Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 12 July 2024
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic From Safety to Sense of Safety View all 9 articles

Pre- and post-pandemic risk perceptions and worries

  • Department of Psychosocial Sciences, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway

Introduction: The present study is a cross-sectional investigation of worry and risk perceptions regarding various hazards and destinations, measured before, during, and after the COVID-19 pandemic.

Methods: Questionnaire data were collected from tourists in Norway during the summer seasons of 2012 (N = 2,669), 2019 (N = 1,666), and 2022 (N = 956), and from a representative sample of Norwegians in 2020 (N = 1,003).

Results: The results show a general decline in the level of worry and risk perceptions post-pandemic compared to those pre-pandemic, with the exception of infectious diseases, whose perceived risk slightly increased in 2022.

Conclusion: The results highlight the importance of employing cross-sectional or longitudinal data to investigate changes in risk perceptions over time. The findings also indicate that pessimistic predictions of a continued decline in tourism appear to be unwarranted.

Introduction

The COVID-19 pandemic has severely impacted travel and tourism, especially during periods of lockdowns and travel restrictions (UNWTO, 2023). Since the start of the pandemic, research has focused on understanding how risk perceptions regarding COVID-19 may influence tourist behavior and travel choices. Reported findings show that increased COVID-19 risk perceptions negatively affect travel intentions (Yoo et al., 2022; Golets et al., 2023; Fuchs et al., 2024). Moreover, tourists may have become more cautious by avoiding destinations perceived as high risk for COVID-19 (e.g., Orîndaru et al., 2021; Jeczmyk et al., 2023), and overall, travel is perceived as riskier during the pandemic (Villacé-Molinero et al., 2021; Susanti et al., 2023). Research also shows changes in tourist preferences due to COVID-19 risk perceptions. Hence, interest in certain destinations increased during the pandemic, while interest in other destinations decreased. For example, interest in nature-based tourism increased while interest in city-based tourism decreased (Huang et al., 2021; Hardt and Glückstad, 2024). While these results are interesting, they are all based on retrospective reports of perceived risk obtained during and after the pandemic. Various cognitive biases can influence risk perceptions that are assessed retrospectively, i.e., risk perceptions may appear elevated only after the hazard has occurred (Wolff et al., 2019). However, comparisons of before and after measures of the same hazard often show no increase in risk perceptions. To our knowledge, no research comparing risk perceptions and worries obtained before the pandemic to those obtained during and after the pandemic is available. This study is unique in that it aims to address this gap.

Materials and methods

Data were collected through surveys conducted among tourists visiting Norway during the summer seasons of 2012, 2019, and 2022, with participants hailing from over 70 different countries. Additionally, in 2020, when data collection among tourists was impossible due to the pandemic, we obtained data from a representative sample of the Norwegian population through an online survey (see Table 1 for sample demographics).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample demographics.

Risk perceptions for various hazards were assessed in 2012 and 2022 using the following questions, followed by a list of hazards: How risky do you consider the trip you are on now to be concerningterrorism or actions of war/food poisoning/infections or infectious diseases (such as for example SARS or HIV)/traffic accidents/violence or other forms of criminality/suffering accidents/petty crime/All in all, how risky do you judge this trip to be?

Risk perceptions for various destinations were measured in 2019 and 2022 using the following question: If you were to visit the following destinations as a tourist, how risky would you judge them to be? Consider each destination and rate it in relation to the general risk of unwanted events. followed by a list of these destinations: Norway and the Nordic countries/Germany, Austria, or Switzerland/Visiting larger cities in the USA/Roundtrip to Israel/Roundtrip to Turkey/Visiting larger cities in Europe/Musical and shopping in London/Roundtrip to the USA/Cultural trips to Spain. Hazards and destinations were rated on a 7-point Likert-type scale, with 1 indicating not risky and 7 indicating very risky.

Tourist worries were assessed in 2019 and 2022 using the Tourist Worry Scale in samples of tourists, and in 2020, this factor was assessed within a representative Norwegian sample (Larsen et al., 2009; Wolff and Larsen, 2013).

