Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 30 July 2024
Sec. Neuropsychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Neuropsychological Testing: From Psychometrics to Clinical Neuropsychology View all 11 articles

Reliability and minimal detectable change of the Yoni task for the theory of mind assessment

  • 1IRCCS Fondazione Don Carlo Gnocchi ONLUS, Milan, Italy
  • 2Africa Unit for Transdisciplinary Health Research (AUTHeR), North-West University, Potchefstroom, South Africa

Introduction: The Theory of Mind (ToM) assessment is becoming essential to evaluate the response to a social cognition intervention and to monitor the progression of social abilities impairment in atypical conditions. In the Italian setting, the Yoni task has been recently validated in its short version (the Yoni-48 task) to evaluate ToM in the clinical setting. The present study aimed to verify the test-retest reliability and the Minimal Detectable Change (MDC) of the Yoni-48 task.

Methods: The Yoni-48 task was administered to 229 healthy adults at two evaluation sessions 3 weeks apart (mean days between sessions = 20.35 ± 1.75) by a psychologist. The test-retest reliability of the Yoni-48 task accuracy and response time was tested by the Intraclass Correlation Coefficient (ICC2,1, two-way random model, absolute agreement type). Then, the MDC95 and MDC90 were computed based on the standard error of measurement. Finally, the 95% limits of agreement were plotted (LOA plot) to visualize the difference and mean score of each pair of measurements.

Results: The total Yoni-48 task accuracy, but not the response time score, showed a high ICC (>0.80), with an MDC of 0.10. By plotting the LOA plot for the accuracy score no systematic trends were observed.

Discussion: This evidence will support the adoption of the Yoni task in longitudinal designs.

1 Introduction

Social cognition is a complex set of abilities enabling the detection and processing of social stimuli from the environment. It allows adequate social behavioral response (Frith, 2008) and successful social relationships, which are essential for physical and psychological well-being (Umberson and Montez, 2010). A core component of social cognition is the Theory of Mind (ToM) or mentalizing, the capacity to infer own and others’ mental states (i.e., emotions, beliefs, and intentions) to predict behavior (Premack and Woodruff, 1978; Wimmer and Perner, 1983). ToM has a multidimensional and multilevel nature. Especially, it consists of an affective (hot) and cognitive (cold) component, which involves the understanding of affective (emotions) and cognitive (beliefs, intentions, thoughts) mental states, respectively (Brothers and Ring, 1992; Abu-Akel and Shamay-Tsoory, 2011). Also, two different levels of complexity of ToM reasoning have been highlighted (Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2005; Kalbe et al., 2010) referring to the first-order ToM, the capacity to represent another person’s emotions/beliefs/intentions, and the second-order ToM, the ability to attribute one person’s belief about another person’s mental state (Happè, 2021).

In recent years, the assessment of ToM in the clinical setting is become essential. ToM deficits are frequently considered markers of social maladaptation linked to a broad range of developmental, psychiatric, and neurological disorders (Bora and Pantelis, 2013; Plana et al., 2014; Cotter et al., 2016). Moreover, ToM performance may serve as a marker of neural deterioration and disease progression. There is evidence that social cognitive impairment characterizes the early stage of many clinical conditions, including the early stage of dementia (Bora et al., 2015; Rossetto et al., 2020, 2022; Yi et al., 2020), and that ToM deficits get worse with the progression of the disease (Bora et al., 2015, 2016), leading to poor social and occupational functioning and reduced quality of life. For this reason, ToM measures have to be included in the neuropsychological battery to monitor the progression of neurocognitive symptoms (as suggested by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edition, American Psychiatric Association, 2013) and to customize the rehabilitation treatment strategies. In fact, social cognition rehabilitation activities may be integrated into cognitive interventions for several neurological and neurodegenerative conditions (Henry et al., 2016), given the flourishing evidence on social abilities impairment in these populations. Finally, ToM measures may be adopted to assess the response to a social cognition intervention. Specific rehabilitation programs targeted to enhance social cognition abilities have been implemented and proposed for people with neuropsychiatric and neurological diseases, such as schizophrenia (d'Arma et al., 2021), traumatic brain injury (Togher et al., 2023), and Multiple Sclerosis (d'Arma et al., 2023). However, few ToM measures have been tested for longitudinal evaluations and to be adopted in rehabilitation settings. In fact, in this context, some psychometric properties, such as the test-retest reproducibility evidence and the estimation of score responsiveness, such as the minimal detectable change, are needed for a good interpretation of the rehabilitation trajectories and responses. Finally, changes in ToM competencies are frequently assessed longitudinally through long and time-consuming composite batteries that attempt to understand the complex nature of the construct.

