Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 05 September 2024
Sec. Performance Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Best Practice Approaches for Mixed Methods Research in Psychological Science - Volume II View all 10 articles

Analysis of motor behavior in piano performance from the mixed methods approach

  • 1Faculty of Psychology, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
  • 2Faculty of Psychology, Institute of Neurosciences, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
  • 3Department of Didactics of Musical, Artistic and Corporal Expression, Faculty of Education and Sport Sciences Melill, University of Granada, Granada, Spain
  • 4Department of Languages and Computer Sciences, School of Computer Science and Engineering, University of Málaga, Málaga, Spain
  • 5Computer and Information Sciences, University of St. Thomas, St. Paul, MN, United States

Introduction: The focus of this study centers on the extraction, analysis, and interpretation of the motor behavior of advanced-level pianists using observational methodology, itself framed within the field of mixed methods, paying particular attention to those aspects that characterize the pressed and struck touch. The aim of this research was to analyze the motor interactions of activation or inhibition associated with the production of a type of touch in the movements of the right upper limb of the participating pianists.

Methods: An ad hoc observational instrument was built that was incorporated into the software Lince Plus for data recording and coding. Data reliability was guaranteed applying Cohen’s Kappa coefficient, and an analysis of polar coordinates was carried out to identify the motor interactions involved in piano playing.

Results: The study provided significant information about the interaction of motor functions linked to two types of touch, such as those that occur in the sliding finger movement over the key in the pressed touch or the lifting finger movement above the key in the struck touch, obtaining clearly identified patterns of piano touch motor behavior.

Discussion: This research represents an innovative perspective of the study of piano-playing movement via the direct and perceptible observation of the pianist’s motor behavior in an everyday context. Observational methodology is distinguished by its low degree of internal control, which makes it possible to scientifically study the spontaneous behavior of pianists in their natural environment. This model allows us to describe and analyze piano touch for its application in the field of piano performance and teaching, emphasizing the practical implications of motor interactions in piano touch.

1 Introduction

The study of piano touch is an essential aspect of piano playing, as much on a pedagogical level as on a biomechanical, technical, and, above all, artistic level. The sound and expressive intentions of the pianist are reflected on the keyboard through motor actions of the upper limbs. The control of the quality and precision of piano playing is acquired over many years of practice and study, geared towards perfecting the pianist’s motor abilities (Furuya et al., 2010; Goebl et al., 2005). The studies of Furuya et al. (2014a, 2014b) highlighted optimized finger movements and a higher independence of these, achieved via musical practice, suggesting a plastic adaptation of the neuromuscular system associated with the control and independence of finger movement. Similarly, the pianist can specify the sound quality—defined from a timbral viewpoint—with gesture and type of touch (Bernays and Traube, 2013; Dogantan-Dack, 2016). In MacRitchie’s multi-disciplinary review of piano touch (2015), it was noted that, in addition to the different individual characteristics of each pianist, the sound of the piano also differed depending on the technical resources used, with type of touch—pressed versus struck—playing a significant role. Goebl et al. (2005) analyzed these two types of touch, observing large differences in the depression of the key and in hammer speed patterns. They examined the contact times of the whole set of pieces that make up the percussion mechanism of the piano, highlighting the importance of touch in movement control. They showed that, at the same speed, there is greater sound control with the pressed touch than the struck touch, with manipulation of the instrument and playing technique influencing perception of the sound quality of the playing. Furthermore, the pianist’s movements are a relevant factor in the effective communication of the musician with the audience (James, 2018; Li, 2022).

Advances in the scientific research of piano touch have revealed that these two types of touch exhibit different motor behaviors of the upper limbs (Goebl et al., 2014; Furuya et al., 2010) that imply different muscular loads, along with different sound qualities. The integration of both types of touch is suggested, although highlighting that a staccato articulation would carry a higher risk of injury, above all in the shoulder (Degrave et al., 2020; Goebl et al., 2004).

MacRitchie and Zicari (2012) highlighted the importance of the connection between the pianist’s musical intention and the resulting physical gesture, concluding that the musician’s conscious control over their body influences the sound produced.

The timbral quality of a performance, from the pianists’ viewpoint, does not only depend on the intensity and duration of the sounds, but includes various aspects that make up the performance: melody, articulation, tempo or dynamics. They use adjectives and metaphors such as shiny, rounded, dry, thick or velvety to describe different timbral nuances (Bernays, 2013; Li and Timmers, 2021). The embodiment of these musical conceptions requires the interaction between the plasticity of the neuronal system and the motor system, characterized by its flexibility in organizing an inordinate number of degrees of freedom in the upper limbs (Bernstein, 1967; Kay et al., 2003; Furuya and Altenmüller, 2013).

In the last few decades there has been a significant increase in research into acoustics and biomechanics in the field of piano playing. The majority of these studies use technical measuring systems equipped with high precision sensors attached to both the pianist’s body and the instrument itself (Goebl, 2017). These studies have provided valuable scientific information that sheds light on the artistic area of piano technique, and specifically—in the case that concerns us—on the knowledge of piano touch procedures. See the research of MacRitchie and McPherson (2015), Bernays and Traube (2014) or Turner et al. (2021), among others, as clarifying examples.

However, it remains a challenge to study the pianist’s body movements in a natural and minimally intrusive environment that does not significantly affect their performance (Goebl, 2017). As MacRitchie (2015) points out, there is a need to develop non-intrusive measurement systems that can be incorporated into piano lessons, outside of a laboratory setting and without restrictions.

The application of observational methodology to this approach is considered appropriate, since it is characterized as a scientific method adapted to the reality of natural situations, in which the control of other methodologies is not suitable, given that it requires the spontaneity of human behavior developed in its usual context (Anguera et al., 2019). Until now, observational methodology has been primarily applied to the study of movement in motor contexts such as physical activity and sports (Anguera et al., 2017). However, it has not been used in the analysis of body movement in the field of instrumental music.

This study seeks to fill this gap in the scientific literature related to this subject by applying observational methodology to the study of piano-playing movement which contributes—in addition to a non-intrusive method of capturing movement—a new analysis of the behavioral flow of piano touch that implies an order of motor actions. Lag sequential analysis and polar coordinate analysis appear as a powerful medium for estimating patterns of behavior that reflect activation and inhibition relationships of movements that precede or follow a particular behavior, in such a way that they reflect behavioral tendencies that can be useful for detecting motor behavior patterns that could be avoided in order to prevent injury or perfect both technique and musical expression. Lag sequential analysis of piano performance is different from other purely quantitative analyses due to its implication in the process of transition and interconnection between some behaviors and others, involving a continual systematic observation process that takes into account the technical and expressive requirements of the musical piece. This analysis can provide a deeper understanding of the piano touch technique.

This project puts forward an innovative approach to the study of the motor behavior of two advanced level pianists, using observational methodology (OM)—considered mixed method in itself—(Anguera et al., 2017, 2018b; Anguera and Hernández-Mendo, 2016), for its adaptability and flexibility in detecting and analyzing, via direct and perceptible observation, the motor behavior patterns that are shown in the expressive production of piano sound in natural contexts. OM integrates the qualitative and quantitative phases (QUAL-QUAN-QUAL) of the research via rigorous procedures (quantitizing) that render it robust by making data analysis possible, not only through the scrutiny of behavior occurrences with the frequency parameter, but with the parameters of order and duration, from the production of a data record in the form of a code matrix (Anguera et al., 2020).

The main aim of this research was to identify relationships between motor behaviors that differently characterize pressed and struck touch. To this end we present and justify the construction of an ad hoc observation instrument based on field format, for its open, multidimensional, multiple-code and self-regulatory character (Anguera, 2010), integrated into the software LINCE PLUS for systematic observational study (Soto-Fernández et al., 2022).

The observation produced a large set of qualitative data that was analyzed quantitatively via lag sequential analysis (Bakeman and Gottman, 1989; Bakeman and Quera, 2011) and polar coordinate analysis (Sackett, 1980). This analysis yielded novel results regarding the detection of motor interactions linked to piano-playing movement.

