters of the participants (75.4%) stated that they chose to have a child before the age of 30. All subjects gave their written informed consent for taking part in the study.
The study was aimed at people who did not yet have children, but were of reproductive age, and was conducted via an online survey. The objectives of the study were communicated to the respondents along with instructions on its duration, anonymity, confidentiality, and the possibility of withdrawing at any time without any consequences. The study had project approval granted by the Research Ethics Committee of the Institute of Psychology at the University of Szczecin (No. 24/2023, 9th November 2023), and it was conducted following the guidelines provided in the Declaration of Helsinki.
The Dark Future Scale (DFS) by Zaleski et al. (2019) is a short version of the Future Anxiety Scale. The DFS measures future anxiety, defined as a fear of anticipated adverse changes in the future (e.g., I am afraid that the problems troubling me now will continue for a long time). Participants evaluate their attitudes toward the future on a seven-point Likert scale, with 0 being decidedly false and 6 being decidedly true. In the current study, the scale presented very good internal reliability with Cronbach’s α = 0.897.
The Multidimensional Scale of Motives for Postponing Parenthood (MSMPP-18) by Szcześniak et al. (under review) is a measure to assess six motives to postpone the decision to have the first child: (1) feeling of uncertainty and incompetence involves a belief in one’s own unpreparedness to act as a parent (e.g., I am afraid I will not be able to cope with parental responsibilities); (2) self-focus refers to self-fulfillment (e.g., I am currently focusing on self-development); (3) parenthood as a burden implies fear of the sacrifices associated with having and caring for a child (e.g., Parenting is taxing); (4) fear of change reflects a fear of the unknown and potential negative consequences co-occurring with the birth of a child (e.g., A woman’s body changes unfavorably after pregnancy); (5) financial security concern is related to the perception of precarity and apprehension of not having sufficient resources to raise a child (e.g., The cost of raising a child is beyond my financial means); (6) worry about a child’s future stems from the fear that the child may suffer from climate change or wars (e.g., I do not want my child to live in unstable times). The MSMPP-18 can also be considered as one factor of deferred parenthood. The scale consists of 18 items, and each item is rated on a seven-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree). The values of internal consistency for all six dimensions and the total score were very good: (1) feeling of uncertainty and incompetence (α = 0.914); (2) self-focus (α = 0.929); (3) parenthood as a burden (α = 0.808); (4) fear of change (α = 0.786); (5) financial security concern (α = 0.914); (6) worry about a child’s future (α = 0.841); and the total score (α = 0.909).
The Generational Time Perspective Questionnaire (GTPQ) by Timoszyk-Tomczak and Próchniak (2024) is a tool used to examine generational time perspective in two aspects: affective and cognitive. The affective dimension, with 5 items, diagnoses negative emotions (e.g., fear, anger, shame) toward threats that may occur in tens or hundreds of years (e.g., I am worried about what Earth will look like in the future). The cognitive dimension, which consists of 6 items, includes the concentration on the future manifested in an interest in the lives of people in future generations (e.g., I wonder how people will live in tens/hundreds of years from now). Respondents decide how much they agree with the statements using a 5-point Likert scale where 1 = I strongly disagree and 5 = I strongly agree. The tool is characterized by good reliability. In the present study, the internal consistency of the generational cognitive perspective is Cronbach’s α = 0.816, and the reliability of generational anxiety is α = 0.892.
The Riverside Satisfaction with Life Scale (RSLS), developed by Margolis et al. (2019) and adapted into Polish by Adamczyk et al. (2020), is a brief, single dominant factor that assesses an individual’s satisfaction with their own life embraced as a whole. The questionnaire contains both regularly scored (e.g., I like how my life is going) and reverse-scored (e.g., I want to change the path my life is on) items. Respondents rate their agreement with each of the statements using a 7-point scale (1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree). In the current study, the RSLS showed good internal consistency with Cronbach’s α = 0.832.
To determine the optimum sample size in advance, an a priori power analysis was performed using G*Power 3.1.9.4 with a bivariate normal model correlation (Kang, 2021). A small effect size of 0.21 with an alpha of 0.05 and a power of 0.90 was set. The analysis showed that the total sample size would demand at least 187 participants. The rationale for using this value was based on meta-analyses that reveal that the average typical effect sizes in social psychology are equivalent to Pearson’s r = 0.21 (Richard et al., 2003).
The Shapiro–Wilk test was applied to evaluate the normality of the data. Since it showed that in all cases, the collected data was significantly different from the normal distribution, Spearman’s Rho correlations were conducted to assess the associations between future anxiety, motives for postponing parenthood and its overall score, generational affectivity, generational cognitive perspective, and satisfaction with life. A multivariable linear regression analysis was used to: (1) determine the degree of multicollinearity in a dataset; (2) test for unusual observations (outliers); (3) control for potential survey-related confounders and to examine whether they were affecting the direct association between the independent (future anxiety) and dependent (motives for postponed parenthood and its total score) variables.
A regression analysis was conducted to analyze the degree of near-linear dependence between the explanatory variables (Daoud, 2017). The tolerance statistics and the variance inflation factor (VIF) were used to calculate the variance inflation. The values showing a sign of multicollinearity were lower than 0.1 for tolerance and larger than 5 for VIF. Mahalanobis distance (p < 0.001) and Cook’s distance (higher than 1) were used to identify influential cases.