The Yoni task (Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2007) has been recently validated and standardized in its 48-item short version (the Yoni-48 task) (Isernia et al., 2022a,b) for widespread use in clinical settings, also for a longitudinal approach. The advantage of this test is the multidimensional and multi-level assessment of ToM by evaluating separately cognitive and affective domains, and first- and second-order mental states attribution. Moreover, it was conceived as a digital measure (Koo and Vizer, 2019): it is administered in a computerized way, allowing the simultaneous collection of both accuracy and response time scores. Especially, each item is scored based on a corrected/uncorrected answer and on the seconds taken to answer. Importantly, the Yoni task consists of visual stimuli minimizing the influence of language, memory, and executive function on the subject’s performance. Moreover, the adoption of the Yoni task in the assessment of ToM in the clinical population has been supported by previous studies. Especially, it has been demonstrated to effectively detect ToM difficulties in localized brain lesions conditions (Abu-Akel and Shamay-Tsoory, 2011), schizophrenia, Parkinson’s Disease, and Mild Cognitive Impairment (Rossetto et al., 2018). Based on this previous evidence, the Yoni task is suggested to be suitable for the clinical setting, such as for supporting neuropsychological assessment.

However, a study testing the reproducibility over time of the Yoni task is needed to provide further proof of reliability. Importantly, an estimate of the Minimal Detectable Change (MDC), that is the minimal magnitude of change beyond which the change is real rather than a random measurement error, is needed for the adoption of the tool in the longitudinal contexts. The MCD is commonly computed for measures of motor functions, which are widely adopted for the monitoring of the performance after a rehabilitation program (e.g., Watson and Petrie, 2010; Lee et al., 2013; Palmer et al., 2017; Negrete et al., 2021). However, it is rarely estimated for cognitive measures (Blackwood et al., 2021; Webb et al., 2022; Chiu et al., 2023), and, to our knowledge, it has been never computed for social cognition tools.

The present study aimed to verify the reproducibility of the Yoni-48 task by estimating the test-retest reliability and the MDC value.

2 Materials and methods

This is a prospective study conducted from November 2022 to December 2023 at the IRCCS Don Gnocchi Foundation (Milan, Italy). The research has been reviewed and approved by the Don Gnocchi Foundation Ethics Committee.

2.1 Participants

Participants were recruited from the university courses (students of Professional Education; Psychology; Nurse; Psychomotricity) and the staff (technical staff; health professionals; interns) of the IRCCS Don Gnocchi Foundation, Santa Maria Nascente Center of Milan (Italy). Inclusion criteria considered to enroll participants was age > 18. Also, the following exclusion criteria were considered as well: (i) presence of neurological and/or psychiatric conditions; (ii) presence of visual and hearing disability able to affect the performance of the task; (iii) presence of pharmacological therapy affecting the evaluation session.

Participation in the study was voluntary and subjects did not receive pecuniary compensation for their involvement in the research.

