Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 18 November 2024
Sec. Decision Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Exploration of Decision Neuroscience Research in the Digital Era View all 3 articles

Naturalistic generative narratives reveal effects of social characteristics on decision-making

  • 1Department of Neurosurgery, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
  • 2Northeastern University, Boston, MA, United States
  • 3Health Sciences and Technology, Harvard-MIT, Boston, MA, United States
  • 4Program in Neuroscience, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
  • 5Department of Neurobiology, German Primate Center, Göttingen, Germany

Introduction: The social characteristics of others can powerfully influence our decisions. They can also be broadly impacted by the social context in which these choices are made, making the effects of these characteristics on decision-making especially challenging to understand.

Methods: Here, we developed a Generative Narrative Survey that provided participants with naturalistic scenarios that richly varied in social context and theme but that also systematically varied the characteristics of the social agents involved, followed by a question. An example of this narrative is “You’re a tourist, and you are trying to take a picture of yourself with your phone. A black male comes up to you and offers to take the photo for you. Do you hand them your phone?”

Results: After validating this approach using feeling thermometer measures, we found that the emotional states of others had the strongest and most consistent effect on the participants’ choices. More notably, whereas most characteristics had independent effects on decision-making, social features such as the inferred socioeconomic status of others significantly influenced the effect that race had on the participant’s judgments. Moreover, the social context of the agent’s interactions with other agents had a significant additive effect, especially when the emotional states of the agents in the scenarios contrasted. The influence of these characteristics on the participants’ choices was also markedly affected by their demographics, especially when contrasting with that of the agents involved, and were often driven by the participants’ reported political views.

Discussion: Together, these findings reveal how the mixture of social characteristics, context, and personal views influence decision-making and highlight the use of naturalistic generative narrative surveys in studying human behavior.

1 Introduction

The social characteristics of others can powerfully influence our decisions (Cunningham and Zelazo, 2007; Fiske and Taylor, 2013). Negative attitudes, for example, are often associated with avoidance, ignoring others, and selective re-interpretation, while positive attitudes are often associated with approaching, selective attention, and preferential information recall. Thus, the evaluation of different characteristics and the context in which the evaluation takes place can profoundly affect how we interpret different situations we face in real life.

Explicit attitudes are pervasive and can be based on another’s race, gender, socioeconomic status, and perceived emotions (Axt, 2018; Charlesworth and Banaji, 2019; Fitzgerald and Hurst, 2017; Kurdi et al., 2019a; Payne et al., 2008). Measures of explicit attitudes often ask participants to express their evaluations deliberately in the form of a survey (Payne et al., 2008), these surveys typically focus on single characteristics such as age or race, but not on multiple characteristics. People are not always aware of their attitudes or might have implicit biases in their evaluation of distinct characteristics (Greenwald and Banaji, 1995). Thus, implicit attitude tests have also been used to reveal people’s implicit attitudes, yet, these tests are rarely presented within the context of real-world scenarios or social contexts and typically focus on contrasting two characteristics at the time.

Although there has been significant progress in our understanding of how explicit and implicit attitudes affect our perception of others (Daumeyer et al., 2019), more needs to be done to understand how these biases may explicitly influence our decision-making. More importantly, more needs to be understood about how our decisions are affected by the complex interaction between traits such as race, gender, and personality. For instance, are certain decisions more likely to be negatively affected if one agent is black and friendly versus black and angry or a well-dressed female versus a poorly-dressed male? We also need to know more about how one’s demographics influence these decisions.

Finally, while an individual’s characteristics may influence our decisions, they can also be strongly impacted by the social context in which they are made (Allen et al., 2010). For example, people compare their attitudes to those of others and might even adjust them based on their perceived similarities (Adolphs, 2003; Heider, 1958; Hovland et al., 1957) or the number of agents that are involved (Bower, 1961). Further, the setting in which interactions between two agents occur and how they manifest can affect our judgment of those individuals (Wang et al., 2023; Yang et al., 2023). While attitudes are often prone to contrast effects (Hovland et al., 1957) and ingroup beliefs (Efferson et al., 2008), these ingroup preferences and outgroup dislikes can also be reflected in our demographics, including our political leaning (Leshin et al., 2022). Thus, contrast effects, ingroup favoritism, and political preferences provide additional rich contexts that can influence attitudes.

Here, we aimed to study how the mixture of social characteristics, contexts, and personal beliefs influence our decisions by developing a Generative Narrative Survey design. We refer to our approach Generative Narrative, as it generates distinct narrative items on every survey based on a set of rules. Specifically, we aimed to determine how different permutations of social characteristics of others (e.g., emotional state, perceived socioeconomic status, race, and gender), their context (e.g., the interaction between social agents), and participant’s demographics may affect participant’s decisions, and how combinations of these factors interact to produce choices across scenarios that are generalizable and robust.

2 Methods

2.1 General design

The experiments were approved by the Ethics Committee of the Georg-Elias-Müller Institute of Psychology of the University of Göttingen. For the study, we developed a Generative Narrative Survey in which a series of brief written scenarios were provided to participants. The participants were instructed to answer a series of questions. Specifically, “For each question, we ask you to imagine yourself in a real-life scenario. In every scenario, you will observe or interact with others. Please answer questions as true to yourself as possible.” The scenarios present situations in which one or two agents take an action. The agents in the scenarios possess two to three different characteristics describing their emotional state, socioeconomic status, race, and gender. To allow for tractable analysis across different combinations of characteristics, each category of characteristics could take two values. For emotion, it was ‘happy’ or ‘angry’; for socioeconomic status, it was ‘poorly-dressed’ or ‘well-dressed’, for race it was ‘white’ or ‘black’; for gender, it was ‘female’ or ‘male.’ After each scenario, the participants were prompted to answer a question deciding between two, three, or four alternatives.