Sex, age, place of residence, a declaration about becoming a parent before the age of 30, and an indication of the most appropriate age to be a parent were included as potential confounding variables. With respect to sex, age, and place of residence, it has been found that there are some disparities between men/women, younger/older adults, and smaller/bigger cities concerning future anxiety and aspects of family functioning. For example, women scored higher than men on future anxiety (Awad et al., 2024; Bujnowska et al., 2019; Jannini et al., 2022) and on the current and ideal number of children (Thompson and Lee, 2011). Moreover, the level of future anxiety was significantly higher among younger emerging adults (18–20 years old) than their older counterparts (21–25 years old) (Koprowicz and Gumowska, 2022). The timing of the first birth was also related to the place of residence (Obeng Gyimah, 2003). Although the appropriate age to become parents should be autonomously chosen by a couple (Bellieni, 2012), according to Polish data released by Statista in 2023, the best age to have the first child for both women and men was between 25 and 29. All five prospective confounders were introduced in Step 1. The generational time perspective in two aspects (affective and cognitive) and life satisfaction were included in Step 2.
The data was analyzed using IBM SPSS Statistics (version 20). A multiple mediation was tested by the PROCESS macro for SPSS (Model No 4) with three parallel mediators, the Bootstrapping 5,000 technique, and 95% confidence intervals (CIs). Future anxiety was the independent variable, and the motives (feeling of uncertainty and incompetence—UNC, self-focus—SF, parenthood as a burden—BUR, fear of change—CH, financial security concern—FIN, worry about a child’s future—WOR) for postponing parenthood and its total score (MPP) were the dependent variables. Generational affectivity (GA), generational cognitive perspective (GCP), and life satisfaction (SAT) were included as mediators. Thus, seven pathways were proposed: FA → GA/GCP/SAT → UNC; FA → GA/GCP/SAT → SF; FA → GA/GCP/SAT → BUR; FA → GA/GCP/SAT → CH; FA → GA/GCP/SAT → FIN; FA → GA/GCP/SAT → WOR; and FA → GA/GCP/SAT → MPP. Mediation was considered successful if the 95% CIs did not encompass zero. Moreover, since it is recommended that not only the statistical significance of indirect effects but also the effect size of a given effect should be reported (Preacher and Kelley, 2011), we used the software application SmartPLS4 to calculate the magnitude of effect size (v2) for all mediation paths in seven models. Following the indications advocated by Ogbeibu et al. (2021) about the squared adjustment v effect, we assumed the value of 0.175 for a large effect, 0.075 for medium, and 0.01 for small as more suitable for indirect effects. All v2 values lower than 0.01 were considered as an indication of no effect.
Descriptive statistics for the mean, standard deviation, values of skewness, kurtosis, the Shapiro–Wilk test of future anxiety, motives for postponing parenthood, generational anxiety, generational cognitive perspective, and satisfaction are presented in Table 1. The Shapiro–Wilk tests for normality showed that all factors presented a significant p value, except future anxiety.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the future anxiety, motives for postponing parenthood, generational anxiety, generational cognitive perspective, and satisfaction (N = 203).
The VIF values ranged below 2 (between 1.047 and 1.644) and the lowest tolerance was 0.608, suggesting no presence of multicollinearity in the data. The Mahalanobis distance for multivariate outlier detection revealed a chi-squared value of less than 0.001 in only two of the 203 cases. Likewise, Cook’s distance values (between 0.000 and 0.279) indicated that the outliers were not problematic.
The linear regression model showed that five categories included in Step 1 explained 24.7% of the variance (R2 = 0.247): sex (β = −0.162, t = −2.744, p = 0.007), age (β = −0.138, t = −2.420, p = 0.016), place of residence (β = −0.009, t = −0.159, p = 0.874), declaration about having a child before the age of 30 (β = 0.320, t = 5.643, p = 0.001), and indication of the most appropriate age to decide to have the first child (β = 0.088, t = 1.579, p = 0.116). Three of the above-mentioned variables had a significant effect, suggesting that motives for postponed parenthood may decrease among men and older adults, and increase among those people who consider having a child before the age of 30. Future perspective (β = 0.305, t = 4.404, p = 0.001), generational affectivity (β = 0.207, t = 3.153, p = 0.002), generational cognitive perspective (β = 0.081, t = 1.346, p = 0.180), and satisfaction with life (β = −0.041, t = −0.641, p = 0.523) predicted a significant amount of the variance (43.8%; F(202,9) = 16.698, p = 0.001), even after controlling for the effects of confounders.
Table 2 illustrates the bootstrap correlation coefficient 95% confidence intervals between variables considered in the study: future anxiety, motives for postponed parenthood and its total score, generational affectivity, generational cognitive perspective, and satisfaction with life. The results are mostly consistent with the hypotheses. Future anxiety correlates significantly and positively with five motives for postponing parenthood (except self-focus) and postponed parenthood overall (H1). It also correlates significantly and positively with generational affectivity and with the generational cognitive perspective at the level of tendency (H2). Future anxiety is associated negatively with life satisfaction (H3).
Table 3 presents the results of the PROCESS macro for SPSS of the relationship between future anxiety and the motives for postponing parenthood. Bootstrap estimates for the indirect effect, based on 5,000 bootstrap samples, showed that zero was outside of the lower and upper bounds of the CIs in all seven models. More precisely, the study found that two factors (generational affectivity and life satisfaction) mediated the relationship between the predictor (future anxiety) and the outcome variables (motives for postponing parenthood/its total score) in five models: FA → GA/SAT → UNC; FA → GA/SAT → BUR; FA → GA/SAT → CH; FA → GA/SAT → FIN; FA → GA/SAT → WOR. Moreover, life satisfaction alone was a mediator in the relationship between future anxiety and self-focus—FA → SAT → SF. Generational affectivity alone was a mediator in the relationship between future anxiety and the total score of postponed parenthood—FA → GA → MPP. The generational cognitive perspective was not a mediator in any model.