2 Pressed touch and struck touch

The study of piano touch covers diverse aspects related to motor behavior, such as the movement of fingers, hands, upper arms and forearms, wrists and shoulders, along with speed or the pressure applied to the keys, analyzed from different pedagogical, biomechanical and technological perspectives. Type of touch is a highly important procedural parameter, given that it produces different sound and expression qualities that are perceived both by the musician and the audience (Goebl, 2017). Pianists are unanimous in distinguishing between pressed and struck touch, with the different motor behaviors that each one generates (Goebl et al., 2014; Furuya et al., 2010) and the repercussion in the quality—both of sound and expression—in piano playing (Goebl et al., 2005). The pressed touch is initiated from contact with the surface of the key, increasing shoulder mobility during the anticipatory rocking movement of pressing the keys (Verdugo et al., 2020, 2022); while the struck touch is characterized by lifting the finger a certain distance from the key before pressing it, producing an additional noise in the piano sound (Goebl et al., 2005; Kinoshita et al., 2007). Furuya et al. (2010) described a sound with a “hard” timbre if the key is pressed after previously lifting the finger and a “soft” timbre if the keystroke starts with the finger in contact with the key. Similarly, Goebl et al. (2014) showed that the timbre and the sound quality of the piano are not only influenced by the speed with which the hammer reaches the string. It was shown that applying the same speed to the keys, but with different touches, it was possible to detect the noise made by the finger-key and key-bottom impact if struck touch was used, which did not happen with the pressed touch. On the other hand, with the pressed touch, the sound reflects a gradual increase in the key speed, while the struck sound involves a sudden initial peak. With the struck touch, the time the finger stays on the key is shorter given that the finger hits the key at speed and leaves it quickly; while in the pressed touch the finger leaves the key by sliding off it in gradual acceleration, remaining on the key longer (Ortmann, 1925; W, 1925) and thus lengthening the acoustic qualities of the piano. While the struck touch is ineffective for this purpose, it is effective for strong sounds and rapid movements (Goebl et al., 2014). In terms of finger movement during the keystroke, Furuya et al. (2010) compared the pressed touch with the struck touch, finding that in the former the finger traces a trajectory from the distal phalanx (DP) to the proximal phalanx (PP), while in the latter it was from the PP to the DP; thus a gentler sound is produced—from a psychoacoustic perspective—in the pressed touch than in the struck touch. The trajectory of the pressed touch is therefore closer to the carezzando style of Chopin and the Lisztian touch, characterized by pressing the key with the finger and releasing it by sliding over it from the fingerpad towards the palm of the hand (Bellman, 2001; Ott, 2003).

James (2012) considers the use of the pressed touch essential for obtaining tonal quality; this is because the contact surface area of the fleshy part of the finger is larger, due to the fingers being slightly extended, providing the musician with greater tactile feedback. Regarding the position of the fingers, Furuya et al. (2012) also concluded that in the pressed touch the fingers are more extended than curved, facilitating a larger surface contact. In this sense, Goebl and Palmer (2008) highlighted that the mechanoreceptors in the skin of the fingerpad transmit sensorial signals to large cortical areas responsible for tactile processing, with the proximity of the fingers to the keys being significant (Dalla Bella and Palmer, 2011). This is essential for the precision and spatial exactitude necessary for controlling the speed of contact with the key, the articulation and tempo, together with obtaining different tonal nuances (James, 2012; Kojucharov, 2014; Kojucharov and Rodà, 2015). According to the pianist, pedagogue and precursor of the scientific study of piano touch, Marie Jaëll, the fingerpad touch enables the musician to obtain an ideal sound via a system of reciprocal influence of movement, thought and internal hearing (Robb, 2022).

With reference to the pianist’s control of the touch, Goebl and Palmer (2008) observed that this depends on the parameters of tempo and dynamics, with the struck touch being more common in faster tempi, whilst in slows tempi pianists have greater control over the type of touch. Furthermore, use of the pressed touch stands out in gentle dynamics, whilst the struck touch is preferred in stronger dynamics (Goebl et al., 2005). In their study of time control and the efficiency of expert musicians’ hand movements, Goebl and Palmer (2013) observed that the fingers act independently at different tempi. They also observed that wrist movements of supination, pronation, flexion and extension remained stable in different tempi, as opposed to the finger joints which had greater ranges of motion. Hadjakos et al. (2008) highlighted shoulder and elbow participation—together with wrist and finger joints—in octave repetition, the forearm being lifted with inertia to avoid tiredness during the pianist’s performance. In terms of wrist action, Oikawa et al. (2011) observed that the flexor muscle in the wrist is what acts as an agonist in the movement, with rapid activation to press the key, while the extensor stabilizes the wrist and finger joints. Using surface EMG, they concluded that a neutral wrist position at fortissimo intensity is the best option for reducing the musculoskeletal load on the forearm during the keystroke. The height of the finger when preparing to press the key also varied with changes in rhythm and dynamics (Dalla Bella and Palmer, 2011). One of the most important aspects of the keystroke is the movement and shape of the fingers. Among the aspects that influence the keystroke, Berman (2002) included: the weight placed on the key, the mass and speed applied, the comparison between the finger that reaches the key bottom and that which superficially touches it, the curve of the fingers and the contact applied to the key by the finger pad or tip; along with the position inside or outside the keyboard with pressure on the keys or remaining at a distance without a strong grip on them. Far from being of little relevance, these characteristics define the timbre quality of a performance and distinguish one pianist from the next (Sandor, 1981). Regarding the shape of the palm area of the hand, Brown (2000) suggested that flattening the arch of the hand at the point of the knuckles creates tension in the forearm and fingers. Following this logic, Del Pueyo (1990) maintains that the hand should be reinforced and balanced with firm fingers, forming a hollow by the marked lowering of the outer fingers. With the purpose of avoiding blocking the hand, Lee (2005) makes reference to the piano technique of Franz Liszt, which involves the suspension and sliding of the fingers—just like the bow of a violin when it falls onto the strings and slides over them—lightening the weight to enable a wide expression of different dynamics, thanks to retropulsive force characterized, according to Ott (2004), as:

A light suspension of the shoulder and upper arm that allows the forearm to tilt its pressure towards the hand while a large fleshy surface of the fingers pulls on the keys, retaining this general retropulsion at the key bottom which provides strength and speed without causing any blocking (p. 37).

The hand no longer stays still but begins to move freely to the right and left, excluding verticality, highlighting the role of the finger flexors for this purpose. Uniformity in the fingers is achieved through gradually supporting the forearm and upper arm, avoiding the heaviness produced by the force of gravity (Ott, 2004). In his treatise about piano playing technique, On piano playing: motion, sound and expression, Sandor (1981) explains free-fall movement: consisting of the upper arm, forearm, hand and fingers lifting and then falling using gravitational force, with no muscular intervention whatsoever during the lowering movement of the arms. When the fingers reach the keyboard a slight elevation of the hand is produced while the fingers remain over the keyboard. On the other hand, regarding the complex motor behavior of piano touch, Bernstein (1967) and Bernstein and Popova (Kay et al., 2003), showed how muscle strengths were reorganized from a biomechanical viewpoint depending on the tempi, in such a way that the forearm participated in a slow tempo, whilst only the wrist flexion intervened in a rapid tempo. This showed the motor system’s capacity for self-organization contingent on mechanical restrictions, along with the possibility that different movements produce similar results (Kursell, 2006). Furuya and Kinoshita (2008) evidenced the importance of training for expert pianists, in their study of motor control of the upper limbs in the interaction of complex multi-joint movement—shoulder, arm, elbow, wrist and metacarpophalangeal joints—during piano touch. Such training is fundamental for achieving greater physiological efficiency, along with a greater exactitude of movement, which implies a necessary muscular control of the upper limbs as a whole. The precursor of scientific research into the piano touch mechanism, Marie Jaëll, in her empirical study of hand movement (Caruso, 2016), considered that musical perfection is achieved through a corporal awareness of piano playing, avoiding the automatic repetition of movements (Guichard, 2004). For Jaëll, isolation and paying attention to minimal variations in touch is similar to tuning in to the aesthetic content of a musical work (Weinstein-Reiman, 2021). Emphasizing this aspect, it is important to underline the importance of conscious activity in order to intervene in muscular counter-tension that counteracts the motion impulse of the arm, hand and fingers. This is achieved via a continuous movement that enables muscular control of finger rebound on the keys, acting in the opposite direction to this, using tension in opposition to the force of gravity that develops muscular elasticity starting from points of support (James, 2012). In short, the pianist’s body movements are related to the control of the kinematic chain from the shoulders to the finger pads, combining speed—according to the tempo and dynamics—with musical expression (Goebl, 2017).

3 Methods

3.1 Design

A mixed method approach was applied to this study using observational methodology (Anguera, 1979). Following the three dichotomous dimensions proffered by Anguera et al. (2011), the design proposed in this research was of an N/P/M type (nomothetic in being aimed at the observation of each of the two participants and the independent study of them being of interest; punctual, as the recordings were carried out in one single session and in the same conditions for both participants although with intra-session monitoring from the start to the end of the session; and multidimensional, since different behaviors are observed which entail different levels of response, with corresponding repercussion in the observation instrument). The systematic observation carried out was non-participatory and the observed components were completely perceptible (Anguera et al., 2018a; Bakeman and Quera, 2011).

3.2 Participants

The participants in this study were two male pianists aged 28 and 27, identified as Participant 1 (P1) and Participant 2 (P2), respectively. Both had advanced qualifications in the specialty of piano, with 21 and 20 years of professional experience, respectively, in the study of classical piano.

Experienced pianists were required with a view to maximizing the degree of control in the sound production in their performance. Both musicians gave their written informed consent in accordance with the principals of the Helsinki Declaration, and a favorable assessment was obtained of the methodological, ethical and legal aspects from the Bioethical Commission of the University of Barcelona.

