Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 30 September 2024
Sec. Quantitative Psychology and Measurement

Assessing basic/fundamental psychological need fulfillment: systematic mapping and review of existing scales to foster cumulative science

  • 1Department of Human-Centered Design, Faculty of Industrial Design Engineering, Delft University of Technology, Delft, Netherlands
  • 2Department of Psychology, Faculty of Arts, University of Ljubljana, Ljubljana, Slovenia

Background: Because the fulfillment of basic/fundamental psychological needs affects people’s motivation and well-being, measuring the degree to which these needs are met is of interest to researchers across various domains. Although numerous self-assessment tools have been developed, no recent comprehensive reviews exist, hindering cumulative scientific progress. The present systematic review aimed to identify and analyze the main approaches to developing self-report scales for assessing basic/fundamental psychological need fulfillment. The objective is to inform readers interested in selecting instruments for their studies and those intending to develop new scales.

Methods: Following PRISMA, we conducted a search of Scopus, Web of Science, PubMed, and ProQuest in August 2023. The following information was extracted from eligible studies: Scale name, abbreviation, theoretical basis, application domain, final scale construction, scale development and validation methodology, and citation count.

Results: Our search identified 32 primary studies, in which 31 original scales were developed and validated, and 89 secondary studies that aimed to modify these original scales. The predominant theoretical basis was Self-Determination Theory, although eight scales were based on alternative need typologies. The scales were either domain-general or specific to contexts such as work, education, or exercise/sports contexts. While most were designed to measure need satisfaction, some also addressed need support, frustration, and thwarting.

Conclusion: Despite significant efforts in developing, adapting, and applying scales to measure need fulfillment, we found several issues resulting from diverse perspectives on conceptualizing psychological needs and need typologies, discordant approaches in developing and validating measures, and other inconsistencies that should be acknowledged and addressed in future research.

1 Introduction

Needs play a crucial role in human functioning, underpinning almost all of our daily goals and actions. For decades, numerous distinguished psychologists such as Maslow (1943), Murray (1938), and more recently Deci and Ryan (2000), have advanced the idea that humans share a common set of basic or fundamental needs, the fulfillment of which is essential for personal growth and wellbeing.

Several theories have been proposed to form need typologies, with foundational work that can be traced back to Maslow’s (1943) Hierarchy of Needs and Murray’s (1938) theory of needs. One of the most prominent frameworks is the Basic Psychological Need Theory (BPNT; Vansteenkiste et al., 2020), which is part of the broader Self-Determination Theory (SDT; Ryan and Deci, 2017). BPNT identifies three basic psychological needs: Autonomy, Competence, and Relatedness. Alongside BPNT, other notable theories include the Existence, Relatedness, and Growth (ERG) Theory (Alderfer, 1969), the Manifest Needs Theory (MNT; McClelland, 1987), the interdisciplinary Detachment-Recovery, Autonomy, Mastery, Meaning, and Affiliation (DRAMMA) model (Newman et al., 2014), and the Psychology of Working Theory (PWT; Duffy et al., 2016).

Early renditions of need systems, such as those developed by Murray (1938) and Maslow (1943), classified human needs into two categories based on how essential they were for survival. The first includes physiological (physical/biological/bodily/viscerogenic) needs, which were considered to be “primary,” “fundamental,” or “basic,” as their fulfillment is essential for survival. The second includes psychological needs, which were deemed “secondary” and prioritized after existential threats are resolved, as they contribute to thriving rather than mere survival (Welzel, 2013). Although the concept of basic bodily needs is relatively easy to grasp, delineating a basic psychological need from a non-basic one remains challenging. Consequently, identifying when and how particular psychological needs are met or unmet poses an even greater challenge.

The diversity of need typologies illustrates a current lack of consensus of what constitutes a fundamental psychological need and how this differs from non-basic needs. Over the years, various criteria have been proposed to address this issue. One of the earliest efforts was published by Baumeister and Leary (1995), comprising nine principles. Later, Ryan and Deci (2017) introduced new criteria to distinguish between basic (“growth”) from non-basic (“deficit”) psychological needs. More recently, Vansteenkiste et al. (2020) presented a more refined list of requirements, consisting of five “basic” and four “associated” criteria.

Another debate revolves around the relationship between basic psychological needs. Maslow (1943) famously proposed a hierarchical relationship, where “higher-level” needs emerge only once “lower-level” needs are satisfied. ERG similarly posits that needs can be interrelated, while Baumeister and Leary (1995) and early SDT researchers held an opposing view, proposing that needs can operated independently. Recent research, however, has indicated possible relationships among the SDT needs. For instance, studies by Baard et al. (2004) and Adie et al. (2012) found that autonomy support positively influences the fulfillment of all three SDT needs.

There are substantial differences in how psychological needs have been operationalized in psychometric scales, which in turn influences how item pools are formulated (see Table 1 for examples). One key distinction is between scales based on a uni-dimensional versus a two-dimensional model of need fulfillment. Early psychometric scales followed a unidimensional model, where a need was either fulfilled or not, measured along a single continuum. These scales focused on the “bright” side of human functioning—the extent to which needs were satisfied (e.g., Reeve and Sickenius, 1994). However, about a decade ago, a conceptual shift occurred toward a two-dimensional perspective, recognizing need frustration as distinct from low need satisfaction, representing the “dark” side of human functioning (Vansteenkiste et al., 2020). The model presented by Vansteenkiste and Ryan (2013) explains the asymmetry between these experiences: While need frustration inherently indicates the absence of need satisfaction, low need satisfaction does not necessarily imply the presence of need frustration.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Four examples of items assessing the need for competence in the context of the workplace.

A second key distinction lies between scales that measure personal experiences of need fulfillment (i.e., need satisfaction and frustration) and those that assess the external conditions for need fulfillment (i.e., need support and thwarting). The latter examines the extent to which social environments either actively promote/facilitate or actively undermine/hinder individuals’ need fulfillment (Vansteenkiste and Ryan, 2013). For example, the behavior of parents, coaches, or teachers can either support or thwart psychological needs, and larger settings like schools, workplaces, or even societies may differ in their impact (Ryan et al., 2019). However, need fulfillment is not solely determined by external conditions; even in supportive environments, individuals may not always experience satisfaction, and in need-thwarting environments, frustration is not guaranteed (Deci and Ryan, 1985b). This highlights that measures of external conditions do not always predict personal experiences of need fulfillment. A potential exception could be those needs that can only be fulfilled by other people (e.g., relatedness, the need for love and belonging).

Given these ongoing debates regarding the definition, categorization, and operationalization of basic psychological needs, various types of need-focused self-assessment tools have emerged over the past decades. Especially in recent years, the interest in assessing basic/fundamental psychological need fulfillment has captivated researchers across domains, such as work, education, sports, leisure, healthcare, design, and technology. Additionally, in the past decade, several papers have provided comprehensive guidelines for designing and validating psychometric scales, alongside the development of novel statistical tools and methods.

Despite this growing interest, we have not found recent reviews dedicated to need-based scale design and development. Elson et al. (2023) recently argued that many measures in psychology are only used once or twice, an issue they referred to as “the toothbrush problem” and deemed a serious barrier to cumulative scientific progress. As one of the main reasons for this proliferation, they identified the difficulty of finding reusable measures in the large, fragmented academic literature. To address this gap, the present study aims to systematically map and review original psychometric scales1 developed to measure psychological need fulfillment, applicable to the general population. By reviewing scales developed over the past several decades, we aim to provide valuable information for prospective users of existing psychometric scales, as well as for developers of future scales.

This manuscript aims to accomplish two primary objectives, each corresponding to a main section of the paper. The first objective (detailed in the “Results” section) is to present a comprehensive overview of published psychometric scales from 1978 to the present. This section serves as a resource for scholars and practitioners seeking existing scales for their research. Building on this overview, we identify the key factors underlying the similarities and differences among the scales, such as their theoretical foundations, scale construction methodologies, item pool generation, and scale validation processes. These factors can guide scholars in making informed decisions about which scale to use. The second objective (reported in the “Discussion” section) is to explore these key factors in greater depth. For each factor, we provide methodological considerations to be made when developing new scales. In addition, we provide practical recommendations supplemented with references to further resources that provide more detailed guidance on specific aspects of psychometric scale development.

2 Methods

2.1 Search strategy

The systematic literature search and review was independently conducted by two researchers (TK and SH) on 21 August 2023, using Scopus, Web of Science, PubMed, and ProQuest. The following keywords were used: “need*” AND either of the words “questionnaire*” OR “assess*” OR “scal*” OR “measure*” OR “tool*” OR “techniq*” in the title, AND either of the terms “psycholog* need*” OR “basic need*” OR “fundamental need*” OR “human need*,” AND “develop*” OR “valid*” in the title, abstract, or keywords. The search string was customized to align with each search engine’s format logic, and the search scope was confined to peer-reviewed scientific papers in the English language.

2.2 Study selection

Papers that did not address psychological need fulfillment of a healthy general population in a social context were excluded. This entailed disregarding studies focusing on non-human needs, the needs of people in very particular situations (such as living under extreme conditions, or with a chronic or terminal disease), and studies of technology-mediated need fulfillment. Furthermore, studies that did not develop, adapt, or validate need-related self-report questionnaires were also omitted.

The resulting compilation of records was thoroughly screened to identify the studies that developed and/or validated original scales (hereinafter referred to as “primary studies”) and to distinguish them from studies that had adapted existing versions of scales (hereinafter referred to as “secondary studies”). Figure 1 offers an overview of the search and screening process. Throughout the various stages of the study selection and review process, any disagreements between the two reviewers were addressed through discussion, culminating in a consensus, with P.M.A.D. serving as a third discussion partner to resolve any remaining disagreements.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram of literature search and study selection (template adopted from Page et al., 2021).

2.3 Data extraction and analysis

The following data were extracted from the primary studies: (1) scale name and abbreviation; (2) theoretical basis for scale development; (3) addressed states of need fulfillment; (4) application domain; (5) final construction of the scale; and study design data, including (6) item pool generation and Content analysis; (7) pilot testing; and (8) scale validation. Missing information was obtained from reliable online sources or the corresponding authors. We also recorded the citation count for all primary and secondary studies on Web of Science and Scopus on 21 August 2023.

3 Results

The systematic review identified 121 eligible studies, of which 32 were primary and 89 secondary. The primary studies described the development of 31 original scales, of which 23 were based on SDT, and the remaining eight were built on other (hereinafter referred to as “alternative”) theories. All identified original scales are presented in Table 2.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Identified original scales (ordered alphabetically) and associated primary studies.