Table 3. Role of the generational affectivity, generational cognitive perspective, and life satisfaction in the relationship between future time perspective and dimensions of postponed parenthood/overall score (N = 203).
Although mediation was supported by the CIs in a significant proportion of the models, the lower bounds of the reported intervals were close to zero in six of eleven cases, thus suggesting that the effect sizes in these models could be weak. In fact, as presented in Table 3, only one of eleven significant mediations (as indicated by lower and upper CIs) showed a small effect size of 0.015 (FA → GA → WOR), slightly overpassing the value of 0.01 indicated by Ogbeibu et al. (2021) as a threshold for a small effect. The remaining significant models displayed negligible effects, providing information that the magnitude of the effect sizes was low. It is important to highlight that all values of v2 were insignificant (except one with a small effect mentioned before) despite values p and CIs being significant. From a theoretical perspective, these findings can be potentially very meaningful (Agler and De Boeck, 2017) and indicate that a tendency to anxiously perceive the future may lead to higher generational affectivity and lower life satisfaction, which in turn may result in various motives for postponing parenthood (H4). At the same time, from a practical perspective, the resulting effect sizes should be interpreted with caution as they are very small.
The study aimed to analyze the relationship between future anxiety and motives for postponing parenthood, considering the mediating role of generational time perspective and/or life satisfaction. We hypothesized that future anxiety is positively related to motives for postponing parenting decisions (H1), future anxiety is positively related to generational time perspective in its two dimensions (affective and cognitive) (H2), future anxiety correlates negatively with life satisfaction (H3), and that generational time perspective and life satisfaction mediate the direct relationship between future anxiety and motives for postponing parenthood (H4).
With respect to hypothesis H1, which was largely confirmed (except self-focus), our findings suggest that personal preoccupation with unfavorable changes in the future coexists with feelings of one’s own unpreparedness to act as a parent, fear of the sacrifices associated with being a parent, fear of change, perception of financial precarity, and worry about the child’s future. Because of the lack of empirical evidence on future anxiety and motives for postponing parenthood, we discuss our findings in the light of research on related concepts and phenomena. A range of studies supports that future negative and future confusion correlate negatively with confidence in one’s own worth (Zhi et al., 2021). Other studies show that the intense experience of anxiety contributes to a threat to one’s self-image and is directly related to uncertainty about one’s self (Joiner et al., 1999; Sowislo and Orth, 2013). The relationship between future anxiety and self-focus can be justified by the positive correlation between negative affect and focus on numerous aspects of the self. The positive correlation of future anxiety with burden and fear of change can be confirmed by the literature on the nature of anxiety related to parenthood. Given that being a parent is considered the most difficult job (Nomaguchi and Milkie, 2020; Panayiotou and Vrana, 2004) anxiety about parenting may lead to its postponement. In fact, previous analyses show that a reluctance to sacrifice and the perception of a significant burden associated with motherhood are two of the main motives of voluntarily childless women (Wilak, 2023). Some young people have a “belief that children detract from the marital relationship by interfering in the leisure time and intimacy of couples” (Hird and Abshoff, 2000, p. 352). If future anxiety means a fear of unfavorable changes, such a concern may result in the expectation that something bad may occur (Zaleski, 1996), and consequently, generate the perception of parenthood as a burden and unwanted change. When it comes to the relationship between anxiety and economic concern, this knowledge is well established, especially in the context of COVID-19 research. It has been proved by different scientists that higher levels of anxiety are significantly associated with financial concern (Haliwa et al., 2021; Oh et al., 2021). Recent findings provide evidence that individuals with high neuroticism scores tend to report higher financial distress (Fachrudin and Latifah, 2022), and emotional instability is one of its strongest predictors (Xu et al., 2015). Finally, some studies confirm the link between anxiety and worry about a child’s future. According to various researchers (Galway and Field, 2023), young people who have negative thoughts and experience negative emotions in connection with climate change (e.g., fear, sadness, anxiety, helplessness, etc.) express doubts about having children. This may be due to the relationship found in previous studies, which indicated that perceived threat correlates with higher levels of worry (Berenbaum et al., 2007).
Regarding hypothesis H2, which was fully confirmed, future anxiety significantly and positively correlates with affective generational time perspective and with the cognitive dimension. This means that young people with increased anxiety about the future anticipate negative consequences regarding their living conditions and the possibility of implementing their plans, not only for their own, but also for future generations. The results obtained in our study are consistent with previous studies, in which the affective dimension of generational time perspective correlated more strongly with future anxiety than the cognitive dimension (Timoszyk-Tomczak and Próchniak, 2024). The positive relationship between these variables is not surprising because all three relate to time perspective, involving anxiety about future events. However, both the theoretical foundations and the low strength of the correlation between them indicate that they are not identical constructs. While future anxiety refers to thoughts about the distant future concerning the person who imagines it (Zaleski, 1996), generational time perspective refers to potentially difficult events that may impact future generations (Timoszyk-Tomczak and Próchniak, 2024).
The third hypothesis, regarding a negative relationship between future anxiety and life satisfaction (H3), was fully confirmed and is, therefore, consistent with the results obtained in previous studies. Fear of the future indicates a predominance of negative attitudes toward the future over positive ones and refers to the anticipation of dangerous and unfavorable changes (Zaleski, 1996). Negative attitudes toward the future are associated with lower well-being and lower life satisfaction, which is confirmed by meta-analyses (Kooij et al., 2018).