3.3 Instruments

3.3.1 Observation instrument

An ad hoc observation instrument was constructed for the observation and record of the motor behaviors of the participants during the piano playing performance. The aim was to detect, select and analyze patterns of motor behavior in the piano touch of each of the two musicians. As it is a multidimensional study, the observation instrument was based on the field format, which contained 13 dimensions deployed in 53 behaviors in total, the mutual exclusivity of these behaviors being governed in each of the dimensions (Table 1). The base dimension for the construction of the observation instrument were the motor functions of the right upper limb in piano playing, under the technical premises of the struck touch and the pressed touch. The segmentation criteria were based on selecting units of behavior according to the technical requirements of the musical excerpt performed, taking into account the specific purposes of the research, which focus on observing the piano touch of two expert pianists during the performance. Nine behavioral segments were delimited after the exploratory phase of the study. These are shown in Dimension 1 of the observation instrument (Table 1), with the molar and molecular levels of the behaviors observed, taking into account the different levels of granularity (Schegloff, 2000) of the units—from larger (molar level) to smaller (molecular level) –, and considering their relative nature, included within the dimensions and behaviors of the observation instrument.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Observational instrument.

Dimension 1 (Musical extract interpreted) was deployed in nine behaviors/elements, each one containing musical extracts selected for observation, taking into account the technical requirements of piano playing, described textually and graphically in the observation instrument. Dimension 2 (Start of keystroke) was deployed in three behaviors related to finger or arm movement prior to the keystroke. Dimension 3 (Fingers) was deployed in five behaviors related to the different flexion or extension behaviors of the metacarpophalangeal joints (MJ) and the proximal (PJ) and distal interphalangeal finger joints (DJ). Dimension 4 (Fingers: surface contact with the key) was deployed in seven behaviors related to the part of the finger that made contact with the key (pad, tip, distal phalanx (DP), distal phalanx and proximal phalanx (DP and PP), lateral part in pronation or supination or no contact with the key). Dimension 5 (Thumb: contact surface) was deployed in four behaviors relating to the surface contact of the thumb with the key, taking into account the morphological difference of the thumb with respect to the other fingers. Dimension 6 (Finger: key release) was deployed in two behaviors, taking into account whether the finger releases the key by sliding over it or lifting it off the key. Dimension 7 (Finger placement), deployed in two behaviors, had the aim of observing the behavior of the fingers in terms of coordination or lack of coordination between them, taking into account similar or disparate placement of each finger with respect to the others. Dimension 8 (Thumb: movements) was deployed in four behaviors, taking into account the thumb movements via rotation or sliding over the key. Dimension 9 (Hand: palm area) was deployed in two behaviors, taking into account the cupped position of the palm, via flexion of the MJ, or the flat position, via extension of the MJ. Dimension 10 (Hand: action) was deployed in six behaviors, taking into account the hand movements or where the hand is placed on the keyboard when it is motionless. Dimension 11 (Wrist), was deployed in three behaviors according to the neutral, flexed or extended position of the wrist. Dimension 12 (Forearm) was deployed in two behaviors according to the position of the forearm in relation to the keyboard, being high when it is sloping down toward the keyboard and low when the elbow is facing the floor. Dimension 13 (Placement of the body in front of the keyboard) was deployed in three behaviors related to the height chosen by the pianist in front of the keyboard, according to the formation of an obtuse, acute or right angle by the upper arm and forearm. The dimensions and behaviors are shown in Table 1.

3.3.2 Recording instruments

Filming was done using two Casio EXF-1 digital cameras which, prior to determining the system of reference, were placed on both sides of the participating pianists, at the height of the keyboard. The recording rate was 100 frames per second.

For the data record and coding process the program LINCE PLUS (v.2.1.0) (Soto-Fernández et al., 2022) was used. Bakeman (1978) established four types of observational data, and this classification is still valid. From these, we used Type IV data in this study, which require a multidimensional observation instrument, as is our case, and also that the duration of each co-occurrence of behaviors (in addition to their frequency and order of presentation) be recorded. They are the most powerful data and have allowed us to obtain a record with the maximum level of information, enabling us to then proceed to a robust analysis of that record. Type IV data are concurrent and time-based; i.e., they incorporate the parameters of order and duration, in addition to co-occurrence, given the multidimensional nature of the observation design.

3.3.3 Audio recording instrument

Audio recording of the musical fragment performed by the participants was done with the software Ableton Live v.8, sound card Roland Edirol UA 101. The sound was recorded with no equalizing or added effects.

3.4 Procedure

For this study a musical fragment was selected, consisting of the first 10 bars of the first movement of Sonata for N° 13 in B-flat major, K. 333/315c, by Wolfgang Amadeus Mozart (Figure 1). Eleven advanced level pianists were offered the opportunity to take part in the recording of this musical fragment, and their written informed consent sought. They were subsequently assessed by a group of three experts, two of these being teachers from a Spanish advanced music school, and the third a teacher from a Belgian music school. The selection was based on the evaluation of sound quality via a description using five adjectives considered by Bernays (2013) as the most common for describing nuances of timbre on the piano. Similarly, each of the audio recordings was given a score by the experts on a scale from 1 to 10. Finally, the two pianists who obtained the highest score from the experts were selected for this study (P1 and P2).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Excerpt of Sonata N° 13 for piano in B-flat major, K. 333/315c, by W. A. Mozart.

The participating pianists were asked to study the musical fragment for 5 min each day for a week. Following this practice time, the participating pianists were then recorded in both video and audio format playing the musical fragment on a Steinway grand piano placed center stage, in one single session which took place in the concert hall of the Auditorio Manuel de Falla in Granada, Spain. Each participant played the selected fragment with their own fingering, without using the pedal and in a tempo of 4 = 116. The recordings were carried out with two cameras placed on the sides of the piano keyboard, in such a way that the right upper limb study object could be observed from two different angles, to enable full inspection of it.

3.4.1 Data quality control: inter-observer agreement

The record of the observed behaviors was done by two observers with a high degree of experience and knowledge of the substantive field of this study (Anguera et al., 2018a). One of them is a music teacher, with a qualification in piano teaching, an advanced qualification in dance and a qualification in the Dalcroze method based on the teaching of music through movement. The other observer is a professor of piano in an advanced music school and also a concert pianist. Both underwent 3 weeks of training in the observation process and use of the recording instrument for this study, prior to recording. Data record reliability was guaranteed by calculating the level of agreement with Cohen’s Kappa coefficient (Cohen, 1960, 1968), via the program Lince Plus, version 2.1.0 (Soto-Fernández et al., 2022). In line with Landis and Koch (1977), the inter-observer agreement level was “almost perfect,” obtaining results of 0.90 with participant P1, and 0.95 with participant P2.

3.4.2 Data analysis

Once data reliability had been confirmed, the data were then analyzed. The robustness provided by observational methodology comes not only through the scrutiny of behavior occurrences with the first frequency parameter, but also because the parameters of order and duration enable lag sequential analysis and polar coordinate analysis, among others, thus rendering it necessary to obtain a data record in the form of a code matrix (Anguera et al., 2020).

Lag sequential analysis (Bakeman and Gottman, 1989; Bakeman and Quera, 2011), which was done from the obtained records, enables the detection of sequential patterns or chains of significant behaviors that show regularities in which patterns of motor behaviors can be observed, associated with a given behavior criterion, which is the behavior proposed in each analysis as the trigger of the behavioral pattern. The results are obtained in the form of adjusted residuals.

Polar coordinate analysis (Sackett, 1980), which requires as data the adjusted residuals obtained from the lag sequential analysis, is a powerful data reduction technique for determining the inhibition or activation relationships between a focal behavior (considered nuclear in each analysis) and one or various conditioned behaviors, which are all those of which we want to determine their activating or inhibiting relationship with the focal behavior. The associations between the behavior dyads are represented graphically via vectors.

Polar coordinate analysis was done in this study with the program HOISAN (Hernández-Mendo et al., 2012), version 2.0, with the observation record files being exported from LINCE PLUS to HOISAN.

As a preliminary step, lag sequential analysis was done to obtain prospective and retrospective patterns of behavior with respect to a given focal behavior. The lag sequential analyses were carried out prospectively (from +1 to +5 lags) and retrospectively (from −1 to −5 lags); the significance level was set at p < 0.05. The calculation of adjusted residuals gives positive (activation effect) or negative (inhibitive effect) values between the criterion behavior (that would play the role of focal in the polar coordinate analysis), and each conditioned behavior, which shows the degree of connection between both behaviors.

The volume of initial results produced was reduced via a powerful algorithm, based on the parameter Zsum = ∑𝛧/√n, proposed by Cochran (1954) which is calculated prospectively (from the focal behavior onwards) and retrospectively (from the focal behavior backwards). This Zsum parameter enables us to obtain the length and angle of the vectors that show the type of inter-relationship between the focal behavior and each conditioned behavior (depending on the quadrant they are in, and therefore the angle), and the intensity of this relationship (depending on the length of the vector).