3.1 Chronological overview

As presented in Figure 2, the earliest identified scale dated back to 1976 and was designed to evaluate need satisfaction in the context of work (Steers and Braunstein, 1976). Fifteen years later, a scale was introduced in the context of education (Williams, 1991), followed by another in the work domain (Kasser et al., 1992). The first domain-general scale emerged in 1994 (Reeve and Sickenius, 1994), and by 2003, two more were developed (Sheldon et al., 2001; Gagné, 2003). The first scales assessing need satisfaction in exercise/sports were published in 2006 (Vlachopoulos and Michailidou, 2006; Wilson et al., 2006). It is noteworthy that approximately half of all primary studies were published after 2013, indicating an increased interest in need scales over the recent decade.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Chronological overview of the reviewed 31 original scales by domain, theoretical basis, and need fulfillment state. Slanted lines between the years 1975 and 1990 indicate a non-calibrated time gap; *related studies; AFS, Activity-feeling states scales; APNSEQ, Adolescent psychological need support in exercise questionnaire; ASBPNSS, Adolescent Students’ basic psychological needs at school scale; BMPN, Balanced measure of psychological needs scale; BNSS, Basic-needs-satisfaction scale; BNSSS, Basic needs satisfaction in sport scale; BNSW-S, Basic needs satisfaction at work scale; BPNES, Basic psychological needs in exercise scale; BPNS, Basic psychological needs scale; BPNSFS, Basic psychological need satisfaction and frustration scale; BPNS-T, Basic psychological needs scale for teachers; BPNWS, Basic psychological needs at work scale; DRAMMA, DRAMMA questionnaire; ERG, Existence, relatedness, and growth needs scale; GNSF, General need satisfaction and frustration scale; IBQ, Interpersonal behaviors questionnaire; LCQ, Learning climate questionnaire; MNQ, Manifest needs questionnaire; NNSS, Novelty need satisfaction scale; NSFS, Need satisfaction and frustration scale - educational and work contexts; NSTSSPE, Need-supportive teaching style scale in physical education; PNFS-PA, Psychological need frustration scale for physical activity; PNSE, Psychological need satisfaction in exercise scale; PNSSPE, Psychological needs satisfaction scale in physical education; PNSS-S, Psychological need states in sport-scale; PNTS, Psychological need thwarting scale; SPNTPAI, Satisfaction of psychological needs through physical activity instrument; Sheldon, “Sheldon scale”; W-BNS, Work-related basic need satisfaction scale; WMF-E, Work Motivation form-employee; WNSS, Work needs satisfaction scales.

Early psychometric scales were designed to only assess the state of need satisfaction or support. In 2011, need thwarting was introduced by Bartholomew et al. (2011a, 2011b), followed by need satisfaction and frustration (Chen et al., 2015), need support and thwarting (Rocchi et al., 2017), and finally, need frustration (Chung et al., 2020). An attempt was also made to introduce the concept of need unfulfillment; however, the results from Bhavsar et al. (2020) failed to confirm this as a distinct dimension.

In addition to the primary studies, we identified 89 secondary studies designed to adapt 20 of the original questionnaires to target various domains or demographic groups, or to include additional dimensions (see Supplementary Table 1). In these cases, scales were translated, item descriptions were modified, and factors/items were removed or added. Furthermore, some researchers attempted to shorten the original scales, while others combined several existing scales to craft more comprehensive ones.

The number and diversity of subsequent studies partially reflects the application value of the 31 identified original scales. Although citation count is not necessarily a reliable indicator of research quality, it does serve as an indicator of scientific impact (Aksnes et al., 2019). Notably, the three most cited studies in our review, according to Web of Science and Scopus, were those describing the development of BNSW-S (with 1,045 and 1,142 citations, respectively; Deci et al., 2001), the BPNSFS (with 1,038 and 1,114 citations, respectively; Chen et al., 2015), and the “Sheldon scale” (with 819 and 1,069 citations, respectively; Sheldon et al., 2001).

3.2 Characteristics of 31 original scales

Characteristics of the reviewed original scales are detailed in the Supplementary Table 2 for SDT-based scales and Supplementary Table 3 for alternative theory-based scales. Key information is summarized in Table 3 below and briefly addressed in the following subsections.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Key characteristics of the identified original scales (ordered alphabetically).

3.2.1 Theoretical basis and included needs

SDT was by far the most favored theoretical basis for the development of questionnaires aimed at assessing need fulfillment, accounting for 23 of the identified studies. Alternative theories that served as bases for scale development included Murray’s theory of needs, Maslow’s Hierarchy of Needs, PWT, ERG, and the DRAMMA model, each contributing to one study. Moreover, two studies were based on both SDT and Maslow’s theory, and one study was based on a combination of various sources.

The classical SDT need trio —autonomy, relatedness, and competence (ARC)— is featured in over two-thirds of the reviewed questionnaires (21 SDT-based, two alternative theory-based). All SDT-based questionnaires include the need for autonomy, and all but two (LCQ, NSTSSPE) include the needs for competence and relatedness. The LCQ is designed to evaluate a single need (i.e., autonomy), which represents the least number of needs assessed among all the questionnaires. One SDT-based scale further decomposes the need for autonomy into its facets, including choice, internal perceived locus of causality, and volition, while another adds dependability as an additional need. Two SDT-based scales include the need for autonomy either as the sole factor or alongside other needs, such as involvement and structure.

In contrast, the needs included in alternative scales are much more diverse. Autonomy is addressed in four alternative questionnaires (BMPN, DRAMMA, MNQ, WNSS), competence in two (BMPN, WNSS), and relatedness in three (BMPN, ERG, WNSS). In addition to these, over 30 other needs are included in the alternative questionnaires, NSTSSPE, and WMF-E. The satisfaction of psychological needs through physical activity instrument was developed to evaluate the most extensive range of needs – 13 in total – among all the reviewed questionnaires. These needs encompass physical comfort, safety, social connection, esteem from others, individual esteem, learning, challenge, entertainment, novelty, creativity, mindfulness, aesthetic appreciation, and morality. It is of note that some of the alternative needs could be construed as constituents of ARC, although they were assigned a different name. A list of all included needs within the reviewed scales is provided in Table 4.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Human needs included in the reviewed scales, the number of scales including each need, and its theoretical basis.

3.2.2 Need fulfillment states across application domains

Of the 31 original questionnaires, 21 were specifically designed to assess the state of need satisfaction. The remaining scales had varied objectives, including the assessment of need satisfaction and frustration (4), need support (3), need frustration (1), need thwarting (1), and need support and thwarting (1). All the alternative questionnaires exclusively focused on assessing need satisfaction.

Six scales that targeted the work domain (BNSW-S, BPNWS, MNQ, W-BNS, WMF-E, WNSS) assessed need satisfaction. Four scales implemented in education assessed need satisfaction (ASBPNSS, BPNS-T, ERG, PNSSPE) and two assessed need support (LCQ, NSTSSPE). Four scales developed for exercise/sports settings assessed need satisfaction (BNSSS, BPNES, PNSE, satisfaction of psychological needs through physical activity instrument). Need support, thwarting, frustration, and satisfaction and frustration were each assessed by one scale (APNSEQ, PNTS, PNFS-PA, PNSS-S, respectively). Six domain-general scales focused on need satisfaction (AFS, BMPN, BNSS, BPNS, DRAMMA questionnaire, the “Sheldon scale”), two assessed need satisfaction and frustration (BPNSFS, GNSF), and one focused on need support and thwarting (IBQ). Two questionnaires were developed and validated across multiple domains: NSFS evaluates need satisfaction and frustration at work and in education, and NNSS assesses need satisfaction in both domain-general and physical education contexts.

3.2.3 Final scale construction

Regardless of their theoretical basis, most of the original scales were established on a three-factor structure (15). Seven scales were developed with six factors, three with four factors, two with five, one with 10, and one with 13 factors. Additionally, two scales adhered to a unidimensional structure. The number of items per scale range from six to 68, with the most common number being 18 items (5), followed by 12 or 15 items (4 each), and 20 or 24 items (3 each). The distribution of items across factors is uniform in 17 cases and non-uniform in 12.

Scales used both unipolar (14) and bipolar (15) response scales. Two studies only reported using a “Likert scale” without specifying the anchors. The most frequently used number of response options was 5 or 7 (each observed in 13 scales). Furthermore, three bipolar scales consisted of 6 points, and two unipolar scales consisted of 4 and 9 points, respectively. Notably, with the exception of one frequency scale and two intensity scales, all response-scales were agreement-based.

3.3 Development and validation of original scales

A comprehensive analysis of the reviewed approaches to scale development and validation is provided in the Supplementary Table 4 for SDT-based scales and Supplementary Table 5 for alternative theory-based scales. A brief summary is provided below.

3.3.1 Item pool generation and content analysis

3.3.1.1 Approach to item generation

In the creation of item pools, 19 SDT and six alternative studies used a deductive approach. This involved deriving items from existing questionnaires and need-related theories. One SDT study used an inductive approach, which was based on information gathered from a survey of the target population. Additionally, three SDT studies and two alternative studies employed a combination of deductive and inductive approaches.

3.3.1.2 Assessing content and face validity

Content validity was assessed in 15 SDT studies. In 13 of these, one to 40 experts in need psychology were consulted, while authors assessed content validity themselves in two studies. Furthermore, one study additionally used 20 non-expert members of the target population. In three studies, the Content Validity Index (CVI) was calculated to guide decisions regarding item retention or elimination (Bartholomew et al., 2011b; Liu and Chung, 2014; Chung et al., 2020). Two other studies relied on Aiken’s V, with one of them also incorporating Cohen’s effect size indices (Ng et al., 2011). The remaining studies employed less rigorous methods, such as expert-led in-person discussions.

Five alternative studies assessed content validity. For this purpose, three and six experts were engaged in two studies, one study recruited three non-expert members of the target population, while the authors assessed content validity themselves in another. The involvement of participants in content validation was not specified in one study. Notably, none of these studies used statistical approaches to assess inter-rater reliability.

Face validity was assessed in nine SDT studies. In six of these, two to 33 judges were drawn from the target population, and in three, one to 10 experts were engaged. Eight of these studies assessed both content and face validity. Additionally, three alternative studies assessed face validity, all of which also included the assessment of content validity by the same panel of judges.

3.3.2 Factor validity

3.3.2.1 Internal consistency reliability

Internal consistency reliability was assessed at various stages in nearly all studies, with only two exemptions (Sheldon et al., 2001; Gonzalez-Cutre et al., 2016). For this purpose, Cronbach’s Alpha (α) was used in 23 studies, Raykov’s Rho (ρ) in two, and McDonald’s ω total (ωt), Guttman split-half, Composite Reliability (CR), and ordinal CR in one study each. All alternative studies assessed internal consistency reliability using Cronbach’s α. Ten SDT and two alternative studies assessed internal consistency reliability on more than one sample.