2.2 Generative narrative survey generation

We generated a unique survey for each participant. It had eight demographic questions, fifty generated narratives, five comprehension questions, and eight feeling thermometer questions. The main test consisted of answering 50 items containing a ‘stem narrative’ in which one or two agents participated. A base set of 46 “template” narratives (Table 1) and 39 comprehension-specific “template” narratives (Table 2) were used in survey generation (7 “template” narratives did not have an obvious comprehension-specific equivalent, see Table 2). To allow for generalizability, we generated unique surveys for each participant in which they answered the same 50 narratives, but each narrative had agents with different characteristics. For example, they may be presented with the narrative “You’re a tourist, and you are trying to take a picture of yourself with your phone. A black male comes up to you and offers to take the photo for you.” We then asked the participants to indicate in a 2-, 3-, or 4-item response their judgment of each narrative. In this example, the question was “Do you hand them your phone?” Other scenarios, by comparison, may contain a narrative such as “You are a juror. The defendant is a poorly-dressed female and, based on the facts, they likely stole millions of dollars.” followed by the question “How many years would you recommend in jail?” Further, these scenarios would randomly vary such that sometimes there would be a white male in one narrative and a white female in another, or a happy male in one and an angry male in another.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Template narratives, question, answers, and assigned scoring.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Template comprehension narratives, questions, answers, and assigned scoring.

Each unique test was generated by pseudo-randomly assigning to each item two or three testable characteristics. If there were two agents in the narrative, then, at least one of their characteristics was complementary. No two narratives that were semantically similar or that included race as a characteristic were presented in succession. Comprehension questions were pseudo-randomly interspersed in the test and were not asked in succession.

Each generated survey included at least one instance of each of the 46 template narratives. To allow for generalizability and diversity of narratives, 20% of the narratives generated included two characteristics (e.g., white male) and 80% included three descriptors (e.g., poorly-dressed white male). If a narrative included two agents, the two agents always contained distinct descriptors from the same category. Within each survey, the narratives were generated so that each descriptor and combination of descriptors were featured in roughly equal numbers.

To confirm that the participants indeed attended to the questions and to test their comprehension, the survey included five narratives in which the question was directly related to the narrative. For example, “You’re the director of a theatrical performance at a local theater. You’re deciding between two candidates for the starring role. The first is a white male and the second is a black male. How many candidates are you deciding between?” Respondents with an accuracy below 60% were excluded from further analyses (n = 2 were excluded).

As an additional convergent validity measure, participants filled in a feeling thermometer after filling in the survey, in which participants rated their feelings toward each characteristic from “very cold and unfavorable” to “very warm and favorable.” Note that in both tests, the participants had to consider statements, evaluate their possible responses, and decide how to best express their responses (Payne et al., 2008).

Finally, the survey contained a series of demographic questions including year of birth, highest education achieved, self-identified race or races, self-identified gender, and household income based on the US census quartiles. Furthermore, based on the social capital hypothesis (Putnam, 1995), we tested if the absence of civic engagement with others is correlated with different attitudes toward specific characteristics. Therefore, we asked the participants if they had voted in the last election and whether they belonged to a social club (e.g., sports club). We further tested if participants’ political views correlate with distinct explicit attitudes toward some characteristics. The survey required participants to self-report their political views from extremely liberal to extremely conservative on a 7-point Likert scale. Demographic questions were not used as exclusion criteria.

2.3 Participants

We recruited participants through the crowdsourcing platform Amazon mTurk. We limited the possible participants to those who had a work approval rate of 80%, were based in the USA, were assigned the Master’s qualification, and had not completed a task for our lab previously. To exclude participants who had completed a task for our lab previously, we downloaded the list of participants in our tasks (HIT; Human Intelligence Tasks), extracted the unique worker ID, updated the list of participants, and assigned them an excluding qualification type in mTurk. Participants received $2 per response. Data were collected during 72 h in August 2023. The respondents remained anonymous throughout the experiment unless they contacted the research team for which they had to use e-mail.

2.4 Data analyses

2.4.1 Scoring

Each of the 46 template narratives was manually curated with a scoring key that matched each of the possible responses. For example, for the following narrative: “You had a terrible cab ride home from the airport, driven by an X. How much would you tip them? A. 0%, B. 10%, C. 20%,” answer choice A was scored as 0 points, choice B as 0.5 points, and choice C as 1 point in favor of characteristic X.

For narratives that contained more than one agent, a scoring key for each character in the narrative was used. For example, for the following narrative: “You’re a beggar on the streets of a major city. You see one person, who is an X about to walk by, and you see another person, who is a Y about to walk by. Who do you ask for money? A. First, B. Second,” answer choice A is scored as 1 point for characteristic X and 0 points for characteristic Y, and answer choice B is scored as 0 points for characteristic X and 1 point for characteristic Y.

In this way, we were able to score decisions as favorable and unfavorable across a broad variety of naturalistic scenarios and social agents involved. The scoring of all narratives was transformed to the interval [0 1] in order to normalize the responses within an interval (see also Table 1). Normalizing the responses facilitated comparability between disparate questions and possible answers. While ordinal responses may not neatly map to a ratio answer, we have assumed that participants responded as if the options were ordinal. With the goal of increasing engagement in the task we provided a variety of response options, including ordinal (e.g., select 0, 10, 20% tip) and categorical (e.g., choose A over B).

2.4.2 Statistics

We built a generalized linear mixed-effects model (LME) that included each category (gender, race, emotion, socioeconomic status) and their two-way interactions as fixed effects, and included the participant’s ‘identity’ and the narrative template number as random effects. For comparing the preference for one characteristic to another within one category, e.g., in the gender category: female vs. male, we conducted an independent samples t-test, after performing an equal variances test. If the test suggested unequal variances, we used Welch’s t-test. To investigate if there were statistical interactions across categories in three-characteristic narratives (e.g., black well-dressed male vs. white poorly dressed male), a 2-way ANOVA was performed. Lastly, paired samples t-test comparing in-group and out-group preferences for Race, Gender, and socioeconomic status were conducted to gauge the effect of in-group/out-group bias on a participant’s response. These were followed by estimating Cohen’s D to then calculate post hoc the statistic’s power using G*Power (Version 3.1.9.7; Faul et al., 2009).