In this study, polar coordinate analysis was used to analyze the focal and conditioned behaviors of the participants P1 and P2 that are shown in Table 2, in line with the aim of this study, where the codes corresponding to each behavior are indicated and the description recorded in the observation instrument (Table 1).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Description and codes of the given focal behaviors and conditioned behaviors/elements of participants P1 and P2 in the polar coordinate analysis.

Polar coordinate analysis allows us to obtain the prospective and retrospective Zsum parameters of each conditioned behavior with respect to the focal behavior, and to visualize it via a vector map divided into four quadrants, where the prospective and retrospective Zsum parameters are represented on the X and Y coordinate axes respectively, and the vector corresponding to each behavior is located in one of the four quadrants. This means there are four possibilities: quadrant I (++) indicates that the focal and conditioned behaviors are mutually activated; quadrant II (− +) indicates the prospective inhibition and retrospective activation of the behaviors, i.e., the focal behavior inhibits the conditioned behavior but is also activated by it; quadrant III (− −) indicates mutual prospective and retrospective inhibition; and quadrant IV (+ −) indicates prospective activation and retrospective inhibition, i.e., the focal behavior activates the conditioned behavior, although this inhibits the focal. The length of the vector (radius) is considered statistically significant (p < 0.05), when it measures from 1.96 onwards. The vector angle (angular coordinate) determines its location in one of the quadrants: I [0 to 90°]; II [91° to 180°]; III [181° to 270°]; IV [271° to 360°].

4 Results and discussion

4.1 Descriptive analysis

A total of 462 visualizations were carried out with participant P1, and 568 visualizations with P2, with the distribution by dimensions, behaviors and percentages of visualizations shown in Tables 3, 4, respectively.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Record of visualizations by participant P1.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Record of visualizations by participant P2.

In line with Anguera (1982), corporal movements are non-verbal behaviors that occur quickly, can produce a large quantity of responses—each varying in frequency of occurrence—and can be present or absent in a given moment. Consequently, the frequency of occurrence measurement becomes relevant, given that it is possible to discover the action probability of a specific response and its comparison with other types of behavior. As the frequency data in Tables 3, 4 shows, there is a different set of motor behavior occurrences in the two participants.

With reference to participant P1, Figure 2 shows the middle finger pressing the “F” key, starting from contact with the key surface (behavior ITP_ Dcontsup achieved 100% of the visualizations), tracing a finger trajectory that goes from the DP to the PP (Furuya et al., 2010). The hand is cupped due to the marked lowering of the thumb and little finger and the flexion of the MJ (the behavior MP_Moq obtained 95.6% of the visualizations), it remains high due to the high position of the forearm (behavior AtBz_AtBzA obtained 93.3% of the visualizations), which lightens the weight avoiding the hand blocking, and all the fingers remain in contact with the keys (the behavior DsCol_DsSim obtained 100% of the visualizations).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. P1 Pressed touch: trajectory from distal phalanx to proximal phalanx.

In the case of participant P2, Figure 3 shows prior finger elevation before pressing the “F” key, separating it from the key via extension of the MJ and flexion of DJ and PJ (the behavior IPT_DelvextM obtained 48.1% of the visualizations), tracing a finger trajectory that goes from PP to DP (Furuya et al., 2010). The hand is flat due to extension of the MJ and its position on the piano is low (the behavior MP_Mpl obtained 94.1% of the visualizations), due to a neutral wrist position (the behavior Mca_McaN obtained 72.5% of the visualizations) and the low position of the forearm (the behavior AtBz_AtBzB obtained 29.4% of the visualizations). The fingers are in disparate positions (observe the elevation of the index finger with the DJ and PJ extended and the middle finger with DJ and PJ flexed) (the behavior DsCol_DsDisp obtained 51% of the visualizations).

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. P2 Struck touch: trajectory from proximal phalanx to distal phalanx.

In keeping with the motor behavior patterns detailed above and those reflected in Tables 3, 4, two different types of touch are apparent—pressed and struck—which correspond to participant P1 and participant P2, respectively.

4.2 Polar coordinate analysis

The results of the polar coordinate analysis of the focal and conditioned behaviors shown in Table 2.

Participant P1. Motor interactions of the fingers that act with MJ flexed and PJ and DJ slightly extended (DS_DfxMligextPD) with the actions of start of keystroke (IPT), finger-key surface contact (DSct), key release actions (DlibT), finger placement (DsCol), shape of the palm area of the hand (MP), wrist position (Mca), forearm position (AtBz) and the placement of the body in front of the piano (CC).

The results of the analysis of the focal behavior DS_DfxMligextPD and the conditioned behaviors expressed in Table 2 for participant P1 were significant, as can be seen in block 1 of Table 5. In the corresponding vector map, shown first on the left in Figure 4, quadrant I shows the mutual activation of the focal behavior with the key release action of the fingers via sliding over it (DLibT_Ddz), along with a cupped palm shape (MP_Moq). The behavior relating to the finger-key surface contact is found in quadrant III, which means that it mutually inhibits the focal behavior, due to the moment of the action, prior to the key release via sliding. High forearm placement (AtBz_ AtBzA) is found in quadrant IV, which means that the fingers in MJ flexion and slight PJ and DJ extension activate the high forearm. At the same time the high forearm retrospectively inhibits the behavior of the fingers with flexed MJ and slightly extended PJ and DJ, which shows the rocking behavior of the forearm that acts in suspension, enabling the free movement of the fingers on regulating the pressure applied to them and avoiding the heaviness of the gravitational force (Ott, 2004). All this shows that the behavior of the fingers with MJ flexed and PJ and DJ slightly extended interacts with the cupped shape of the hand and the behavior of the high forearm. These behaviors facilitate key release via a sliding motion, motor behaviors that are characteristic of the pressed touch (Bellman, 2001; Furuya et al., 2012; Ortmann, 1925; Ott, 2003).

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Results of the polar coordinate analysis of participant P1.

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. P1. Vector representation of the interactions between four given behaviors (focal) and their paired behaviors (conditioned). The panels from right to left correspond to the behaviors indicated in Table 2. Purple vectors represent significant interactions; red vectors represent highly significant interactions; and blue vectors represent slightly significant interactions (corresponding to the values marked with one or two asterisks, according to the significance level in Table 5).

Participant P1. Motor interactions of the fingers that act with flexion of the MJ, PJ and DJ, (DS_DfxMPD), with the actions of start of keystroke (IPT), finger-key surface contact (DSct), key release action (DlibT), finger placement (DsCol), shape of the palm area of the hand (MP), wrist position (Mca), forearm position (AtBz) and the placement of the body in front of the piano (CC).

The results of the analysis of focal behavior DS_DfxMPD and the conditioned behaviors expressed in Table 2 for participant P1 were not significant in behaviors DSct_DcfgDP, DSct_DNoc, DLibT_Ddz and CC_CCa, as can be observed in block 2 of Table 5, while the rest of the results were significant. Second from the left in the corresponding vector map represented in Figure 4, clearly different motor behaviors and their location in opposing quadrants (activation–inhibition) are shown. In quadrant I the mutual activation of the focal behavior with the finger-key surface contact via the fingerpad, the wrist flexed and the forearm high is opposite to the behaviors in quadrant III, with the inhibition of the conditioned behavior relating to the finger-key contact with the DP and a neutral wrist position. In quadrant II we can see the shape of the palm area is cupped, indicating that the behavior of the fingers with MJ, PJ, and DJ flexed inhibits the cupped palm area prospectively, at the same time that the cupped hand retrospectively activates the finger action with the MJ, PJ, and DJ flexed. The action of the start of the keystroke from contact with its surface is located in quadrant IV, which means that the focal behavior (DS_DfxMPD) prospectively activates the conditioned behavior (IPT_Dcontsup), at the same time that the conditioned behavior retrospectively inhibits the focal. All this reveals that the motor behavior of the fingers with MJ, PJ and DJ flexed interacts with the finger-key contact of the fingerpad, the high forearm, and wrist flexion, which can be attributed to the fact that, in pressed keystrokes, the wrist anticipates the attack through wrist extension and steadily augments its contribution via wrist flexion (Oikawa et al., 2011; Goebl et al., 2005). Contact with the fingerpad provides greater tactile feedback, essential for spatial and temporal precision, along with the production of the pianist’s sound intentions (Dalla Bella and Palmer, 2011; Goebl and Palmer, 2008; James, 2012; Robb, 2022); these being motor behaviors characteristic of the pressed touch (Goebl et al., 2014; Furuya et al., 2010; MacRitchie, 2015; Verdugo et al., 2020).

Participant P1. Motor interactions of the action of thumb in lateral contact (PScT_PcL) with thumb actions (P), shape of the palm area of the hand (MP), hand action (Macc), wrist position (Mca), forearm position (AtBz) and placement of the body in front of the piano (CC).