3.3.2.2 Exploratory factor analysis (EFA)

EFA was performed in 10 studies, eight of which were based on SDT and two on alternative theories. Factors were extracted by means of Principal-Axis Factoring (PAF) in seven cases, twice in combination with promax rotation, twice with direct oblimin rotation, once with oblimin rotation with Kaiser normalization, and once with equamax and an unspecified oblique rotation. Maximum Likelihood estimation (ML) with promax rotation was used in three studies; in one case, Kaiser normalization was employed. Two SDT studies used an unspecified extraction method with oblique rotation. Across all studies, sample sizes for EFA ranged between 185 and 646 (median 382).

3.3.2.3 Principal-components analysis (PCA)

As an alternative to EFA, PCA was used in the exploratory phase of one alternative and three SDT studies, twice with varimax rotation, once with promax rotation, and once with direct oblimin rotation. Sample sizes ranged between 115 and 560 (median 239).

3.3.2.4 Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA)

CFA was conducted in 23 (19 SDT, four alternative) studies; in 13 of these on multiple samples. A variety of software tools were employed, including IBM SPSS Amos (7), Mplus (6), EQS (5), and Lisrel (4). ML was used in nine studies (once with bootstrapped parameter estimates), Robust ML in six, and one study reported the use of MIMIC models. Six studies did not specify the method employed for CFA. The sample sizes for CFA ranged between 81 and 1,185 (median 371), and in one study, CFA was only performed on the same sample of respondents as EFA. In nine studies, only CFA was conducted without a prior EFA or PCA.

3.3.2.5 Exploratory structural equation modeling (ESEM)

ESEM was used as an alternative to EFA and CFA in three SDT studies with robust ML; geomin rotation was opted for in two of these. All studies performed ESEM using Mplus. Sample sizes ranged between 301 and 605 (median 330).

3.3.2.6 Model fit indices for CFA and ESEM

The most frequently used model fit indices included the Comparative Fit Index (CFI; 23 studies), Root Mean-Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA; 23 studies), Chi-square (χ2; 18 studies), Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR; 17 studies), and the Non-normed Fit Index (NNFI), also referred to as the Tucker Lewis Index (TLI; 15 studies).

3.3.3 Other validity tests

Eleven SDT studies assessed criterion validity and 17 evaluated construct validity (for details, please see Supplementary Tables 4 and 5). Furthermore, 12 studies tested the invariance of the measure, with nine focusing on invariance across genders, four across ages, two across samples, and single studies assessed invariance across time, cultures, languages, social agents, grades, qualifications, workplace types, income, financial and health satisfaction, sport types, and competitive levels and experiences. Six alternative studies assessed criterion validity (four of them on multiple samples), and six construct validity. Invariance of the measure was tested in two studies; one assessed invariance across genders and ages, and the other invariance across gender, races, social classes, and income.

4 Discussion

This systematic review of measures assessing basic/fundamental psychological need fulfillment identified 31 original scales, 23 based on SDT and eight on alternative theories, including Murray’s and Maslow’s theory, PWT, ERG, and the DRAMMA model. The earliest scale dates back to 1976 and the latest was published in 2023. In addition to the classical SDT needs trio, over 30 other needs are included across the reviewed scales. Two-thirds of them were designed to assess need satisfaction, and more recent scales included both satisfaction and frustration, or support and thwarting of needs. The number of factors ranges from one to 13, but the majority of scales were established on a three-factor structure. The approach to item generation was mostly deductive and content analysis was performed using various methods. Internal consistency reliability was assessed in all but two studies, factor analyses were performed using EFA, PCA, CFA, and ESEM, and additional validation tests were performed in some studies.

Our results resonate with the concerns raised by Elson et al. (2023) for the context of basic/fundamental need fulfillment scales, including the so-called “Jingle and Jangle fallacies” (i.e., the assumption that a measure’s name is representative of its content or what it measures) and the potential for undisclosed measurement flexibility (e.g., altering the item pool, or using unique scoring strategies without fully understanding the psychometric implications). The authors highlighted the importance of revisiting previous work to better inform the interpretation of existing findings and increase psychological measures’ future reuse potential. The present review supports this call for a closer examination of how basic/fundamental psychological needs are conceptualized, how need fulfillment states are defined, and how scales are designed and validated. We address each of these topics in the following sections.

4.1 Need typologies

4.1.1 Conceptualization of basic/fundamental psychological needs

While there is a diversity of concepts addressed in the literature, most authors referred to them as “basic psychological needs.” A few studies use alternative terms like “psychological needs,” “basic needs,” “fundamental needs,” or simply “needs.” This variation highlights the absence of a consensus on the criteria used to classify needs as “basic/fundamental.” Such differences in terminology can create confusion, particularly when the same term is used in the name of scales based on different need typologies (e.g., the BNSSS-based on SDT-and BNSS-based on Maslow’s theory-, both claim to assess “basic needs,” but cover substantially different content).

The main differences between existing need typologies appear to lie in scholars’ position on the innateness, universality, and interrelatedness of basic psychological needs. SDT posits that basic psychological needs are innate, while other theories consider them to be learned (Murray, 1938; McClelland, 1985; Arogundade and Olunubi, 2013). If basic psychological needs are intrinsic, they should apply universally to all human beings, aligning with SDT’s perspective, which ascribes less importance to individual differences in need strength. Most contemporary research supports the idea that people across diverse cultures and regions share the same set of basic psychological needs (Miller, 1999; Martela and Sheldon, 2019).

4.1.2 Attempted modifications of SDT-based need typologies

Most scales based on SDT include ARC. However, continuous efforts are observed to refine this classical trio by either introducing new candidate needs, replacing existing needs, or deconstructing needs into their constituent facets. The rationale behind introducing new candidate needs stems from the observation that the frustration of ARC alone may not adequately predict individuals’ ill-being. Consequently, there have been attempts to expand the list of basic psychological needs with, e.g., the need for novelty (−variety) (Gonzalez-Cutre et al., 2016; Bagheri and Milyavskaya, 2020), beneficence (Martela and Ryan, 2016), and morality (Prentice et al., 2019).

In some cases, researchers have adapted individual elements of ARC to suit specific research purposes. For example, Haerens et al. (2013) substituted competence with structure; and Liu and Chung (Liu and Chung, 2017) replaced competence and relatedness with structure and involvement. Ng et al. (2011) took a different approach by deconstructing the need for autonomy into its three facets to cover the unique but correlated aspects of this need. Their approach aligns with a recent conjecture that facets of a single need can represent unique aspects of the need concept, adding nuance to enhance content coverage (Vansteenkiste et al., 2020).

While there have been ongoing efforts to refine need typologies, several important challenges still need to be addressed. These include the precise definition of need facets, and the associated methodologies required for their assessment. Future research aimed at adapting existing need typologies for scale development should carefully balance eligibility criteria for basic psychological needs with the intended purpose of measurement. In turn, lessons learned from these new explorations can help refine current standards and may also generate new insights for evaluation.

4.2 States of need fulfillment

4.2.1 States of need fulfillment in scale design

Before 2011, the development of measurement tools primarily focused on the “bright side” of need fulfillment (satisfaction and support). Consequently, almost all scales developed before 2015 adopted a one-dimensional structure. Need satisfaction was the first state of interest, and it remains the most extensively explored aspect to date, presumably because it has been empirically proven to predict people’s overall well-being (Bartholomew et al., 2011b). However, scholars later found that low scores on need satisfaction did not adequately capture one’s perceived state of negative experiences, stressing the importance of also measuring the “darker side” of need fulfillment (Bartholomew et al., 2011b). Sheldon and Gunz (2009) suggested the measurement of need frustration, as it was found to predict one’s motivation to fulfill unmet needs through particular activities.

In the past decade, there has been increasing attention to assessing the perceived disruptive influence of social contextual factors on need fulfillment, encompassing both need support and thwarting (Bartholomew et al., 2011b). In the current review, two studies evaluated need-supportive teaching styles or instructive behaviors as perceived by students (Williams, 1991; Liu and Chung, 2017), and one addressed both need-supportive and thwarting interpersonal behaviors (Rocchi et al., 2017). This finding is consistent with advancements in motivation research, as an increasing number of studies are delving into the multivariate environmental factors that affect one’s need fulfillment (Assor et al., 2018; Mabbe et al., 2018).

4.2.2 Need satisfaction, frustration, support, and thwarting

In our review, we observed that the terms “satisfaction/frustration” and “support/thwarting” were at times used interchangeably, which could lead to confusion (e.g., Costa et al., 2015; Bhavsar et al., 2020). To ensure clarity in research findings, it is crucial to distinguish between these technical terms, especially because the states of need fulfillment they describe can co-occur (e.g., Bartholomew et al., 2011b).

According to Vansteenkiste and Ryan (2013), need thwarting occurs at the interpersonal level, where socializing agents (e.g., caregivers, teachers, employers) are actively antagonistic toward one’s need satisfaction. In other words, need support/thwarting involves an active and direct way of fostering/undermining one’s needs by the social environment. Conversely, need satisfaction/frustration is experienced at the intrapersonal level, influenced by one’s socialization experiences and genetic factors (Vansteenkiste and Ryan, 2013); an individual’s interpretation of need satisfaction/frustration may be shaped by their personality style and internalized beliefs.

Although need support typically leads to need satisfaction and wellbeing, and need thwarting typically leads to need frustration and ill-being, cross-paths can also occur (Vansteenkiste and Ryan, 2013). For example:

“The people in my life pressure me to do things their way” (autonomy thwarting), but “I do things the way I want” (autonomy satisfaction).

“The people in my life give me the freedom to make my own choices” (autonomy support), but “I do a lot of things I do not want to do” (autonomy frustration).

“The people in my life send me the message that I am incompetent” (competence thwarting), but “I feel confident that I can do things well” (competence satisfaction).

“The people in my life tell me that I can accomplish things” (competence support), but “I feel incapable” (competence frustration).

Our review also revealed concerns regarding the accuracy of some scales in capturing the intended constructs. Specifically, some items of the reviewed questionnaires appear to combine elements of both need frustration and thwarting, despite being designed to measure one or the other. For example, the items “I feel incompetent (frustration) because of the things I am told (thwarting)” or “I doubt my ability to overcome challenges (frustration) because of the comments I receive (thwarting)” (adapted from PNFS-PA) combine two constructs in a single statement. Respondents’ ratings of such double-barreled items may also be misinterpreted (e.g., disagreement with the former item could be interpreted as “I do not feel incompetent because of the things I am told” or as “I feel incompetent for some other reason”). Based on this, a comprehensive investigation into need fulfillment may necessitate a measure that encompasses both levels: the interpersonal (need support/thwarting) to guide necessary modifications in the social environment, and the intrapersonal (need satisfaction/frustration) to inform required interventions at the individuals’ personal level.

