- 1Faculty of Educational Sciences, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
- 2Pedagogy and Medical Psychology Department, Institute of Psychology and Social Work, Sechenov University, Moscow, Russia
- 3Laboratory for Cognitive Research, HSE University, Moscow, Russia
- 4Faculty of Graduate Studies, York University, Toronto, ON, Canada
Despite the extensive history of investigation, characterization and diagnostics of giftedness is still a point of debate. The lack of understanding of the phenomenon affects the identification process of gifted children, development of targeted educational programs and state of research in the field of gifted education. In the current systematic review, we seek to delineate the specific aspects in which gifted children differ from their typically developing peers in cognitive abilities, psychophysiology and psychological characteristics. Secondly, we aim to document the prevalence and criteria of intelligence tests used to assess gifted children and adolescents. We reviewed 104 articles from more than 25 countries that examined a total of 77,705 children ages 5–18 years. Results reveal a discernible trend toward adopting more culturally appropriate measures for assessing giftedness in children. Findings highlight that gifted children generally outperform their peers in several cognitive domains such as verbal working memory, inhibition, geometric problem solving, attention-switching and elemental information processing, showcasing an accuracy-reaction time trade-off. Psychophysiological assessments demonstrate heightened and accelerated brain activity during complex effortful cognitive processes. Psychological and behavioral measures reveal that gifted children score higher on tests measuring intrinsic motivation, self-efficacy, and openness to experience; as well as achieving higher grades in school and employing better problem-solving strategies. Our systematic review can be beneficial in educational and research contexts, giving directions in assessment of giftedness and designing future research.
Introduction
Superior cognitive abilities or ‘giftedness’ have attracted the interest of philosophers (Comte, 1844; Saint-Simon, 1813), educators (Gardner, 2004; Harris, 1867), psychologists, psychiatrists (Galton, 1870; Lombroso, 1895; Renzulli, 1978), economists (Bui et al., 2011) and neuroscientists (Geake, 2008; Geschwind and Galaburda, 1985) for a long time. Despite the extensive history of investigation, characterization and diagnostics of giftedness is still a point of debate: components of giftedness and criteria for outstanding abilities lack operationalization and precision (Robinson et al., 2000; Sternberg and Davidson, 2005; Subotnik et al., 2011). While traditional views linked giftedness closely with high IQ scores, contemporary theories understand it as a multifaceted phenomenon influenced by both cognitive and non-cognitive factors. Renzulli’s Three-Ring Conception (Renzulli, 2011) suggests that giftedness arises from the interaction of above-average ability, creativity, and task commitment. Gagné’s Differentiated Model of Giftedness and Talent (Gagné, 2004) distinguishes between innate natural abilities (gifts) and systematically developed skills (talents), emphasizing the influence of environmental and personal factors in transforming gifts into talents. Sternberg’s Triarchic Theory (Sternberg, 1985) proposes that giftedness involves a balance of analytical, creative, and practical intelligences, indicating that traditional intelligence tests may not fully capture an individual’s capabilities. Nevertheless, till now intelligence tests serve as a major giftedness detection tool used by researchers and educators (Hodges et al., 2018), especially when selecting for educational programs. Besides not considering more comprehensive approach to giftedness, overall intelligence tests criteria and thresholds are not well-determined. Such practices differ among countries, states, and even educational institutions (e.g., Bélanger and Gagné, 2006; Mcclain and Pfeiffer, 2012). Cutoff criteria are typically based on either a percentage of children (e.g., the top 5% or 1%) or specific test scores (e.g., an IQ of 130 on classic IQ tests). This variability in selecting gifted children makes it challenging to compare and compile research data, thereby impacting the state of the art in the field. Gifted education is one of the areas that is closely connected with intelligence testing. Thus, lack of consistency in testing criteria affects the identification process of gifted children and the development of educational programs to accommodate the specific needs of gifted children (Silverman and Gilman, 2020; Subotnik et al., 2011).
Furthermore, understanding of giftedness imply uncovering its manifestations in cognitive abilities, motivation, personal traits, and other areas (Berg and McDonald, 2018; Topçu and Leana-Taşcılar, 2018). Nevertheless, it is still unclear whether some characteristics contribute more to the concept of giftedness and what underlying factors are responsible for them. The literature on the traits distinguishing gifted children from those with average abilities is rife with ambiguity. For example, Arffa (2007) suggested superior levels of inhibition in gifted children, while other studies do not support these findings (Duan et al., 2010; Rocha et al., 2020). Vogl and Preckel (2013) demonstrated that higher cognitive ability was correlated with increased social self-concept of assertiveness, whereas Kroesbergen et al. (2015) found no difference in social acceptance and self-concept between gifted and typically developing children. Additionally, Casino-García et al. (2021) revealed even lower scores on family, social, and physical self-concept in gifted children. The literature on mathematical skills (Giofrè et al., 2014; Paz-Baruch et al., 2022) and wellbeing (Košir et al., 2016; Kroesbergen et al., 2015; Vogl and Preckel, 2013) in gifted and mainstream children also contains significant ambiguity.
In the current systematic review we aim to provide greater clarity on what constitutes intellectual giftedness. We seek to delineate the specific aspects in which gifted children differ from their typically developing peers. Secondly, we aim to investigate which tests are most commonly used and what criteria are employed to differentiate between gifted children and their control peers to clarify the trend for practitioners and researchers.
To answer these questions in our systematic review we set the following goals:
a. to document the prevalence of intelligence tests used as selection tool for gifted and control groups in children and adolescents younger than 18 years;
b. to discuss comparisons between gifted and control children focusing on cognitive functions, psychophysiology, and psychological characteristics;
c. to highlight existing methodological, conceptual, and reporting gaps in current research for gifted children.
This synthesized knowledge can be beneficial in educational and research contexts, giving directions in assessment and understanding of giftedness and designing future research.
Methods
Review design and eligibility criteria
The objectives of this review was to synthesize current evidence on the cognitive domains, psychophysiological patterns, and psychological traits that distinguish gifted children and adolescents; and to document the prevalence and criteria of intelligence tests used in assessing giftedness among children and adolescents. The relevancy of this systematic review comes from ongoing debate surrounding the lack of a unified understanding of giftedness and gaps in identification processes. By systematically analyzing existing studies, we aim to inform educational practices and highlight areas necessitating further investigation.
A systematic review was prepared in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA; Moher et al., 2009; Page et al., 2021) and the checklist (Hutton et al., 2015). Studies included in the final set of eligible articles adhered to the following criteria: (a) written in English; (b) participants were children and adolescents younger than 18 years; (c) participants had no pre-existing medical or psychological disorders; (d) giftedness was determined using well-established intelligence tests discussed in peer-reviewed journals; (e) the intellectual giftedness was main focus of the study; (f) publications were empirical studies such as experimental studies, observational studies, and psychometric evaluations.
Including non-English articles was not feasible without compromising the quality and accuracy of the translations. By focusing on English-language articles, we ensured that the studies included in our review are accessible to a broad international audience. Second, medical and psychological disorders can significantly influence cognitive functioning, physiological responses, and psychological wellbeing (McCutcheon et al., 2023; Snyder, 2013; Webb et al., 2016). Excluding participants with pre-existing conditions reduces variability unrelated to giftedness, enhancing the internal validity of our review and allowing for more consistent comparisons across studies. Third, we considered “well-established intelligence tests” to be standardized assessments that demonstrate strong reliability and validity (e.g., internal consistency, test–retest reliability above 0.80) in measuring intelligence or cognitive abilities, supported by extensive psychometric research. This includes evidence from test manuals (e.g., Wechsler, 2014), independent peer-reviewed studies, academic textbooks and handbooks (Flanagan and Harrison, 2022), and reviews in authoritative sources like the Mental Measurements Yearbook (Buros Center for Testing, n.d.). Eligible intelligence tests were standardized on a large, representative sample, providing normative data for accurate comparison across diverse populations. These test were commonly used in the identification and assessment of gifted children in both research and applied settings and widely discussed and critiqued in peer-reviewed academic journals, indicating acceptance within the scientific and educational communities (e.g., Watkins and Canivez, 2016). Examples of such tests are the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-V), Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales (SB5), Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (KABC-II), Raven’s Progressive Matrices, and Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities. Forth, primary outcomes included differences in cognitive domains (e.g., working memory, inhibition, geometric problem-solving), psychophysiological assessments (e.g., ERPs, oscillatory power, BOLD signal, gray and white matter volumes), and psychological measures (e.g., motivation and self-efficacy). We did not deem eligible papers where giftedness was measured only by high performance or talent in a particular subject area studied at school. Lastly, we did not include theoretical papers or conceptual analyses, literature reviews and meta-analyses, editorials, commentaries and opinion pieces, case studies, qualitative studies without empirical data, unpublished work, book and book chapters, non-peer-reviewed articles. We did not impose any restrictions on the publication date; all studies published up to the date of our search were considered for inclusion.
Search strategy and information sources
The literature search was conducted using Web of Science,1 covering literature up to February 4, 2022. Additional sources included gray literature identified through reference lists of relevant articles. The search strategy included a combination of keywords ‘cognitive’, ‘gifted’, ‘intelligence’, and ‘talent’ in title and abstract. Boolean operators “AND” and “OR” were used to refine the search. The finalized keyword string was as follows:
(cognitive[Abstract] AND gifted[Abstract]) OR (intelligence[Abstract] AND gifted[Abstract]) OR (intelligence[Abstract] AND talent[Abstract]) OR (cognitive[Abstract] AND talent[Abstract]),
and included a filter on English-language articles. We did not consult with a librarian, as coauthor MA has extensive experience in conducting systematic reviews and played a key role in developing and refining the search strategy.
Study selection and data extraction
This search yielded a total of 1,214 articles, which underwent a series of selection criteria. After duplicates were removed, studies were first screened based on their title and abstract by three authors (AL, EK, NZh). To increase the inter-rater reliability (IRR), a third of papers was independently assessed by three reviews (Al, EK and NZh). After comparing the assessment results, the IRR was high (>80%). The rest of the papers were split between those reviewers. Reviewers were free to leave the papers uncategorized due to some doubts concerning inclusion criteria. At the end of the screening uncategorized papers were examine by all three reviewers and consensus was reached by discussion. Full-text articles were retrieved for further assessment. A total of 104 articles survived criteria and were included in the review. One of the main reasons for exclusion was the choice of the test to measure giftedness or the absence of it (n = 117). We only deemed eligible standardized intelligence tests, which have been discussed in peer-reviewed journals. No method of selection was mentioned in 81 articles. In some articles, authors recruited children that were already identified as gifted; in that case we included the paper only if the identification measure was indicated and met our criteria. The second most common exclusion reason was the type of giftedness (n = 40). We rejected articles which focused solely on outstanding achievements or giftedness in other areas rather than intellect (e.g., sports, art, or a particular subject at school).