The results of the analysis of focal behavior PScT_PcL and the conditioned behaviors expressed in Table 2 for participant P1 were significant, as can be seen in block 3 of Table 5. Third from the left in the corresponding vector map represented in Figure 4, we can see in quadrant I the mutual activation of the focal behavior with the neutral wrist position (Mca_McaN), together with mutual activation, although to a lesser degree but also significant, of the cupped hand shape (MP_Moq) and the action of the hand on the white keys (Macc_MaccBlan). Quadrant III shows the mutual inhibition of the conditioned behaviors relating to the high placement of the body in front of the keyboard (CC_CCa), high placement of the forearm (AtBz_AtBzA), movement of the hand via sliding (Macc_MdlzD) and a high wrist position (Mca_McaF). Quadrant IV shows the prospective activation of sliding the thumb over the key to enable the other fingers to pass over it, allowing lateral movement of the hand without this intervening, as is shown in quadrant III, whilst keeping its position in the white keys area and its cupped shape. Two clearly opposing activation actions are shown (neutral wrist, in quadrant I) and inhibition actions (flexed wrist, in quadrant III), due to the fact that the wrist does not use rotation or flexion for this movement. At the beginning of the sonata where there are six descending consecutive notes (G-F-E♭-D-C-B♭), P1 presses the “D” key with the thumb and allows the middle finger to press the “C” key by sliding over the “D” key. The hand does not change its placement or position, so that it moves laterally towards the left of the keyboard without a pronated turn. On the last beat of the second bar, the group of semiquavers (F-E♭-D-C) ends with the note “C,” on which the thumb carries out the same sliding action to allow the “B♭” at the beginning of the following bar to be played. The results show the interaction of the motor behavior of the thumb in lateral contact, with the thumb sliding over the key, the hand cupped, the hand moving via the thumb sliding over the key and the wrist in neutral position (see Figure 5).

Figure 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. P1 Hand movement via thumb sliding.

Participant P1. Motor interactions of the action carried out in the musical extract of the 9th demarcation of the fragment of the Mozart sonata (ExMu_Mzt1_9) with the actions of start of keystroke (IPT), finger position (DS), finger-key surface contact (Dsct), thumb surface contact with the key (PScT), key release actions (DlibT), finger placement (DsCol), shape of the palm area of the hand (MP), hand action (Macc), wrist position (Mca), forearm position (AtBz) and the placement of the body in front of the piano (CC).

The results of the analysis of focal behavior ExMu_Mzt1_9 and the conditioned behaviors expressed in Table 2 for participant P1 were significant, as can be observed in block 4 of Table 5, with the exception of DLibT_Ddz and CC_CCa. Fourth from the left in the corresponding vector map represented in Figure 4, we can see in quadrant I the mutual activation of the focal behavior with the action of the thumb making contact with the tip and the placement of the fingers flexing MJ and extending PJ and DJ. The start of the keystroke with the rest of the fingers is done from contact of the DP and PP with the key surface; the hand is flat and moves via sliding the fingers without modifying its position (Macc_MdlzD). In quadrant II the behavior focal prospectively inhibits the conditioned behaviors of finger position with extension of MJ, PJ and DJ, the wrist flexed and the forearm high, at the same time that the conditioned behaviors retrospectively activate the action of the focal behavior. Quadrant III shows the mutual inhibition of the focal behavior and the conditioned behaviors relating to finger contact via the finger pad (DSct_Dcy), lateral thumb contact (PScT_PcL), cupped palm shape (MP_Moq) and the action of the hand over the white keys (Macc_Blan). Quadrant IV shows the prospective activation of the neutral wrist position, while this retrospectively inhibits the focal behavior.

In focal behavior ExMu_Mzt1_9 we can observe the actions produced in the whole of the 9th bar and the first two beats of the 10th bar of the sonata. At the start of the 9th bar there is a syncope that begins with an octave (F-F), in the strong part of the first beat. The tip of the thumb remains in contact throughout the whole syncope (F, minim figure) due to the distance maintained with the “F” key an octave higher and the following keys “E♭-D” and “D.” Here what is notable is the action of the ring finger sliding over the “E♭” key until the little finger presses the “D” key. This motor action is significant, considering that the hand is moving towards the left of the keyboard, without the ring finger moving to the middle, which is what is immediately to its left, but the ring finger slides over the key to allow the little finger to move, which is on its right, an action which enables the sound continuity of the syncope (Bellman, 2001; Ott, 2003). Finally, a succession of descending sixths is played by sliding the fingers over the keys. The results show the sliding action of the fingers which interact with the rest of the motor behaviors (see Figure 6).

Figure 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Movement of ring finger to little finger (“E-flat” – “D”) via sliding over the key.

Participant P2. Motor interactions of the fingers that act with extension of the MJ and flexion of the PJ and DJ (DS_DextMfxPD) with the actions of start of keystroke (IPT), finger-key surface contact (DSct), key release actions (DlibT), finger placement (DsCol), shape of the palm area of the hand (MP), wrist position (Mca), forearm position (AtBz) and the placement of the body in front of the piano (CC).

The results of the analysis of focal behavior DS_DextMfxPD and the conditioned behaviors expressed in Table 2 for participant P2 were significant, with the exception of IPT_DelvextM, DSct_DcfgDP and Mca_McaEx, as shown in block 1 of Table 6. First on the left in the corresponding vector map represented in Figure 7, we can see the behaviors with the longest vectors in each of the quadrants. Thus, quadrant I shows the mutual activation of the focal behavior with the conditioned behaviors of no finger-key contact (DSct_DNoc), neutral wrist position (Mca_McaN) and low forearm (AtBz_AtBzB). Quadrant III shows the mutual inhibition of the behaviors relating to the start of the keystroke with prior lifting of the arm, along with finger-key contact with the DP, flexed wrist and high forearm. Quadrant II shows the prospective inhibition of the conditioned behavior relating to finger-key contact via the fingertip (DSct_Dcpt), at the same time that this retrospectively activates the action of the fingers with extension of the MJ, and flexion of the PJ and DJ. The key release action via lifting the finger is located in quadrant IV, which means that the focal behavior (DS_DextMfxPD) prospectively activates the conditioned behavior (DLibT_Delv), while the conditioned behavior inhibits the focal retrospectively. These interactions are related to the flattening of the hand arch that occurs with the given focal behavior, necessitating the raising of the fingers or forearm to release or touch the key due to the tension created in the forearm and fingers (Brown, 2000).

Table 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Results of the polar coordinate analysis of participant P2.

Figure 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. P2. Vector representation of the interactions between four given behaviors (focal) and their paired behaviors (conditioned). The panels from right to left correspond to the behaviors indicated in Table 2. Purple vectors represent significant interactions; red vectors represent highly significant interactions; and blue vectors represent slightly significant interactions (corresponding to the values marked with one or two asterisks, according to the significance level in Table 6).

The results show the interaction of the motor behavior of fingers with extended MJ and flexed PJ and DJ, with fingertip finger-key contact (Berman, 2002), and key release via lifting the finger and the start of the keystroke from a certain distance from the keys, via the action of no finger-key contact (Furuya et al., 2010); these being behaviors characteristic of the motor behavior of the struck touch (see Figure 8).

Figure 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. P2 Key release via finger elevation and start of keystroke from a certain distance from the keys.

Participant P2. Motor interactions of the fingers that act with flexion of the MJ, PJ and DJ, (DS_DfxMPD), with the actions of start of keystroke (IPT), finger-key surface contact (DSct), key release actions (DlibT), finger placement (DsCol), shape of the palm area of the hand (MP), wrist position (Mca), and forearm position (AtBz).

The results of the analysis of focal behavior DS_DfxMPD and the conditioned behaviors expressed in Table 2 for participant P2 were significant, with the exception of behaviors IPT_Dcontsup, IPT_Levprevbz, DSct_Dcpt, DSct_DcfgDP, DSct_DNoc, Mca_McaEx, AtBz_AtBzA and AtBz_AtBzB, which were not significant, as can be seen in block 2 of Table 6. Second from the left in the corresponding vector map, represented in Figure 7, we can see the behaviors with the longest vectors in each of the quadrants. Thus, quadrant I shows the mutual activation of the focal behavior with a similar finger placement (DsCol_DsSim), a behavior opposite to that shown in quadrant III, relating to the inhibition of disparate finger placement (DsCol_DsDisp). Quadrant II shows a flat palm area (MP_Mpl), indicating that the focal behavior prospectively inhibits the conditioned behavior (MP_Mpl), at the same time that the conditioned behavior—i.e., the flat hand—retrospectively activates the focal behavior, that is, the action of the fingers with flexion of the MJ, PJ, and DJ. The flexed wrist position (Mca_McaF) is located in quadrant IV, as opposed to the neutral wrist position (Mca_McaN) of quadrant II, which means that the focal behavior (DS_DfxMPD) prospectively activates the conditioned behavior (Mca_McaF), at the same time that the conditioned behavior inhibits the focal, just the opposite as to what happens in quadrant II (Mca_McaN). This motor interaction of the wrist can be attributed to the fact that, in struck keystrokes, wrist flexion assists the downward movement of the fingertip prior to the attack. Its contribution diminishes upon finger-key contact and increases towards the end of the attack (Verdugo et al., 2020). Quadrant IV also shows the prospective action of lifting the arm prior to the start of the keystroke (IPT_Levprevbz), along with finger-key contact via the fingertip (DSct_Dcpt), while in quadrant III it inhibits—both prospectively and retrospectively—the behavior that characterizes the pressed touch, i.e., the start of the keystroke from contact with the surface of the key (IPT_Dcontsup).