Additionally, it is important to recognize that prior to 2011, “need frustration” was considered synonymous with “low need satisfaction,” as the two-dimensional model of need fulfillment had not yet been introduced. This historical conflation has been a major source of confusion, since early references to “need frustration” do not align with how frustration is conceptualized in the two-dimensional model. Thus, caution is required when selecting instruments and interpreting research data. This caution is relevant even for scales developed after 2011. A noteworthy example is the BPNSFS scale. The original version, described in Chen (2013), was designed to measure only need satisfaction, with 24 items across three factors (one for each BPNT need), with four positively and four negatively coded items per factor. A later version was developed to assess both satisfaction and frustration. In this revision, the 12 negatively coded items were repurposed to represent need frustration, resulting in a six-factor model (see Van Der Kaap-Deeder et al., 2020). While the BPNSFS remains a widely-used and valuable tool, it is important to recognize that its original items were not developed with the more recent conceptual distinctions in mind. Consequently, some of the new items appear to better reflect low need satisfaction than true need frustration (Murphy et al., 2023). As Murphy et al. (2023, p. 129) argue, “[…] the apparent distinction between the Satisfaction and Frustration scales is likely primarily driven by item-keying direction, not by substantive content distinguishing constructs of need satisfaction and need frustration.”

When selecting or designing scales for research on psychological needs, it is crucial to critically evaluate whether the items genuinely capture the constructs of need satisfaction and frustration. The above-described examples illustrate that researchers should avoid relying solely on item-keying or earlier scale versions without ensuring that the scales accurately reflect the distinct processes of active need thwarting and low need satisfaction.

4.2.3 Need dissatisfaction and unfulfillment

In 2011, Bartholomew et al. (2011a, 2011b) suggested that low scores on need satisfaction might reflect need dissatisfaction rather than frustration. Additionally, in 2020, Bhavsar et al. (2020) attempted to introduce a third state of need fulfillment, the so-called need unfulfillment, which they defined as “the experience of a lack of need fulfillment.”

As is the case with frustration and thwarting, the term “dissatisfaction” has sometimes been used interchangeably with the term “frustration” (Sheldon and Hilpert, 2012; Neubauer and Voss, 2018), leading to ambiguity in scale design, application, and the reporting of research results. Notably, unlike need frustration, unfulfillment results from need-indifferent interpersonal behaviors that do not actively thwart others’ needs but rather leave them overlooked or neglected (Cheon et al., 2019). Bhavsar et al. (2020) did not find empirical support for a tripartite conceptualization of need states, and Sheldon and Hilpert (Sheldon and Hilpert, 2012) suggested that the state of unfulfillment might already be included in bipolar scales designed to assess need satisfaction. To resolve this, further empirical evidence is needed to determine whether need unfulfillment genuinely impacts individuals’ wellbeing or if it merely results in disengagement, characterized by indifference or having no feelings.

4.3 Item number, scale polarity, item wording, and response format

4.3.1 Item number

Most reviewed scales featured an equal number of items across all factors. However, justification for this approach was provided in only four studies, and it appears that the decision was based on the assumption that scales need to be “balanced” in number of items, as per Sheldon and Hilpert (2012). According to Nielsen and Kreiner (2011), a balanced number of items across subscales is only required when self-assessment tests are to be scored and interpreted by the respondents themselves, in order to make scoring and interpretation less complicated and more transparent to them. Although rules of thumb exist regarding the required number of items in a scale,2 Hadžibajramović et al. (2022) pointed out that general recommendations may not always be useful, as this number depends on the complexity of the measured construct.

Thurstone (1947) stressed the importance of parsimony in psychometric scales, advocating for the simplest possible factor structure with the minimum number of items to adequately cover the construct of interest. Typically, there are two-to four-times as many items in the initial item pool as expected in the final scale set (Streiner et al., 2014; DeVellis, 2017) to ensure that the pool reflects all aspects of the construct. To reduce the number of items to the required minimum, those that measure the same aspect of a concept as other items without contributing any additional information (i.e., “redundant items”) must be excluded. Including such items produces high (>0.40) inter-item correlations (Piedmont, 2014), which can increase internal consistency reliability, but results in very low validity of the scale (Kline, 1979). In addition, items with very low inter-item correlation (<0.2) should be excluded, as they may not represent the same content domain (Piedmont, 2014). Inter-item correlation between 0.20 and 0.40 suggests that the items are reasonably homogenous and simultaneously contain sufficiently unique variance (Piedmont, 2014) - they are reasonably “similar,” while at the same time sufficiently “dissimilar” to be retained.

One approach to generating items that thoroughly cover a construct is by identifying different facets of the concept (Oosterveld, 1996), which share a common foundation, but also present unique aspects (Vansteenkiste et al., 2020). For example, the three facets of autonomy identified by Ng et al. (2011) and included in BNSSS entail perceived choice, internal perceived locus of causality, and volition; according to Bauer and McAdams (Bauer and McAdams, 2000), competence consists of four facets: Having an impact on self, others, and the environment, achieving desired goals, self-mastery, and independence; and Hackmann (2019) decomposed relatedness into five facets: Friendship, love, interpersonal dialog or sharing, connection with groups, and caring for or helping others. We can also distinguish between a giving and a receiving facet of relatedness, and need fulfillment can be experienced at different levels, such as the individual, group, societal, global, and universal level (Vansteenkiste et al., 2020). Ideally, each facet of a need would be covered by one item of the scale.

Given that the complexity of the measured construct should dictate the maximum number of items per scale (Robinson, 2018), and multidimensional psychometric instruments can measure constructs of varying complexities, as demonstrated above, predetermining the number of items and adding or removing them to reach an equal number of items per subscale possibly introduces systematic error.

4.3.2 Scale polarity

Need fulfillment, like many other psychological concepts, is conceived as having varying degrees of magnitude represented by a construct continuum (Jebb et al., 2021). This continuum can be either unipolar or bipolar, depending on what its endpoints represent (Jebb et al., 2021). In a unipolar scale, the lower pole represents the absence of the construct (e.g., no agreement), while in a bipolar scale, the lower pole represents the presence of an opposing construct (e.g., disagreement). It is important to distinguish between unipolar and bipolar response scales, as they can result in significantly different answer distributions (Höhne et al., 2021).

Our review revealed that early scales developed for measuring need satisfaction and frustration (e.g., W-BNS) treated these two states as the endpoints of a single continuum (Van den Broeck et al., 2010). However, with advancements in motivation research, the “bright” and “dark” sides of need fulfillment were recognized as distinctive experiences that should be examined and interpreted separately (Chen et al., 2016), because the lack of one (e.g., need satisfaction) does not necessarily indicate the presence of the other (e.g., need frustration; Vansteenkiste and Ryan, 2013).

4.3.3 Item wording

This review also revealed inconsistencies in the wording of scale items designed to address different need fulfillment states. For example, ten scales assessing need satisfaction included some negatively-worded items that were reverse-scored. While Sheldon and Gunz (2009) interpreted these as the “absence of satisfaction,” Van den Broeck et al. (2010) suggested that they represented “need frustration.” It is worth noting that their response scales used were uni-and bipolar, respectively. We assume that negative wording in need satisfaction and support scales was used to avoid acquiescence bias, as explicitly reported by Van den Broeck et al. (2010), although earlier studies concluded that negatively-worded items reduce the validity of responses (Schriesheim and Hill, 1981) and may introduce systematic error to a scale (Jackson et al., 1993; McPherson and Mohr, 2005). According to Jebb et al. (2021), reverse-worded items are acceptable if the construct is bipolar, but contaminate measurement if it is unipolar.

It should also be stressed that it is more appropriate to use positive descriptions of need frustration rather than negative descriptions of need satisfaction in need frustration scales (De Vaus, 2014; Longo et al., 2016). Longo et al. (2016) pointed out the problem of negative wording in W-BNS and BNSW-S, and Murphy et al. (2023) recently deemed BPNSFS invalid for use as a measure of the dual-dimension theory precisely for this reason.

In addition to negative wording, we found that some items intended to evaluate need satisfaction/dissatisfaction actually assess need support/thwarting (e.g., “There were people telling me what I had to do” in BMPN). It is of note that such items can reduce content validity, affect factor structure of the scale (Longo et al., 2016), and lead to inaccurate interpretations of measurement results.

4.3.4 Response format

The choice of response scale can importantly influence measurement results and should correspond with the continuum of the construct being measured. Approximately half of the reviewed questionnaires assessing need satisfaction and support used unipolar response scales, while the other half used bipolar scales, with four being even-numbered. It remains unclear whether the responses to these different scales can be interpreted equally. As discussed above, the lower poles of uni-and bipolar scales may not convey the same meaning. In addition, the midpoint of an odd bipolar agreement scale (“neither agree nor disagree”) and the low pole of a unipolar scale (“do not agree”) could both be interpreted as “a lack of agreement with the statement.” In the case of need satisfaction scales, this would imply a lack of need satisfaction. The efforts by Bhavsar et al. (2020) to include the state of need unfulfillment as a separate dimension may have encountered challenges precisely due to the use of a bipolar response scale for assessing all three states: Satisfaction, frustration, and unfulfillment.

Similarly, satisfaction and frustration of needs was assessed using both types of response scales, while thwarting and frustration were assessed using bipolar response scales, all composed of an odd number of response options. Here, again, we encounter the issue of midpoint interpretation: If the respondent neither agrees nor disagrees with their need being satisfied, this could be understood as “need unfulfillment;” but how should the midpoint on a frustration or thwarting scale be interpreted? This remains an open question that should be revisited in future research.

Labeling of response scales can also influence measurement outcomes. Traditional response scales are fully-labeled (i.e., each response point has a verbal anchor), but end-labeled scales (i.e., those with verbal anchors only at the endpoints) appear to be more common in contemporary psychometric scale development. Although end-labeling can simplify the selection of verbal anchors, it increases the difficulty of responding for participants (Darbyshire and McDonald, 2004). Fully-labeled response scales, on the other hand, tend to increase acquiescence but also reduce extreme responses, increase the clarity of reverse-coded items, and yield higher test–retest reliability (Weng, 2004; Weijters et al., 2010), making them generally preferable. It is also worth noting that Höhne et al. (2021) found comparability of answers on fully-and end-labeled unipolar scales, but not on bipolar scales, where full labeling was associated with positivity bias and end labeling with a middle response style. End-labeled unipolar and bipolar scales were found to be comparable (Höhne et al., 2021).