Results

Regression analyses were conducted to assess the predictive power of age, female gender, organized group travel, and year of data collection on risk perceptions for various hazards and destinations. The results showed a significant decline in risk perceptions for all hazards from 2012 to 2022, except for the perceived risk of infectious diseases, which increased significantly (see Table 2). Additionally, risk perceptions for six out of nine tourist destinations decreased, while they remained unchanged for three destinations from 2019 to 2022 (see Table 3). A MANCOVA was used to assess whether scores on the Tourist Worry Scale varied depending on the year of data collection while controlling for differences in age and gender distribution across the samples. The results also showed that scores peaked in 2020 but were significantly lower in 2022 than before the pandemic in 2019 and during the pandemic in 2020 (see Table 4).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Regression analyses predicting risk perceptions for various hazards.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Regression analyses predicting risk perceptions for various destinations.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. MANCOVA comparing mean values on the tourist worry scale for various years, controlling for age and gender distribution differences.

Discussion

The comparison of pre-and post-pandemic risk perceptions for various hazards, destinations, and worries shows decreased scores in 2022, the first year after travel restrictions were lifted in many parts of the world, including most of Europe. The only exception to this trend is the perceived risk of infectious diseases, which was rated higher in 2022 than in previous years. The data also show that worries peaked during the pandemic in 2020. However, these data were collected from a representative sample of the Norwegian population who were not traveling at the time of data collection. Previous research has found that scores on the Tourist Worry Scale tend to be elevated in non-traveling samples compared to traveling ones (Larsen et al., 2009; Wolff and Larsen, 2017). The authors attribute this to the impact bias, a cognitive bias where individuals tend to overestimate the intensity of future emotions (Wilson and Gilbert, 2003). Therefore, the elevated worry scores observed in 2020 should be considered in the context of this bias.

The results are in line with earlier findings (Larsen et al., 2011; Wolff and Larsen, 2014, 2016), showing that increased risk perceptions and worries following dramatic events like natural disasters or terrorism are short-lived and limited to the affected destination. Risk perceptions might decrease in special circumstances even after a crisis (Wolff and Larsen, 2014, 2017, 2021). The findings are also in line with those of Grillo et al. (2023), who found robust risk habituation in COVID-19 risk perceptions in a longitudinal investigation. These authors found that risk perceptions decreased over time, even though objective threat indicators showed increased risk. This highlights the importance of longitudinal and cross-sectional risk measures to document changes in perceived risk. Research in the tourism domain appears to be dominated by studies that rely on people's memories for pre-pandemic risk assessment (Wolff et al., 2019). However, memories are likely flawed by rosy retrospection (Mitchell et al., 1997), which refers to people's tendency to rate events retrospectively more positively than they did during their occurrence. For example, respondents rated the world as safer in the past, while continuous and prospective measures of perceived risk remain constant (Brun et al., 2011; Wolff and Larsen, 2014).

It is also interesting to note that the level of risk perceptions and worries is not only “back to normal” in 2022 but is, in fact, lower than pre-pandemic. Such a decrease could be explained by the relief experienced following the pandemic or by mechanisms like the gambler's fallacy (see Wolff and Larsen, 2014, 2017, 2021). However, the observed decrease could partly be explained by a selection bias. People who choose to travel in 2022, almost immediately after the lifting of travel restrictions, might be the ones who worry the least and have the lowest risk perceptions. However, the number of international tourists arriving in Norway in 2022 was only 16% down from 2019 (UNWTO, 2023), and the composition of the pre and post-pandemic samples is very similar (see Table 1).

The findings are also in line with those of Larsen et al. (2011), who claimed that tourists do not judge the risk of various hazards independently of each other. They observed that, while risk perceptions for certain forms of travel increased, they decreased for other types of holiday. In the present data, risk perceptions for infectious diseases are increased. However, all other hazards are rated as less risky.