A PRISMA flow chart illustrates the steps taken the study selection process, detailing the number of records identified, screened, assessed for eligibility, and included in the review (Figure 1).
Data were extracted using a standardized form capturing details on author(s), publication year, country where the study was conducted, age and gender of participants, sample size, what tests were used to select intellectually gifted children for each study, criterion of giftedness. The combined total number of participants across all relevant studies included a substantial sample of 77,705 individuals. In our systematic review we aimed to provide a comprehensive synthesis of the existing literature. To achieve this, we included studies both with and without control groups. Studies without control groups were studied to address our methodological research question concerning the intelligence tests used in identifying giftedness. Our main results regarding the characteristics of giftedness were based on studies including both gifted and control groups as it allows direct comparisons between gifted children and their non-gifted peers. Thus, if the study contained a control group we proceeded with evaluation of further methods and results, including experimental method, the target variable of analysis, the task/questionnaire chosen, and statistical differences reported between the gifted and control groups. We ensured that the control groups were demographically and contextually similar to the gifted groups in terms of age, gender, socioeconomic status, and ethnic background. This approach aimed to attribute observed differences specifically to giftedness. The combined total number of participants in articles with a control group included 27,309 individuals. To ensure accuracy, all three reviewers cross-checked the extracted data and all the terms were taken as they were used in the original articles.
Quality assessment and data synthesis
We conducted an informal quality assessment by critically reviewing each included study’s methodology, sample characteristics, data analysis methods and reported findings. Data from the included studies were synthesized by creating a comprehensive summary (Supplementary Table 1S), which summarizes demographic, methodological choices and research findings reported by article. We organized articles in blocks based on the topic of interest: cognitive functions, psychophysiology, psychological characteristics, and other. By organizing the data in this manner, we facilitated a systematic comparison of studies, enabling us to identify patterns, similarities, and differences across various research contexts. The summary table served as the foundation for a narrative synthesis, as the heterogeneity of study designs and outcome measures precluded a quantitative meta-analysis.
Results
Giftedness assessments
Supplementary Table 1S summarizes information for 104 articles, including author, year of publication, topic of interest, country, number of participants, age group, measurement of giftedness, criteria of giftedness, experimental method, target of analysis, dependent variable/s and significant differences between gifted and control children. Articles were published between 1930 and 2022. Main topics of interest were: Cognitive abilities (n = 42), Psychophysiological data (n = 16), Psychological characteristics (n = 20), and Other (n = 54). Some of the articles considered more than one topic of interest. In most articles that reported participant gender, the proportion of boys and girls in the sample was comparable (n = 62), girls outnumbered boys in five articles, and boys outnumbered girls in 26 articles. Most of the articles were published in the USA (n = 38), followed by China (n = 12), Israel (n = 9), Spain (n = 5), Germany (n = 5), South Korea (n = 5), Canada (n = 4), Denmark (n = 4), France (n = 4), Australia (n = 2), Iran (n = 3), Turkey (n = 2), Sweden (n = 2), and Netherlands (n = 2). Other countries produced one eligible article.
For 59 out of 104 studies the target group was primary school children (from 6 to 11 y.o.), for 45 studies—younger adolescents (from 12 to 14 y.o.), for 32—older adolescents (15–18 y.o.), for 22—younger children (<6 y.o.). Some articles considered children from more than one age group.
Figure 2 illustrates the distributions of different tests used to select gifted and control groups in eligible articles. The Wechsler Intelligence Scale was the most popular test used (n = 51). Subscales of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale Test were specified, where possible. Second most popular test among eligible papers was the Raven’s Matrices Test (n = 25). Stanford-Binet was used 16 times, Cattell’s Culture Fair Test—five, CogAT—four and Naglieri Nonverbal Ability Test—three. Other tests (n = 27) were used in less than three articles and composed the category “Other tests.”
Figure 2. Commonly used intelligence tests for giftedness identification. CogAT Test, Cognitive Abilities Test; WAIS, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; RCPM, Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices.
Within the category of the Wechsler scales the most frequently used test was the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC). The classical version of WISC was used in seven studies, the revised WISC version—in 18 studies, WISC-III—in five studies, WISC-IV—in five studies, and WISC-V in one study. The criteria of giftedness for most of the studies stayed in range from 120 to 130 (see Supplementary Table 1S, column “Criterion of giftedness”). One study set the criteria to 160 and two studies selected the top 5% and top 1% of the participants who took part in the test. The scale was used with all age groups. Another type of scale was the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI) which was given to younger children, primary school children, and younger adolescents in seven studies. Two papers used Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale and administered it to older adolescents. Four papers did not specify the type of scale.
The three versions of Raven’s Progressive Matrices were used in the final sample of the review. The Advanced Progressive Matrices (RAMP; n = 13) were given mainly to older adolescents with the criteria of scoring higher than 26–33. The Standard Progressive Matrices (RSPM, n = 5) tested abilities of younger adolescents and primary school children by indicating the criteria of giftedness as top 5% or top 10% of children participated in the test. Four papers did not provide information on the criterion. The criteria for the Colored Progressive Matrices (n = 3) were the top 2% and standardized score more than 110 and 130. The test was given to primary school children and younger children. The other four papers did not specify the version of the Raven’s Progressive Matrices.
The criteria for the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Test (n = 16) mostly fell within the scores 120–135. One study set the criteria to top 5% and one study—to 150. Children from all the age groups took this test.
Cognitive abilities
Of the 104 eligible articles, 42 examined cognitive abilities, 27 of which involved a control group. Of the 27 articles with a control group, samples of 15 studies were gender balanced, five included more males, three were exclusively male, one had more females, and three did not provide this information. One study focused on younger children, 13 studies on primary school children, 14 on younger adolescents, and nine studies evaluated older adolescents.
Working memory
Out of six articles that used tasks related to working memory, five of them found significant differences between gifted and control children (Table 1). Significant differences in accuracy were found in forward and backward parts of the Digit span task (Fard et al., 2016; Leikin et al., 2013), as well as in the working memory task where children had to remember words (Coyle et al., 1998) and the last item of a series (Calero et al., 2007; Zhang et al., 2017). Gifted group did not perform better than the control group in the working memory task with digits and letters and both parts of the Corsi task (Leikin et al., 2013; Paz-Baruch et al., 2016).
Attention
Three articles found differences in various types of attention (Table 2). Gifted children performed significantly better on the d2 test of selective attention (Paz-Baruch et al., 2016). Higher scores in accuracy for gifted primary school children were found for sustained, supervisory, and divided attention, but not in focused, alertness, spatial, and switching attention (Zhang et al., 2016). At the same time gifted children were faster on attentional switching, alertness, spatial and divided attention tasks but not on focused, sustained and supervisory attention tasks (Zhang et al., 2016). In another study, both groups performed at a high level in terms of accuracy on attentional switching task, and gifted children demonstrated shorter reaction time in comparison with controls (Duan and Shi, 2014).
Inhibition
Three articles out of four found differences on inhibition between the groups (Table 3). Johnson et al. (2003) found differences in processing speed between groups with high and average levels of intelligence on the Spatial location task, the Stroop task, and Trail making test where gifted children were faster in giving their responses. Gifted primary school children made less errors in the ignored repetition condition and the Stroop condition in the Stroop task. Significant differences in reaction time but not in accuracy were observed between the groups in effortful inhibition measured by the Trail making test and automatic inhibition measured by the spatial location task. Montoya-Arenas et al. (2018) research did not find significant differences in accuracy on the Stroop task, no reaction time scores were reported. Younger adolescents scored higher and reacted faster in cognitive control and conflict control tasks (Liu et al., 2011a, 2011b).
Abstract reasoning and planning
Both groups of primary school children showed comparable time and numbers of moves in solving the Tower of Hanoi/London (Montoya-Arenas et al., 2018; Vogelaar et al., 2019; Table 4). In the mental rotation task gifted younger and older adolescents showed higher accuracy, whereas there was no significant difference in reaction time between the groups (Anomal et al., 2020). Results for the Wisconsin Card-Sorting Test that covers a set of executive functions demonstrate the controversy: significant differences between gifted children and control children were found in one article (Tanabe et al., 2014) but not another (Montoya-Arenas et al., 2018). Researchers found higher accuracy of the gifted group in geometry problem solving (Vogelaar et al., 2017; Waisman et al., 2016), and inductive reasoning task (Zhang et al., 2015).
Elementary cognitive processes
Elementary cognitive tasks are often basic tasks that pose limited requirements on cognitive processes that are not commonly considered as working memory, attention, and inhibition tasks. In elementary processing tasks (Table 5), the gifted groups showed better results in cross-out of numbers that measured the speed of processing (Paz-Baruch et al., 2014) as well as in choice reaction time task (Duan et al., 2013) but not in other speed of processing tasks such as the visual matching test, the digit-symbol test, and the symbol search test (Paz-Baruch et al., 2014, 2016). Investigating speed and efficiency of elemental information processing, Kranzler et al. (1994) detected an advantage of gifted younger adolescents in reaction time in the simple reaction time and odd-man paradigm tasks but not in the choice reaction time task. Notably, the authors also analyzed movement time, the interval between releasing the home button and depressing the push button, which resulted in showing differences between the groups in all three tasks. In line with this study, Duan et al. (2013) reported differences in the speed of information processing between gifted and control primary school children using perceptual and processing tasks. They used several measures such as the inspection time task (deciding which stimulus line is longer), the choice reaction time task (judging whether the sample stimulus appeared in the line of other stimuli), and the abstract matching task (choosing appropriate patterns based on the sample one). For all three measures, gifted children were quicker than those in the control group.