The fourth hypothesis, about the mediating role of generational time perspective and life satisfaction in the relationship between future anxiety and postponed parenthood (H4), was largely confirmed. The findings obtained in the current research show that future anxiety is associated with postponing parenthood through fear of how future generations will function and life satisfaction. Although there are no analyses relating to possible conditions for postponing parenthood related to civilizational changes, future time perspective, and anxiety about upcoming changes, research in eco-anxiety indicates that its severity may be associated, among other things, with a deterioration of mental health and reluctance to have children (Boluda-Verdú et al., 2022). If the fear for future generations increases and is related to expected problematic living conditions or difficulties in achieving goals, it may limit the creation of plans for the future and decrease life satisfaction, especially if these plans are related to having children. The obtained results indicate that people who are anxious about the future, with higher generational anxiety and lower levels of life satisfaction, tend to postpone parenthood. It is well documented that the skills and opportunities to plan for future desired outcomes are critical to well-being, motivation, and behavior (Kooij et al., 2018). Future time perspective is associated with affective traits and personality predispositions. People who are open, extroverted, conscientious, full of hope, and positive have a longer future perspective and, therefore, are more motivated to implement different activities (Kooij et al., 2018).
It needs to be noted that our mediational results, although demonstrating the existence of mediation supported by CIs around the indirect effect, should be treated with caution due to the small magnitude of effect sizes shown in the study. It can be assumed that the mediators we chose as variables that could explain the relationship between future anxiety and postponed parenthood, although important and theoretically justified, are not necessarily essential ones. In fact, as Funder and Ozer (2019) observe, human psychology is naturally complex, and not all constructs have a large effect on mental actions, emotions, or behaviors. Moreover, it is empirically confirmed (Walters, 2019) that effect sizes in the context of mediation analysis are almost always small. Since there are no studies on a similar topic in which authors reported effect sizes for the indirect effects, it is difficult to draw far-reaching conclusions.
Undoubtedly, our study adds new knowledge about the direct relationship between future anxiety and motives for postponing parenthood, and indicates the mediating role of generational affectivity and life satisfaction in this relationship. However, it also presents some limitations. Due to the cross-sectional nature of the study, we cannot talk about the causal type of direct relationships and indirect effects through the demonstrated mediations. Although the rationale for the direction of these relationships was based on previous, particularly predictive, research, longitudinal studies are needed to confirm this trajectory. The findings can also be affected by the confounding variables that we included in the study. Motives for postponed parenthood may be influenced not only by future anxiety, generational affectivity, and life satisfaction, but also by sex, age, and the consideration of having a child before or after the age of 30. The results regarding the magnitude of effect size seem to support this perspective.
While research shows that anxiety is a predictor of indecision or even inhibits decision-making (Jia et al., 2020), we know less about the phenomena that may play an important role in the relationship between future anxiety and motives for postponing parenthood (Szcześniak et al., 2024). For this reason, the current study advances the understanding of how the subjective future time perspective is related to delayed parenthood through generational concern and reduced life satisfaction. It also has important implications for the developmental aspects of emerging adulthood, which is considered the most unstable period of the life span (Arnett et al., 2014). Our findings may indicate that despite the importance of sociodemographic variables in postponing parenthood (e.g., lack of housing, financial instability, acquiring knowledge, education, etc.), variables related to personality and time perspective may also play a very important role in postponing the decision to have a child.
Moreover, the future can be a source of worries about very personal and global situations (Zaleski, 1996). Further research requires clarification of what type of anxiety about the future is more important for postponing parenthood. Further analyses could be aimed at understanding the structure of anxiety about the future, and what type of anticipated threats are the most inhibiting in making decisions about having children. Are these concerns about one’s own skills related to the role of a parent, social and living restrictions, fears of losing previous opportunities and a sense of freedom, or potential threats to future generations and the world? Research could also include identifying factors that minimize anxiety about the future. This may be important for individual development and the realization of life goals characteristic of emerging adulthood, and it could also help regulate fertility, which decreases precisely where conditions and opportunities for children are greater, i.e., in high-income countries and developing societies.
Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found at: https://osf.io/gpb63/?view_only=d1623ffca4264589bb80cd8b0e0db200.
The studies involving humans were approved by Ethics Committee of the Institute of Psychology at the University of Szczecin (No. 24/2023, 9th November 2023). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
MS: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Supervision, Resources, Methodology, Investigation, Formal analysis, Data curation, Conceptualization. CT-T: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Supervision, Resources, Methodology, Investigation, Formal analysis, Data curation, Conceptualization. JŁ: Writing – original draft, Resources, Methodology, Investigation, Data curation. MG: Writing – original draft, Resources, Methodology, Investigation, Data curation.