We examined the possible relationship between distinct self-reported demographics and the evaluation of each characteristic using an LME. We included as fixed-effects variables the demographic variables that we had collected. Namely: age, education level, having voted in the previous election, membership in a social club, political leaning, gender, household income, and race. Race and gender were encoded as effects dummy variables. Thus, there are n-1 dummy variables in these categories. At the same time, the participant’s ‘identity’ and the narrative template number were treated as random effects.

To assess post hoc the power of the LME model to estimate a true effect on narrative scores we proceeded as follows. We generated 1,000 replicates of synthetic data that reflected the study design, with 250 participants responding to 50 items. The percentage of items testing each characteristic per synthetic participant was similar to the original study (100% tested gender; ~60% tested race, emotion, dress, or an interaction between gender and race, emotion, or dress; ~25% tested an interaction between race, emotion, and dress). For simplicity, responses were binary. Synthetic responses had normal noise, while one fixed effect or interaction between parameters had a true effect of 0.05 (in narrative score units). We then fitted the LME model to the simulated data. Finally, to estimate the power of the LME, we calculated the proportion of simulations where the model correctly detects significant effects, using α = 0.05, for the interaction terms and fixed effects. The power of our approach was 100% for either fixed effects or interactions.

Political leaning had a significant effect on the overall participants’ responses, with increasing conservatism related to lower scores. To explore this relationship further we performed a partial correlation between narrative score and political leaning for each characteristic, controlling for all other characteristics. Throughout we set α = 0.05, report the uncorrected p-values, and correct for multiple comparisons using False Discovery Rate. Data analyses were performed on R (Version 4.1.1), SPSS (Version 27), and Matlab (Version 9.10).

3 Results

3.1 Responses to generative narrative survey and validation measures

We analyzed the responses of 257 US-based participants, recruited through mTurk, to a survey about attitudes toward four different categories of characteristics: gender, race, emotion, and socio-economic status. For convergent validity, we used two instruments to assess the participants’ attitudes: (1) a feeling thermometer and (2) a generative narrative survey. In the feeling thermometer, participants reported how warm they felt at that moment toward each characteristic using a feeling thermometer question “Please rate how warm or cold you feel towards ~ insert characteristic here ~ people (0 = coldest feelings, 5 = neutral, 10 = warmest feelings).” These feeling thermometer questions have been used to measure attitudes explicitly (Alwin, 1997), and responses to these items are positively correlated with other responses to other instruments. In the generative narrative survey, we presented brief scenarios in which agents with distinctive characteristics take an action and the participants play a role and are then asked to decide their course of action. Finally, to confirm that the participants indeed attended to the narratives, we asked comprehension questions in which the questions related to the narratives could be answered unambiguously from the information contained in the narrative.

First, to confirm the convergent validity of the questionnaires, we examined the responses of the participants to the comprehension questions. Overall, of the 257 participants, we found that 255 (99.3%) displayed a comprehension performance above 60% and were therefore included in the remainder of the analyses (Table 3). Second, we confirmed that responses provided for the narrative questions as assessed through the narrative scores (0–1 with 0 being unfavorable, and 1 being favorable) correlated with their thermometer scores (with 0 being cold feelings and 1 being warm feelings) across the participants. Overall, participants felt more positive than negative about all characteristics (i.e., with feeling thermometer ratings >0.5), except for the adjective ‘angry’ [0.3239 ± 0.0125, mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM); n = 255, t(254) = 14.084, p = 1.108e-33, one-sample t-test with mean 0.5]. The participant’s responses to the same characteristics for the characters in the narratives showed a similar preference. This was true when considering all characteristics simultaneously (r = 0.523, p = 4.102e-14; Pearson correlation), or each one individually (Figure 1). Thus, the narratives elicited similar evaluations of each characteristic to asking participants how warm they felt toward each characteristic. Together this suggests that the participants’ answers to the narrative questions reflected their subjective judgments on a participant-by-participant level.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Cross-tabulation of participants based on their reported gender, age, and race.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Convergent validity between thermometer ratings and narrative scores. Scatterplot of participant’s (n = 255) thermometer ratings and narrative scores for each characteristic. Each panel contains the Pearson correlation coefficient and the associated p-value between these two variables.

Across narratives, the choices of the participants were influenced by the social characteristics of the agents described in the narratives. After grouping characteristics by category, we discovered that participants showed a significant preference for one characteristic in gender, emotion, and dress (our proxy for socioeconomic status), but not for race. There was no significant difference in preference for ‘white’ versus’ black’ (t(508) = 0.890, p = 0.374; independent samples t-test), but participants significantly preferred ‘female’ over ‘male’ (t(508) = 8.203, p = 1.935e-15), ‘happy’ over ‘angry’ (t(507) = 29.405, p = 9.977e-112), and ‘well-dressed’ over ‘poorly-dressed’ (t(508) = 19.083, p = 1.304e-61) agents (Figure 2). Therefore, when taken together, we confirmed that the participants (1) comprehended the survey questions, (2) their responses were significantly influenced by the agents described in the narratives, and that (3) their responses corresponded with those elicited by the feeling thermometer measurement.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Narrative scores for each characteristic, grouped by characteristic category. Each circle is the mean narrative score of a participant; the boxplot shows the median, interquartile range, and the 95% confidence interval for the median. The asterisk in the boxplot denotes the mean and the line the mean ± 1 standard deviation. The distribution is a kernel density. Asterisks on the bar denote significant differences using an independent samples t-test, p < 0.05.