Other
Examining self-control abilities using facial expressions, Urben et al. (2018) revealed shorter reaction times of average ability children for neutral and happy faces but no difference for sad faces. Higher accuracy for gifted children in the tasks related to memory and learning was found in auditory verbal learning (Fard et al., 2016), the learning potential test (Calero et al., 2011), and metacognitive competences (Tibken et al., 2022). Researchers also revealed higher skills of the gifted group in reading comprehension (Vogelaar et al., 2017), verbal fluency (Montoya-Arenas et al., 2018), fluid intelligence measured by RSPM (Li et al., 2020), concrete and formal operations (Carter and Ormrod, 1982) and strategic thinking (Coyle et al., 1998; Yun et al., 2011) in comparison with their peers. Higher accuracy in gifted children was observed in mental-attention capacity (Johnson et al., 2003), pattern recognition task (Paz-Baruch et al., 2016), self-regulation and concentration (Calero et al., 2007), and simple arithmetic exercises that measured the speed of processing (Paz-Baruch et al., 2014).
Psychophysiological data
Sixteen articles examined psychophysiological data, 14 of them focused on differences between gifted and control groups.
Event related potentials (ERP) studies
There were eight articles investigating event-related potentials (ERPs) related to cognitive functions (Table 6). Six of them demonstrated significant differences between gifted and control children in at least one ERP component.
One out of two articles analyzing the P1 component of ERPs (peaks ~100 ms after stimulus onset) found a significant difference between the gifted and the control group. Amplitude of the parietal P1 was higher in gifted adolescent boys when solving geometric problems (Waisman et al., 2016). Gifted children also experienced the peak of P1 later than the control group, in other words, the latency of P1 in this study was higher in gifted children (Waisman et al., 2016). Another paper, a facial expression identification study, revealed no difference in P1 response between gifted and control groups of younger adolescent boys during the early visual processing stage (Liu et al., 2015).
Both articles investigating the N2 component of ERP (~200–350 ms post-stimulus) found a significant difference between the gifted and the control groups. The N2 component had larger amplitude in gifted children for both classic conflict imposed by the Eriksen flanker task (Liu et al., 2011b) and emotional conflict by face-word Stroop task (Liu et al., 2011b) and by emotional Simon task (Li et al., 2020). The emotional Simon task also revealed lower N2 latency in gifted children (Li et al., 2020).
Three out of six articles investigating ERPs analyzed the P3 component (roughly 250–500 ms after stimulus onset). Two articles revealed increased Р3 amplitude in gifted children, both in primary school students and younger adolescents, both in response to a stimulus and in response to a cue providing information about future stimulus (Liu et al., 2011b, Liu et al., 2011a; Zhang et al., 2006). One out of these three articles showed that gifted children demonstrate shorter latency of the P3 component (Liu et al., 2011b; Zhang et al., 2006). However, no difference was found neither in P3 amplitude nor in latency in the study of Duan and Shi (2014) who investigated attentional switching performance in younger adolescents. One article focused on the P3a (~250–280 ms after stimulus onset, mostly associated with the processing of novelty) and the mismatch negativity components (200–400 ms). For both components, a larger amplitude in gifted primary school students and younger adolescents was observed (Liu et al., 2007). One study investigated mental rotation-related negativity (~400 ms post-stimulus onset) and the late discriminative negativity (400–700 ms; Anomal et al., 2020; Liu et al., 2007). For both components greater amplitudes in gifted primary school students and younger adolescents was observed. Overall, gifted children tend to show greater ERP amplitudes during problem solving.
Electroencephalography (EEG) oscillations studies
Two articles examined brain oscillations using EEG. Both articles investigating EEG oscillations related to cognitive functions show significant differences between gifted and control children in gamma frequency band.
When performing easy tasks for reasoning, gamma rhythm power (30–45 Hz) was lower in gifted older adolescents, whereas when performing difficult tasks, gifted children exhibited significantly increased gamma power compared with a control group (Zhang et al., 2015). Only the gifted group showed a significant increase in gamma power with task difficulty (Zhang et al., 2015). Zhang et al. (2014) analyzed temporal binding of the gamma-band (30–60 Hz) synchronization between frontal and parietal cortices in adolescents with exceptional mathematical ability. Compared with the average-ability participants, the math-gifted adolescents showed a highly integrated fronto-parietal network due to distant gamma phase-locking oscillations. Gifted adolescents also demonstrated more stable frontal–parietal gamma phase dynamics (Zhang et al., 2014).
Other physiological methods
One article examined functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging metrics with the aim to investigate the neural bases for intellectual giftedness in adolescents. The authors showed that the blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal in the posterior parietal cortex was significantly stronger in gifted older adolescents than in their control peers (Lee et al., 2006). Two articles investigated Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI) indicators. Such topological characteristics of the brain network as levels of global and local efficiency were higher in gifted older adolescents (Ma et al., 2017). Axial diffusivity, reflecting white matter integrity, was higher in gifted primary school students and young adolescents (Nusbaum et al., 2017). Another article focused on skin conductance in response to an orientation reflex, which was increased in gifted primary school students (Kimmel and Deboskey, 1978).
Psychological characteristics
Twenty articles examined psychological characteristics, 12 of which focused on differences between gifted and control children (Table 7). Four articles were devoted to motivation characteristics of gifted children. Three of them showed higher scores of intrinsic motivation in the gifted group (Bergold et al., 2020; Gottfried and Gottfried, 1996; Guez et al., 2018). One article investigated achievement motivation in older adolescence and found it to be enhanced in the gifted group (Wirthwein et al., 2019). One article focused on extrinsic motivation and demonstrated significantly higher scores in gifted younger adolescents (Guez et al., 2018).
Three articles investigated psychological characteristics related to self-efficacy (Table 8). Academic self-efficacy (Guez et al., 2018), math ability self-concept (Bergold et al., 2020), self-esteem intelligence scores (Wirthwein et al., 2019) and self-regulatory efficacy (Guez et al., 2018) were higher in gifted children in comparison with control children. However, there was no difference in social self-efficacy between the groups (Guez et al., 2018). One article also did not obtain a significant difference in social adaptation in children of different ages (López and Sotillo, 2009).
Two studies analyzed the Big 5 personality factors. Both of them found that gifted adolescents were significantly more open to experience and did not differ from ordinary peers in Extraversion, Agreeableness and Conscientiousness (Limont et al., 2014; Wirthwein et al., 2019). One article investigated the relationship between giftedness and perfectionism (Lavrijsen et al., 2021). Gifted younger adolescents had significantly higher scores on the multidimensional perfectionism scale, and, at the same time, lower concern over mistakes.
The level of life satisfaction did not differ between gifted adolescents and the control group (Bergold et al., 2015, 2020). One article focused on different characteristics of overexcitability (Limont et al., 2014). The authors found that gifted adolescents did not differ from their peers in emotional overexcitability, however gifted adolescents surpassed control children in other types of overexcitability (Sensual, Intellectual, Psychomotor, Imaginational).
According to four studies focusing on strategic behavior, gifted children come up with a better strategy when playing games (Chung et al., 2011; Yun et al., 2011) and solving tasks (Coyle et al., 1998; Zhang et al., 2017) than their control peers. Also gifted children tend to cooperate more, be less sensitive to loss (Chung et al., 2011) and stick to one particular strategy (Coyle et al., 1998). The monetary acceptance rate and earnings in the Ultimatum game conducted by Yun et al. (2011) was lower for gifted than control children (Table 9).
Creativity was measured in four studies, with only one evaluating differences between gifted and control children (Kershner and Ledger, 1985). Kershner and Ledger (1985) used the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking to measure verbal and figural originality, fluency and flexibility. Authors found significant differences between gifted children and controls only in the verbal originality dimension.
Other categories
Besides three main groups of studies listed above, there were four other categories we identified. Seven studies examined participants’ achievements, both during the school years and later in adult life. Four of them focused on differences between gifted and control children. According to results, gifted children have higher grades at school (Li et al., 2020; Wirthwein et al., 2019) and on important examinations (Guez et al., 2018). The probability to get a Master’s degree is also higher among gifted children in comparison with controls (Bergman et al., 2014). However, differences in future income level are noticeable only when comparing gifted and control boys, with gifted boys earning more (Table 10).
Five studies investigated aspects related to family environment, three of them focused on differences between gifted and control children. According to Weissler and Landau (1993) parents of gifted children are less authoritarian and tend to expose their children to a wider variety of things (toys, books, works of art, traveling) and different sources of information. In general, they pay more attention to the child’s development and cultivation of intelligence. Landau and Weissler (1993) conducted the similar research in the same year and confirmed that at least fathers of gifted children are more educated and liberal, while at the same time the level of parents’ assertiveness was higher in families with gifted students. No differences were found in such dimensions as socioeconomic status, atmosphere at home, cognitive interaction between parents and children, diversity of parents’ interests and parents’ level of stress. In the third study comparing the environment in families with and without gifted children no differences were found at any of the dimensions, including democratic/authoritarian family style, achievement orientation, intellectual-cultural orientation, communication and organization (Schilling et al., 2006).
Fifteen studies focus on methodology of assessing gifted children, reporting significant correlations between different intelligence tests, and comparing consistency of results among them. None of these studies compared gifted and control group performance as such.
The last category combined studies (n = 27) which topics could not be included in one of the above–listed categories. There were various studies looking for an association between IQ and breakfast consumption (Hisam et al., 2015), TV comprehension (Abelman, 1995), height (Hollingworth, 1930), handedness (Aliotti, 1981), sleep behavior (Demirhan et al., 2018; Piro et al., 2021). Some of the studies revealed unexpected significant differences in gifted and intellectually average children, for example in height (Hollingworth, 1930) and TV comprehension (Abelman, 1995), while in other fields gifted children did not differ much from their intellectually average peers, e.g., in breakfast consumption (Hisam et al., 2015) and sleep behaviors (Piro et al., 2021).
Discussion
The objective of this study was to enhance the understanding of giftedness identification, along with the cognitive, physiological, and psychological characteristics associated with gifted children. We systematically reviewed the literature to document the prevalent tests used to identify intellectual giftedness in children and synthesized data from studies comparing gifted and control children in cognitive, physiological and psychological domains. In accordance with the three questions outlined in the Introduction, we synthesize the key findings as follows: (а) although the Wechsler Intelligence Scale Test remains the most popular choice for identifying giftedness in children, there is a discernible trend, particularly from the 1990s onwards, favoring culturally independent inclusive tests with visual or non-verbal stimuli such as Raven’s Matrices; (b) Results indicate that gifted children outperform their peers in cognitive tasks related to verbal working memory, inhibition, attentional switching, geometric problem solving, and elemental information processing, while physiological studies reveal differences in brain activity and structure, showing increased activity in late components of evoked potentials and during complex problem-solving processes; furthermore, gifted individuals exhibit higher intrinsic motivation, self-efficacy, openness to experience, and better school achievements and problem-solving strategies; (c) Significant methodological, conceptual, and reporting gaps exist in current research on gifted children, including variability in measurement approaches, lack of standardization in assessment methods, controversial binary group divisions, inconsistent terminology for cognitive functions and inconsistencies in reporting styles that hinder reliable comparisons and synthesis of findings. The ensuing discussion maps assessments and attributes of children with outstanding intellectual abilities compared to their same age peers in control groups in cognitive functions, psychophysiology, psychological and behavioral characteristics. Concluding, we point to limitations in methodological practices and considerations for future research.