Another factor to consider is the direction of the response scales, meaning the position of the “negative”/“null” and “positive” poles. For example, the BNSS employs a 4-point unipolar response scale where the verbal anchor “a lot” is assigned to the numerical value “1” and “nothing” is assigned to “4.” Cases like this can confuse respondents because we usually associate larger numbers with larger quantities. Jebb et al. (2021) recommend aligning the response format with the polarity of the assessed continuum. For example, a 7-point scale is intuitively perceived as unipolar when numbered from 0 to 6 (with 0 at the lower pole representing “nothing” and 6 at the upper pole representing the largest value) and as bipolar when numbered from-3 to +3 (−3 implying a negative and + 3 a positive response at the lower and upper poles, respectively).3

Our review revealed that some secondary studies modified the response scale format of the primary study. For example, the original BPNSFS, which assessed need satisfaction and frustration in general, used a unipolar 5-point scale (1 = “not true at all” - 5 = “completely true”). However, subsequent versions for use in sport and physical education employed a unipolar 7-point response scale with the original end labels, and the “Romantic partners” version used a bipolar 7-point scale [0 = “totally disagree” - 6 = “totally agree” (Van Der Kaap-Deeder et al., 2020)]. In addition to the fact that the lack of agreement was represented with “1” on the unipolar scale and the opposite of agreement with “0” on the bipolar scale, we once again encounter the issue of comparability of responses at the lower poles across these different scales. Therefore, when using existing psychometric scales as templates for developing new ones, it is best to retain the response format of the original scale. If a different response scale seems more appropriate, the practical implications of changing it should be thoroughly examined.

Regarding the number of response options, the majority of reviewed questionnaires use 5 or 7 points, aligning with previous research indicating that response scales with 5–7 points produce most reliable results. Data quality substantially decreases when the number of response options exceeds 7 or drops below 5 (Givon and Shapira, 1984; Krosnick and Presser, 2009), as seen in the Satisfaction of Psychological Needs Through Physical Activity Instrument (9 points) and BNSS (4 points), respectively. Shorter scales within the 5–7-point range tend to induce less respondent frustration, thereby possibly increasing the response rate (Babakus and Mangold, 1992), whereas longer response scales lead to stronger scale direction effects (Yan et al., 2018). Boateng et al. (2018) recommend the use of 5 points for unipolar response scales and 7 points for bipolar scales. Among the 5-and 7-point scales included in this review (26 in total), only 17 adhered to this recommendation.

4.4 Reviewed domains and contexts

The specific context in which need fulfillment is experienced is one of the key points for consideration when developing a scale. The earliest scale found in this review, the MNQ, is applied to work settings. It is based on Murray’s system of needs, which is primarily a personality-based approach to study human motivation. In line with this, the MNQ was expressly created as a tool for use in organizational settings, serving as an instrument for selecting and assigning people appropriate roles.

The relatively late emergence of instruments tailored to educational contexts could be explained by a shift in focus from need satisfaction/frustration to need support/thwarting. In particular, the relationship between teachers’ instructional styles and students’ need fulfillment has been increasingly investigated since the 2000’s (Reeve and Jang, 2006) with the aim of enhancing student motivation and performance. Interestingly, domain-general scales were introduced after those developed for work and education settings. The domain of exercise/sport was the last to emerge, possibly because of the late establishment of sports psychology (i.e., 1996) and the growing interest in motivation for physical activity (Deci and Ryan, 1985a; Vallerand, 1997; Vallerand, 2007).

In addition to educational institutions, sports centers, and workplaces, it is worthwhile to examine other daily-life contexts, such as those encountered within the home environment. Hewett et al. (2017) found a significant compensatory value of the home environment for meeting needs that are under-satisfied in other areas of life. As the overall experience of need fulfillment in life often results from the interplay between segments that span various life domains, e.g., unsatisfied needs in one domain can inspire compensation in others (Petrou and Bakker, 2016; de Bloom et al., 2020), an emerging area of interest involves investigating holistic need fulfillment. This is reflected in the DRAMMA model proposed by Newman et al. (2014), which served as a basis for the reviewed DRAMMA questionnaire (de Bloom et al., 2017), as well as the newly developed Needs-based Off-job Crafting Scale (NOCS; Kujanpää et al., 2022).

4.5 Scale validation

The findings of this review suggest that, in general, more rigorous approaches were used to validate SDT-based scales as compared to those based on alternative theories. To date, several authors have published detailed guidelines for the development and validation of psychometric scales (e.g., Worthington and Whittaker, 2006; Roopa and Rani, 2012; Morgado et al., 2017; Mourougan and Sethuraman, 2017; Boateng et al., 2018; Kyriazos and Stalikas, 2018; Kishore et al., 2021). For in-depth guidance, we refer the reader to these resources. The following section provides a brief overview of scale-validation issues that stood out most during our review.

4.5.1 Content analysis

About two-thirds of the reviewed studies assessed content validity of the proposed items (considering factors such as representativeness, relevance, essentiality). Around one-third of the studies assessed face validity (considering criteria like clarity, readability, comprehensibility, appropriateness of wording for the target population).4 Interestingly, quantitative approaches to content analysis were employed only in five SDT studies, four of which used an inductive approach to generate item pools. Surprisingly, quantitative methods were never used to assess face validity.

Establishing content validity of the proposed item pool is a vital step to support the validity of a psychometric scale (Haynes et al., 1995; Yusoff, 2019a), and insufficient face validity can cause the respondents to misunderstand the items, which highlights the importance of thorough content analysis. More rigorous approaches to assess content validity include calculating the CVI based on a sufficient number of expert opinions (Yusoff, 2019a). Similarly, face validity can be established using the Face Validity Index (FVI), and the panel of judges should include a sufficient number of the target respondents (Yusoff, 2019b). Alternative methods also exist, such as the use of Aiken’s V or Cohen’s effect size indices, as demonstrated by Ng et al. (2011). Although the common practice is to only present the judge panel with a description of the measured construct and a list of candidate scale items, also including the proposed response scale in the analysis could help ensure adequate interpretability and avoid issues outlined in previous sections.

4.5.2 Internal consistency reliability

Among the psychometric properties assessed in the reviewed studies, internal consistency reliability was the most frequently reported. However, the interpretation of Cronbach’s coefficient α varied across studies, which highlights the importance of aligning interpretations with established guidelines. For example, Steers and Braunstein (1976) reported α values between 0.56 and 0.66 for three subscales of MNQ, describing these as “acceptable […], given the type of measure.” Similarly, Saeednia (2011) described the BNSS subscales as “internally consistent” with α values ranging from 0.43 to 0.65. These interpretations could benefit from further consideration of recommended thresholds. According to Ab Hamid et al. (2017), Cronbach’s α values of 0.70 or higher are generally considered indicative of good internal consistency, while values between 0.60 and 0.70 may be acceptable in exploratory research stages.

It should also be noted that a high α does not always indicate a high level of internal consistency. Very short scales, for example, often result in low α values due to their brevity, and adding more items for the same concept can increase α (Tavakol and Dennick, 2011). Williams and Woodward (1980) assumed this to be the case for MNQ, and Dreher and Mai-Dalton (1983) cautioned against using the scale in its current form. Very high Cronbach’s α values (>0.90) should, however, also be avoided, as they may suggest item redundancy (Tavakol and Dennick, 2011). For these reasons, some authors argue that Cronbach’s α cannot simply be interpreted as an index of internal consistency (Green et al., 1977; Green and Thompson, 1989; Boyle, 1991; Cortina, 1993).

4.5.3 Factor validity

Of the reviewed studies, only 14 reported performing exploratory analyses to assess factorial structure. Among these, four used PCA, which is not recommended, as it may overestimate factor loadings (Costello and Osborne, 2005; Worthington and Whittaker, 2006). The most suitable EFA method for data that severely violates multivariate normality is PAF, while ML is best for normally-distributed data (Costello and Osborne, 2005; Worthington and Whittaker, 2006).

The practice of factor retention and item deletion based on Eigenvalues, though often-used, is generally considered one of the less accurate methods and is, therefore, not recommended (Costello and Osborne, 2005). For example, Sheldon et al. (2001) encountered challenges in extracting the expected 10 factors for their well-known scale using this method. They opted to proceed, explaining: “[…] our approach does not require that all candidate needs suggested by existing theories emerge as empirically distinct […]” This example highlights the importance of considering alternative approaches when the anticipated factor structure does not emerge clearly from the data. Despite these challenges, Sheldon’s scale has been influential, contributing to the development and validation of subsequent scales for assessing need fulfillment (e.g., ERG, BMPN). Worthington and Whittaker (2006) advise that when the desired factor structure is not adequately reproduced by EFA, researchers should either adopt the solution supported by the data or revisit item generation and earlier steps in the development process.

CFA was performed in 23 studies, in nine of these without a prior exploratory analysis, and in one study using the same sample as for EFA, which conflicts with the established guidelines. We observed that ESEM was used in three more recent studies to combine EFA and CFA; this appears to be an emerging new approach to statistical testing of psychometric scales.

Most EFAs and CFAs in the reviewed studies were performed on sufficiently large respondent samples. According to Worthington and Whittaker (2006), a sample size of 300 or more respondents is generally sufficient in most cases. Sample sizes of 150–200 respondents suffice when communalities are higher than 0.50, or when there are 10:1 items per factor with factor loadings at |0.4|, and 100–150 respondents may be adequate if all communalities are 0.60 or greater or when there are at least 4:1 items per factor with factor loadings larger than |0.6|. Fewer than 100 respondents or a participant-to-item ratio below 3:1 are always considered inadequate (Worthington and Whittaker, 2006). This was the case for CFA of the DRAMMA questionnaire (85 respondents), and for one of multiple CFAs of BPNS (81 respondents) and GNSF (84 respondents). Increasing the sample size can help when item communalities are low (<0.4), when there are several cross-loading items, or when there are three items per factor (Costello and Osborne, 2005).

4.6 Referencing issues, inconsistent scale names, and abbreviations

Throughout the review process, we encountered several challenges related to referencing practices, inconsistent scale names, and abbreviations. Although these issues may seem minor, they required substantial investigative work to trace the origins of certain scales and connect subsequent studies to their primary sources. We mention them to underscore the importance of precision and consistency in referencing and naming conventions to facilitate clarity and reduce ambiguity in the field. They include:

1. Misidentification of primary studies: In some instances, primary studies were incorrectly cited, leading to difficulties in accurate data reporting. For example, in some cases the reference for BPNWS was used when referring to W-BNS (e.g., Autin et al., 2019). Misidentifications complicate the process of understanding the evolution of scales and their proper application.

2. Inconsistent questionnaire abbreviations: Another issue was the inconsistent use of abbreviations for the same questionnaire across different studies. For instance, different abbreviations were sometimes used to refer to the same questionnaire (e.g., when referring to the questionnaire developed by Gagné (2003), subsequent studies used two different abbreviations, BPNS and BNSG-S,5) leading to confusion and making it difficult to track the correct references and ensure research continuity.