2.2 Measures

2.2.1 The Yoni-48 task

The Yoni task is a computerized measure of ToM originally developed by Shamay-Tsoory and Aharon-Peretz (2007). The task is composed of visual static stimuli, in which a face (“Yoni”) appears at the center of the screen, surrounded by 4 elements (fruits, characters, animals…). For each stimulus, based on a written instruction on the top of the screen, the subject is invited to click on the element Yoni refers to, having not more than 60 s maximum per item. Therefore, the subjects are required to infer cognitive (cognitive ToM items: e.g., “Yoni is thinking of…”) and affective (affective ToM items: e.g., “Yoni loves…”) mental states of Yoni. The gaze direction and the facial expression of Yoni are informative cues to choose the right answer. Also, control stimuli are included in which the subject is invited to perform a physical inference (control items: e.g., “Yoni is close to…”). Moreover, stimuli show two levels of ToM recursive thinking, assessing first- (e.g., “Yoni is thinking of …”) and second-order ToM (e.g., “Yoni is thinking about the fruit that … wants”), respectively. In this study, the Italian version of the task (48-item; Isernia et al., 2022a,b) was administered. This version is constituted of 42 ToM and 6 control items. The ToM items are divided into 21 affective and 21 cognitive ToM; 16 first- and 26 second-order ToM items. The accuracy and response time scores have been separately computed based on Italian scoring instructions and adjusted for demographic variables, such as sex, age, and education (Isernia et al., 2022b). The following composite scores have been calculated: accuracy composite score (ACC, range 0–1), and response time composite score (RT, range 0–1).

2.3 Procedure

The Yoni task was administered at two evaluation sessions (test and retest sessions) three weeks apart (mean days between sessions = 20.35 ± 1.75) by a psychologist. The evaluation sessions were conducted in the same setting using the same technological device to perform the task (Figure 1). Within the test session, participant demographics were also collected.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The study procedure.

2.4 Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was performed using IBM SPSS software (version 28.0) and R (version 4.1.2). Descriptive statistics (frequencies, means, medians, standard errors, and standard deviations) were reported to detail the demographics of the participants group and their performance in the Yoni task at the test and retest sessions.

Before reliability analyses, outliers were identified considering the Yoni task performance under 2 standard deviations from the norm (Isernia et al., 2022b) (see Figure 2). Then, to observe the extent of the Yoni task score fluctuation (practice effect) between the test and re-test session, the effect size (point-biserial correlation coefficient, rpbs) of paired-sample comparison (Wilcoxon rank test) between test and retest performance was extracted. Also, the correlation between test-retest ∆ change and the mean of the two assessments (Massessment) was run, and the 95% limits of agreement were plotted (LOA plot, Bland and Altman, 1986) to visualize the difference and mean score of each pair of measurements.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Flow chart of the study.

The repeatability of the Yoni task accuracy and response time was tested by the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC2, 1, two-way random model, absolute agreement type). An ICC score ≥ 0.80, 0.79–0.60, and <0.59 was interpreted as a high, moderate, and poor agreement, respectively.

Then, the minimal detectable change (MDC) value of the Yoni task scores was computed as agreement parameters to be used to determine consistent improvement or decrement in the ToM ability, net to the measure oscillations. To this purpose, the standard error of measurement (SEM), the MDC95, and MDC90 were calculated using the following formula:

S E M = S D all testing score x 1 I C C
M D C 95 = 1.96 x 2 x S E M
M D C 90 = 1.65 x 2 x S E M

Then, the amount of random measurement error (MCD95%; MCD90%) was computed by dividing MCD95/MCD90 by the maximum score and multiplying it by 100.

As additional analyses, to confirm the validity and inter-item reliability of the Yoni task, internal consistency (Cronbach α), split-half reliability, Pearson reliability, mean infit and outfit were computed at T1 and T2. Moreover, construct validity was assessed at T1 using a confirmatory factor analysis. Firstly, the factorial scores representing the construct have been computed separately considering the affective and cognitive items. Afterward, the Spearman correlations were reported to portray the association between the item and the factor score. The cfa function of the R lavaan package was used to perform the confirmatory factor analysis and, the option ordered = TRUE was used to consider the categorical nature of the items (Rosseel, 2012).

3 Results

3.1 Participants

A total of 229 healthy adults took part in the research. Among these, 215 participants attended both the test and retest sessions (Table 1). Four people were identified as outliers and were excluded from the analysis since they reported a Yoni task performance far from the norm (z score ≤ 2 sd of the normative population; Isernia et al., 2022b). In total, 211 participants were included in the analyses [53 males, mean age = 25.53 ± 9.24; mean education (y) = 13.99 ± 2.11]. Figure 2 depicts the flow chart of the study.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographics of the participants in the study.