Giftedness assessments
In our systematic review we demonstrate that classic intelligence tests have been used for many decades and remain popular in evaluation of superior intelligence. About 39% of studies used a version of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale test for assessing intellectual giftedness in children. We also reveal a trend for visual–spatial assessments that has become prominent in the last 30 years. The Raven’s Matrices Test is the second most popular assessment method in our systematic review: it evaluates visual–spatial abilities and minimizes cultural, and verbal confounds. It is often used to collect data in non-English speaking countries such as China, Spain, Israel, and Iran. This is consistent with reports that demonstrate an advantage of nonverbal tasks for screening and identifying gifted children from White, Hispanic and African American backgrounds (Lewis, 2001). The third most popular test is the Stanford-Binet, which was the one of the first tests to determine intellectual capacity in children and had a major influence on the future development of intelligence testing (Boake, 2002; Pichot, 1948). These findings highlight the importance of adopting more inclusive methods for identifying giftedness, as traditional approaches may inadvertently overlook the diverse talents of children from various cultural backgrounds. By prioritizing assessments that reduce verbal and cultural biases, we might begin to foster a more equitable framework for recognizing and nurturing gifted potential across all demographics. Such an approach not only broadens the definition of giftedness but also could lead to educational practices and policies that are more attuned to the needs of a diverse student population.
Notably, different studies employed varying threshold scores to define high intelligence. Most studies included the top 5–10 percent of scorers in the gifted group. Although there are methodological considerations for obtaining substantial sample sizes, variation in thresholds could potentially lead to differences in outcomes and interpretations of research results. The implications of establishing such thresholds are also profound for educators, specialists in gifted education, and program developers, as standardized criteria could enhance identification processes, inform targeted interventions that meet the specific needs of gifted children and promote equitable access to gifted programs across diverse populations. Thus, the need for a universally agreed-upon threshold for defining giftedness remains.
Giftedness involves a balance of analytical, creative, and practical intelligences (Sternberg, 1985). Therefore, despite their wide popularity traditional intelligence tests may not fully capture an individual’s capabilities. It would be beneficial to adopt a more comprehensive approach to identifying giftedness that goes beyond traditional assessments. For example, utilizing existing tools that measure creativity, and practical problem-solving abilities can provide a more complete picture. Besides, incorporating qualitative measures such as teacher evaluations, peer reviews, self-assessments, and observational data might be advantageous in providing a holistic view of a student’s abilities. Lastly, encouraging educational policymakers to revise identification criteria for gifted programs to include multiple intelligences could lead to more inclusive and effective educational strategies.
Cognitive abilities
In this section, we summarize the findings related to the cognitive characteristics of gifted children, highlighting their significance in advancing our understanding of giftedness. By examining these results, we aim to provide insights that can inform both theoretical frameworks and practical applications in the field of gifted education.
Group performance was significantly different between gifted children and control children in cognitive tasks in about 70% of comparisons evaluating reaction time, 53% evaluating accuracy, and 59% evaluating both accuracy and reaction time. These findings suggest that reaction times may be a more sensitive metric in distinguishing between gifted and control groups in the cognitive tasks.
Considering working memory tasks, 63% of comparisons showed significant differences in accuracy between gifted and control children. Friedman et al. (2006) specifically showed that working memory is highly correlated with intelligence and showcasing the value of working memory tasks for assessing giftedness. In our review, gifted children showed better results on both forward and backward digit span task (Calero et al., 2007; Fard et al., 2016; Leikin et al., 2013), on recall of categorized words (Coyle et al., 1998; Zhang et al., 2017), as well as manipulating them (Calero et al., 2007; Fard et al., 2016; Leikin et al., 2013). However, gifted children did not show better accuracy in the Corsi task that involves visual–spatial working memory (Leikin et al., 2013; Paz-Baruch et al., 2016). Thus, on working memory tasks gifted children exhibit superior accuracy in storage, manipulation and retrieval, but not in spatial domain, specifically in the Corsi block-tapping task. This discrepancy in results may be attributed to the Corsi task’s greater cognitive demands and its motor component, while the digit span and recall tasks are typically less effortful (Piccardi et al., 2019). However, it is worth noting that the studies utilizing the Corsi task did not analyze reaction times, which may limit a comprehensive comparison between gifted and control children; if reaction times had been measured, it is possible that gifted children could have shown advantages in the spatial domain as well, as spatial attention tasks revealed more differences in reaction time than accuracy between gifted and control groups.
Results on attention revealed a trade-off pattern between speed and accuracy. Enhanced speed, rather than accuracy, has been demonstrated by gifted children in alertness, spatial attention (Zhang et al., 2016) and attentional switching (Duan and Shi, 2014; Zhang et al., 2016). While the enhanced precision at expense of superior speed was established in sustained and supervisory attention domains (Zhang et al., 2016). In divided attention gifted children outperformed their control children in both speed and accuracy (Zhang et al., 2016).
A speed-accuracy trade-off was also revealed for gifted group in both automatic and effortful inhibition tasks (Johnson et al., 2003), where they demonstrated enhanced speed rather than accuracy; and go-no-go task (Liu et al., 2011a), in which gifted children answered more accurately than their peers but demonstrate similar reaction time. Gifted children outperformed in both accuracy and reaction time in Eriksen flanker task (Liu et al., 2011b). Critically, inhibition processes that characterize Flanker tasks are considered more cognitively loaded, as competing cognitive processes need to be suppressed, whereas inhibition to the go-no-go task involves the suppression of a dominant response (Hung et al., 2018). Thus, children in the gifted group seem to show better performance on tasks with higher cognitive demands and overall tend to be either faster or more accurate in tasks of inhibition.
Planning and strategy tasks are also typically considered as more cognitively demanding. Gifted children outperform their control peers in geometric problem solving (Vogelaar et al., 2017; Waisman et al., 2016). However, gifted children did not solve the Tower of Hanoi/London planning task with less moves than their peers in control groups (Montoya-Arenas et al., 2018; Vogelaar et al., 2019). This difference in outcomes may be related to varying levels of control within the experimental designs and the differing time constraints during task execution; for example, the relatively unstructured nature of the Tower task allows for more flexibility in the problem-solving process, including variations in time and the strategies employed which could introduce additional confounding variables and make it more challenging to draw definitive comparisons between groups.
A reaction time advantage was demonstrated in 83% of comparisons examining elemental information processing using tasks such as simple reaction time, odd discrimination and abstract matching (Duan et al., 2013; Kranzler et al., 1994). This fact goes in line with our ERP results, which demonstrated shorter ERP latency in gifted children during performance of tasks that require inhibition of irrelevant information (Li et al., 2020; Liu et al., 2011b; Table 6).
Overall, our results demonstrate a consistent advantage of gifted children in verbal working memory, inhibition and geometric problem solving, and shorter reaction time in attentional switching and elemental information processing. Our results align with a recent mini-review that showed the advantages of gifted children in verbal working memory and attentional switching based on the sample of 15 eligible studies (Bucaille et al., 2022). Other researchers (Friedman et al., 2006) specifically showed that working memory is highly correlated with intelligence and showcasing the value of working memory tasks for assessing giftedness, which is also consistent with our results. These findings suggest that verbal working memory and attentional flexibility could be potential cognitive characteristics of giftedness identification. Besides, our results highlight a speed-accuracy trade off pattern, which is mostly prominent in attention and inhibition tasks. We speculate that higher cognitive capacity in gifted children enables them to process the task faster or improve accuracy, particularly in more complex tasks. This mechanism may explain higher reaction time in some tasks and higher accuracy in others in our review. Insights gleaned from psychophysiology data may clarify this trade-off, thereby enriching the discourse on cognitive characteristics associated with giftedness.
Psychophysiology
In this section, we summarize findings on the psychophysiological characteristics of gifted children which might underlie cognitive characteristics mentioned above. By examining brain patterns, researchers can identify specific regions and networks that are more active or efficient in gifted children. This helps pinpoint the neural basis of advanced skills in problem-solving, memory, and attention.
EEG studies of ERPs show that gifted children differ significantly from the control group in both early and late ERP components. The earliest component of the ERP—P1 (100 ms after stimulus onset)—reflects the earliest stages of information processing. Amplitude of the parietal P1 in response to a stimulus when solving geometric problems was increased in gifted older adolescent boys (Waisman et al., 2016). The authors explain this effect by early analytical activation and more focused attention related to the stimuli in gifted in comparison with control children. Meanwhile, latency of P1 in this study was also higher in the gifted group, which shows a possibly compensatory slower brain reaction in gifted adolescents. However, another study that used the identification of facial expressions task found no differences in P1 between gifted and control younger adolescent boys (Liu et al., 2015). This inconsistency may be explained by the fact that the authors used an affective task, whereas previous studies focused on cognitive tasks, which typically show more stable differences. The absence of differences in P1 for younger adolescents might suggest that they have not yet developed the same cognitive processing capabilities as older adolescents. Furthermore, the differing methods used to identify gifted children—RAPM in one study and Cattell’s culture fair test in another—could contribute to the varying findings across studies.
Articles also examined ERP component N2, which is believed to be related to both classic response conflict (Liu et al., 2011b) and emotional conflict (Li et al., 2020). According to our review, N2 had a larger amplitude in gifted children, which can be interpreted as enhanced conflict processing. The results are consistent between the two studies despite the fact that authors studied different age groups and used different methods for determining giftedness. The latest study by Li et al. (2020) also revealed lower N2 latency in gifted children, which can be interpreted as a faster reaction to the conflict.