3.2 The mixed effects of social characteristics on decision making

Our approach to generating novel narratives combined with broad sampling allowed us to test the interaction of characteristic categories (gender, race, emotion, and dress). We considered all four categories simultaneously by building a generalized linear mixed-effects (LME) model that included each category and their interactions as fixed effects and included participants’ identities and the narrative template as random effects. Here, we observed that gender, emotion, and dress were captured by coefficients highly significantly different from zero (all p < 1e-32), while race was not (p = 0.22; Table 4). Moreover, among all two-way interactions, only the interaction between race and socioeconomic status tended toward a significant effect (p = 0.054, uncorrected). These results suggested that gender, socioeconomic status, and emotional state of an agent had independent effects on how participants decided.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Main and interaction effects predicting narrative scores.

Here, we tested the participant’s preferences for combinations of characteristics with a two-way ANOVA. For race and dress, there was an independent significant effect of dress, but not of race, while there was a significant interaction when not correcting for multiple comparisons using FDR [F(1, 4,907) = 278.450, p < 0.001, Ω2 = 2.51%; F(1, 4,907) = 2.069, p = 0.150, Ω2 = 0.01%; F(1, 4,907) = 4.030, p = 0.045, Ω2 = 0.03%, respectively]. For race and gender, there was an independent significant effect of gender, but not of race and no significant interaction [F(1, 11,009) = 39.995, p < 0.001, Ω2 = 0.17%; F(1, 11,009) = 0.893, p = 0.345, Ω2 < 0.00%; F(1, 11,009) = 0.011, p = 0.917, Ω2 < 0.00%, respectively]. For race and emotion, there was an independent effect of emotion but not of race or their interaction [F(1,4,873) = 384.393, p < 0.001, Ω2 = 3.56%; F(1, 4,873) < 0.001, p = 0.987, Ω2 < 0.00%; F(1, 4,873) = 0.691, p = 0.406, Ω2 = −0.003%, respectively]. Both, emotion and gender showed a significant independent effect, but their interaction was not significant [F(1, 10,931) = 921.597, p < 0.001, Ω2 = 3.8%; F(1, 10,931) = 37.849, p = 8e-10, Ω2 = 0.15%; F(1, 10,931) = 0.318, p = 0.573, Ω2 < 0.00%]. Similarly, both gender and dress showed a significant effect, but their interaction was not significant [F(1, 10,972) = 64.3, p = 1.18e-15, Ω2 = 0.27%; F(1, 10,972) = 407.072, p = 6.4e-89, Ω2 = 1.71%; F(1, 10,972) = 0.675, p = 0.411, Ω2 = 2.51%]. Finally, both dress and emotion showed a significant effect but their interaction was not significant [F(1, 4,842) = 115.746, p = 1.08e-26, Ω2 = 1.07%; F(1, 4,842) = 392.110, p = 5.5e-84, Ω2 = 3.63%; F(1, 4,842) = 0.493, p = 0.483, Ω2 < 0.00%; Figure 3]. Taken together, the emotional states of the agents therefore had the largest and most consistent effect on the participant’s choices. Further, whereas most characteristics had independent effects on decision making, the interaction of the agents’ perceived socioeconomic status and race affected the participant’s choices.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Narrative scores for combinations of characteristics. Line plots illustrate the relationship between narrative scores and distinct pairs of characteristics. Each point is the mean and error bars illustrate the standard error of the mean (SEM). The lines with an asterisk illustrate a significant difference in that factor, all p < 0.05, Two-way ANOVA.

3.3 Social contrast effect

An advantage of the generative narrative survey approach is that it allows us to test participant’s preferences when one or two agents participate in the narrative. The effect of social characteristics on the participant’s choices was highly context-dependent. Dissonance theory (Festinger, 1954), for example, suggests that our judgments of others should be affected not only by their social characteristics but also by how those characteristics relate to other agents they may interact with. Therefore, to test this hypothesis, we calculated the difference in scores between characteristics of each category when there was only one character and in which there were two. First, we tested if the narrative scores for each character were higher than chance when the narratives had only one agent. The narrative scores of characters that were female, happy, and well-dressed were higher than chance [t(5164) = 3.39, p = 0.0006; t(3054) = 14.69, p = 6.36e-49; t(3147) = 11.43, p = 1.06e-29, respectively; one sample t-test against 0.5]. While the scores of male, angry, and poorly-dressed characters were significantly lower than chance [t(5201) = 5.94, p = 2.87e-9; t(3148) = 21.12, p = 1e-92; t(3068) = 12.75, p = 2.5e-36, respectively; one sample t-test against 0.5]. Neither the scores of Black or White characters were significantly different from chance [t(3050) = 0.62, p = 0.53; t(3123) = 1.15, p = 0.24, respectively; one sample t-test against 0.5]. We observed that when participants considered two agents in the gender, emotion, and socioeconomic status, there was a significant increase in their preferences compared to when only considering one [t(508) = 6.18, p = 1.251e-09; t(508) = 18.65, p = 1.592e-59; t(508) = 11.013, p = 1.922e-25, respectively]. Thus, for example, the participants were significantly more likely to favor a well-dressed male when that agent interacted with a poorly-dressed male than when considered in separate narratives. There were no significant changes in preference related to the race characteristics (t(508) = 0.569, p = 0.57; Figure 4). Therefore, the effect of all characteristics, aside from race, was significantly influenced by the social context of the agent’s interactions.

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Social contrast effect in the difference in narrative scores when there was one character vs. two characters for each characteristic category. The Y-axis is the difference in narrative scores between the preferred and the non-preferred characteristics in each attribute category. Data in ocher shows the difference between the responses for the preferred vs. the least preferred characteristic when the narrative has only one character per participant. Data in red shows the difference between the average response when the narrative contains two characters. Each circle is a participant’s average narrative score; the boxplot shows the median, interquartile range, and the 95% confidence interval for the median. The asterisk in the boxplot denotes the mean and the line the mean ± 1 standard deviation. The distribution is a kernel density. The lines with an asterisk illustrate a significant difference (independent samples t-test, p < 0.05). n = 255 participants for each plot.