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Adachi, T., Endo, M., and Ohashi, K. (2020). Regret over the delay in childbearing decision negatively associates with life satisfaction among Japanese women and men seeking fertility treatment: A cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health. 20:886. doi: 10.1186/s12889-020-09025-5
Adamczyk, K., Trepanowski, R., Celejewska, A., and Ganclerz, M. (2020). Development of the polish-language Riverside life satisfaction scale and its further validation. J. Pers. Assess. 102, 817–832. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2019.1674317
Agler, R., and De Boeck, P. (2017). On the interpretation and use of mediation: multiple perspectives on mediation analysis. Front. Psychol. 8:1984. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01984
Arnett, J. J., Žukauskienė, R., and Sugimura, K. (2014). The new life stage of emerging adulthood at ages 18-29 years: implications for mental health. Lancet Psychiatry 1, 569–576. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(14)00080-7
Avignon, V., Gaucher, L., Baud, D., Legardeur, H., Dupont, C., and Horsch, A. (2023). What do mothers think about their antenatal classes? A mixed-method study in Switzerland. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 23:741. doi: 10.1186/s12884-023-06049-8
Awad, E., Malaeb, D., Fekih-Romdhane, F., Hallit, S., and Obeid, S. (2024). The moderating effect of psychological distress in the association between temperaments and dark future among young adults. BMC Psychiatry 24:18. doi: 10.1186/s12888-023-05486-1
Baird, B. (2010). Life satisfaction across life the lifespan: findings from two nationally representative panel studies. Soc. Indic. Res. 99, 183–203. doi: 10.1007/s11205-010-9584-9
Baruffol, E., Gisle, L., and Corten, P. (1995). Life satisfaction as a mediator between distressing events and neurotic impairement in a general population. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 92, 56–62. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1995.tb09543.x
Bellieni, C. V. (2012). Neonatal risks from in vitro fertilization and delayed motherhood. World J. Clin. Pediatr. 1, 34–36. doi: 10.5409/wjcp.v1.i4.34
Benzies, K., Tough, S., Tofflemire, K., Frick, C., Faber, A., and Newburn-Cook, C. (2006). Factors influencing women’s decisions about timing of motherhood. J. Obstet. Gynecol. Neonatal. Nurs. 35, 625–633. doi: 10.1111/j.1552-6909.2006.00079.x
Berenbaum, H., Thompson, R. J., and Bredemeier, K. (2007). Perceived threat: exploring its association with worry and its hypothesized antecedents. Behav. Res. Ther. 45, 2473–2482. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2007.03.015
Bodin, M., Holmström, C., Plantin, L., Schmidt, L., Ziebe, S., and Elmerstig, E. (2021). Preconditions to parenthood: changes over time and generations. Reprod. Biomed. Soc. Online 13, 14–23. doi: 10.1016/j.rbms.2021.03.003
Boehnke, K., Schwartz, S., Stromberg, C., and Sagiv, L. (1998). The structure and dynamics of worry: theory, measurement, and cross-national replications. J. Pers. 66, 745–782. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.00031
Boivin, J., Rice, F., Hay, D., Harold, G., Lewis, A., van den Bree, M. M. B., et al. (2009). Associations between maternal older age, family environment and parent and child wellbeing in families using assisted reproductive techniques to conceive. Soc. Sci. Med. 68, 1948–1955. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.02.036
Boluda-Verdú, I., Senent-Valero, M., Casas-Escolano, M., Matijasevich, A., and Pastor-Valero, M. (2022). Fear for the future: eco-anxiety and health implications, a systematic review. J. Environ. Psychol. 84, 101904–101917. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2022.101904
Bradley, R. H., and Corwyn, R. F. (2004). Life satisfaction among European American, African American, Chinese American, Mexican American, and Dominican American adolescents. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 28, 385–400. doi: 10.1080/01650250444000072
Bujnowska, A. M., Rodríguez, C., Garcia, T., Areces, D., and Marsh, N. V. (2019). Parenting and future anxiety: the impact of having a child with developmental disabilities. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 16:668. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16040668
Carelli, M. G., Wiberg, B., and Âström, E. (2011). Broadening the TP profile: future negative time perspective. In M. Stolarski, N. Fieulaine, and W. Beekvan (Eds.). Time perspective theory; review, research and application: Essays in honor of Philip G. Zimbardo (pp. 87–97). Springer International Publishing.
Chen, X., Cai, Z., He, J., and Fan, X. (2020). Gender differences in life satisfaction among children and adolescents: a meta-analysis. J. Happiness Stud. 21, 2279–2307. https://10.1007/s10902-019-00169-9. doi: 10.1007/s10902-019-00169-9
Clarke, S., McKenzie, H., Haintz, G. L., and Graham, M. (2020). Social support and women’s reproductive decision making: testing emergent fit of “optimizing support for the preservation of self”. Qual. Health Res. 30, 975–987. doi: 10.1177/1049732320904713
Commodari, E., La Rosa, V. L., and Nania, G. S. (2022). Pregnancy, modtherhood and partner support in visually impaired women: a qualitative study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:4308. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19074308
Coudin, G., and Lima, M. L. (2011). Being well as time goes by: future time perspective and well-being. Int. J. Psychol. Psychol. Therapy 11, 219–232.
Cowan, C. P., and Cowan, P. A. (1995). Interventions to ease the transition to parenthood: why they are needed and what they can do. Fam. Relat. 44, 412–423. doi: 10.2307/584997
Cunningham, K. F., Zhang, J. W., and Howell, R. T. (2015). Time perspectives and subjective well-being: a dual-pathway framework. In M. Stolarski, N. Fieulaine, and W. Beekvan (Eds.). Time perspective theory; review, research and application: Essays in honor of Philip G. Zimbardo (pp. 403–415). Springer International Publishing.