Most articles devoted their analyses to a rather late ERP component P3 related to attention allocation processes (Polich, 2007). Most studies revealed increased Р3 amplitude in gifted primary school students and younger adolescents, both in response to a stimulus and in response to a cue (Liu et al., 2011a, 2011b; Zhang et al., 2006), while using different cognitive tasks—from visual search to cognitive control tasks. Also P3 in gifted children had shorter latency (Liu et al., 2011b; Zhang et al., 2006). This is consistent with behavioral findings showing shorter reaction times to cognitive tasks in favor of gifted children. Together, these findings may indicate enhanced and accelerated processes of voluntary or effortful attention processes in gifted children. The increase in P300 during top-down switching was also more pronounced in gifted younger adolescents (Duan and Shi, 2014). Thus, the amplitude and latency of P300 may account for the accelerated reaction times in attentional tasks among gifted children described in the Cognitive ability section.
The N400 is a negative deflection in the ERP waveform of the brain’s electrical activity generally linked with language processing, object recognition, facial recognition, action processing, gesture processing, mathematical cognition, semantic memory, recognition memory, and different developmental and acquired disorders (Kutas and Federmeier, 2011). Our synthesis of past studies showed increased amplitudes in the N400 associated with mental rotation (Anomal et al., 2020) and stimulus discrimination (Liu et al., 2007), presumably reflecting the ability of gifted individuals to devote more cognitive resources to these processes.
In summary, ERP research indicates that gifted children may exhibit more advanced information processing in the later processing stages, such as conflict resolution, top-down attention, discrimination of target stimuli, and mental rotation. Contradictory data have been obtained for the earliest stages of perception. These contradictions might be resolved by considering such experimental factors as age, the specifics of the task, and the method of assessing giftedness. Notice also that gifted children demonstrated increased speed of information processing in the later, more complex processing stages related to attention and conflict resolution, rather than earlier stages of perception (i.e., P1). This may suggest that cognitive differences between gifted children and their peers are more pronounced in complex, late-stage descending processes.
In addition to ERP, EEG activity can be studied by analyzing oscillations obtained using spectral analysis of the EEG signal and reflecting phase-synchronized fluctuations of the membrane potential of neurons (Siegel et al., 2012). Unlike ERPs, studies of EEG oscillations demonstrate not only increased activation related to cognitive processes in gifted children, but also decreased activation. When performing easy tasks for reasoning, gamma rhythm power (30–45 Hz) was lower in gifted older adolescents, which can be interpreted within the framework of neural efficiency hypothesis (Zhang et al., 2015). At the same time, gamma rhythm power increased when the task became more difficult in gifted older adolescents (Zhang et al., 2015). These inconsistent results might also be explained by the relation between task difficulty and the neural efficiency effect and indicate the need to take into account task difficulty in EEG studies of giftedness. It is also important to note that in this study, giftedness was partly determined through mathematical ability. A recent EEG study has shown that when studying the effects of neuronal efficiency, it is important to consider whether math or general giftedness is being assessed (Waisman et al., 2023).
Overall, one could decipher that gifted children demonstrate consistently enhanced and accelerated brain reaction only when performing complex tasks (such as complex reasoning), complex effortful processes (such as cognitive control when processing words) and complex (reflected in late ERP components) stimulus processing processes. This phenomenon might imply the ability to enhance complex information processing that distinguishes gifted children from control children. Our synthesis of results suggests that gifted children demonstrate the ability to activate top-down processing faster and more intensively (excluding at least switching processes). This may indicate an advantage for gifted children in using the limited cognitive resources required to implement this processing. Alternatively, one can entertain that perhaps gifted children have either more resources available or a more optimal functioning of the mechanism for mental effort allocation (Shenhav et al., 2017).
Other indicators of functional brain activity of gifted children in comparison with control children are sparse. BOLD activity in the posterior parietal cortex was increased in gifted older adolescents during a reasoning task (Lee et al., 2006), indicating that superior cognitive ability may stem from enhanced functionality within the fronto-parietal network rather than activation of additional brain regions. Supporting this, Ma et al. (2017) found that gifted children exhibit higher local connection density while relying less on brain hub regions. Furthermore, Nusbaum et al. (2017) demonstrated enhanced inter- and intra-hemispheric white matter integrity in gifted primary school children and young adolescents in frontal, central, and associative pathways, aligning with studies identifying the fronto-parietal network as crucial for intelligence (Dunst et al., 2014; Navas-Sánchez et al., 2014). Additionally, Kimmel and Deboskey (1978) reported that gifted children showed larger initial skin conductance responses and slower habituation compared to average peers, highlighting a connection between autonomic reactivity and intellectual functioning in children.
Overall, the difference in brain activity between the groups is detected only in the late components of evoked potential and in complex top-down processes. This might be explained by the fact that gifted children easily perform better than control children in simple tasks but in order to outperform on complex ones they need to employ additional neural resources. Speculatively, it can be assumed that this brain functioning specificity allows gifted children to outperform their peers in effortful cognitive tasks. However, it is still unclear if it is connected to the greater amount of such neural resources or to their increased motivation in response to the complex task. We will shed some light on this question in the next chapter on psychological characteristics of gifted children.
Psychological characteristics
In this section, we present findings related to the psychological characteristics of gifted children. A deeper understanding of these traits can provide valuable insights into gifted children’s motivations and self-perceptions, enabling educators to create optimal conditions for unlocking abilities and ensuring mental wellbeing of such children.
Motivation is believed to be highly correlated with giftedness (Lens and Rand, 2000). Some researchers have even included motivation in the definition of giftedness (Gottfried and Gottfried, 2004). Moreover, according to some theories, motivation is a catalyst or resource for the development of giftedness (Gagné, 1985; Sternberg and Lubart, 1993), and low motivation is considered to be the reason for the academic underachievement of gifted children (Whitmore, 1986).
Studies comparing gifted children with a control group have all found higher scores on intrinsic motivation in gifted children (Bergold et al., 2020; Gottfried and Gottfried, 1996; Guez et al., 2018). Intrinsic motivation can be defined as the most self-determined form of motivation, where a student engages in a behavior spontaneously, out of interest and enjoyment. In developmental studies, intrinsic motivation is usually investigated as academic intrinsic motivation, which includes enjoyment of school learning and characterized by curiosity; mastery achievement motivation; persistence in learning; striving for new challenging tasks (Gottfried et al., 2001). External motivation, i.e., exogenous motivation in which a behavior is driven by external factors like encouragement or punishment, is also increased in gifted younger adolescents according to Guez et al. (2018). It is important to note that the causal relationship between motivation and giftedness remains unknown. Moreover, motivation can increase the results of intelligence tests (Duckworth et al., 2011), thus influencing the selection of gifted children. Achievement motivation as an independent construct was also found to be more pronounced in gifted children (Wirthwein et al., 2019). Same results were reported by Gottfried et al. (2006), although the article did not pass the criteria of our review.
Often along with motivation, researchers investigate perceived self-efficacy, a characteristic which can be defined as self-esteem of capabilities in various fields of activity (Bandura and Cervone, 1986). Some authors even include it in the concept of motivation (Schunk and Pajares, 2002), while those who consider self-efficacy a separate personality characteristic have shown that it is increased in gifted children. In particular, this result is observed for academic self-efficacy (Guez et al., 2018), math ability self-concept (Bergold et al., 2020), self-esteem intelligence (Wirthwein et al., 2019), as well as self-regulatory efficacy (i.e., self-esteem of the ability to resist peer pressure to exhibit deviant risk behavior). These consistent results indicate that gifted children highly appreciate their abilities in various areas. It is not clear, however, whether high self-esteem is a consequence or a cause of giftedness. Only for social self-efficacy there was no difference between the groups (Guez et al., 2018). Social self-efficacy was defined as self-esteem of different social abilities. This divergent finding may be explained by López and Sotillo’s (2009) results, which indicated that gifted children did not differ from the control group in measures of social adaptation based on various questionnaires, thus confirming their self-assessment. Speculatively, it can be assumed that enhanced motivation and self-efficacy are related to the specificity of effort allocation mechanism that allow gifted children to outperform their peers in effortful cognitive functions.
Studies using the Big 5 factors found that gifted adolescents were significantly more Open to experience and did not differ from their peers in Extraversion, Agreeableness and Conscientiousness (Limont et al., 2014; Wirthwein et al., 2019). The authors suggested that openness as the desire for new experience might be necessary for the development of giftedness. However, it is important to note that the results obtained do not indicate whether the revealed psychological characteristics of gifted children are a consequence or a cause of giftedness.
In summary, motivation seems to be consistently associated with giftedness, with gifted children showing higher levels of both intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, as well as enhanced self-efficacy—except for social self-efficacy. Gifted adolescents are also more open to experience.
To summarize the reviewed studies on the distinctive cognitive, psychophysiological, and psychological characteristics of gifted children, we highlight their unique traits across various domains. Our findings suggest a general advantage of gifted children in either reaction time or accuracy across key cognitive domains, including verbal working memory and attentional switching but also inhibition, geometric problem solving and elemental information processing. This advantage aligns with enhanced and accelerated brain activity during complex effortful processes, presumably due to their greater availability of brain resources or higher motivation in gifted children. Our findings further support the notion of generally higher intrinsic motivation in gifted children in comparison with their peers in the control group. Additionally, our investigation revealed heightened self-efficacy and openness to experience in gifted children as well as higher school achievements and different problem-solving strategies.
The importance of these results comes both from insights gained in basic research by uncovering characteristics of giftedness, and from the contribution these findings may make to the development of educational programs tailored for gifted individuals. Advantage in reaction times and accuracy across specific mentioned cognitive domains can aid in developing more targeted assessment methods for children’s abilities. The observation that gifted children exhibit enhanced brain activity during complex tasks but roughly equivalent one in comparison with their peers during simple tasks, might suggest to educators that the most developmental environments for gifted students are those that provide increased intellectual challenges. Additionally, by examining the brain patterns of gifted children in general, researchers can clarify the underlying mechanisms behind high intellectual potential. Gifted children’s heightened intrinsic motivation, self-efficacy, and openness to experience can offer educators valuable insights into creating environments that support their potential and wellbeing.
In the next section we elaborate on the future research directions to advance the state of the art in the field of intellectual giftedness.
Considerations for future studies
Cognitive function terminology and measurement variability
The review of the literature and the generalized interpretation of the results of research are greatly hampered by inconsistent terminology describing cognitive functions. Different authors rely on different theoretical models of cognitive abilities, which gives rise to an unlimited number of studied functions and their terms. We recorded approximately 50 different terms that describe higher top-down information processing (see column ‘Target of analysis’ in Supplementary Table 1S). Most terms denoted some form of attention, both in the broadest sense (as cognitive control or executive functions), and its various aspects (divided, selective, spatial, etc.). Many authors have also studied higher-level cognitive abilities, such as reading, abstract reasoning, mental rotation, etc. It was often difficult to decipher specific semantic differences among studied functions, for example, between analogical and abstract reasoning, attentional control and cognitive control. To mitigate this limitation, we used terminology proposed by original articles. We also encourage future studies to add a short paragraph outlining how the terminology they use relates to others in the field (Table 11, step 1).