3.4 Demographic responses and ingroup and outgroup comparisons

We investigated how the participant’s own reported demographic characteristics influenced their responses. While we found variations in the participant’s responses based on their demographics and in particular their political leaning (Table 5), we examined the relationship between self-reported demographics and the evaluation of each characteristic using a LME model. We included as fixed-effects variables demographic variables that we had collected. Namely: age, education level, having voted in the previous election, membership in a social club, political leaning, gender, household income, and race. Race and gender were encoded as effects dummy variables. Thus, there were n-1 dummy variables of these categories. At the same time, participant ‘identity’ and narrative template were treated as random effects. Here, we observed that only political leaning was a significant factor in the model (p = 0.973, p = 0.891 for gender-related coefficients; p = 0.138, p = 0.537, p = 0.226, p = 0.517, for race-related coefficients; p = 0.881, p = 0.183, p = 0.372, p = 0.184, p = 0.00008; age, education level, having voted in the previous election, membership in a social club, and political leaning, respectively; t-test on the estimated coefficient vs. null).

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Narrative scores for each characteristic by each aggregated demographic.

The survey required participants to self-report their political views from extremely liberal to extremely conservative on a 7-point Likert scale, with higher scores indicating more conservative views. The results from the LME model indicated that political leaning had a significant effect on the overall participants’ responses, with increasing conservatism related to lower narrative scores. To explore this relationship further we fitted a LME model that included political leaning, all main characteristics, and the two-way interactions between politics and each characteristic as fixed effects and the individual participants and narrative templates as random effects (Table 6). Here, we observed that increasingly conservative views were associated with lower scores for female, black, poorly-dressed, or happy characters. Correspondingly, increasing conservative views were associated with higher scores for male, white, richly-dressed agents, or happy characters. As a follow up to this result, we focused on measuring the relationship between political leaning and each characteristic, controlling for all other characteristics, using partial correlation. Here, we found a significant negative correlation between conservatism and the narrative scores of female, black, happy, and poorly dressed agents (r = −0.067, p = 4.25e-6; r = −0.1408, p = 1e-12; r = −0.0709, p = 3.43e-4; r = −0.084, p = 2.17e-5, respectively, Figure 5, partial correlation, n = 255 participants). Conversely, we found a positive correlation between conservatism and the narrative score of white agents (r = 0.061, p = 0.002, partial correlation, n = 255 participants). All other partial correlations were not significant (male: r = 0.003, p = 0.79; angry: r = 0.006, p = 0.72; richly-dressed: r = 0.02, p = 0.27). Therefore, political leaning influenced how the social characteristics of others affected the participants’ decisions.

Table 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. LME results after fitting the model.

Figure 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Relationship between political view and each characteristic. Scatter plot with means and SEM of the narrative score for each characteristic parsed by respondents’ self-reported political leaning, with higher numbers indicating being more conservative. n = [39, 56, 45, 36, 34, 30, 15], per level of self-reported political view. Partial correlations and associated p-values are included for each characteristic.

Finally, we focused on the characteristics that could lead participants to perceive the characters in the narratives as belonging to either their ingroup or outgroup. Here, we find that both female and male participants preferred female agents [t(137) = 5.0828, p = 1.20e-06, power = 99%; t(113) = 5.9032, p = 3.81e-08, power = 100%, respectively; Paired samples t-test, Figure 6A]. While our main analyses indicate that there were no differences in preferences regarding the race of the agents (Figures 24), we found significant differences in the ingroup preferences. Specifically, black respondents showed an ingroup preference (t(18) = 3.59, p = 0.00207, power = 90.9%, ingroup vs. outgroup; Paired samples t-test), while white respondents did not show a preference for either group (t(209) = 0.3945, p = 0.69359, power = 6.0%; Paired samples t-test, Figure 6B). Lastly, respondents in both the upper quartile (t(28) = 9.63, p = 2.19e-10, power = 100%; Paired samples t-test) and the lower quartile (t(73) = 7.7372, p = 4.38e-11, power = 100%; Paired samples t-test) of household income preferred agents that were well dressed over those that were poorly dressed (Figure 6C).

Figure 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Ingroup favoritism is illustrated by the difference in narrative scores when the respondent could have considered the narrative characters as ingroup or outgroup based on their self-reported demographics. The Y-axis is the difference in narrative scores between the ingroup and outgroup for each demographic dimension: Male: n = 138, Female: n = 114; White: n = 210, Black: n = 19; Upper-income Quartile: n = 29; Lower-income Quartile: n = 74. The boxplot shows the median, interquartile range, and the 95% confidence interval for the median. The asterisk in the boxplot denotes the mean and the line the mean ± 1 standard deviation. The distribution is a kernel density.

4 Discussion

We designed a generative narrative survey in which we permuted distinct social characteristics from four distinct classes in many different interactive contexts to test simultaneously several hypotheses. Explicit and implicit attitudes regarding gender, race, and socioeconomic status have been extensively studied (Charlesworth and Banaji, 2019; Cunningham and Zelazo, 2007; Gilberstadt et al., 2020; Navajas et al., 2019; Pratto et al., 1994; Stanley et al., 2011). At the same time, it has long been acknowledged that other’s emotions have a strong impact on how we evaluate distinct situations (Klüver and Bucy, 1939; Papez, 1937; Sadedin et al., 2023; Zych and Gogolla, 2021). Yet, we need to learn more about how our decisions are affected by the interaction of distinct social characteristics of others within real-world scenarios (e.g., offering a tip to a poorly-dressed black male), their comparison (e.g., a poorly-dressed black male vs. a well-dressed white female), or the observer’s demographics.