4.7 Limitations

The present study attempted to systematically review the extensive body of literature on the measurement of psychological need fulfillment across key life domains and life in general. Although we aimed to be as inclusive as possible, it is conceivable that some studies may not have been identified, given the constraints of our search criteria. A deliberate choice was made to exclude scales designed to assess need fulfillment of people facing particular circumstances. Examples are migrants, refugees, people affected by natural disasters, patients with various chronic or terminal diseases, and psychiatric disorders. These exclusions were implemented as they fell outside the scope of the present review.

5 Conclusion

With this systematic review, we aimed to analyze the evolutionary paths, key characteristics, and development methodologies of 31 original psychometric scales designed to assess basic/fundamental need fulfillment. Our search returned 24 eligible primary studies based on SDT and eight primary studies based on other need typologies, complemented with 89 secondary studies. Based on these, we identified the trends and issues in existing practice, and provided suggestions for future development. We focused on several core issues, including the conceptualization of basic/fundamental psychological needs, the interpretation of need fulfillment states—including the curious case of need unfulfillment—scale construction methodologies, existing and emerging domains and contexts, and the scale validation procedures. The purpose of this review was to provide a comprehensive overview of the considerations necessary when selecting or developing need assessment scales. Consequently, our discussion is broad rather than deep, setting the stage for future reviews to explore specific issues we have identified in greater detail.

One such critical issue that warrants further investigation is the distinction between need fulfillment, frustration, support, and thwarting, as well as the significant implications these distinctions hold for accurate measurement. Our review uncovered numerous factors contributing to ambiguity and confusion in this area. While these terms represent different constructs, satisfaction/frustration and supporting/thwarting have been often used interchangeably, blurring the lines between their meanings. Additionally, the historical conflation of need frustration with low need satisfaction, the overlap of cross-paths between need support, thwarting, satisfaction, and frustration, and the use of double-barreled items in several scales further complicates the interpretation of measurement results. Added to this is the confusion surrounding the concepts of need dissatisfaction and the more recent attempt to define need unfulfillment, which has introduced additional confusion in the field. If our review highlights one key point, it is the urgent need to reduce this ambiguity and confusion in order to support more precise measurements that better capture the nuanced nature of psychological need states. This will, in turn, enhance the validity of future research and contribute to a clearer understanding of need fulfillment across various contexts. Please note that we refer to the theoretical transition from the one-dimensional to the two-dimensional model of need fulfillment solely to contextualize the reviewed scales. We neither endorse nor critique this theoretical shift, nor do we take a position on which model is superior for measurement purposes. This issue remains unresolved within the field. While we acknowledge the growing body of literature advocating for the two-dimensional model, the evidence remains inconclusive, indicating that both models may offer value depending on their context of application. Further research is required to clarify these conceptual boundaries and determine which model provides the most reliable and valid assessment of psychological need fulfillment. Given the importance of this issue, it is imperative for researchers and practitioners developing or using need measurement scales to stay informed and engage with these discussions.

On the one hand, our examination of the available evidence revealed commendable efforts in developing, adapting, and applying scales to measure various states of need fulfillment within different life domains. On the other hand, it is crucial to acknowledge the existence of varying perspectives on conceptualizing psychological needs and need typologies, as well as discordant approaches in developing and validating measures, and other inconsistencies. These disparities warrant attention, as they can introduce ambiguity in the interpretation of scales and can lead to misinterpretation of research results.

By providing an overview of existing scales designed to assess need-centered experiences, this study can serve as a resource for researchers and practitioners who seek to apply such scales in their research or practice. We intentionally avoided ranking the scales, as it is not feasible to suggest that any single scale is universally superior. Instead, each scale is tailored to specific purposes and contexts, with its own set of strengths and limitations. However, our analysis of the factors that account for the differences and similarities between these scales is intended to support informed decision-making in selecting the most appropriate tool for a given research or practice context.

In addition, this overview can serve as a resource for scholars aiming to develop new or modified scales. Given the diversity of research objectives and the absence of universally accepted procedures for scale development, it is not feasible to recommend a one-size-fits-all approach. The steps required for scale development will vary depending on the intended purpose of the scale. Moreover, many factors involved in scale construction do not have a generally accepted approach. Therefore, we recommend that researchers make informed decisions for each factor, guided by our specific recommendations and suggested literature provided in this review. Furthermore, we advise that these decisions be explicitly reported during scale development process, as transparency will enhance the reproducibility and rigor of the research.

Although there is no universally recommended procedure, two of the reviewed questionnaires stood out as examples of good practice: The IBQ (Rocchi et al., 2017) and the GNSF (Neubauer et al., 2022). These scales can serve as valuable starting points for researchers new to the field. From a scale construction point of view, the IBQ uses appropriate item wording to describe need support and thwarting, along with an appropriate response format (i.e., a 7-point unipolar response scale). Similarly, the GNSF uses appropriate item wording to describe need satisfaction and frustration, as well as an appropriate response format (i.e., a 5-point unipolar response scale). Content analysis for IBQ was performed by 10 experts; for GNSF, content was analyzed multiple times, with face validity assessed by 24 members of the target population and content validity by three experts. On the negative side, neither study utilized quantitative approaches to content analysis. For factor validation, the authors of both scales opted for CFA without a prior EFA, as they based their hypothesized six-factor models on SDT. When items are expected to load onto predetermined factors with a strong theoretical basis, this approach is justified. In addition to factor validation, convergent and discriminant validity, predictive validity, and invariance across genders were also assessed for IBQ, and convergent validity, criterion validity, and invariance across ages were assessed for GNSF.

The present systematic review also offers guidance for those intending to develop new self-report measurement tools. In essence, this work ideally serves as a foundational resource that not only maps the past but also charts a course for the future of research in this vital domain of human psychology.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

TK: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AA: Formal analysis, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing. SH: Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Writing – original draft. PD: Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by the VICI grant 453-16-009 of The Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO), Division for the Social and Behavioral Sciences, awarded to P.M.A. Desmet.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1427478/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

1. ^We use the terms “scale” and “questionnaire” interchangeably.

2. ^According to Marsh et al. (1998), three items can yield reliable solutions; and according to Costello and Osborne (2005), five or more strongly-loading items (≥0.50) indicate a solid factor.

3. ^We intentionally avoid referring to the poles as “left” and “right” due to cultural and language differences.

4. ^Although face validity is often considered an element of content validity, we distinguish between the two based on the preferred type of judges: Content validity is best established by experts in the field, and face validity by the target population.

5. ^For consistency, the present review adopted the name BPNS in line with most follow-up studies addressing this questionnaire.

References

Ab Hamid, M. R., Sami, W., and Mohmad Sidek, M. H. (2017). Discriminant validity assessment: use of Fornell and Larcker criterion versus Htmt criterion. J. Phys. Conf. Ser. 890:012163. doi: 10.1088/1742-6596/890/1/012163

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Adie, J. W., Duda, J. L., and Ntoumanis, N. (2012). Perceived coach-autonomy support, basic need satisfaction and the well-and ill-being of elite youth soccer players: a longitudinal investigation. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 13, 51–59. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2011.07.008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Aksnes, D. W., Langfeldt, L., and Wouters, P. (2019). Citations, citation indicators, and research quality: an overview of basic concepts and theories. SAGE Open 9:215824401982957. doi: 10.1177/2158244019829575

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Alderfer, C. P. (1969). An empirical test of a new theory of human needs. Organ. Behav. Hum. Perform. 4, 142–175. doi: 10.1016/0030-5073(69)90004-X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Alvarado, R. A., Wilhelmi, M. R., and Ruiz, O. B. (2021). Construct validity: basic psychological needs scale for teachers. J. Educ. Teachers Trainers 12, 1–10. doi: 10.47750/jett.2021.12.02.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Arogundade, O., and Olunubi, A. (2013). A psychological appraisal of manifest needs as predictors of job involvement among selected workers in Lagos state. Res. Psychol. Behav. Sci. 1, 61–66. doi: 10.12691/rpbs-1-5-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Assor, A., Feinberg, O., Kanat-Maymon, Y., and Kaplan, H. (2018). Reducing violence in non-controlling ways: a change program based on self determination theory. J. Exp. Educ. 86, 195–213. doi: 10.1080/00220973.2016.1277336

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Autin, K. L., Duffy, R. D., Blustein, D. L., Gensmer, N. P., Douglass, R. P., England, J. W., et al. (2019). The development and initial validation of need satisfaction scales within the psychology of working theory. J. Couns. Psychol. 66, 195–209. doi: 10.1037/cou0000323

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baard, P. P., Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (2004). Intrinsic need satisfaction: a motivational basis of performance and well-being in two work settings. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 34, 2045–2068. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2004.tb02690.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Babakus, E., and Mangold, W. G. (1992). Adapting the Servqual scale to hospital services: an empirical investigation. Health Serv. Res. 26, 767–786

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bagheri, L., and Milyavskaya, M. (2020). Novelty–variety as a candidate basic psychological need: new evidence across three studies. Motiv. Emot. 44, 32–53. doi: 10.1007/s11031-019-09807-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartholomew, K. J., Ntoumanis, N., Ryan, R. M., Bosch, J. A., and Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C. (2011a). Self-determination theory and diminished functioning: the role of interpersonal control and psychological need thwarting. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 37, 1459–1473. doi: 10.1177/0146167211413125

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartholomew, K. J., Ntoumanis, N., Ryan, R. M., and Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C. (2011b). Psychological need thwarting in the sport context: assessing the darker side of athletic experience. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 33, 75–102. doi: 10.1123/jsep.33.1.75

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bauer, J. J., and Mcadams, D. P. (2000). Competence, relatedness, and autonomy in life stories. Psychol. Inq. 11, 276–279.

Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., and Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychol. Bull. 117, 497–529. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhavsar, N., Bartholomew, K. J., Quested, E., Gucciardi, D. F., Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C., Reeve, J., et al. (2020). Measuring psychological need states in sport: theoretical considerations and a new measure. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 47:101617. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2019.101617

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boateng, G. O., Neilands, T. B., Frongillo, E. A., Melgar-Quiñonez, H. R., and Young, S. L. (2018). Best practices for developing and validating scales for health, social, and behavioral research: a primer. Front. Public Health 6:149. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2018.00149

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boyle, G. J. (1991). Does item homogeneity indicate internal consistency or item redundancy in psychometric scales? Personal. Individ. Differ. 12, 291–294. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(91)90115-R

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brien, M., Forest, J., Mageau, G. A., Boudrias, J. S., Desrumaux, P., Brunet, L., et al. (2012). The basic psychological needs at work scale: measurement invariance between Canada and France. Appl. Psychol. Health Well Being 4, 167–187. doi: 10.1111/j.1758-0854.2012.01067.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, B. (2013). The universal and culture specific character of basic psychological need satisfaction [Doctoral dissertation]. Universiteit Ghent.