Naturally, the present data have their limitations in that the respondents were not asked about COVID-19 specifically. It was also not possible to conclude that the observed decline in risk perceptions and worries was caused by the pandemic. However, the fact that measures were obtained before and after the onset of the pandemic circumvents another problem that flaws the data collected post-pandemic only, e.g., rosy retrospection.

Another limitation concerns the fact that the samples differ slightly in some characteristics. While the analysis did control for possible effects of age, gender, and travel mode, there are also some variations in the accommodations utilized. Most notably, cruise tourists are underrepresented in the post-pandemic sample, which is surprising as cruise arrivals to Bergen increased in 2022 compared to the pandemic and pre-pandemic years (kystdatahuset, 2024). This discrepancy is, therefore, most likely an artifact of the data collection.

Finally, one might argue that, even though data were collected in a city (i.e., Bergen) in Norway, the country itself could be considered a destination low in COVID-19 risk. Therefore, it is uncertain whether the results can be generalized to destinations high in COVID-19 risk.

Conclusion

In summary, our findings show a decrease in risk perceptions after the COVID-19 pandemic compared to before the pandemic. The results are in line with previous research showing that increases in risk perceptions following dramatic events are short-lived and limited. Findings are also in line with human nature in that people adapt to hazards and show decreased risk perceptions even when the objective risk remains constant or even increases (e.g., Grillo et al., 2023). This allows for the optimistic (or pessimistic) supposition that, as with earlier crises, travel behavior will return to pre-pandemic levels rather quickly. Our findings also highlight the importance of not relying solely on after-measures. Future research should examine whether risk perceptions display similar patterns in destinations perceived as higher risk than Norway.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent from the patients/participants or patients/participants' legal guardian/next of kin was not required to participate in this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.

Author contributions

KW: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. SL: Conceptualization, Investigation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This research was funded by the University of Bergen.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Brun, W., Wolff, K., and Larsen, S. (2011). Tourist worries after terrorist attacks: report from a field experiment. Scand. J. Hosp. Tour. 11, 387–394. doi: 10.1080/15022250.2011.593365

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuchs, G., Efrat-Treister, D., and Westphal, M. (2024). When, where, and with whom during crisis: the effect of risk perceptions and psychological distance on travel intentions. Tour. Manag. 100:104809. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2023.104809

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Golets, A., Farias, J., Pilati, R., and Costa, H. (2023). COVID-19 pandemic and tourism: the impact of health risk perception and intolerance of uncertainty on travel intentions. Curr. Psychol. 42, 2500–2513. doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-02282-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grillo, T. L., Henderson, T., and Ward, A. F. (2023). Habituation to threats in times of crisis: lessons from the temporal trajectory of risk perceptions throughout COVID-19. Res. Square [preprint]. doi: 10.21203/rs.3.rs-3320706/v1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardt, D., and Glückstad, F. K. (2024). A social media analysis of travel preferences and attitudes, before and during Covid-19. Tour. Manag. 100:104821. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2023.104821

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, S. S., Shao, Y., Zeng, Y., Liu, X., and Li, Z. (2021). Impacts of COVID-19 on Chinese nationals' tourism preferences. Tour. Manag. Perspect. 40:100895. doi: 10.1016/j.tmp.2021.100895

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeczmyk, A., Uglis, J., Zawadka, J., Pietrzak-Zawadka, J., Wojcieszak-Zbierska, M. M., and Kozera-Kowalska, M. (2023). Impact of COVID-19 pandemic on tourist travel risk perception and travel behaviour: a case study of Poland. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 20:5545. doi: 10.3390/ijerph20085545

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

kystdatahuset (2024). Havneinformasjon: Cruiseankomster. Available online at: https://kystdatahuset.no/tallogstatistikk/cruise/dashboard