Here, we found that gender, perceived socioeconomic status, and emotion but not race had a significant influence on how participants rated fictional agents. More notably, when evaluating two agents with different characteristics, participants’ preferences were stronger compared to only one character for the same categories in gender, perceived socioeconomic status, and emotion, but not race. While our participants preferred females over males and well-dressed over poorly dressed agents, regardless of their demographic characteristics, only black respondents showed ingroup favoritism. Finally, we identified a robust negative correlation between self-reported conservative political views and the narrative scores of distinct social characteristics: including being female, black, happy, and poorly dressed. We also found a positive correlation between conservatism and the narrative score of white agents; together revealing a remarkably detailed interrelationship between the effects of social characteristics, context, and sociodemographics on decision making.

In our panel, participants reported more positive feelings toward females than males and decided in favor of females over males. Past research indicates that attitudes toward females are more positive than those toward males (Eagly and Mladinic, 1994). While our instruments do not directly test prejudices or stereotypical behaviors, this positive evaluation might derive from stereotypical attitudes (Amodio and Cikara, 2021; Eagly and Mladinic, 1994). This deferential behavior is commonly expressed across cultural settings but varies by individual sociodemographic characteristics (Kågesten et al., 2016). Relatedly, we discovered that participants reporting more conservative viewpoints showed less positive views of females than liberal participants did. Furthermore, while boys and girls show ingroup preferences early in development, as males mature they show a preference for females (Dunham et al., 2016). Similarly, we did not find evidence of own-gender preferences in our panel of adult US-based participants. Thus, our results confirm the widely observed preference for females over males.

We tested attitudes toward two basic emotions with different valences: happiness and anger emotions (Tracy and Randles, 2011). Both happiness and anger are emotions that are relevant to the perceiver, and they can trigger approach and avoidance reactions (Paulus and Wentura, 2016). In line with previous findings, we observed that participants in our panel evaluated favorably happy over angry agents. Furthermore, these two emotions can be considered to be certain, vs. emotions associated with uncertainty like hopefulness and anxious (Tiedens and Linton, 2001). The certainty of happiness and anger might facilitate their judgment in others’ behaviors as observed in the large difference in narrative scores we observed. Similarly, a happy face may signal a wish for affiliation, and an angry face a wish to attack (Hess and Thibault, 2009), the context in which an emotion is perceived matters (Barrett et al., 2011). In our vignettes, inspired by real-life situations, participants showed a robust preference for happy agents over angry ones. Overall, these results underscore the robustness of our novel approach.

Overall, we found little evidence in favor of a strong preference based on race. This finding is in line with contemporary studies of attitudes toward race (Charlesworth and Banaji, 2019). The absence of differences in race may relate to a sensitive domain that elicits social desirability bias (An, 2015) that can be better predicted with other methods, like the implicit association test (Greenwald and Banaji, 1995; Kurdi et al., 2019a; Kurdi et al., 2019b). This absence might reflect the responders’ attitudes –as also shown in the feeling thermometer. On the other hand, black responders showed ingroup favoritism for black agents, while no other ingroup favoritism was observed. However, we are also cautious about strong inferences based on this result due to the relatively low number of self-reported black respondents.

Generally, people of higher socioeconomic status receive preferential treatment (Lott and Saxon, 2002). Sociodemographics, like political leaning, also influence how rich people are perceived with liberals less supportive of richer people (Parker, 2012). However, implicit rather than explicit biased attitudes toward the upper class are usually expressed (e.g., Horwitz and Dovidio, 2017). With the generative narrative survey, we found that across our sample-encompassing all family income quartiles-participants decided positively in favor of richly dressed characters. Furthermore, we observed a trend in higher narrative scores for black and richly dressed compared to white and richly dressed characters. While not statistically significant, we speculate that it might relate to a combination of prejudices and values commonly observed in the United States, from where we collected the data. On one hand, generally, people in the United States would like to be rich, the so-called American dream; and this value is also imbued with the concept of meritocracy, in which anyone can be rich if they have the merits (Kasser and Ryan, 1993). On the other, generally, people in this country have an implicit negative bias toward black people (Charlesworth and Banaji, 2019; Kubota et al., 2012; Stanley et al., 2011). Thus, we speculate that higher narrative scores for richly dressed black characters might be related to characters that achieved a widely held positive value. This finding and the associated hypotheses deserve further investigation.

Enhanced responses when contrasting two agents may relate to cognitive dissonance theory, in which we show preferences for attitudes that are consonant with our beliefs and attitudes (Egan et al., 2007). In particular, it suggests that the effect of social characteristics on decision making does not manifest in isolation but is rather strongly influenced by their social context. These findings also demonstrate the disparate roles that specific characteristics play and highlight the powerful influence that the behavior and emotional states of others have on our decisions.

Finally, in line with population-based surveys (Gilberstadt et al., 2020) we found that participants who report more conservative political leaning tend to evaluate black agents less favorably and to a lesser extent lower socioeconomic status, happy and more favorably white agents. It also demonstrates the variable effects that one’s demographics play when considering in vs. out-group characteristics.

Altogether, the convergent validity of the generative narrative survey with the feeling thermometer ratings, which have been shown to correlate with other measures of explicit attitudes (Axt, 2018; Payne et al., 2008), provides a novel approach for investigating explicit attitudes. Thus, it is likely that the evaluations in both the feeling thermometer ratings and the survey were conscious, effortful, and involved critical thinking (Morewedge and Kahneman, 2010). These evaluations correspond to explicit attitudes, in contrast to implicit attitudes. One intriguing possibility is to constrain the response time to probe the role of fast, automatic, and intuitive cognitive processing in these evaluations and correlate them with implicit measures of attitudes. While the generative narrative survey is designed to limit the participants’ response choices, two aspects should be taken into account in future studies. First, users should control for possible social desirability bias in how participants report their choices. Second, users should carefully design the number of options in the answers as these are not necessarily treated as ordinal options by the participants and users should consider increasing them to 10 or more options to strengthen their studies (e.g., Leung, 2011; Simms et al., 2019). The generative narrative survey approach can be flexibly used to test single or multiple characteristics. Analytical consideration should be taken into account when testing multiple characteristics. An important step in using the Generative Narrative approach in future studies is to simulate plausible results in order to set all experimental design parameters appropriately a priori (e.g., Lakens and Caldwell, 2021). Another possibility when using this approach is to re-use specific templates for contrasting participants’ preferences, although it should be considered that participants might read the narratives with less attention as they will be more similar and might not notice the subtle differences between them. Here, our approach efficiently tested multiple attitudes explicitly, and showed robust convergent validity, and revealed that respondents exhibited contrast effects. The generative narrative method also has the potential to be useful in obtaining attitude correlates in physiological testing contexts in which single-participant testing is time-restricted.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the Ethics Committee of the Georg-Elias-Müller Institute of Psychology of the University of Göttingen. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