The results show the interaction of the motor behavior of fingers with flexions MJ, PJ and DJ with a similar finger placement, a flat hand, prior elevation of the arm before the start of the keystroke, and finger-key contact via the fingertip, which are motor behaviors associated with the struck touch (Goebl et al., 2014; Furuya et al., 2010; Verdugo et al., 2020).

Participant P2. Motor interactions of the action of thumb in lateral contact (PScT_PcL) with thumb actions (P), shape of the palm area of the hand (MP), hand action (Macc), wrist position (Mca), forearm position (AtBz) and placement of the body in front of the piano (CC).

The results of the analysis of focal behavior PScT_PcL and the conditioned behaviors expressed in Table 2 for participant P2 were significant, with the exception of behaviors Mca_McaN and Mca_McaF, that were not significant, as can be observed in block 3 of Table 6. Third from the left in the corresponding vector map represented in Figure 7, we can see the behaviors with the longest vectors in each of the quadrants. Quadrant I shows the mutual activation of the hand action in the white keys area. This behavior is not visible in the vector map due to the long length of the vector (6.62), as can be seen in Table 6. Quadrant III shows the inhibition of the conditioned behaviors Macc_MaccFond, MP_Mpl, Macc_Mgirsup, P_PpsR, AtBz_AtBzA and Macc_MaccSalt. Quadrant IV shows the prospective activation of the hand action with pronated turn (Macc_Mgirpro), the thumb acting as a pivot to allow the movement of the other fingers (P_ PpivtPsDs), visible in Table 6 by the long length of its vector (6.34), hand action with the palm away from the keyboard (Macc_MaccFuer) and wrist lowered, via joint extension (Mca_McaEx), while the conditioned behavior inhibits the focal behavior. In movements carried out in the succession of descending sounds by consecutive degrees, the thumb remains fixed on the key and turns in pronation which the hand follows to reposition itself after the fingers have passed. The thumb releases the key via a lifting of the finger, when the rest of the fingers press the next key. The results show the interaction of the lateral thumb contact behavior with motor behaviors related to rotation of the thumb and the hand, together with thumb key release via elevation (Goebl et al., 2005).

Participant P2. Motor interactions of the action carried out in the musical extract of the 9th demarcation of the fragment of the Mozart sonata (ExMu_Mzt1_9) with the actions of start of keystroke (IPT), finger position (DS), finger-key surface contact (Dsct), thumb surface contact with the key (PScT), key release actions (DlibT), finger placement (DsCol), shape of the palm area of the hand (MP), hand action (Macc), wrist position (Mca), forearm position (AtBz) and the placement of the body in front of the piano (CC).

The results of the analysis of focal behavior ExMu_Mzt1_9 and the conditioned behaviors expressed in Table 2 for participant P2 were significant, with the exception of DSct_Dcpt, which were not significant, as can be seen in block 4 of Table 6. Fourth from the left in the corresponding vector map represented in Figure 7, we can see the behaviors with the longest vectors in each of the quadrants, where almost all the behaviors are presented in quadrants I and III, which are opposites (mutual activation and mutual inhibition). Thus, quadrant I shows the motor behavior of the fingers with extension of MJ, PJ and DJ (DS_DextMPD), the start of the keystroke via prior lifting of the fingers with extension of MJ (IPT_DelvextM), start of keystroke via prior lifting of the arm (IPT_Levprevbz), the action of thumb contact via the tip (PScT_PcP), the surface contact of the rest of the fingers with the DP (DSct_DcfgD), similar finger placement (DsCol_DsSim), hand action at the back of the keyboard (Macc_MaccFond), jumping hand movement (Macc_MaccSalt), flexed wrist position (Mca_McaF) and high forearm (AtBz_AtBzA). Quadrant II shows that the focal behavior inhibits the conditioned behaviors of finger position with MJ flexion and PJ and DJ extension (DS_DfxMextPD), thumb not in contact with the key surface (PScT_PNoc) and flat palm area (MP_Mpl), at the same time that the conditioned behaviors activate the focal behavior action. Quadrant III shows the inhibition of the conditioned behaviors relating to the action of the fingers not in contact with the surface of the keys (DSct_DNoc), lateral thumb contact (PSct_PcL), disparate finger placement (DsCol_DsDisp) and wrist position with joint extension (Mca_McaEx). Quadrant IV shows the prospective activation of the key release action via lifting the fingers over the keys (DLibT_Delv), while the conditioned behavior inhibits the focal behavior. As previously outlined, in the focal behavior ExMu_Mzt1_9 we can observe the motor actions involved in the playing of the syncope, where we can see the thumb tip in contact with the key until the end of the syncope. The forearm moves forward to press the “D” note of the syncope by placing the finger on the narrow part of the white key, between the two black keys, with the hand acting at the back of the keyboard. This is followed by a succession of descending sixths which are played via jumping hand movements to move from one sixth to the next. The keys are released by lifting the hand, which produces a sound discontinuation in the passage from some sounds to others.

The results show the interaction of the focal behavior with the motor behaviors of the fingers with extension MJ, PJ and DJ, thumb tip contact with the key, hand action at the back of the keyboard, with jumping hand movements, a flexed wrist position and key release via lifting the fingers and the forearm.

The results obtained with respect to frequency in the scrutiny of motor behavior occurrences produced by participants P1 and P2 clearly correspond with the polar coordinate analysis of the motor behaviors that characterize the pressed touch and the struck touch, respectively.

5 Conclusion

The aim of this study was to detect and analyze the interactions of motor behaviors that distinctively characterize pressed and struck touch in piano performance. Additionally, it aimed to show the huge potential offered by research into motor behavior in piano playing from the scientific focus of mixed methods.

Systematic observation is an ideal medium for defining the piano touch procedure and for interpreting the expressive significance of body movement, which goes beyond anatomical and biomechanical knowledge (Goebl, 2017). Observational methodology, which brings a novel perspective to artistic research (Anguera, 2010). The possibility of using computer applications both in the musical research and professional fields, opens up a pathway of innovation and improvement that unites research and practice, implying an effective transfer of knowledge and therefore the evolution towards refinement in the field of musical performance.

Lag sequential analysis was used to examine the strength of association between behaviors using HOISAN software. Polar coordinate analysis, based on adjusted residuals from lag sequential analysis, identified activating or inhibitory relationships between a behavior of interest, known as a focal behavior, and other behaviors, known as conditioned behaviors.

Through observational methodology, this study has extracted relevant defining patterns of motor behavior in piano touch procedure. The findings reveal novel interactions between motor behaviors associated with two types of touch, contributing to the existing literature on motor control in piano performance.

Thus, in the pressed touch, the flexion of the metacarpophalangeal joints (knuckles) and slight extension of the P and D joints interact with the cupped hand and the finger-key contact surface centered on the finger pads. The contact surface of the fleshy part of the finger is larger, which allows for more precise control during performance (Dalla Bella and Palmer, 2011; Goebl and Palmer, 2008; James, 2012; Robb, 2022). The cupped position of the hand interacts with the key release behavior through the sliding of the fingers, which in turn interacts with the raised forearm, facilitating the cupping of the hand while avoiding the heaviness of the arm weight by keeping it in suspension, resulting in an improvement in sound quality. The study has highlighted the importance of sliding the fingers over the keys towards the palms of the hands in a circular, chained motion that goes from the DP to the PP, and in the case of the thumb, its sliding facilitates the passage of the other fingers. Pressure comes from contact made with the keys before they are pressed. The position of the hand is high, with slightly curved fingers in alignment (Del Pueyo, 1990). The movement is controlled by suspension of the forearm, enabling sound continuity. The position of the hand is not altered by the movements over the keyboard thanks to the sliding motion of the fingers over the keys (Lee, 2005; Ott, 2004). The hand is close to the keyboard, which avoids the noise produced by the contact of the fingers with the keys. At the same time a greater economy of movement is produced since there are no big movements (Goebl et al., 2005).