3.2 The Yoni task performance in the test and retest sessions

Tables 2 and 3 show the performance of participants at the Yoni task in the test and retest sessions. Both the accuracy and the response time scores were high in the test session and tended to increase in the retest session (see Figure 3). The Wilcoxon W test reported a statistically significant difference between the two sessions’ performance in all scores except for the first-order and cognitive accuracy scores. The effect size (rpbs) suggested a slight practice effect in the accuracy performance and a moderate effect in the response time.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Comparison between the Yoni task accuracy in test and retest sessions.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Comparison between the Yoni task response time in test and retest sessions.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Delta change trend of ACC and RT scores.

By plotting the test-retest ∆ change against the mean score of the assessments (Bland–Altman plot, Figure 4) for ACC and RT scores, no systematic trends were observed.

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Bland-Altman plot of differences in scores against the mean scores of the two assessments.

3.3 The Yoni task reliability

3.3.1 Repeatability results

Table 4 reports the ICC values of the Yoni task scores. Results suggested a good reliability of all the accuracy scores except for the ToM first-order score. Specifically, the ToM total (ACC), second-order, and cognitive accuracy scores showed a high repeatability (ICC > 0.80), while the ToM affective score revealed a moderate test-retest reliability. The ToM first-order score, instead, showed poor repeatability (ICC < 0.59). Concerning the response time scores, we observed poor reliability (ICC < 0.59) both in the total response time score (RT) and sub-scores (Table 5).

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Reliability and minimal detectable change of the Yoni-48 task accuracy scores.

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Reliability and minimal detectable change of the Yoni-48 task RT scores.

3.3.2 Agreement parameters

The MDC values of accuracy and response time scores suggested an acceptable-to-excellent random measurement error (Tables 4, 5). Especially, the accuracy total score, which reported also high repeatability, showed an MDC% equal to 11.48 for a 95% confidence level, and equal to 9.66 for a 90% confidence level. In particular, a fluctuation >/< 0.10 in the ACC score can be interpreted as a consistent improvement/decrement in the ToM performance.

3.3.3 Inter-item reliability, item discrimination ability and construct validity

To further explore reliability, the Yoni task internal consistency, split-half reliability, and item discrimination ability were explored at T1 and T2. The Yoni task showed a high inter-item reliability at both times: an internal consistency Cronbach’s α = 0.80 at T1 and T2, and a good median split-half reliability in both times (T1: ϱSP = 0.81 and a 95% HDI = 0.73–0.86; T2: ϱSP = 0.81 and a 95% HDI = 0.72–0.86). Also, the dichotomous Rash model analysis revealed a Pearson reliability of the test equal to 0.613 at T1 and 0.55 at T2. Finally, the items showed a mean infit equal to 0.93 ± 0.27 at T1 and 0.93 ± 0.28 at T2, and a mean outfit equal to 0.99 ± 0.75 at T1 and 1.06 ± 0.89 at T2. Construct validity was confirmed by the confirmatory factor analysis: ϱ Spearman’s correlation coefficient reported significant associations between the affective ToM and cognitive ToM latent factors and affective and cognitive items, respectively (see Supplementary Table S1). Three items reported weak/absent associations with the latent factor: items 8, 13, and 38.

4 Discussion

The Yoni-48 task has been proposed as a digital tool for the assessment of ToM, which has been recently validated for the Italian population (Isernia et al., 2022b) and may be suitable to be adopted as an outcome measure in social cognition interventions. Its digital administration complies with the recent advantage of digital neuropsychology (Bilder, 2011), allowing the norm-based administration of test batteries via computers, tablets and mobiles (Koo and Vizer, 2019). Especially, the Yoni task has been conceived as a computerized tool, able to facilitate agile data recording and scoring.

The present study tested the reproducibility of the Yoni-48 task to evaluate its reliability for the assessment of social cognition in longitudinal contexts, such as in the pre- and post-evaluation of rehabilitation and intervention programs.