EW: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. OW: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. ZW: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Supervision. RB-M: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by the European Union (ERC Starting Grant, NEUROGROUP, 101041799). Views and opinions expressed are however those of the authors only and do not necessarily reflect those of the European Union or the European Research Council Executive Agency. Neither the European Union nor the granting authority can be held responsible for them.

Acknowledgments

A preprint version of this article can be found at https://osf.io/preprints/psyarxiv/9xw84 (Wong et al., 2024).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Adolphs, R. (2003). Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 165–178. doi: 10.1038/nrn1056

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, T. J., Sherman, J. W., and Klauer, K. C. (2010). Social context and the self-regulation of implicit bias. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 13, 137–149. doi: 10.1177/1368430209353635

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Alwin, D. F. (1997). Feeling thermometers versus 7-point scales: which are better? Sociol. Methods Res. 25, 318–340. doi: 10.1177/0049124197025003003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Amodio, D. M., and Cikara, M. (2021). The social neuroscience of prejudice. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 72, 439–469. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010419-050928

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

An, B. P. (2015). The role of social desirability bias and racial/ethnic composition on the relation between education and attitude toward immigration restrictionism. Soc. Sci. J. 52, 459–467. doi: 10.1016/j.soscij.2014.09.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Axt, J. R. (2018). The best way to measure explicit racial attitudes is to ask about them. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 9, 896–906. doi: 10.1177/1948550617728995

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, L. F., Mesquita, B., and Gendron, M. (2011). Context in emotion perception. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 20, 286–290. doi: 10.1177/0963721411422522

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bower, G. H. (1961). A contrast effect in differential conditioning. J. Exp. Psychol. 62, 196–199. doi: 10.1037/h0048109

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Charlesworth, T. E. S., and Banaji, M. R. (2019). Patterns of implicit and explicit attitudes: I. Long-term change and stability from 2007 to 2016. Psychol. Sci. 30, 174–192. doi: 10.1177/0956797618813087

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cunningham, W. A., and Zelazo, P. D. (2007). Attitudes and evaluations: a social cognitive neuroscience perspective. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 97–104. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.12.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Daumeyer, N. M., Onyeador, I. N., Brown, X., and Richeson, J. A. (2019). Consequences of attributing discrimination to implicit vs. explicit bias. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 84:103812. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2019.04.010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., and Banaji, M. R. (2016). The development of implicit gender attitudes. Dev. Sci. 19, 781–789. doi: 10.1111/desc.12321

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eagly, A. H., and Mladinic, A. (1994). Are people prejudiced against women? Some answers from research on attitudes, gender stereotypes, and judgments of competence. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 5, 1–35. doi: 10.1080/14792779543000002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Efferson, C., Lalive, R., and Fehr, E. (2008). The coevolution of cultural groups and ingroup favoritism. Science 321, 1844–1849. doi: 10.1126/science.1155805

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Egan, L. C., Santos, L. R., and Bloom, P. (2007). The origins of cognitive dissonance. Psychol. Sci. 18, 978–983. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02012.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., and Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G* power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 41, 1149–1160. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Hum. Relat. 7, 117–140. doi: 10.1177/001872675400700202

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiske, S. T., and Taylor, S. E. (2013). Social cognition: from brains to culture. 4th Edn. Sage.

Google Scholar

Fitzgerald, C., and Hurst, S. (2017). Implicit bias in healthcare professionals: a systematic review. BMC Med. Ethics 18:19. doi: 10.1186/s12910-017-0179-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilberstadt, H., Hartig, H., Jones, B., Dunn, A., Doherty, C., Kiley, J., et al. (2020). Voters’ attitudes about race and gender are even more divided than in 2016 (P. R. Center, Ed.). Available at: https://www.pewresearch.org/politics/2020/09/10/voters-attitudes-about-race-and-gender-are-even-more-divided-than-in-2016/

Google Scholar

Greenwald, A. G., and Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychol. Rev. 102, 4–27. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.102.1.4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Heider, F. (1958). “Perceiving the other person” in The psychology of interpersonal relations. ed. F. Heider (Hoboken, NJ, USA: John Wiley & Sons Inc), 20–58.

Google Scholar

Hess, U., and Thibault, P. (2009). Darwin and emotion expression. Am. Psychol. 64, 120–128. doi: 10.1037/a0013386

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Horwitz, S. R., and Dovidio, J. F. (2017). The rich—love them or hate them? Divergent implicit and explicit attitudes toward the wealthy. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 20, 3–31. doi: 10.1177/1368430215596075

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hovland, C. I., Harvey, O. J., and Sherif, M. (1957). Assimilation and contrast effects in reactions to communication and attitude change. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 55, 244–252. doi: 10.1037/h0048480

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kågesten, A., Gibbs, S., Blum, R. W., Moreau, C., Chandra-Mouli, V., Herbert, A., et al. (2016). Understanding factors that shape gender attitudes in early adolescence globally: a mixed-methods systematic review. PLoS One 11:e0157805. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0157805