To examine cognitive functions of gifted and control children a wide variety of tasks was created by researchers. Variability in measurements of cognitive abilities complicates the feasibility of conducting a quantitative meta-analysis due to the heterogeneity of data. Further, the use of complex tasks that activate several cognitive mechanisms at the same time might complicate the comparison between the studies. Although not always possible because of time and resource restrictions it is advisable to include well-established cognitive tasks that can build the literature for ultimately identifying convergence across studies using quantitative meta-analyses.
Lack of standardization in methods
The field “Other tests” takes the second position in the list of widely used test to identify giftedness in children (Figure 2). This category includes all the validated tests which were used for selection. The profusion of tests, even those with established credibility, could potentially impact the coherence among them, further making interpretations more challenging. The use of thresholds in different studies to define giftedness varies significantly, reflecting differences in assessment tools, age groups, and research objectives. This lack of standardization suggests a need for more consistency and clarity in defining and measuring high intelligence in research and educational settings. To ultimately reach consensus we propose that future studies provide a rational for assessment, metrics and score criteria used to classify groups (Table 11, step 2).
Binary group division
We only analyzed data from studies that compared psychological measures between the groups of gifted and control children. However, the idea of dividing participants into two groups may be controversial in its origin. Though, it is necessary to determine the cut-off point when selecting gifted children to study in a gifted class, for research purposes there is no clear ground for such division as linear regression models are always available to identify the relation of giftedness with other characteristics of participants. Besides, categorizing intelligence through precise numerical divisions is somewhat contrived, given that intelligence spans a broad spectrum. Therefore, we suggest that future studies consider correlational approaches or organizing three or more groups specified criteria (Table 11, step 6).
Reporting style
The variability in the reporting styles of the studies also affects the result of systematic reviews and meta-analyses. Missing indication on the method of selection (or unclear definition), age and number of participants, and task descriptions limits the ability to conduct comprehensive and reliable syntheses of existing research. Variability in reporting style also has several implications on research in general. It may hinder the ability of other researchers to replicate the study accurately. Besides, inconsistencies in reporting make it challenging to compare results across different studies and may impact the generalizability of study findings. For these reasons, in our seven steps for studying outstanding performers we propose that future studies clearly define sample recruitment details and demographics, provide details paradigm administration, report statistical significance thresholds and tabulate descriptive statistics.
In summary, current research on gifted children reveals notable methodological, conceptual, and reporting gaps. These include variations in measurement techniques, a lack of standardization in assessment methods, contentious binary group classifications, inconsistent terminology for cognitive functions, and disparities in reporting styles, all of which impede reliable comparisons and synthesis of findings.
Limitations
The review has some important limitations. First, the study included search only from one database Web of Science due to resource constraints. Web of Science is one of the most popular and widely used databases for academic research and publication, which is known for its interdisciplinary coverage. Web of Science is also recognized for hosting high-impact journals and authoritative sources in gifted education and intelligence fields. Prior to finalizing our database selection, we conducted preliminary scoping searches in databases Web of Science and PubMed databases. The search in PubMed revealed substantial overlap with the articles indexed in Web of Science and did not yield a significant number of additional unique studies relevant to our topic. Therefore, we determined that Web of Science alone was sufficient to capture the breadth of literature necessary for our review. Nevertheless, we acknowledge that limiting our search to single database might bias the coverage of the relevant literature. Second, we acknowledge that restricting our review to English-language articles may introduce language bias, limit the comprehensiveness of our findings and affect the generalizability of our conclusions. Future research could benefit from including non-English studies or collaborating with multilingual researchers to provide a more exhaustive overview of the literature.
Third, the study includes only dichotomized information (gifted or non-gifted children), thus ignoring literature that examined correlation between constructs. We acknowledge that this fact diminishes the variability in the data and makes the results harder to interpret, especially with the differing cutoffs across studies. However, in the educational context identification of talented children is always a practical task: children either meet the criterion established by a specific program or not. Thus, in the current systematic review we rely on binary group allocation.
Forth, all the eligible studies were given the same weight when summarizing the results. It was done to ensure that a diverse range of perspectives and findings are considered in the review, which is particularly relevant as the research of giftedness is broad and there is limited consensus in the literature. Assigning equal weight might also help to mitigate potential biases that could arise from selectively emphasizing studies with larger sample sizes or more complex methodologies. This approach promotes a more balanced representation of the available evidence. Nevertheless, giving equal weight to all relevant papers may potentially affect the overall robustness of the review findings. In addition, the review did not analyze effect sizes of the included studies, since in many articles it was not indicated or was calculated by different metrics.
Conclusion
This systematic review has provided a synthesis of the prevalent intelligence tests in identifying intellectual giftedness in children and the clearer understanding of cognitive, physiological and psychological characteristics which distinguish gifted individuals from their control peers. Our findings delineate a noticeable shift from traditional intelligence tests toward the utilization of more culturally appropriate measures. Comparisons among gifted and control children reveal superior abilities of the former ones in verbal working memory, inhibition, geometric problem solving, increased speed in attention-switching and elemental information processing. Consistently, psychophysiological assessments demonstrate heightened and accelerated brain activity during effortful cognitive processes. Psychological and behavioral tests further demonstrate that gifted children score higher on tests measuring intrinsic motivation, self-efficacy, and openness to experience, as well as achieving higher grades in school and employing better problem-solving strategies. We propose a simple seven step checklist for studying outstanding performers as our review emphasizes the need for continued research and refinement in assessment methodologies to advance the state of the art in the field of intellectual giftedness and address the unique needs of gifted children in educational context.
Data availability statement
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.
Author contributions
EK: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AL: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. NZ: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. MA: Conceptualization, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work is an output of a research project implemented as part of the Basic Research Program at the National Research University Higher School of Economics (HSE University).
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1411981/full#supplementary-material
Footnotes
References
Abelman, R. (1995). Gifted, LD, and gifted/LD children’s understanding of temporal sequencing on television. J. Broadcast. Electron. Media 39, 297–312. doi: 10.1080/08838159509364308
Aliotti, N. C. (1981). Intelligence, handedness, and cerebral hemispheric preference in gifted adolescents. Gifted Child Quart. 25, 36–41. doi: 10.1177/001698628102500107
Anomal, R. F., Brandão, D. S., Porto, S. B., de Oliveira, S. S., de Souza, R. F. L., De Fiel, J., et al. (2020). The role of frontal and parietal cortex in the performance of gifted and average adolescents in a mental rotation task. PLoS One 15:e0232660. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0232660
Arffa, S. (2007). The relationship of intelligence to executive function and non-executive function measures in a sample of average, above average, and gifted youth. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 22, 969–978. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.08.001
Bandura, A., and Cervone, D. (1986). Differential engagement of self-reactive influences in cognitive motivation. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 38, 92–113. doi: 10.1016/0749-5978(86)90028-2
Bélanger, J., and Gagné, F. (2006). Estimating the size of the gifted/talented population from multiple identification criteria. J. Educ. Gift. 30, 131–163. doi: 10.4219/jeg-2006-258
Berg, D. H., and McDonald, P. A. (2018). Differences in mathematical reasoning between typically achieving and gifted children. J. Cogn. Psychol. 30, 281–291. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2018.1457034
Bergman, C., Ferrer-Wreder, L., and Modig, K. (2014). High IQ in early adolescence and career success in adulthood: findings from a Swedish longitudinal study. Res. Hum. Dev. 11, 165–185. doi: 10.1080/15427609.2014.936261
Bergold, S., Wirthwein, L., Rost, D. H., and Steinmayr, R. (2015). Are gifted adolescents more satisfied with their lives than their non-gifted peers? Front. Psychol. 6:1623. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01623
Bergold, S., Wirthwein, L., and Steinmayr, R. (2020). Similarities and differences between intellectually gifted and average-ability students in school performance, motivation, and subjective well-being. Gifted Child Quart. 64, 285–303. doi: 10.1177/0016986220932533
Boake, C. (2002). From the Binet–Simon to the Wechsler–Bellevue: tracing the history of intelligence testing. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 24, 383–405. doi: 10.1076/jcen.24.3.383.981
Bucaille, A., Jarry, C., Allard, J., Brochard, S., Peudenier, S., and Roy, A. (2022). Neuropsychological profile of intellectually gifted children: a systematic review. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 28, 424–440. doi: 10.1017/S1355617721000515
Bui, S. A., Craig, S. G., and Imberman, S. A. (2011). Is gifted education a bright idea? Assessing the impact of gifted and talented programs on achievement. National Bureau of Economic Research. Available at: https://www.nber.org/papers/w17089 (Accessed February 10, 2024).
Buros Center for Testing. (n.d.). University of Nebraska Press. Available at: https://www.nebraskapress.unl.edu/9780910674683/the-twenty-first-mental-measurements-yearbook (Accessed October 26, 2024).
Calero, M. D., Belen, G.-M. M., and Robles, M. A. (2011). Learning potential in high IQ children: the contribution of dynamic assessment to the identification of gifted children. Learn. Individ. Differ. 21, 176–181. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2010.11.025
Calero, M. D., García-Martín, M. B., Jiménez, M. I., Kazén, M., and Araque, A. (2007). Self-regulation advantage for high-IQ children: findings from a research study. Learn. Individ. Differ. 17, 328–343. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2007.03.012
Carter, K. R., and Ormrod, J. E. (1982). Acquisition of formal operations by intellectually gifted children. Gifted Child Quart. 26, 110–115. doi: 10.1177/001698628202600304
Casino-García, A. M., Llopis-Bueno, M. J., and Llinares-Insa, L. I. (2021). Emotional intelligence profiles and self-esteem/self-concept: an analysis of relationships in gifted students. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:1006. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18031006
Chung, D., Yun, K., Kim, J. H., Jang, B., and Jeong, J. (2011). Different gain/loss sensitivity and social adaptation ability in gifted adolescents during a public goods game. PLoS One 6:e17044. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017044
Coyle, T. R., Read, L. E., Gaultney, J. F., and Bjorklund, D. F. (1998). Giftedness and variability in strategic processing on a multitrial memory task: evidence for stability in gifted cognition. Learn. Individ. Differ. 10, 273–290. doi: 10.1016/S1041-6080(99)80123-X
Demirhan, E., Randler, C., Beşoluk, Ş., and Horzum, M. B. (2018). Gifted and non-gifted students’ diurnal preference and the relationship between personality, sleep, and sleep quality. Biol. Rhythm. Res. 49, 103–117. doi: 10.1080/09291016.2017.1333568
Duan, X., Dan, Z., and Shi, J. (2013). The speed of information processing of 9- to 13-year-old intellectually gifted children. Psychol. Rep. 112, 20–32. doi: 10.2466/04.10.49.PR0.112.1.20-32
Duan, X., and Shi, J. (2014). Attentional switching in intellectually gifted and average children: effects on performance and ERP. Psychol. Rep. 114, 597–607. doi: 10.2466/04.10.PR0.114k21w8
Duan, X., Wei, S., Wang, G., and Shi, J. (2010). The relationship between executive functions and intelligence on 11- to 12-year-old children. Psychol. Test Assess. Model. 52, 419–431.