Google Scholar

Chen, B., Ma, H., Qin, L.-Y., Gao, F., Chan, K.-M., Law, S.-W., et al. (2016). Recent developments and challenges of lower extremity exoskeletons. J. Orthop. Transl. 5, 26–37. doi: 10.1016/j.jot.2015.09.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, B., Vansteenkiste, M., Beyers, W., Boone, L., Deci, E. L., Van Der Kaap-Deeder, J., et al. (2015). Basic psychological need satisfaction, need frustration, and need strength across four cultures. Motiv. Emot. 39, 216–236. doi: 10.1007/s11031-014-9450-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheon, S. H., Reeve, J., Lee, Y., Ntoumanis, N., Gillet, N., Kim, B. R., et al. (2019). Expanding autonomy psychological need states from two (satisfaction, frustration) to three (dissatisfaction): a classroom-based intervention study. J. Educ. Psychol. 111, 685–702. doi: 10.1037/edu0000306

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chung, P. K., Zhong, T., Liu, J. D., Zhang, C. Q., and Wong, M. Y. C. (2020). Development and initial validation of the psychological need frustration scale for physical activity. PeerJ 8:e9210. doi: 10.7717/peerj.9210

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cortina, J. M. (1993). What is coefficient alpha? An examination of theory and applications. J. Appl. Psychol. 78, 98–104. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.78.1.98

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, S., Ntoumanis, N., and Bartholomew, K. J. (2015). Predicting the brighter and darker sides of interpersonal relationships: does psychological need thwarting matter? Motiv. Emot. 39, 11–24. doi: 10.1007/s11031-014-9427-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Costello, A. B., and Osborne, J. W. (2005). Best practices in exploratory factor analysis: four recommendations for getting the most from your analysis. Prac. Assessment Res. Eval. 10:7. doi: 10.7275/jyj1-4868

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Darbyshire, P., and Mcdonald, H. (2004). Choosing response scale labels and length: guidance for researchers and clients. Austr. J. Market Res. 12, 17–26.

Google Scholar

De Bloom, J., Syrek, C., Lamers, E., and Burkardt, S. (2017). But the memories last forever. Vacation reminiscence and recovery from job stress: a psychological needs perspective. Wirtschaftspsychologie 19, 64–78.

Google Scholar

De Bloom, J., Vaziri, H., Tay, L., and Kujanpää, M. (2020). An identity-based integrative needs model of crafting: crafting within and across life domains. J. Appl. Psychol. 105, 1423–1446. doi: 10.1037/apl0000495

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

De Vaus, D. (2014). Surveys in social research. 6th Edn. New York, NY: Routledge/Taylor and Francis Group.

Google Scholar

Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (1985a). The general causality orientations scale: self-determination in personality. J. Res. Pers. 19, 109–134. doi: 10.1016/0092-6566(85)90023-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (1985b). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York, NY: Springer New York.

Google Scholar

Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (2000). The" what" and" why" of goal pursuits: human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychol. Inq. 11, 227–268. doi: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Deci, E. L., Ryan, R. M., Gagné, M., Leone, D. R., Usunov, J., and Kornazheva, B. P. (2001). Need satisfaction, motivation, and well-being in the work organizations of a former Eastern Bloc country: a cross-cultural study of self-determination. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 27, 930–942. doi: 10.1177/0146167201278002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

DeVellis, R. F. (2017). Scale development: Theory and applications. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Google Scholar

Dreher, G. F., and Mai-Dalton, R. R. (1983). A note on the internal consistency of the manifest needs questionnaire. J. Appl. Psychol. 68, 194–196. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.68.1.194

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Duffy, R. D., Blustein, D. L., Diemer, M. A., and Autin, K. L. (2016). The psychology of working theory. J. Couns. Psychol. 63, 127–148. doi: 10.1037/cou0000140

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunton, G. F., Do, B., Crosley-Lyons, R., Naya, C. H., Hewus, M., and Kanning, M. (2023). Assessing basic and higher-level psychological needs satisfied through physical activity. Front. Psychol. 14:1023556. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1023556

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Elson, M., Hussey, I., Alsalti, T., and Arslan, R. C. (2023). Psychological measures aren’t toothbrushes. Commun. Psychol. 1:25. doi: 10.1038/s44271-024-00086-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Emm-Collison, L. G., Standage, M., and Gillison, F. B. (2016). Development and validation of the adolescent psychological need support in exercise questionnaire. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 38, 505–520. doi: 10.1123/jsep.2015-0220

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gagné, M. (2003). The role of autonomy support and autonomy orientation in prosocial behavior engagement. Motiv. Emot. 27, 199–223. doi: 10.1023/A:1025007614869

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Givon, M. M., and Shapira, Z. (1984). Response to rating scales: a theoretical model and its application to the number of categories problem. J. Mark. Res. 21, 410–419. doi: 10.1177/002224378402100406

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzalez-Cutre, D., Sicilia, A., Sierra, A. C., Ferriz, R., and Hagger, M. S. (2016). Understanding the need for novelty from the perspective of self-determination theory. Personal. Individ. Differ. 102, 159–169. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.06.036

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, S. B., Lissitz, R. W., and Mulaik, S. A. (1977). Limitations of coefficient alpha as an index of test unidimensionality. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 37, 827–838. doi: 10.1177/001316447703700403

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, S. B., and Thompson, M. S. (1989). “Structural equation modeling in clinical psychology research” in Handbook of research methods in clinical psychology. eds. M. C. Roberts and S. S. Ilardi (Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing Ltd).

Google Scholar

Hackmann, M. (2019). Need-satisfaction of the components of the self-determination theory and its reflection in psychological well-being: Exploring the fulfilment of the needs of the Sdt on basis of the life story interview. Enschede: University of Twente.

Google Scholar

Hadžibajramović, E., Schaufeli, W., and De Witte, H. (2022). Shortening of the burnout assessment tool (bat)—from 23 to 12 items using content and Rasch analysis. BMC Public Health 22:560. doi: 10.1186/s12889-022-12946-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haerens, L., Aelterman, N., Van Den Berghe, L., De Meyer, J., Soenens, B., and Vansteenkiste, M. (2013). Observing physical education teachers’ need-supportive interactions in classroom settings. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 35, 3–17. doi: 10.1123/jsep.35.1.3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haynes, S. N., Richard, D. C. S., and Kubany, E. S. (1995). Content validity in psychological assessment: a functional approach to concepts and methods. Psychol. Assess. 7, 238–247. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.7.3.238

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hewett, R., Haun, V. C., Demerouti, E., Rodríguez Sánchez, A. M., Skakon, J., and De Gieter, S. (2017). Compensating need satisfaction across life boundaries: a daily diary study. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 90, 270–279. doi: 10.1111/joop.12171

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Höhne, J. K., Krebs, D., and Kühnel, S. M. (2021). Measurement properties of completely and end labeled unipolar and bipolar scales in Likert-type questions on income (in)equality. Soc. Sci. Res. 97:102544. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2021.102544

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, P. R., Wall, T. D., Martin, R., and Davids, K. (1993). New measures of job control, cognitive demand, and production responsibility. J. Appl. Psychol. 78, 753–762. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.78.5.753

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jebb, A. T., Ng, V., and Tay, L. (2021). A review of key Likert scale development advances: 1995–2019. Front. Psychol. 12:637547. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.637547

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kasser, T., Davey, J., and Ryan, R. M. (1992). Motivation and employee-supervisor discrepancies in a psychiatric vocational rehabilitation setting. Rehabil. Psychol. 37, 175–188.

Google Scholar

Kishore, K., Jaswal, V., Kulkarni, V., and De, D. (2021). Practical guidelines to develop and evaluate a questionnaire. Indian Dermatol. Online J. 12, 266–275. doi: 10.4103/idoj.IDOJ_674_20

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kline, P. (1979). Psychometrics and psychology. London: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Krosnick, J. A., and Presser, S. (2009). “Question and questionnaire design” in Handbook of survey research. eds. J. D. Wright and P. V. Marsden. 2nd ed (San Diego, CA: Elsevier).

Google Scholar

Kujanpää, M., Syrek, C., Tay, L., Kinnunen, U., Mäkikangas, A., Shimazu, A., et al. (2022). Needs-based off-job crafting across different life domains and contexts: testing a novel conceptual and measurement approach. Front. Psychol. 13:959296. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.959296

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kyriazos, T., and Stalikas, A. (2018). Applied psychometrics: the steps of scale development and standardization process. Psychology 9, 2531–2560. doi: 10.4236/psych.2018.911145

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, J. D., and Chung, P. K. (2014). Development and initial validation of the psychological needs satisfaction scale in physical education. Meas. Phys. Educ. Exerc. Sci. 18, 101–122. doi: 10.1080/1091367X.2013.872106

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, J. D., and Chung, P. K. (2017). Factor structure and measurement invariance of the need-supportive teaching style scale for physical education. Percept. Mot. Skills 124, 864–879. doi: 10.1177/0031512517712803

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Longo, Y., Gunz, A., Curtis, G. J., and Farsides, T. (2016). Measuring need satisfaction and frustration in educational and work contexts: the need satisfaction and frustration scale (Nsfs). J. Happiness Stud. 17, 295–317. doi: 10.1007/s10902-014-9595-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mabbe, E., Soenens, B., Vansteenkiste, M., Van Der Kaap-Deeder, J., and Mouratidis, A. (2018). Day-to-day variation in autonomy-supportive and psychologically controlling parenting: the role of parents’ daily experiences of need satisfaction and need frustration. Parenting 18, 86–109. doi: 10.1080/15295192.2018.1444131

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., Hau, K. T., Balla, J. R., and Grayson, D. (1998). Is more ever too much? The number of indicators per factor in confirmatory factor analysis. Multivar. Behav. Res. 33, 181–220. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3302_1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martela, F., and Ryan, R. M. (2016). The benefits of benevolence: basic psychological needs, beneficence, and the enhancement of well-being. J. Pers. 84, 750–764. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12215

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martela, F., and Sheldon, K. M. (2019). Clarifying the concept of well-being: psychological need satisfaction as the common core connecting eudaimonic and subjective well-being. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 23, 458–474. doi: 10.1177/1089268019880886

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Maslow, A. H. (1943). A theory of human motivation. Psychol. Rev. 50, 370–396. doi: 10.1037/h0054346

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McClelland, D. C. (1985). How motives, skills, and values determine what people do. Am. Psychol. 40, 812–825. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.40.7.812

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McClelland, D. C. (1987). Human motivation. Cambridge: CUP Archive.