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kasser, T., and Ryan, R. M. (1993). A dark side of the American dream: correlates of financial success as a central life aspiration. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 65, 410–422. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.65.2.410

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klüver, H., and Bucy, P. C. (1939). Preliminary analysis of functions of the temporal lobes in monkeys. Arch. Neurol. Psychiatr. 42, 979–000. doi: 10.1001/archneurpsyc.1939.02270240017001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kubota, J. T., Banaji, M. R., and Phelps, E. A. (2012). The neuroscience of race. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 940–948. doi: 10.1038/nn.3136

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurdi, B., Gershman, S. J., and Banaji, M. R. (2019a). Model-free and model-based learning processes in the updating of explicit and implicit evaluations. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 116, 6035–6044. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1820238116

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurdi, B., Mann, T. C., Charlesworth, T. E. S., and Banaji, M. R. (2019b). The relationship between implicit intergroup attitudes and beliefs. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 116, 5862–5871. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1820240116

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lakens, D., and Caldwell, A. R. (2021). Simulation-based power analysis for factorial analysis of variance designs. Adv. Methods Pract. Psychol. Sci. 4:2515245920951503. doi: 10.1177/2515245920951503

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leshin, R. A., Yudkin, D. A., Van Bavel, J. J., Kunkel, L., and Rhodes, M. (2022). Parents’ political ideology predicts how their children punish. Psychol. Sci. 33, 1894–1908. doi: 10.1177/09567976221117154

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leung, S.-O. (2011). A comparison of psychometric properties and normality in 4-, 5-, 6-, and 11-point Likert scales. J. Soc. Serv. Res. 37, 412–421. doi: 10.1080/01488376.2011.580697

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lott, B., and Saxon, S. (2002). The influence of ethnicity, social class, and context on judgments about US women. J. Soc. Psychol. 142, 481–499. doi: 10.1080/00224540209603913

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morewedge, C. K., and Kahneman, D. (2010). Associative processes in intuitive judgment. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 435–440. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.07.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Navajas, J., Álvarez Heduan, F., Garrido, J. M., Gonzalez, P. A., Garbulsky, G., Ariely, D., et al. (2019). Reaching consensus in polarized moral debates. Curr. Biol. 29, 4124–4129.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.10.018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Papez, J. W. (1937). A proposed mechanism of emotion [article]. Arch. Neurol. Psychiatr. 38, 725–743. doi: 10.1001/archneurpsyc.1937.02260220069003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, K. (2012). Yes, the rich are different Washington, DC, USA: Pew Research Center.

Google Scholar

Paulus, A., and Wentura, D. (2016). It depends: approach and avoidance reactions to emotional expressions are influenced by the contrast emotions presented in the task. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 42, 197–212. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000130

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Payne, B. K., Burkley, M. A., and Stokes, M. B. (2008). Why do implicit and explicit attitude tests diverge? The role of structural fit. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 94, 16–31. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.94.1.16

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., Stallworth, L. M., and Malle, B. F. (1994). Social-dominance orientation - a personality variable predicting social and political-attitudes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 67, 741–763. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.4.741

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Putnam, R. D. (1995). Bowling alone: America’s declining social capital. J. Democr. 6, 65–78. doi: 10.1353/jod.1995.0002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sadedin, S., Duenez-Guzman, E. A., and Leibo, J. Z. (2023). Emotions and courtship help bonded pairs cooperate, but emotional agents are vulnerable to deceit. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 120:e2308911120. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2308911120

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Simms, L. J., Zelazny, K., Williams, T. F., and Bernstein, L. (2019). Does the number of response options matter? Psychometric perspectives using personality questionnaire data. Psychol. Assess. 31, 557–566. doi: 10.1037/pas0000648

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanley, D. A., Sokol-Hessner, P., Banaji, M. R., and Phelps, E. A. (2011). Implicit race attitudes predict trustworthiness judgments and economic trust decisions. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 108, 7710–7715. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1014345108

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tiedens, L. Z., and Linton, S. (2001). Judgment under emotional certainty and uncertainty: the effects of specific emotions on information processing. J Pers Soc Psychol. 81, 973–988. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.81.6.973

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tracy, J. L., and Randles, D. (2011). Four models of basic emotions: a review of Ekman and Cordaro, izard, Levenson, and Panksepp and watt. Emot. Rev. 3, 397–405. doi: 10.1177/1754073911410747

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X., Chen, Z., Van Tongeren, D. R., DeWall, C. N., and Yang, F. (2023). Permitting immoral behaviour: a generalized compensation belief hypothesis. Br. J. Psychol. 114, 21–38. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12593

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, E., Williams, O., Williams, Z. M., and Báez-Mendoza, R. (2024). Naturalistic generative narratives reveal effects of social characteristics on decision-making. PsyArXiv. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/9xw84

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, H., Tang, C., and Wang, D. (2023). Is it true that negative emotions cause more utilitarian judgements? From the influence of emotion and cognition. Cognit. Emot. 37, 1248–1260. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2023.2258572

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zych, A. D., and Gogolla, N. (2021). Expressions of emotions across species. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 68, 57–66. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2021.01.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: explicit attitude, decision making, contrast effect, social context, sociodemographic, emotional state, socioeconomic status, generative narrative survey

Citation: Wong E, Williams O, Williams ZM and Báez-Mendoza R (2024) Naturalistic generative narratives reveal effects of social characteristics on decision-making. Front. Psychol. 15:1412131. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1412131

Received: 04 April 2024; Accepted: 04 November 2024;
Published: 18 November 2024.

Edited by:

Jing Luan, Beijing Jiaotong University, China

Reviewed by:

Christoph W. Korn, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Germany
Atsushi Noritake, National Institute for Physiological Sciences (NIPS), Japan

Copyright © 2024 Wong, Williams, Williams and Báez-Mendoza. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Raymundo Báez-Mendoza, UkJhZXpNZW5kb3phQGRwei5ldQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.