Google Scholar

Larsen, S., Brun, W., and Øgaard, T. (2009). What tourists worry about–Construction of a scale measuring tourist worries. Tour. Manag. 30, 260–265. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2008.06.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Larsen, S., Brun, W., Øgaard, T., and Selstad, L. (2011). Effects of sudden and dramatic events on travel desire and risk judgments. Scand. J. Hosp. Tour. 11, 268–285. doi: 10.1080/15022250.2011.593360

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, T. R., Thompson, L., Peterson, E., and Cronk, R. (1997). Temporal adjustments in the evaluation of events: the “rosy view.” J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 33, 421–448. doi: 10.1006/jesp.1997.1333

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Orîndaru, A., Popescu, M. F., Alexoaei, A. P., Căescu, Ș. C., Florescu, M. S., and Orzan, A. O. (2021). Tourism in a post-COVID-19 era: sustainable strategies for industry's recovery. Sustainability 13:6781. doi: 10.3390/su13126781

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Susanti, C. E., Hermanto, Y. B., and Suwito, B. (2023). The effect of tourist destination image (TDI) on intention to visit through tourism risk perception (TRP) of COVID-19 in the tourism industry in the new normal era in Indonesia: case study in East Java. J. Risk Finan. Manag. 16:76. doi: 10.3390/jrfm16020076

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

UNWTO (2023). International Tourism and Covid-19. Available online at: https://www.unwto.org/tourism-data/international-tourism-and-covid-19 (accessed April 27, 2023).

Google Scholar

Villacé-Molinero, T., Fernández-Muñoz, J. J., Orea-Giner, A., and Fuentes-Moraleda, L. (2021). Understanding the new post-COVID-19 risk scenario: outlooks and challenges for a new era of tourism. Tour. Manag. 86:104324. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2021.104324

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, T. D., and Gilbert, D. T. (2003). Affective forecasting. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 35, 345–411. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(03)01006-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolff, K., and Larsen, S. (2013). Tourist worries–Here and now vs. there and then: the effect of item wording in the Tourist Worry Scale. Tour. Manag. 35, 284–287. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2012.09.013

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolff, K., and Larsen, S. (2014). Can terrorism make us feel safer? Risk perceptions and worries before and after the July 22nd attacks. Ann. Tour. Res. 44, 200–209. doi: 10.1016/j.annals.2013.10.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolff, K., and Larsen, S. (2016). Flux and permanence of risk perceptions: tourists' perception of the relative and absolute risk for various destinations. Scand. J. Psychol. 57, 584–590. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12326

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolff, K., and Larsen, S. (2017). A taxonomy of terror-about the effect of different kinds of terror on risk perceptions. Scand. J. Hosp. Tour. 17, 111–128. doi: 10.1080/15022250.2015.1137487

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolff, K., and Larsen, S. (2021). Frequency and randomness predict changes in perceived risk. Ann. Tour. Res. 89:103075. doi: 10.1016/j.annals.2020.103075

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolff, K., Larsen, S., and Øgaard, T. (2019). How to define and measure risk perceptions. Ann. Tour. Res. 79:102759. doi: 10.1016/j.annals.2019.102759

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoo, J. W., Park, J., Lee, J. H., and Park, H. (2022). Recovering from the COVID-19 shock: the role of risk perception and perceived effectiveness of protective measures on travel intention during the pandemic. Serv. Bus. 16, 557–580. doi: 10.1007/s11628-021-00476-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Tourist Worry Scale, COVID-19, risk perceptions, pre- and post monitoring, travel and tourism

Citation: Wolff K and Larsen S (2024) Pre- and post-pandemic risk perceptions and worries. Front. Psychol. 15:1412252. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1412252

Received: 04 April 2024; Accepted: 19 June 2024;
Published: 12 July 2024.

Edited by:

Anne Birgitta Pessi, University of Helsinki, Finland

Reviewed by:

Vladimir Cvetković, University of Belgrade, Serbia
Nathaniel James Siebert Ashby, Harrisburg University of Science and Technology, United States

Copyright © 2024 Wolff and Larsen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Katharina Wolff, katharina.wolff@uib.no

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.