Google Scholar

Mcpherson, J., and Mohr, P. (2005). The role of item extremity in the emergence of keying-related factors: an exploration with the life orientation test. Psychol. Methods 10, 120–131. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.10.1.120

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, J. G. (1999). Cultural psychology: implications for basic psychological theory. Psychol. Sci. 10, 85–91. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00113

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morgado, F. F. R., Meireles, J. F. F., Neves, C. M., Amaral, A. C. S., and Ferreira, M. E. C. (2017). Scale development: ten main limitations and recommendations to improve future research practices. Psicol. Ref. Crítica 30:3. doi: 10.1186/s41155-016-0057-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mourougan, S., and Sethuraman, K. (2017). Enhancing questionnaire design, development and testing through standardized approach. IOSR J. Bus. Manag. 19, 1–8. doi: 10.9790/487X-1905010108

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, B. A., Watts, A. L., Baker, Z. G., Don, B. P., Jolink, T. A., and Algoe, S. B. (2023). The basic psychological need satisfaction and frustration scales probably do not validly measure need frustration. Psychol. Assess. 35, 127–139. doi: 10.1037/pas0001193

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, H. A. (1938). Explorations in personality. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Neubauer, A. B., Kramer, A. C., and Schmiedek, F. (2022). Assessing domain-general need fulfillment in children and adults: introducing the general need satisfaction and frustration scale. Psychol. Assess. 34, 1022–1035. doi: 10.1037/pas0001169

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Neubauer, A. B., and Voss, A. (2018). The structure of need fulfillment: separating need satisfaction and dissatisfaction on between-and within-person level. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 34, 220–228. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000326

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Newman, D. B., Tay, L., and Diener, E. (2014). Leisure and subjective well-being: a model of psychological mechanisms as mediating factors. J. Happiness Stud. 15, 555–578. doi: 10.1007/s10902-013-9435-x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ng, J. Y. Y., Lonsdale, C., and Hodge, K. (2011). The basic needs satisfaction in sport scale (Bnsss): instrument development and initial validity evidence. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 12, 257–264. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2010.10.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nielsen, T., and Kreiner, S. (2011). Reducing the item number to obtain same-length self-assessment scales: a systematic approach using result of graphical log linear Rasch modeling. J. Appl. Meas. 12, 310–323

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Oosterveld, P. (1996). Questionnaire design methods. Amsterdam: University of Amsterdam.

Google Scholar

Page, M. J., Mckenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., et al. (2021). The Prisma 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 372:n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrou, P., and Bakker, A. B. (2016). Crafting one’s leisure time in response to high job strain. Hum. Relat. 69, 507–529. doi: 10.1177/0018726715590453

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Piedmont, R. L. (2014). “Inter-item correlations” in Encyclopedia of quality of life and well-being research. ed. A. C. Michalos (Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands).

Google Scholar

Poulou, M. S., and Norwich, B. (2019). Adolescent students’ psychological needs: development of an existence, relatedness, and growth needs scale. Int. J. Sch. Educ. Psychol. 7, 75–83. doi: 10.1080/21683603.2018.1479320

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Prentice, M., Jayawickreme, E., Hawkins, A., Hartley, A., Furr, R. M., and Fleeson, W. (2019). Morality as a basic psychological need. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 10, 449–460. doi: 10.1177/1948550618772011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reeve, J., and Jang, H. (2006). What teachers say and do to support students' autonomy during a learning activity. J. Educ. Psychol. 98, 209–218. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.209

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reeve, J., and Sickenius, B. (1994). Development and validation of a brief measure of the three psychological needs underlying intrinsic motivation: the Afs scales. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 54, 506–515. doi: 10.1177/0013164494054002025

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, M. A. (2018). Using multi-item psychometric scales for research and practice in human resource management. Hum. Resour. Manag. 57, 739–750. doi: 10.1002/hrm.21852

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rocchi, M., Pelletier, L., Cheung, S., Baxter, D., and Beaudry, S. (2017). Assessing need-supportive and need-thwarting interpersonal behaviours: the interpersonal behaviours questionnaire (Ibq). Personal. Individ. Differ. 104, 423–433. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.08.034

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roopa, S., and Rani, M. (2012). Questionnaire designing for a survey. J. Indian Orthodontic Soc. 46, 273–277. doi: 10.1177/0974909820120509S

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2017). Self-determination theory: basic psychological needs in motivation, development, and wellness. London: Guilford Publications.

Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., Ryan, W. S., and Di Domenico, S. I. (2019). Effects of rewards on self-determination and intrinsic motivation: Revisiting Deci (1971). In Personality and individual differences: Revisiting the classic studies. Ed. P. J. Corr , London: Sage.

Google Scholar

Saeednia, Y. (2011). Generating a scale measuring hierarchy of basic needs. Procedia. Soc. Behav. Sci. 15, 3084–3094. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2011.04.250

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schriesheim, C. A., and Hill, K. D. (1981). Controlling acquiescence response Bias by item reversals: the effect on questionnaire validity. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 41, 1101–1114. doi: 10.1177/001316448104100420

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheldon, K. M., Elliot, A. J., Kim, Y., and Kasser, T. (2001). What is satisfying about satisfying events? Testing 10 candidate psychological needs. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 80, 325–339. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.2.325

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheldon, K. M., and Gunz, A. (2009). Psychological needs as basic motives, not just experiential requirements. J. Pers. 77, 1467–1492. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00589.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheldon, K. M., and Hilpert, J. C. (2012). The balanced measure of psychological needs (Bmpn) scale: an alternative domain general measure of need satisfaction. Motiv. Emot. 36, 439–451. doi: 10.1007/s11031-012-9279-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Steers, R. M., and Braunstein, D. N. (1976). A behaviorally-based measure of manifest needs in work settings. J. Vocat. Behav. 9, 251–266. doi: 10.1016/0001-8791(76)90083-X

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Streiner, D. L., Norman, G. R., and Cairney, J. (2014). Health measurement scales: A practical guide to their development and use. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Tavakol, M., and Dennick, R. (2011). Making sense of Cronbach's alpha. Int. J. Med. Educ. 2, 53–55. doi: 10.5116/ijme.4dfb.8dfd

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thurstone, L. L. (1947). Multiple-factor analysis: A development and expansion of the vectors of mind. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Google Scholar

Tian, L., Han, M., and Huebner, E. S. (2014). Preliminary development of the adolescent students' basic psychological needs at school scale. J. Adolesc. 37, 257–267. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2014.01.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallerand, R. J. (1997). Toward a hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. Advances in experimental social psychology. New York, NY: Elsevier.

Google Scholar

Vallerand, R. J. (2007). “A hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation for sport and physical activity” in Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in exercise and sport. eds. M. S. Hagger and N. L. D. Chatzisarantis (New York, NY: Human Kinetics).

Google Scholar

Van Den Broeck, A., Vansteenkiste, M., De Witte, H., Soenens, B., and Lens, W. (2010). Capturing autonomy, competence, and relatedness at work: construction and initial validation of the work-related basic need satisfaction scale. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 83, 981–1002. doi: 10.1348/096317909X481382

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Der Kaap-Deeder, J., Soenens, B., Ryan, R., and Vansteenkiste, M. (2020). Manual of the basic psychological need satisfaction and frustration scale (Bpnsfs). Belgium: Ghent University.

Google Scholar

Vansteenkiste, M., and Ryan, R. M. (2013). On psychological growth and vulnerability: basic psychological need satisfaction and need frustration as a unifying principle. J. Psychother. Integr. 23, 263–280. doi: 10.1037/a0032359

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vansteenkiste, M., Ryan, R. M., and Soenens, B. (2020). Basic psychological need theory: advancements, critical themes, and future directions. Motiv. Emot. 44, 1–31. doi: 10.1007/s11031-019-09818-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vlachopoulos, S. P., and Michailidou, S. (2006). Development and initial validation of a measure of autonomy, competence, and relatedness in exercise: the basic psychological needs in exercise scale. Meas. Phys. Educ. Exerc. Sci. 10, 179–201. doi: 10.1207/s15327841mpee1003_4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Weijters, B., Cabooter, E., and Schillewaert, N. (2010). The effect of rating scale format on response styles: the number of response categories and response category labels. Int. J. Res. Mark. 27, 236–247. doi: 10.1016/j.ijresmar.2010.02.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Welzel, C. (2013). Freedom rising. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Weng, L.-J. (2004). Impact of the number of response categories and anchor labels on coefficient alpha and test-retest reliability. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 64, 956–972. doi: 10.1177/0013164404268674

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, G. C. (1991). Self-determination and attrition from a weight loss program. Msc Master's thesis. Rochester, NY: University of Rochester.

Google Scholar

Williams, A. P. O., and Woodward, S. (1980). A note on Steers and Braunstein's behaviourally based measure of manifest needs. J. Occup. Psychol. 53, 219–221. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8325.1980.tb00028.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, P. M., Rogers, W. T., Rodgers, W. M., and Wild, T. C. (2006). The psychological need satisfaction in exercise scale. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 28, 231–251. doi: 10.1123/jsep.28.3.231

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Worthington, R. L., and Whittaker, T. A. (2006). Scale development research: a content analysis and recommendations for best practices. Couns. Psychol. 34, 806–838. doi: 10.1177/0011000006288127

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yan, T., Keusch, F., and He, L. (2018). The impact of question and scale characteristics on scale direction effects. Surv. Pract. 11, 1–14. doi: 10.29115/SP-2018-0008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yusoff, M. S. B. (2019a). ABC of content validation and content validity index calculation. Educ. Med. J. 11, 49–54. doi: 10.21315/eimj2019.11.2.6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yusoff, M. S. B. (2019b). Abc of response process validation and face validity index calculation. Educ. Med. J. 11, 55–61. doi: 10.21315/eimj2019.11.3.6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: psychological needs, need satisfaction, need frustration, psychometric scale development, psychometric scale validation

Citation: Kermavnar T, Avsec A, Huang S and Desmet PMA (2024) Assessing basic/fundamental psychological need fulfillment: systematic mapping and review of existing scales to foster cumulative science. Front. Psychol. 15:1427478. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1427478

Received: 03 May 2024; Accepted: 19 September 2024;
Published: 30 September 2024.

Edited by:

Gudberg K. Jonsson, University of Iceland, Iceland

Reviewed by:

Magdalena Kochanowska, Academy of Fine Arts in Warsaw, Poland
Brett Murphy, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, United States

Copyright © 2024 Kermavnar, Avsec, Huang and Desmet. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Tjaša Kermavnar, tjasa.kermavnar@outlook.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.