Duckworth, A. L., Quinn, P. D., Lynam, D. R., Loeber, R., and Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (2011). Role of test motivation in intelligence testing. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 108, 7716–7720. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1018601108
Dunst, B., Benedek, M., Koschutnig, K., Jauk, E., and Neubauer, A. C. (2014). Sex differences in the IQ-white matter microstructure relationship: a DTI study. Brain Cogn. 91, 71–78. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2014.08.006
Fard, E. K., Keelor, J. L., Bagheban, A. A., and Keith, R. W. (2016). Comparison of the Rey auditory verbal learning test (RAVLT) and digit test among typically achieving and gifted students. Iran. J. Child Neurol. 10:26.
Flanagan, D., and Harrison, P. (2022). Contemporary intellectual assessment: fourth edition: theories, tests, and issues. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Corley, R. P., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., and Hewitt, J. K. (2006). Not all executive functions are related to intelligence. Psychol. Sci. 17, 172–179. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01681.x
Gagné, F. (1985). Giftedness and talent: reexamining a reexamination of the definitions. Gifted Child Quart. 29, 103–112. doi: 10.1177/001698628502900302
Gagné, F. (2004). Transforming gifts into talents: the DMGT as a developmental theory. High Abil. Stud. 15, 119–147. doi: 10.1080/1359813042000314682
Galton, S. F. (1870). Hereditary genius: an inquiry into its laws and consequences. New York, NY: D. Appleton.
Geake, J. G. (2008). The neurobiology of giftedness. Wystąpienie Na Konferencji 10th Asia-Pacific conference on giftedness, Singapore.
Geschwind, N., and Galaburda, A. M. (1985). Cerebral lateralization: biological mechanisms, associations, and pathology: I. A hypothesis and a program for research. Arch. Neurol. 42, 428–459. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1985.04060050026008
Giofrè, D., Mammarella, I. C., and Cornoldi, C. (2014). The relationship among geometry, working memory, and intelligence in children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 123, 112–128. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.01.002
Gottfried, A. E., Fleming, J. S., and Gottfried, A. W. (2001). Continuity of academic intrinsic motivation from childhood through late adolescence: a longitudinal study. J. Educ. Psychol. 93, 3–13. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.93.1.3
Gottfried, A. E., and Gottfried, A. W. (1996). A longitudinal study of academic intrinsic motivation in intellectually gifted children: childhood through early adolescence. Gifted Child Quart. 40, 179–183. doi: 10.1177/001698629604000402
Gottfried, A. E., and Gottfried, A. W. (2004). Toward the development of a conceptualization of gifted motivation. Gifted Child Quart. 48, 121–132. doi: 10.1177/001698620404800205
Gottfried, A. W., Gottfried, A. E., and Guerin, D. W. (2006). The Fullerton longitudinal study: a long-term investigation of intellectual and motivational giftedness. J. Educ. Gift. 29, 430–450. doi: 10.4219/jeg-2006-244
Guez, A., Peyre, H., Le Cam, M., Gauvrit, N., and Ramus, F. (2018). Are high-IQ students more at risk of school failure? Intelligence 71, 32–40. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2018.09.003
Hisam, A., Rahman, M. U., Mashhadi, S. F., Bilal, A., and Anam, T. (2015). Regular breakfast consumption associated with high intelligence quotient: myth or reality? Pak. J. Med. Sci. 31, 1084–1088. doi: 10.12669/pjms.315.7728
Hodges, J., Tay, J., Maeda, Y., and Gentry, M. (2018). A meta-analysis of gifted and talented identification practices. Gifted Child Quart. 62, 147–174. doi: 10.1177/0016986217752107
Hollingworth, L. S. (1930). Do intellectually gifted children grow toward mediocrity in stature? Pedagog. Semin. J. Genet. Psychol. 37, 345–360. doi: 10.1080/08856559.1930.9944161
Hung, Y., Gaillard, S. L., Yarmak, P., and Arsalidou, M. (2018). Dissociations of cognitive inhibition, response inhibition, and emotional interference: Voxelwise ALE meta-analyses of fMRI studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 4065–4082. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24232
Hutton, B., Salanti, G., Caldwell, D. M., Chaimani, A., Schmid, C. H., Cameron, C., et al. (2015). The PRISMA extension statement for reporting of systematic reviews incorporating network meta-analyses of health care interventions: checklist and explanations. Ann. Intern. Med. 162, 777–784. doi: 10.7326/M14-2385
Johnson, J., Im-Bolter, N., and Pascual-Leone, J. (2003). Development of mental attention in gifted and mainstream children: the role of mental capacity, inhibition, and speed of processing. Child Dev. 74, 1594–1614. doi: 10.1046/j.1467-8624.2003.00626.x
Kershner, J. R., and Ledger, G. (1985). Effect of sex, intelligence, and style of thinking on creativity: a comparison of gifted and average IQ children. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 48, 1033–1040. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.48.4.1033
Kimmel, H. D., and Deboskey, D. (1978). Habituation and conditioning of the orienting reflex in intellectually gifted and average children. Physiol. Psychol. 6, 377–380. doi: 10.3758/BF03326742
Košir, K., Horvat, M., Aram, U., and Jurinec, N. (2016). Is being gifted always an advantage? Peer relations and self-concept of gifted students. High Abil. Stud. 27, 129–148. doi: 10.1080/13598139.2015.1108186
Kranzler, J. H., Whang, P. A., and Jensen, A. R. (1994). Task complexity and the speed and efficiency of elemental information processing: another look at the nature of intellectual giftedness. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 19, 447–459. doi: 10.1006/ceps.1994.1032
Kroesbergen, E. H., Van Hooijdonk, M., Viersen, S. V., Middel-Lalleman, M. M. N., and Reijnders, J. J. W. (2015). The psychological well-being of early identified gifted children. Gifted Child Quart. 60, 16–30. doi: 10.1177/0016986215609113
Kutas, M., and Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62, 621–647. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123
Landau, E., and Weissler, K. (1993). Parental environment in families with gifted and nongifted children. J. Psychol. Interdis. Appl. 127, 129–142. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1993.9915549
Lavrijsen, J., Soenens, B., Vansteenkiste, M., and Verschueren, K. (2021). Is intelligence related to perfectionism? Multidimensional perfectionism and parental antecedents among adolescents across varying levels of cognitive ability. J. Pers. 89, 652–671. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12606
Lee, K. H., Choi, Y. Y., Gray, J. R., Cho, S. H., Chae, J.-H., Lee, S., et al. (2006). Neural correlates of superior intelligence: stronger recruitment of posterior parietal cortex. NeuroImage 29, 578–586. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.036
Leikin, M., Paz-Baruch, N., and Leikin, R. (2013). Memory abilities in generally gifted and excelling-in-mathematics adolescents. Intelligence 41, 566–578. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2013.07.018
Lens, W., and Rand, P. (2000). Motivation and future time perspective. Theory, research, and application. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of Self-Regulation, San Diego, CA: Academic Press. 221–235.
Lewis, J. D. (2001). Language isn’t needed: nonverbal assessments and gifted learners. Available at: https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED453026 (Accessed March 5, 2024).