First, the accuracy score (ACC) of the Yoni task showed good test-retest reliability, demonstrating high stability over time and minimal learning effects. To date, only a few studies investigated the test-retest reliability of ToM measures, reporting mixed results. In this regard, the Yoni-48 task revealed a higher reproducibility than other ToM tools. Especially, it showed slightly higher reliability than ToM measures already estimated as highly reproducible (Yeh et al., 2021), such as the Hinting Task (Corcoran et al., 1995) and the Faux-pas test (Stone et al., 1998, 2003). Also, the Yoni-48 task reliability was far greater than other widely used social cognition tools, such as the False Beliefs test and the Story tests (Chen et al., 2017). Based on the study of Altschuler and Faja (2022), the Yoni-48 task reliability outperformed also the second-order false belief test (Muris et al., 1999) and the Social Attribution Task (Klin, 2000), which demonstrated good reproducibility in a cohort of 7 to 11 year-old children within autism spectrum disorder. Finally, the stability over time of the Yoni-48 task was equally good as the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test, as reported by Vellante et al. (2013), which is one of the most used ToM tests in the Italian context.

By considering the Yoni-48 sub-scores, we observed different levels of test-retest reliability. Especially, the second-order ToM score showed a higher stability than the first-order score. This result might be related to the greater sensitivity of the second-order items than the first-order ones, as suggested by previous works (Isernia et al., 2022a,b). Also, although both cognitive and affective ToM scores were fairly stable, the cognitive ToM score showed a higher reliability. This result may be likely explained by the major relevance of visual cues in the affective than cognitive ToM items, which required subjects to capture the affective mental states based on the facial expressions and could be more influenced by visual processing and related habituation effects (Breiter et al., 1996; Pirastru et al., 2023).

Although the accuracy score of the Yoni-48 task has been found to be reliable, the response time score (RT) did not reach acceptable stability. In fact, our findings suggested that the RT score was affected by the learning effect and increased over time. This result was expected and may be related to the familiarity with the stimuli modality and the task instructions, which influenced the subjects’ processing speed (Balas et al., 2007). Globally, this evidence is suggestive of the reliability of the Yoni-48 task and its application as a reliable ToM measure in longitudinal design studies by considering the accuracy and not the response time score. Especially, the global accuracy score (ACC) would be used in future studies to monitor ToM ability. Although we found a high reproducibility of the second-order and cognitive ToM accuracy score, focusing on only one sub-score (such as cognitive ToM and not affective ToM) may be avoided unless under a strict theory-driven hypothesis.

After exploring the reliability of the test, the minimal detectable change was estimated to obtain a measure of the minimal magnitude change of the tool. This value will be useful to capture significant variations in the ToM performance that may not be associated with the measurement error (de Vet et al., 2006). Especially, our findings indicated that an oscillation of 0.11 points in the ACC score should be interpreted as an informative change and may suggest a significant increment/decrement of the performance over time. This datum will be considered as a reference point for the ToM monitoring, training, and rehabilitation.

This study is not without limitations. Our participants were healthy young adults with a high ToM ability. Future studies may include people with ToM difficulties to give clues about the reliability of the Yoni-48 task in clinical populations (e.g., schizophrenia, autism spectrum disorder, neurological conditions). Also, further description of the demographic characteristics of the participants, such as the specific work activity, marital status, and ethnicity, should have been collected, as well as subclinical conditions such as depression and autism spectrum symptoms to test the impact of these variables on the Yoni task performance. Moreover, our participants’ group was composed of a higher rate of females than males, and gender differences were not considered. Finally, our results on minimal detectable change (ACC score change of 0.11) may be interpreted solely as a reference point to capture Yoni-48 real changes and not clinically meaningful changes. For this latter purpose, future research may include a measure of the health status and estimate the minimal clinically important difference in a clinical population target.

5 Conclusion

In conclusion, to our knowledge, this is the first study that estimated the test-retest reliability of the Yoni task and computed the minimal detectable change for a ToM measure. This evidence will support future studies on social cognition trainings and will sustain the interpretation of the Yoni task scores in longitudinal designs.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the Don Gnocchi Ethics Committee. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

SI: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft. DC: Data curation, Writing – original draft. FR: Data curation, Writing – review & editing. CR: Methodology, Writing – review & editing. FB: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by the Italian Ministry of Health (Ricerca Corrente 2022-2024).