In the struck touch, the extension of the metacarpophalangeal joints with flexion of the P and D joints interacts with the flat shape of the hand and the finger-key contact surface centered on the fingertip. The keystroke begins with the finger at a certain distance from the key, raised vertically via extension of the MJ before the key is pressed, going from the PP to the DP, or via elevation of the forearm, producing a noise upon contact that affects sound quality. Wrist activity is increased to stabilize the impact of the key’s inertia when struck (Verdugo et al., 2020). The finger releases the key vertically, via extension of the MJ or via elevation of the forearm, reducing the time it stays on it (Furuya et al., 2010; Goebl et al., 2014). The key makes contact with the fingertip, the contact surface of which is smaller and so, therefore, is the tactile feedback about sound control (Goebl et al., 2014). Movements across the keyboard are made via thumb, hand and forearm turns. These changes of position alter the sound continuity (Goebl et al., 2005).

The described behavioral interactions were also supported by the descriptive analysis of observed behaviors in both pianists, measured by occurrence frequency, which is relevant in body movements (Anguera, 1982). Through the percentage of response visualization, the most significant behaviors were evidenced in both participants, highlighting behaviors associated with two types of touch, pressed and struck, corresponding to participants P1 and P2, respectively.

The results obtained in this study using observational methodology have successfully addressed the established aims, delving into the analysis of the right upper limb movement in the participating professional pianists. Furthermore, the study introduces new perspectives for supporting piano teachers, showing the effectiveness of systematic observation of pianist motor behavior. Future studies could be based on a bigger sample, along with the direct observation of the whole kinematic chain of piano-playing motion.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Ethics Committee of the University of Barcelona. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. Written informed consent was not obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article because in accordance with the ethical principles and guidelines of the Ethics Committee of the University of Barcelona, the privacy and anonymity of participants in this study were preserved.

Author contributions

IS: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. MA: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. JG-V: Conceptualization, Investigation, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. JP-B: Software, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. The second author acknowledged the support of the Generalitat de Catalunya Research Group, Grup de Recerca i innovació en Dissenys (GRID) (Grant no. 2021 SGR 00718) (2022-2024). The authors gratefully acknowledge financial support received for the publication of this article. This work was funded by the University of Barcelona through the 2024 Open Access Publishing Grant Program, as part of the University’s policy to support the free dissemination of knowledge.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank José María Ramos and Alejandro Gómez for their selfless collaboration in the recording of the musical fragments performed by the participants in this work, to whom we are equally grateful for their participation. We would also like to express our gratitude to the expert participants in this work for their knowledge and expertise. Last but not least, we would also like to thank the administration of the Auditorio Manuel de Falla in Granada, where the recording took place, for their kindness and generous contribution to the development of research in the musical-artistic field.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Anguera, M. T. (1979). Observational typology. Quality and quantity. Eur. Am. J. Methodol. 13, 449–484.

Google Scholar

Anguera, M.T. (1982). Metodología de la Observación en las Ciencias Humanas. Barcelona: Ediciones Cátedra. [The methodology of observation in the human sciences. Cátedra editions]

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T. (2010). Posibilidades y relevancia de la observación sistemática por el profesional de la Psicología. Papeles del Psicólogo [Possibilities and relevance of systematic observation by Psychology professionals]. Psychol. Papers 31, 122–130.

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., Blanco-Villaseñor, A., Hernández-Mendo, A., and Losada, J. (2011). Diseños observacionales: ajuste y aplicación en psicología del deporte. Cuadernos de Psicología del Deporte, [Observational designs: adjustment and application in Sports Psychology]. Sports Psychol. Booklets 11, 63–76.

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., Blanco-Villaseñor, A., Jonsson, G. K., Losada, J. L., and Portell, M. (2019). Editorial: systematic observation: engaging researchers in the study of daily life as it is lived. Front. Psychol. 10:864. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00864

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., Blanco-Villaseñor, A., Losada, J. L., and Portell, M. (2018a). Pautas Para elaborar trabajos que utilizan la metodología observacional. Anuario de Psicología [Guidelines for the elaboration of works that use observational methodology]. Psychol. Yearb. 48, 9–17. doi: 10.1016/j.anpsic.2018.02.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., Blanco-Villaseñor, A., Losada, J. L., and Sánchez-Algarra, P. (2020). Integración de elementos cualitativos y cuantitativos en metodología observacional. Ámbitos. Rev. Int. Comun. 49, 49–70.

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., Blanco-Villaseñor, A., Losada, J. L., Sánchez-Algarra, P., and Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2018b). Revisiting the difference between mixed methods and multimethods: is it all in the name? Qual. Quant. 52, 2757–2770. doi: 10.1007/s11135-018-0700-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., Camerino, O., Castañer, M., Sánchez-Algarra, P., and Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2017). The specificity of observational studies in physical activity and sports sciences: moving forward in mixed methods research and proposals for achieving quantitative and qualitative symmetry. Front. Psychol. 8:2196. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02196

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., and Hernández-Mendo, A. (2016). Avances en estudios observacionales de Ciencias del Deporte desde los mixedmethods. Cuadernos de Psicología del Deporte [Advances in observational studies of Sports Science from mixed methods]. Sports Psychol. Booklets 16, 17–30.

Google Scholar

Bakeman, R. (1978). “Untangling streams of behavior: sequential analysis of observation data” in Observing behavior, vol. II: data collection and analysis methods. ed. G. P. Sackett (Baltimore (USA): University Park Press), 63–78.

Google Scholar

Bakeman, R., and Gottman, J. M. (1989). “Observación de la interacción. Introducción al análisis secuencial” in Observation of interaction. An introduction to sequential analysis (Madrid: Morata).

Google Scholar

Bakeman, R., and Quera, V. (2011). Sequential analysis and observational methods for the behavioral sciences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Bellman, J. (2001). Frédéric Chopin, Antoine de Kontski and the Carezzando touch. Early Music 398:407. doi: 10.1093/earlyj/XXIX.3.398

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Berman, B. (2002). Notes from the Pianist’s bench. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

Google Scholar

Bernays, M. (2013). The expression and production of piano timbre: gestural control and technique, perception and verbalisation in the context of piano performance and practice. (Doctoral Thesis). Canada: University of Montreal.

Google Scholar

Bernays, M., and Traube, C. (2013). “Expressive production of piano timbre: touch and playing techniques for timbre control in piano performance” in Proceedings of the 10th sound and music computing conference (SMC2013) (Stockholm, Sweden: KTH Royal Institute of Technology), 341–346.

Google Scholar

Bernays, M., and Traube, C. (2014). Investigating pianists' individuality in the performance of five timbral nuances through patterns of articulation, touch, dynamics, and pedaling. Front. Psychol. 5:67307. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00157

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernstein, N. (1967). The coordination and regulation of movements. London: Pergamon.

Google Scholar

Brown, S. (2000). “Promoting a healthy keyboard technique” in Medical problems of the instrumentalist musician. eds. R. Tubiana and P. Amadio (UK: Martin Dunitz Ltd), 559–7l.

Google Scholar

Caruso, G. (2016). La femme au toucher ineffable: la pianiste, pédagogue et chercheuse Marie Trautmann, épouse Jaëll. Cahiers Internationaux de Symbolisme [The woman with the ineffable touch: Marie Trautmann, pianist, pedagogue, and researcher, wife of Jaëll]. Int. J. Symb. Noteb. 143, 27–42.

Google Scholar

Cochran, W. G. (1954). Some methods for strengthening the common χ2 tests. Biometrics 10, 417–451. doi: 10.2307/3001616

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 20, 37–46. doi: 10.1177/001316446002000104

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1968). Weighted kappa: nominal scale agreement with provision for scaled disagreement of partial credit. Psychol. Bull. 70, 213–220. doi: 10.1037/h0026256

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalla Bella, S., and Palmer, C. (2011). Rate effects on timing, key velocity, and finger kinematics in piano performance. PLoS One 6:e20518. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020518

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Degrave, V., Verdugo, F., Pelletier, J., Traube, C., and Begon, M. (2020). Time history of upper-limb muscle activity during isolated piano keystrokes. J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol. 54:102459. doi: 10.1016/j.jelekin.2020.102459

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Pueyo, E. (1990). Entretiens Sur le piano et son enseignement. [Interview on the piano and its teaching]. Duculot.

Google Scholar

Dogantan-Dack, M. (2016). “In the beginning was gesture: piano touch and the phenomenology of the performing body” in New perspectives on music and gesture (London: Routledge), 243–266.

Google Scholar

Furuya, S., and Altenmüller, E. (2013). Flexibility of movement organization in piano performance. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 1–10. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00173

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Furuya, S., Altenmüller, E., Katayose, H., and Kinoshita, H. (2010). Control of multi-joint arm movements for the manipulation of touch in keystroke by expert pianists. BMC Neurosci. 11, 1–15. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-11-82

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Furuya, S., Aoki, T., Nakahara, H., and Kinoshita, H. (2012). Individual differences in the biomechanical effect of loudness and tempo on upper-limb movements during repetitive piano keystrokes. Hum. Mov. Sci. 31, 26–39. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2011.01.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Furuya, S., and Kinoshita, H. (2008). Expertise-dependent modulation of muscular and non-muscular torques in multi-joint arm movements during piano keystroke. Neuroscience 156, 390–402. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.07.028

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Furuya, S., Nakamura, A., and Nagata, N. (2014a). Acquisition of individuated finger movements through musical practice. Neuroscience 275, 444–454. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.06.031

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Furuya, S., Nakamura, A., and Nagata, N. (2014b). Extraction of practice-dependent and practice-independent finger movement patterns. Neurosci. Lett. 577, 38–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2014.06.012

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Goebl, W. (2017). “Movement and touch in piano performance” in Handbook of human motion. eds. B. Muller and S. I. Wolf (Berlin: Springer), 1–18.