Li, D., Liu, T., and Shi, J. (2020). Fluid intelligence and neural mechanisms of emotional conflict adaptation. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 152, 1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2020.03.004
Limont, W., Dreszer-Drogorób, J., Bedyńska, S., Śliwińska, K., and Jastrzębska, D. (2014). ‘Old wine in new bottles’? Relationships between overexcitabilities, the big five personality traits and giftedness in adolescents. Personal. Individ. Differ. 69, 199–204. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.06.003
Liu, T., Shi, J., Zhang, Q., Zhao, D., and Yang, J. (2007). Neural mechanisms of auditory sensory processing in children with high intelligence. Neuroreport 18, 1571–1575. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282ef7640
Liu, T., Xiao, T., Li, X., and Shi, J. (2015). Neural mechanism of facial expression perception in intellectually gifted adolescents. Neurosci. Lett. 592, 22–26. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2015.02.062
Liu, T., Xiao, T., Shi, J., and Zhao, D. (2011a). Response preparation and cognitive control of highly intelligent children: a Go-Nogo event-related potential study. Neuroscience 180, 122–128. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.02.022
Liu, T., Xiao, T., Shi, J., and Zhao, D. (2011b). Conflict control of children with different intellectual levels: an ERP study. Neurosci. Lett. 490, 101–106. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.12.035
López, V., and Sotillo, M. (2009). Giftedness and social adjustment: evidence supporting the resilience approach in Spanish-speaking children and adolescents. High Abil. Stud. 20, 39–53. doi: 10.1080/13598130902860739
Ma, J., Kang, H. J., Kim, J. Y., Jeong, H. S., Im, J. J., Namgung, E., et al. (2017). Network attributes underlying intellectual giftedness in the developing brain. Sci. Rep. 7:11321. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-11593-3
Mcclain, M.-C., and Pfeiffer, S. (2012). Identification of gifted students in the United States today: a look at state definitions, policies, and practices. J. Appl. Sch. Psychol. 28, 59–88. doi: 10.1080/15377903.2012.643757
McCutcheon, R. A., Keefe, R. S. E., and McGuire, P. K. (2023). Cognitive impairment in schizophrenia: aetiology, pathophysiology, and treatment. Mol. Psychiatry 28, 1902–1918. doi: 10.1038/s41380-023-01949-9
Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., and Altman, D. G.the PRISMA Group (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. Ann. Intern. Med. 151, 264–9, W64. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-151-4-200908180-00135
Montoya-Arenas, D. A., Aguirre-Acevedo, D. C., Díaz Soto, C. M., and Pineda Salazar, D. A. (2018). Executive functions and high intellectual capacity in school-age: completely overlap? Int. J. Psychol. Res. 11, 19–32. doi: 10.21500/20112084.3239
Navas-Sánchez, F. J., Alemán-Gómez, Y., Sánchez-Gonzalez, J., Guzmán-De-Villoria, J. A., Franco, C., Robles, O., et al. (2014). White matter microstructure correlates of mathematical giftedness and intelligence quotient. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 2619–2631. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22355
Nusbaum, F., Hannoun, S., Kocevar, G., Stamile, C., Fourneret, P., Revol, O., et al. (2017). Hemispheric differences in white matter microstructure between two profiles of children with high intelligence quotient vs. controls: a tract-based spatial statistics study. Front. Neurosci. 11:173. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00173
Page, M. J., McKenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., et al. (2021). The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. Int. J. Surg. 88:105906. doi: 10.1016/j.ijsu.2021.105906
Paz-Baruch, N., Leikin, M., Aharon-Peretz, J., and Leikin, R. (2014). Speed of information processing in generally gifted and excelling-in-mathematics adolescents. High Abil. Stud. 25, 143–167. doi: 10.1080/13598139.2014.971102
Paz-Baruch, N., Leikin, R., and Leikin, M. (2016). Visual processing in generally gifted and mathematically excelling adolescents. J. Educ. Gift. 39, 237–258. doi: 10.1177/0162353216657184
Paz-Baruch, N., Leikin, M., and Leikin, R. (2022). Not any gifted is an expert in mathematics and not any expert in mathematics is gifted. Gift. Talented Int. 37, 25–41. doi: 10.1080/15332276.2021.2010244
Piccardi, L., D’Antuono, G., Marin, D., Boccia, M., Ciurli, P., Incoccia, C., et al. (2019). New evidence for gender differences in performing the Corsi test but not the digit span: data from 208 individuals. Psychol. Stud. 64, 411–419. doi: 10.1007/s12646-019-00512-3
Pichot, P. (1948). French pioneers in the field of mental deficiency. Am. J. Ment. Defic. 53, 128–137.
Piro, J. M., Ortiz, C., and Manouvrier, L. (2021). Sleep behaviors and handedness in gifted and non-gifted children. Dev. Neuropsychol. 46, 425–434. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2021.1973473
Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin. Neurophysiol. 118, 2128–2148. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019
Renzulli, J. S. (2011). What makes giftedness?: reexamining a definition. Phi Delta Kappan 92, 81–88. doi: 10.1177/003172171109200821
Robinson, N. M., Zigler, E., and Gallagher, J. J. (2000). Two tails of the normal curve: similarities and differences in the study of mental retardation and giftedness. Am. Psychol. 55, 1413–1424. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.12.1413
Rocha, A., Almeida, L., and Perales, R. (2020). Comparison of gifted and non-gifted students’ executive functions and high capabilities. J. Educ. Gifted Young Sci. 8, 1397–1409. doi: 10.17478/jegys.808796
Saint-Simon, H. (1813). Mémoire sur la science de l’homme. Available at: https://cir.nii.ac.jp/crid/1130282272322131072 (Accessed February 11, 2023).
Schilling, S. R., Sparfeldt, J. R., and Rost, D. H. (2006). Families with gifted adolescents. Educ. Psychol. 26, 19–32. doi: 10.1080/01443410500340959
Schunk, D. H., and Pajares, F. (2002). “The development of academic self-efficacy” in Development of achievement motivation (United States: Elsevier), 15–31.
Shenhav, A., Musslick, S., Lieder, F., Kool, W., Griffiths, T. L., Cohen, J. D., et al. (2017). Toward a rational and mechanistic account of mental effort. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 40, 99–124. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-072116-031526
Siegel, M., Donner, T. H., and Engel, A. K. (2012). Spectral fingerprints of large-scale neuronal interactions. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 121–134. doi: 10.1038/nrn3137
Silverman, L. K., and Gilman, B. J. (2020). Best practices in gifted identification and assessment: lessons from the WISC-V. Psychol. Sch. 57, 1569–1581. doi: 10.1002/pits.22361
Snyder, H. R. (2013). Major depressive disorder is associated with broad impairments on neuropsychological measures of executive function: a meta-analysis and review. Psychol. Bull. 139, 81–132. doi: 10.1037/a0028727
Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Beyond IQ: a triarchic theory of human intelligence. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, xvi, 411.
Sternberg, R. J., and Davidson, J. E. (2005). Conceptions of giftedness, vol. 2. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Sternberg, R. J., and Lubart, T. I. (1993). Creative giftedness: a multivariate investment approach. Gifted Child Quart. 37, 7–15. doi: 10.1177/001698629303700102
Subotnik, R. F., Olszewski-Kubilius, P., and Worrell, F. C. (2011). Rethinking giftedness and gifted education: a proposed direction forward based on psychological science. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 12, 3–54. doi: 10.1177/1529100611418056
Tanabe, M. K., Whitaker, A. M., O’Callaghan, E. T., Murray, J., and Houskamp, B. M. (2014). Intellectual ability as a predictor of performance on the Wisconsin card-sorting test. Appl. Neuropsychol. Child 3, 275–283. doi: 10.1080/21622965.2012.757700
Tibken, C., Richter, T., Von Der Linden, N., Schmiedeler, S., and Schneider, W. (2022). The role of metacognitive competences in the development of school achievement among gifted adolescents. Child Dev. 93, 117–133. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13640
Topçu, S., and Leana-Taşcılar, M. Z. (2018). The role of motivation and self-esteem in the academic achievement of Turkish gifted students*. Gift. Educ. Int. 34, 3–18. doi: 10.1177/0261429416646192
Urben, S., Camos, V., Habersaat, S., and Stéphan, P. (2018). Faces presenting sadness enhance self-control abilities in gifted adolescents. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 36, 514–520. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12241
Vogelaar, B., Bakker, M., Hoogeveen, L., and Resing, W. C. M. (2017). Dynamic testing of gifted and average-ability children’s analogy problem solving: does executive functioning play a role? Psychol. Sch. 54, 837–851. doi: 10.1002/pits.22032
Vogelaar, B., Resing, W. C., Stad, F. E., and Sweijen, S. W. (2019). Is planning related to dynamic testing outcomes? Investigating the potential for learning of gifted and average-ability children. Acta Psychol. 196, 87–95. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2019.04.004
Vogl, K., and Preckel, F. (2013). Full-time ability grouping of gifted students. Gifted Child Quart. 58, 51–68. doi: 10.1177/0016986213513795
Waisman, I., Brunner, C., Grabner, R. H., Leikin, M., and Leikin, R. (2023). (lack of) neural efficiency related to general giftedness and mathematical excellence: an EEG study. Neuropsychologia 179:108448. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2022.108448
Waisman, I., Leikin, M., and Leikin, R. (2016). Brain activity associated with logical inferences in geometry: focusing on students with different levels of ability. ZDM 48, 321–335. doi: 10.1007/s11858-016-0760-5
Watkins, M., and Canivez, G. (2016). Review of the Wechsler intelligence scale for children–fifth edition: critique, commentary, and independent analyses, New York, NY: The Guilford Press. 683–702.
Webb, J. T., Amend, E. R., and Beljan, P. (2016). Misdiagnosis and dual diagnoses of gifted children and adults: Adhd, bipolar, Ocd, Asperger’s, depression, and other disorders. 2nd Edn. Tucson, AZ: SCB Distributors.
Wechsler, D. (2014). Wechsler intelligence scale for children | Fifth Edition. Available at: https://www.pearsonassessments.com/store/usassessments/en/Store/Professional-Assessments/Cognition-%26-Neuro/Wechsler-Intelligence-Scale-for-Children-%7C-Fifth-Edition-/p/100000771.html (Accessed October 23, 2024).
Weissler, K., and Landau, E. (1993). Characteristics of families with no, one, or more than one gifted child. J. Psychol. Interdis. Appl. 127, 143–152. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1993.9915550
Whitmore, J. R. (1986). Intellectual giftedness in young children: recognition and development, vol. 18. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Wirthwein, L., Bergold, S., Preckel, F., and Steinmayr, R. (2019). Personality and school functioning of intellectually gifted and nongifted adolescents: self-perceptions and parents’ assessments. Learn. Individ. Differ. 73, 16–29. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2019.04.003
Yun, K., Chung, D., Jang, B., Kim, J. H., and Jeong, J. (2011). Mathematically gifted adolescents have deficiencies in social valuation and mentalization. PLoS One 6:e18224. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018224
Zhang, L., Gan, J. Q., and Wang, H. (2014). Optimized gamma synchronization enhances functional binding of fronto-parietal cortices in mathematically gifted adolescents during deductive reasoning. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:430. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00430
Zhang, L., Gan, J. Q., and Wang, H. (2015). Localization of neural efficiency of the mathematically gifted brain through a feature subset selection method. Cogn. Neurodyn. 9, 495–508. doi: 10.1007/s11571-015-9345-1
Zhang, H., He, Y., Tao, T., and Shi, J.-N. (2016). Intellectually gifted rural-to-urban migrant children’s attention. High Abil. Stud. 27, 193–209. doi: 10.1080/13598139.2016.1242064
Zhang, Q., Shi, J., Luo, Y., Zhao, D., and Yang, J. (2006). Intelligence and information processing during a visual search task in children: an event-related potential study. Neuroreport 17, 747–752. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000215774.46108.60
Keywords: giftedness, intelligence, children, intelligence tests, cognitive abilities
Citation: Kuznetsova E, Liashenko A, Zhozhikashvili N and Arsalidou M (2024) Giftedness identification and cognitive, physiological and psychological characteristics of gifted children: a systematic review. Front. Psychol. 15:1411981. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1411981
Edited by:
Douglas F. Kauffman, Consultant, Greater Boston area, MA, United StatesReviewed by:
István Szamosközi, Babeș-Bolyai University, RomaniaIsmail Xodabande, Kharazmi University, Iran
Copyright © 2024 Kuznetsova, Liashenko, Zhozhikashvili and Arsalidou. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Elizaveta Kuznetsova, elizaveta.kuznetsova@helsinki.fi
†These authors have contributed equally to this work