Daoud, J. (2017). Multicollinearity and regression analysis. J. Phys. Conf. Ser. 949:012009. doi: 10.1088/1742-6596/949/1/012009
Datta, J., Maxwell, K. J., Mitchell, K. R., Lewis, R., and Wellings, K. (2023). Factors shaping the timing of later entry into parenthood: Narratives of choice and constraint. Soc Scien & Hum Open. 8:100700. doi: 10.1016/j.ssaho.2023.100700
Díaz, M. Y. (2021). “It’s hard to become mothers”: the moral economy of postponing motherhood in neoliberal Chile. Br. J. Sociol. 72, 1214–1228. doi: 10.1111/1468-4446.12901
Díaz, M. Y., and Abufhele, A. (2023). Beyond early motherhood: trends and determinants of late fertility in Chile. Int. Sociol. 39, 27–49. doi: 10.1177/02685809231195956
Diener, E., Inglehart, R., and Tay, L. (2013). Theory and validity of life satisfaction scales. Soc. Indic. Res. 112, 497–527. https://10.1007/s11205-012-0076-y. doi: 10.1007/s11205-012-0076-y
Dion, K. K. (1995). Delayed parenthood and women’s expectations about the transition to parenthood. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 18, 315–333. doi: 10.1177/016502549501800208
Dupray, A., and Pailhé, A. (2017). Does employment uncertainty particularly impact fertility of children of north African immigrants in France? A gender perspective. J. Ethn. Migr. Stud. 44, 401–424. doi: 10.1080/1369183X.2017.1313107
Eysenck, M. W., Payne, S., and Santos, R. (2006). Anxiety and depression: past, present, and future events. Cognit. Emot. 20, 274–294. doi: 10.1080/02699930500220066
Fachrudin, K. A., and Latifah, S. (2022). Relationship between individual characteristics, neurotic personality, personal financial distress, and financial behavior. Cogent Bus. Manag. 9:2105565. doi: 10.1080/23311975.2022.2105565
Funder, D. C., and Ozer, D. J. (2019). Evaluating effect size in psychological research: sense and nonsense. Adv. Methods Pract. Psychol. Sci. 2, 156–168. doi: 10.1177/2515245919847202
Galway, L., and Field, E. (2023). Climate emotions and anxiety among young people in Canada: a national survey and call to action. J. Climate Change Health 9:100204. doi: 10.1016/j.joclim.2023.100204
Haliwa, I., Wilson, J., Lee, J., and Shook, N. J. (2021). Predictors of change in mental health during the COVID-19 pandemic. J. Affect. Disord. 291, 331–337. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2021.05.045
Hao, H., Li, X., Shen, X., Jiang, H., Xiao, H., and Lyu, H. (2024). Reciprocal relationships between future time perspective and self-control among adolescents in China: a longitudinal study. Curr. Psychol. 43, 11062–11070. doi: 10.1007/s12144-023-05222-8
Headey, B., Kelley, J., and Wearing, A. J. (1993). Dimensions of mental health: life satisfaction, positive affect, anxiety and depression. Soc. Indic. Res. 29, 63–82. doi: 10.1007/BF01136197
Hickman, C., Marks, E., Pihkala, P., Clayton, S., Lewandowski, R. E., Mayall, E. E., et al. (2021). Climate anxiety in children and young people and their beliefs about government responses to climate change: a global survey. Lancet Planetary Health 5, e863–e873. doi: 10.1016/S2542-5196(21)00278-3
Hird, M. J., and Abshoff, K. (2000). Women without children: a contradiction in terms? J. Comp. Fam. Stud. 31, 347–366. doi: 10.3138/jcfs.31.3.347
Hwayan, O. A. (2020). Predictive ability of future anxiety in professional decision-making skill among a Syrian refugee adolescent in Jordan. Occup. Ther. Int. 2020, 1–6. doi: 10.1155/2020/4959785
Jannini, T. B., Rossi, R., Socci, V., and Di Lorenzo, G. (2022). Validation of the dark future scale (DFS) for future anxiety on an Italian sample. J. Psychopathodol. 28, 86–93. doi: 10.36148/2284-0249-457
Jia, Y., Hou, Z., Zhang, H., and Xiao, Y. (2020). Future time perspective, career adaptability, anxiety, and career decision-making difficulty: exploring mediations and moderators. J. Career Dev. 49, 282–296. doi: 10.1177/0894845320941922
Joiner, T. E., Katz, J., and Lew, A. (1999). Harbingers of depressotypic reassurance seeking: negative life events, increased anxiety, and decreased self-esteem. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 25, 632–639. doi: 10.1177/0146167299025005008
Kang, H. (2021). Sample size determination and power analysis using the G*power software. J. Educ. Eval. Health Prof. 18:17. doi: 10.3352/jeehp.2021.18.17
Kooij, D. T., Kanfer, R., Betts, M., and Rudolph, C. W. (2018). Future time perspective: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Appl. Psychol. 103, 867–893. doi: 10.1037/apl0000306
Koprowicz, A., and Gumowska, I. (2022). Is the future frightening? Anxiety among young people in care in Poland as they move to independence. Adopt. Fost. 46, 302–317. doi: 10.1177/03085759221116412
Lazarus, G., Pshedetzky-Shochat, R., Zahavi-Lupo, T., Gleason, M. E. J., and Rafaeli, E. (2022). Emotion differentiation during the transition to parenthood – concurrent and prospective positive effects. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 39, 592–614. doi: 10.1177/02654075211043291
Lens, W., Paixao, M. P., Herrera, D., and Grobler, A. (2012). Future time perspective as a motivational variable: content and extension of future goals affect the quantity and quality of motivation. Jpn. Psychol. Res. 54, 321–333. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-5884.2012.00520.x
Luo, L., Yuan, J., Bi, S., Wang, Y., Cao, Y., Wen, S., et al. (2024). The impact of emerging adulthood characteristics on college students’ anxiety: the mediating role of life satisfaction and internet addiction. Curr. Psychol. 43, 17332–17342. doi: 10.1007/s12144-024-05615-3