Google Scholar

Goebl, W., Bresin, R., and Fujinaga, I. (2014). Perception of touch quality in piano tones. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 136, 2839–2850. doi: 10.1121/1.4896461

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Goebl, W., Bresin, R., and Galembo, A. (2004). Once again: the perception of piano touch and tone. Can touch audibly change piano sound independently of intensity. Proceedings of the International Symposium on Musical Acoustics (pp. 332–335). Nara: Japan.

Google Scholar

Goebl, W., Bresin, R., and Galembo, A. (2005). Touch and temporal behavior of grand piano actions. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 118, 1154–1165. doi: 10.1121/1.1944648

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Goebl, W., and Palmer, C. (2008). Tactile feedback and timing accuracy in piano performance. Exp. Brain Res. 186, 471–479. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1252-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Goebl, W., and Palmer, C. (2013). Temporal control and hand movement efficiency in skilled music performance. PLoS One 8:e50901. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0050901

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Guichard, C. (2004). Marie Jaëll: the magic touch, piano music by mind training : Algora Publishing.

Google Scholar

Hadjakos, A., Aitenbichler, E., and Mühlhäuser, M. (2008). Potential use of inertial measurement sensors for piano teaching systems: motion analysis of piano playing patterns. In Proceedings of the 4th i-Maestro Workshop on Technology- Enhanced Music Education, (pp. 61–68).

Google Scholar

Hernández-Mendo, A., López-López, J. A., Castellano, J., Morales-Sánchez, V., and Pastrana, J. L. (2012). Hoisan 1.2: programa informático para uso en metodología observacional. Cuadernos de Psicología del Deporte [Hoisan 1.2: computer program for use in observational methodology]. Sports Psychol. Bookl. 12, 55–78. doi: 10.4321/s1578-84232012000100006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

James, B. (2012). The art of pianism meets science, sustainable performance: use of arm weight. Aust. J. Music. Educ. 2, 92–101.

Google Scholar

James, B. (2018). Pianism: performance communication and the playing technique. Front. Psychol. 9:383083. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02125

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kay, B. A., Turvey, M. T., and Meijer, O. G. (2003). An early oscillator model: studies on the biodynamics of the piano strike (Bernstein y Popova, 1930). Motor Contr. Champ. 7, 1–45. doi: 10.1123/mcj.7.1.1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kinoshita, H., Furuya, S., Aoki, T., and Altenmüller, E. (2007). Loudness control in pianists as exemplified in keystroke force measurements on different touches. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 121, 2959–2969. doi: 10.1121/1.2717493

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kojucharov, E. (2014). Aspects of touch and timbre in piano performance. Anal. Riv. Online Studi Music. 7, 11–21.

Google Scholar

Kojucharov, E., and Rodà, A. (2015). Towards an empirically based definition of piano touch: touch/timbre relationship and key motion measurements. In Proceedings of the 9th Triennial Conference of the European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music (ESCOM2015), eds J. Ginsborg, A. Lamont, M. Phillips, and S. Bramley. Manchester, UK (pp. 516–521).

Google Scholar

Kursell, J. (2006). Piano Mécanique and piano Biologique: Nikolai Bernstein’s neurophysiological study of piano touch. ConFiguretions 14, 245–273. doi: 10.1353/con.0.0015

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Landis, J. R., and Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics 33, 159–174. doi: 10.2307/2529310

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J. (2005). A pedagogical approach to Franz Liszt's second ballade. Ann Arbor: University of Cincinnati.

Google Scholar

Li, S. (2022). “Developing timbre on the piano: interactions between sound, body, and concepts” in Sound teaching (London: Routledge), 48–60.

Google Scholar

Li, S., and Timmers, R. (2021). Teaching and learning of piano timbre through teacher–student interactions in lessons. Front. Psychol. 12:576056. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.576056

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

MacRitchie, J. (2015). The art and science behind piano touch: a review connecting multi-disciplinary literature. Music. Sci. 19, 171–190. doi: 10.1177/1029864915572813

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

MacRitchie, J., and McPherson, A. P. (2015). Integrating optical finger motion tracking with surface touch events. Front. Psychol. 6:134939. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00702

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

MacRitchie, J., and Zicari, M. (2012). The intentions of piano touch. In Proceedings of the 12th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition, 8th Triennial Conference of the European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music, (pp. 636–643). Thessaloniki, Greece: Aristotle University of Thessaloniki.

Google Scholar

Oikawa, N., Tsubota, S., Chikenji, T., Chin, G., and Aoki, M. (2011). Wrist positioning and muscle activities in the wrist extensor and flexor during piano playing. Hong Kong J. Occup. Ther. 21, 41–46. doi: 10.1016/j.hkjot.2011.06.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ortmann, O. (1925) in The physical basis of piano touch and tone: an experimental investigation of the effect of the player's touch upon the tone of the piano. ed. K. Paul (New York: EP Dutton).

Google Scholar

Ott, B. (2003). Marie Jaëll, una enigmática revelación listziana. Música y Educación [Marie Jaëll, an enigmatic listzian revelation]. Music Educ. 16, 123–148.

Google Scholar

Ott, B. (2004). La technique pianistique de Liszt, une technique occultée et trahie par le piano lui-même. [Liszt's pianistic technique, a technique obscured and betrayed by the piano itself.]. Mede. Arts Santé Artistes 48, 24–38.

Google Scholar

Robb, H. (2022). Marie Jaëll: Pioneer of musical embodiment studies. Ninet. Cent. Music 45, 220–243. doi: 10.1525/ncm.2022.45.3.220

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sackett, G. P. (1980). “Lag sequential analysis as a data reduction technique in social interaction research” in Exceptional infant. Psychosocial risks in infant-environment transactions. eds. D. B. Sawin, R. C. Hawkins, L. O. Walker, and J. H. Penticu (New York: Brunner/Mazel), 300–340.

Google Scholar

Sandor, G. (1981). On piano playing. New York and London: Schirmer Books.

Google Scholar

Schegloff, E. (2000). On granularity. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 26, 715–720. doi: 10.1146/annurev.soc.26.1.715

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Soto-Fernández, A., Camerino, O., Anguera, M. T., Iglesias, X., and Castañer, M. (2022). LINCE PLUS software for systematic observation studies of sports and health. Behav. Res. Methods 54, 1263–1271. doi: 10.3758/s13428-021-01642-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, C., Visentin, P., Oye, D., Rathwell, S., and Shan, G. (2021). Pursuing artful movement science in music performance: single subject motor analysis with two elite pianists. Percept. Mot. Skills 128, 1252–1274. doi: 10.1177/00315125211003493

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Verdugo, F., Begon, M., Gibet, S., and Wanderley, M. M. (2022). Proximal-to-distal sequences of attack and release movements of expert pianists during pressed-staccato keystrokes. J. Mot. Behav. 54, 316–326. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2021.1962237

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Verdugo, F., Pelletier, J., Michaud, B., Traube, C., and Begon, M. (2020). Effects of trunk motion, touch, and articulation on upper-limb velocities and on joint contribution to endpoint velocities during the production of loud piano tones. Front. Psychol. 11:1159. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01159

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

W (1925). Review of the physical basis of piano touch and tone: an experimental investigation of the effect of the Player’s touch upon the tone of the piano, by O. Ortmann. Music. Times 66, 421–422. doi: 10.2307/912997

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinstein-Reiman, M. (2021). Printing piano pedagogy: experimental psychology and Marie Jaëll's theory of touch. Ninet. Cent. Music Rev. 18, 427–453. doi: 10.1017/S1479409819000715

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: piano performance, mixed methods, observational methodology, pressed touch, struck touch

Citation: Santisteban IE, Anguera MT, Granda-Vera J and Pastrana-Brincones JL (2024) Analysis of motor behavior in piano performance from the mixed methods approach. Front. Psychol. 15:1433441. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1433441

Received: 17 May 2024; Accepted: 12 August 2024;
Published: 05 September 2024.

Edited by:

Andrea Schiavio, University of York, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Laura Bishop, University of Oslo, Norway
Claudia Bullerjahn, Justus-Liebig-University Giessen, Germany

Copyright © 2024 Santisteban, Anguera, Granda-Vera and Pastrana-Brincones. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Isabel E. Santisteban, isabelasantisteban@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.