Acknowledgments

The authors thanks all the participants who took part in the research.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1412560/full#supplementary-material

References

Abu-Akel, A., and Shamay-Tsoory, S. (2011). Neuroanatomical and neurochemical bases of theory of mind. Neuropsychologia 49, 2971–2984. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.07.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Altschuler, M. R., and Faja, S. (2022). Brief report: test-retest reliability of cognitive, affective, and spontaneous theory of mind tasks among school-aged children with autism Spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 52, 1890–1895. doi: 10.1007/s10803-021-05040-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM V). Washington, DC, USA: American Psychiatric.

Google Scholar

Balas, B., Cox, D., and Conwell, E. (2007). The effect of real-world personal familiarity on the speed of face information processing. PLoS One 2:e1223. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001223

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bilder, R. M. (2011). Neuropsychology 3.0: evidence-based science and practice. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 17, 7–13. doi: 10.1017/s1355617710001396

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blackwood, J., Rybicki, K., and Huang, M. (2021). Cognitive measures in older cancer survivors: an examination of validity, reliability, and minimal detectable change. J. Geriatr. Oncol. 12, 146–151. doi: 10.1016/j.jgo.2020.06.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bland, J. M., and Altman, D. G. (1986). Statistical methods for assessing agreement between two methods of clinical measurement. Lancet 1, 307–310

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bora, E., and Pantelis, C. (2013). Theory of mind impairments in first-episode psychosis, individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis and in first-degree relatives of schizophrenia: systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophr. Res. 144, 31–36. https://10.1016/j.schres.2012.12.013. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.12.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bora, E., Velakoulis, D., and Walterfang, M. (2016). Meta-analysis of facial emotion recognition in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia: comparison with Alzheimer disease and healthy controls. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 29, 205–211. doi: 10.1177/0891988716640375

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bora, E., Walterfang, M., and Velakoulis, D. (2015). Theory of mind in behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer's disease: a meta-analysis. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 86, 714–719. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2014-309445

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Breiter, H. C., Etcoff, N. L., Whalen, P. J., Kennedy, W. A., Rauch, S. L., Buckner, R. L., et al. (1996). Response and habituation of the human amygdala during visual processing of facial expression. Neuron 17, 875–887. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80219-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brothers, L., and Ring, B. (1992). A neuroethological framework for the representation of minds. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 4, 107–118. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1992.4.2.107

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, K. W., Lee, S. C., Chiang, H. Y., Syu, Y. C., Yu, X. X., and Hsieh, C. L. (2017). Psychometric properties of three measures assessing advanced theory of mind: evidence from people with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 257, 490–496. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.08.026

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiu, E. C., Wang, Y. C., Huang, S. L., Hsueh, I. P., Chiang, H. Y., and Hsieh, C. L. (2023). Test-retest reliabilities and minimal detectable changes of 5 versions of the Alzheimer's disease assessment scale-cognitive subscale in people with dementia. Disabil. Rehabil. 45, 1398–1404. doi: 10.1080/09638288.2022.2060334

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Corcoran, R., Mercer, G., and Frith, C. D. (1995). Schizophrenia, symptomatology and social inference: investigating "theory of mind" in people with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 17, 5–13. doi: 10.1016/0920-9964(95)00024-g

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cotter, J., Firth, J., Enzinger, C., Kontopantelis, E., Yung, A. R., Elliott, R., et al. (2016). Social cognition in multiple sclerosis: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurology 87, 1727–1736. https://10.1212/WNL.0000000000003236. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000003236

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

d'Arma, A., Isernia, S., Di Tella, S., Rovaris, M., Valle, A., Baglio, F., et al. (2021). Social cognition training for enhancing affective and cognitive theory of mind in schizophrenia: a systematic review and a Meta-analysis. J. Psychol. 155, 26–58. doi: 10.1080/00223980.2020.1818671

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

d'Arma, A., Valle, A., Massaro, D., Baglio, G., Isernia, S., Di Tella, S., et al. (2023). A cultural training for the improvement of cognitive and affective theory of mind in people with multiple sclerosis: a pilot randomized controlled study. Front. Psychol. 14:1198018. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1198018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

de Vet, H. C., Terwee, C. B., Ostelo, R. W., Beckerman, H., Knol, D. L., and Bouter, L. M. (2006). Minimal changes in health status questionnaires: distinction between minimally detectable change and minimally important change. Health Qual. Life Outcomes 4:54. doi: 10.1186/1477-7525-4-54

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Frith, C. D. (2008). Social cognition. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 363, 2033–2039. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Happè, F. (2021). “Attributions (first order/second order)” in Encyclopedia of Autism Spectrum Disorders, 394–395.