Margolis, S., Schwitzgebel, E., Ozer, D. J., and Lyubomirsky, S. (2019). A new measure of life satisfaction: the Riverside life satisfaction scale. J. Pers. Assess. 101, 621–630. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2018.1464457
Marks, J., Schneider, S., and Voigt, B. (2021). Future-oriented cognition: links to mental health problems and mental wellbeing in preschool-aged and primary-school-aged children. Front. Psychol. 14:1211986. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1211986
Martin, L. J. (2020). Delaying, debating and declining motherhood. Cult. Health Sex. 23, 1034–1049. doi: 10.1080/13691058.2020.1755452
Mooney, A., Earl, J. K., Mooney, C. H., and Beatman, H. (2017). Using balanced time perspective to explain well-being and planning in retirement. Front. Psychol. 8:1781. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01781
Nisén, J., Bijlsma, M. J., Martikainen, P., Wilson, B., and Myrskylä, M. (2022). The gendered impacts of delayed parenthood: a dynamic analysis of young adulthood. Adv. Life Course Res. 53:100496. doi: 10.1016/j.alcr.2022.100496
Nomaguchi, K., and Milkie, M. A. (2020). Parenthood and well-being: a decade in review. J. Marriage Fam. 82, 198–223. doi: 10.1111/jomf.12646
Obeng Gyimah, S. (2003). A cohort analysis of the timing of first birth and fertility in Ghana. Popul. Res. Policy Rev. 22, 251–266. doi: 10.1023/A:1026008912138
Ogbeibu, S., Chiappetta Jabbour, C. J., Gaskin, J., Senadjki, A., and Hughes, M. (2021). Leveraging STARA competencies and green creativity to boost green organisational innovative evidence: a praxis for sustainable development. Bus. Strateg. Environ. 30, 2421–2440. doi: 10.1002/bse.2754
Okine, R., Hughes, L. M., Smith, G., Bonus, M. L., Feinberg, E. C., and Bernardi, L. A. (2023). Undergraduate students have low fertility knowledge and high anxiety regarding future fertility: An opportunity for education. Heliyon. 16:14623. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2023.e14623
Oh, H., Marinovich, C., Rajkumar, R., Besecker, M., Shou, S., Jacob, L., et al. (2021). COVID-19 dimensions are related to depression and anxiety among US college students: findings from the healthy minds survey 2020. J. Affect. Disord. 292, 270–275. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2021.05.121
Panayiotou, G., and Vrana, S. R. (2004). The role of self-focus, task difficulty, task self-relevance, and evaluation anxiety in reaction time performance. Motiv. Emot. 28, 171–196. doi: 10.1023/B:MOEM.0000032313.69675.0d
Panuccio, F., Berardi, A., Marquez, M. A., Messina, M. P., Valente, D., Tofani, M., et al. (2020). Development of the pregnancy and motherhood evaluation questionnaire (PMEQ) for evaluating and measuring the impact of physical disability on pregnancy and management of motherhood: a pilot study. Disabil. Rehabil. 44, 1474–1480. doi: 10.1080/09638288.2020.1802520
Paolini, L., Yanez, A. P., and Kelly, W. E. (2006). An examination of worry and life satisfaction among college students. Individ. Differ. Res. 4, 331–339.
Pihkala, P. (2020). Anxiety and the ecological crisis: an analysis of eco-anxiety and climate anxiety. Sustain. For. 12:7836. https://3390/su12197836. doi: 10.3390/su12197836
Preacher, K. J., and Kelley, K. (2011). Effect size measures for mediation models: quantitative strategies for communicating indirect effects. Psychol. Methods 16, 93–115. doi: 10.1037/a0022658
Proctor, C. L., Linley, P. A., and Maltby, J. (2009). Youth life satisfaction: a review of the literature. J. Happiness Stud. 10, 583–630. https://10.1007/s10902-008-9110-9. doi: 10.1007/s10902-008-9110-9
Regnoli, G. M., Tiano, G., and De Rosa, B. (2024). How is the fear of war impacting Italian adults’ mental health? The mediating role of future anxiety and intolerance of uncertainty. Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 14, 838–855. doi: 10.3390/ejihpe14040054
Reuter, S. Z. (2019). Certainty as social justice: understanding childless academic women’s reproductive decisiveness. Women’s Stud. Int. Forum 74, 104–113. doi: 10.1016/j.wsif.2019.03.010
Richard, F. D., Bond, C. F. Jr., and Strokes-Zoota, J. J. (2003). One hundred years of social psychology quantitatively described. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 7, 331–363. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.7.4.331
Rönnlund, M., Âström, E., Westlin, W., Unger, A., Papastamatelou, J., and Carelli, M. G. (2021). A time to sleep well and be contended: time perspective, sleep quality, and life satisfaction. Front. Psychol. 12:627836. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.627836
Schneider-Mayerson, M., and Leog, K. L. (2020). Eco-reproductive concerns in the age of climate change. Clim. Chang. 163, 1007–1023. doi: 10.1007/s10584-020-02923-y
Schlesinger, B., and Schlesinger, R. (1989). Postponed parenthood: Trends and issues. J. Comp. Fam. Stud. 20, 355–363. doi: 10.3138/jcfs.20.3.355
Sebaie, S. R. E., Aziz, M. M. A. E., and Atia, S. M. M. (2024). Positive thinking, resourcefulness skills, and future anxiety among the caregivers of children with intellectual disability: an intervention study. Middle East Curr. Psychiatry 31:24. doi: 10.1186/s43045-024-00412-x
Shek, D. T., and Li, X. (2016). Perceived school performance, life satisfaction, and hopelessness: a 4-year longitudinal study of adolescents in Hong-Kong. Soc. Indic. Res., 126, 921–934, doi: 10.1007/s11205-015-0904-y
Skirbekk, V. (2022). Decline and prosper!Changing global birth rates and the advantages of fewer children. Berlin: Springer Nature.