Google Scholar

Henry, J. D., von Hippel, W., Molenberghs, P., Lee, T., and Sachdev, P. S. (2016). Clinical assessment of social cognitive function in neurological disorders. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 12, 28–39. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2015.229

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Isernia, S., Rossetto, F., Blasi, V., Massaro, D., Castelli, I., Ricci, C., et al. (2022a). Measuring cognitive and affective theory of mind with the Italian Yoni task: normative data and short versions. Curr. Psychol. 42, 23519–23530. doi: 10.1007/s12144-022-03457-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Isernia, S., Rossetto, F., Shamay-Tsoory, S., Marchetti, A., and Baglio, F. (2022b). Standardization and normative data of the 48-item Yoni short version for the assessment of theory of mind in typical and atypical conditions. Front. Aging Neurosci. 14:1048599. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.1048599

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalbe, E., Schlegel, M., Sack, A. T., Nowak, D. A., Dafotakis, M., Bangard, C., et al. (2010). Dissociating cognitive from affective theory of mind: a TMS study. Cortex 46, 769–780. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.07.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klin, A. (2000). Attributing social meaning to ambiguous visual stimuli in higher-functioning autism and Asperger syndrome: the social attribution task. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 41, 831–846. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00671

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koo, B. M., and Vizer, L. M. (2019). Mobile Technology for Cognitive Assessment of older adults: a scoping review. Innov. Aging 3:igy038. doi: 10.1093/geroni/igy038

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, P., Liu, C. H., Fan, C. W., Lu, C. P., Lu, W. S., and Hsieh, C. L. (2013). The test-retest reliability and the minimal detectable change of the Purdue pegboard test in schizophrenia. J. Formos. Med. Assoc. 112, 332–337. doi: 10.1016/j.jfma.2012.02.023

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Muris, P., Steerneman, P., Meesters, C., Merckelbach, H., Horselenberg, R., van den Hogen, T., et al. (1999). The TOM test: a new instrument for assessing theory of mind in normal children and children with pervasive developmental disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 29, 67–80. doi: 10.1023/a:1025922717020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Negrete, R., Simms, S., Gross, J., Nunes Rabello, L., Hixon, M., Zeini, I. M., et al. (2021). The test re-test reliability of a novel single leg hop test (T-Drill hop test). Int. J. Sports Phys. Ther. 16, 724–731. doi: 10.26603/001c.23677

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Palmer, S., Manns, S., Cramp, F., Lewis, R., and Clark, E. M. (2017). Test-retest reliability and smallest detectable change of the Bristol impact of hypermobility (BIoH) questionnaire. Musculoskelet. Sci. Pract. 32, 64–69. doi: 10.1016/j.msksp.2017.08.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pirastru, A., Di Tella, S., Cazzoli, M., Esposito, F., Baselli, G., Baglio, F., et al. (2023). The impact of emotional valence and stimulus habituation on fMRI signal reliability during emotion generation. NeuroImage 284:120457. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2023.120457

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Plana, I., Lavoie, M. A., Battaglia, M., and Achim, A. M. (2014). A meta-analysis and scoping review of social cognition performance in social phobia, posttraumatic stress disorder and other anxiety disorders. J. Anxiety Disord. 28, 169–177. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.09.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Premack, D., and Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav. Brain Sci. 1, 515–526. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00076512

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosseel, Y. (2012). lavaan: a brief user’s guide. Index of/yrosseel/lavaan.

Google Scholar

Rossetto, F., Baglio, F., Massaro, D., Alberoni, M., Nemni, R., Marchetti, A., et al. (2020). Social cognition in rehabilitation context: different evolution of affective and cognitive theory of mind in mild cognitive impairment. Behav. Neurol. 2020, 5204927–5204929. doi: 10.1155/2020/5204927

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossetto, F., Castelli, I., Baglio, F., Massaro, D., Alberoni, M., Nemni, R., et al. (2018). Cognitive and affective theory of mind in mild cognitive impairment and Parkinson's disease: preliminary evidence from the Italian version of the Yoni task. Dev. Neuropsychol. 43, 764–780. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2018.1529175

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossetto, F., Isernia, S., Cabinio, M., Pirastru, A., Blasi, V., and Baglio, F. (2022). Affective theory of mind as a residual ability to preserve mentalizing in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: a 12-months longitudinal study. Front. Neurol. 13:1060699. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2022.1060699

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., and Aharon-Peretz, J. (2007). Dissociable prefrontal networks for cognitive and affective theory of mind: a lesion study. Neuropsychologia 45, 3054–3067. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.05.021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Aharon-Peretz, J., and Levkovitz, Y. (2007). The neuroanatomical basis of affective mentalizing in schizophrenia: comparison of patients with schizophrenia and patients with localized prefrontal lesions. Schizophr. Res. 90, 274–283. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.09.020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Tomer, R., Berger, B. D., Goldsher, D., and Aharon-Peretz, J. (2005). Impaired "affective theory of mind" is associated with right ventromedial prefrontal damage. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 18, 55–67. doi: 10.1097/01.wnn.0000152228.90129.99

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., Calder, A., Keane, J., and Young, A. (2003). Acquired theory of mind impairments in individuals with bilateral amygdala lesions. Neuropsychologia 41, 209–220. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00151-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., and Knight, R. T. (1998). Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 640–656. doi: 10.1162/089892998562942

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Togher, L., Douglas, J., Turkstra, L. S., Welch-West, P., Janzen, S., Harnett, A., et al. (2023). INCOG 2.0 guidelines for cognitive rehabilitation following traumatic brain injury, part IV: cognitive-communication and social cognition disorders. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 38, 65–82. doi: 10.1097/HTR.0000000000000835

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Umberson, D., and Montez, J. K. (2010). Social relationships and health: a flashpoint for health policy. J. Health Soc. Behav. 51, S54–S66. doi: 10.1177/0022146510383501

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vellante, M., Baron-Cohen, S., Melis, M., Marrone, M., Petretto, D. R., Masala, C., et al. (2013). The "Reading the mind in the eyes" test: systematic review of psychometric properties and a validation study in Italy. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 18, 326–354. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2012.721728

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, P. F., and Petrie, A. (2010). Method agreement analysis: a review of correct methodology. Theriogenology 73, 1167–1179. doi: 10.1016/j.theriogenology.2010.01.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Webb, K. L., Ryan, J., Wolfe, R., Woods, R. L., Shah, R. C., Murray, A. M., et al. (2022). Test-retest reliability and minimal detectable change of four cognitive tests in community-dwelling older adults. J. Alzheimers Dis. 87, 1683–1693. doi: 10.3233/jad-215564

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wimmer, H., and Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition 13, 103–128. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeh, Y. C., Lin, C. Y., Li, P. C., Hung, C. F., Cheng, C. H., Kuo, M. H., et al. (2021). A systematic review of the current measures of theory of mind in adults with schizophrenia. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:7172. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18137172

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yi, Z., Zhao, P., Zhang, H., Shi, Y., Shi, H., Zhong, J., et al. (2020). Theory of mind in Alzheimer's disease and amnestic mild cognitive impairment: a meta-analysis. Neurol. Sci. 41, 1027–1039. doi: 10.1007/s10072-019-04215-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: social cognition, mentalizing, test-retest, reliability, rehabilitation

Citation: Isernia S, Cacciatore DM, Rossetto F, Ricci C and Baglio F (2024) Reliability and minimal detectable change of the Yoni task for the theory of mind assessment. Front. Psychol. 15:1412560. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1412560

Received: 05 April 2024; Accepted: 18 July 2024;
Published: 30 July 2024.

Edited by:

Alessio Facchin, Magna Graecia University, Italy

Reviewed by:

Elena Cavallini, University of Pavia, Italy
Antonia Meyer, University Hospital of Basel, Switzerland

Copyright © 2024 Isernia, Cacciatore, Rossetto, Ricci and Baglio. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Federica Rossetto, frossetto@dongnocchi.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.