Sobotka, T. (2010). “Shifting parenthood to advanced reproductive ages: trends, causes and consequences” in A young generation under pressure? ed. J. Tremmel (Springer-Verlag), 129–154. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-03483-1_7
Sowislo, J. F., and Orth, U. (2013). Does low self-esteem predict depression and anxiety? A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychol. Bull. 139, 213–240. doi: 10.1037/a0028931
Steckermeier, L. C. (2021). The value of autonomy for the good life. An empirical investigation of autonomy and life satisfaction in Europe. Soc. Indic. Res. 154, 693–723. doi: 10.1007/s11205-020-02565-8
Stolarski, M., Fieulaine, N., and Zimbardo, P. G. (2018). “Putting time in a wider perspective: The past, the present and the future of time perspective theory” in The SAGE handbook of personality and individual differences: The science of personality and individual differences. eds. V. Zeigler-Hill and T. K. Shackelford, Sage. 592–628. doi: 10.4135/9781526451163.n28
Szcześniak, M., Falewicz, A., and Brodowska, D. (under review). Multidimensional scale of motives for postponing parenthood (MSMPP-18): development, factorial structure and psychometric properties. Sci Rep.
Szcześniak, M., Falewicz, A., Meisner, M., and Grodecka, K. (2024). The mediating effect of maturity on anxiety and the motives for postponing parenthood. Sci. Rep. 14:20258. doi: 10.1038/s41598-024-71043-9
Szymańska, P. (2021). The role of siblings in the process of forming life satisfaction among young adults – moderating function of gender. Curr. Psychol. 40, 6132–6144. doi: 10.1007/s12144-020-00776-3
Tamarit, A., De la Barrera, U., Schoeps, K., Castro-Calvo, J., and Montoya-Castilla, I. (2023). Analyzing the role of resilience and life satisfaction as mediators of the impact of COVID-19 worries on mental health. J. Community Psychol. 51, 234–250. doi: 10.1002/jcop.22900
Thompson, R., and Lee, C. (2011). Sooner or later? Young Australian men’s perspectives on timing of parenthood. J. Health Psychol. 16, 807–818. doi: 10.1177/1359105310392091
Timoszyk-Tomczak, C., and Próchniak, P. (2024). Construction and validation of a new generational time perspective questionnaire. Sci. Rep. 14:13279. doi: 10.1038/s41598-024-64185-3
Tucholska, K., Gulla, B., Grubowska, A., and Major, P. (2022). Time perspective, future anxiety, and hope in individuals awaiting bariatric surgery. Health Psychol. Rep. 10, 111–121. doi: 10.5114/hpr.2021.105259
Tydén, T., Svanberg, A. S., Karlström, P. O., Lihoff, L., and Lampic, C. (2006). Female university students’ attitudes to future motherhood and their understanding about fertility. Eur J Contracept Reprod Health Care 11, 181–189. doi: 10.1080/13625180600557803
Waldenström, U. (2016). Postponing parenthood to advanced age. Ups J. Med. Sci. 121, 235–243. doi: 10.1080/03009734.2016.1201553
Walters, G. D. (2019). Why are mediation effects so small? Int. J. Soc. Res. Methodol. 22, 219–232. doi: 10.1080/13645579.2018.1517232
Wilak, J. (2023). Why do polish women want to stay childless? Analysis of voluntary childlessness motives. Prace Naukowe Uniwersytetu Ekonomicznego Wrocławiu 67, 130–141. doi: 10.15611/pn.2023.3.12
Wilkie, J. R. (1981). The trend toward delayed parenthood. J. Marriage Fam. 43, 583–591. doi: 10.2307/351759
Xu, Y., Beller, A. H., Roberts, B. W., and Brown, J. R. (2015). Personality and young adult financial distress. J. Econ. Psychol. 51, 90–100. doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2015.08.010
Zabak, S., Varma, A., Bansod, S., and Pohane, M. R. (2023). Exploring the complex landscape of delayed childbearing: factors, history, and long-term implications. Cureus 15:e46291. doi: 10.7759/cureus.46291
Zacchini, F., Sampino, S., Ziętek, M., and Chan, A. (2022). Delayed parenthood and its influence on offspring health: What have we learned from the mouse model. Biol. Reprod. 106, 58–65. doi: 10.1093/biolre/ioab202
Zaleski, Z. (1996). Future anxiety: concept, measurement, and preliminary research. Personal. Individ. Differ. 21, 165–174. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(96)00070-0
Zaleski, Z., Sobol-Kwapinska, M., Przepiorka, A., and Meisner, M. (2019). Development and validation of the dark future scale. Time Soc. 28, 107–123. doi: 10.1177/0961463X16678257
Zdybek, P., and Haimann, M. (2017). Future anxiety as a predictor of the fear of childbirth. Family Forum 7, 127–143. doi: 10.25167/FF2017/127-143
Zhi, K., Yang, J., Chen, Y., Akebaijiang, N., Liu, M., Yang, X., et al. (2021). The relationship between future time perspective and psychological violence among Chinese college students. Front. Psychol. 12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.585837
Keywords: future anxiety, postponed parenthood, delayed parenthood, generational time perspective, life satisfaction
Citation: Szcześniak M, Timoszyk-Tomczak C, Łoś J and Grzeczka M (2025) Future anxiety and the motives for postponing parenthood: generational time perspective and life satisfaction as mediators. Front. Psychol. 15:1441927. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1441927
Received: 03 June 2024; Accepted: 23 September 2024;
Published: 12 February 2025.
Edited by:
Iwona Nowakowska, The Maria Grzegorzewska University, PolandReviewed by:
Elena Commodari, University of Catania, ItalyCopyright © 2025 Szcześniak, Timoszyk-Tomczak, Łoś and Grzeczka. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Małgorzata Szcześniak, bWFsZ29yemF0YS5zemN6ZXNuaWFrQHVzei5lZHUucGw=
†These authors share first authorship
‡ORCID: Małgorzata Szcześniak, https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6637-2304
Celina Timoszyk-Tomczak, https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8314-0476
Julia Łoś, https://orcid.org/0009-0004-4303-6940
Monika Grzeczka, https://orcid.org/0009-0004-2978-9997
Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Research integrity at Frontiers
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.