Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Psychol., 30 May 2024
Sec. Perception Science

How is visual separation assessed? By counting distance units

  • Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, George Washington University, Washington, DC, United States

How does the human visual system assess the separation between pairs of stimuli in a frontal plane? According to the direct (or subtractive) view the system finds the difference between the positions of the stimuli in a localization system. According to the indirect (or additive) view the system finds the number of instances of a distance unit lying between representations of the stimuli. Critically, position is explicitly represented under the direct view, with separation being derived from position. Position is not explicitly represented under the indirect view; separation is consequently inferred by counting an internal unit of distance. Recent results favor the indirect over the direct view of separation assessment. Dissociations between assessments of separation and position, various context effects in the assessment of separation, and suggestions that position information is not cleanly accessed argue against the direct view. At the same time, various context effects in separation assessment argue for the indirect view. Recent findings regarding the brain bases of vision are consistent with the indirect view. In short, recent results suggest that assessing the separation between two frontal stimuli involves integrating distance units between representations of the stimuli.

Introduction

How does the human visual system assess the separation between pairs of stimuli in a frontal plane? Two sorts of explanation have been proposed (Watt, 1992). According to direct (or subtractive) accounts the visual system finds the difference between the positions of the stimuli in a localization system (Morgan and Regan, 1987; Burbeck and Yap, 1990; Stuart et al., 1993; Burbeck et al., 1996; Kohly and Regan, 2000). The localization system for separation assessment is sometimes understood in terms of local sign: Stimulation at a particular retinal position registers as a particular local sign. A particular local sign can be represented by a particular neural element (Matin, 1972; Rose, 1999) or a correlation among the patterns of activity in a set of neural elements (Koenderink, 1990, 2019). The separation between two stimuli can be derived from differences in local sign. For example, Burbeck and Hadden (1993) proposed that separation is assessed by a system of linked position encoders. When a pair of test stimuli is presented, multiple pairs of position encoders are activated, with the position encoders in each pair being linked by a pre-existent connection giving a particular separation. The separation between the test stimuli is derived from the relative degree of activation for position-encoder pairs associated with different separations (See also Burbeck and Pizer, 1995).

According to indirect (or additive) accounts the visual system assesses the separation between two stimuli by finding the number of instances of a distance unit lying between representations of the stimuli (Tsal and Shalev, 1996; McGraw and Whitaker, 1999; MacEvoy and Fitzpatrick, 2006). For example, Hisakata et al. (2016) proposed that separation is assessed in terms of neural elements that express local distance units. The separation between two stimuli is assessed by integrating the units that fall between representations of the stimuli.

To highlight the differences between the direct and indirect accounts of separation assessment, it is useful to compare the way the two accounts might explain a fundamental fact about separation assessment – that the refinement of separation discrimination decreases in proportion to the separations being discriminated (often called Weber’s law for separation) (Wolfe, 1923). Direct accounts have attributed the Weber-like relationship to decreases in the resolution of position information with increases in eccentricity (Levi et al., 1988) and separation (Burbeck and Hadden, 1993). Indirect accounts could attribute the relationship to increases in the numbers of integration errors with increases in the magnitude of the assessed separation.

Supporting the indirect view

The present paper argues for the indirect view of separation assessment. In support of this view note, first, the evidence that has been reported against the direct view. Assessments of separation and position are often dissociated in ways difficult to explain if position information supports separation assessment. For example, although observers make errors in assessing the separation between the vertices of a Müller-Lyer figure, observers fail to make errors in indicating the positions of those vertices (Gillam and Chambers, 1985). In addition, a recent study found reports of remembered separations and positions to be dissociated in a way that is difficult to explain if separations are mentally represented in terms of positions. In the crucial task participants remembered the separations and positions of points on an axis. Although the to-be-remembered positions determined the to-be-remembered separations, memories for the two sorts of information were dissociated; memory for the separations was subject to bias and scaling effects from which memory for the positions was free (Dopkins and Collier, 2024).

Additional evidence against the direct view comes from various context effects in separation assessment. For example, the separation between pairs of test stimuli depends on the separation between each of the individual test stimuli and flanker stimuli. The test–test separation is assessed as relatively large when the test-flanker separation is smaller than the test–test separation and relatively small when the test-flanker separation is larger than the test–test separation (Hess and Badcock, 1995). Similarly, the separation between pairs of stimuli is assessed as smaller the smaller the sizes of the stimuli and the greater the spatial frequency of their background (McGraw et al., 2012). Contextual dependence such as this is difficult to square with a comparison of position information. Further evidence against the direct view comes from a task in which participants make judgments of horizontal or vertical separation with respect to pairs of dots that differ on horizontal and vertical axes. The precision of a given horizontal or vertical separation judgment decreases with increases in the vertical or horizontal separation, respectively, between the dots. By implication, participants cannot selectively access information about horizontal or vertical position. This is inconsistent with a simple direct account, under which the horizontal and vertical position information, giving horizontal and vertical separation, respectively, should be selectively accessible (Dopkins, 2005; Dopkins and Sargent, 2014; Dopkins and Hoyer, 2015, 2018).

Also incompatible with the direct view is evidence against the local sign idea. A number of factors can shift the perceived location of a stimulus away from the location given by its retinal position (Suzuki and Cavanagh, 1997; Whitney and Cavanagh, 2000; Zimmermann et al., 2013). Further, evidence has been advanced that position is represented implicitly rather than explicitly. Analysis of the correspondence between stimuli and their retinal images implies that the visual system represents differential spatial relations between stimulus aspects rather than the aspects themselves (Lappin and Craft, 2000). In addition, physiological data support the capacity of the visual system for the implicit representation of position; the responses of large-receptive-field cells in lateral intraparietal monkey cortex are consistent with such representation (Sereno and Lehky, 2011). And imaging data suggest the use of such representation. Attention increases receptive field size and position discriminability in the ventral stream, with the conjunction of these increases suggesting implicit representation of position (Kay et al., 2015).

At the same time, support has been accumulating for the indirect view of separation assessment. A number of context effects have been reported that are easier to square with the indirect than the direct view. In separation assessment, a pair of dots is evaluated as being closer together following perception of a dynamic random dot array than following perception of a blank field (Hisakata et al., 2016). The effect only occurs when the dot pair is close to the array (Jovanovic et al., 2022), where the closeness of the dot pair to the array can be assessed in world-centered as well as retinotopic terms. Similar results have been observed as a function of motor adaptation (as a byproduct of tapping) (Petrizzo et al., 2020). To explain these results researchers have proposed that separation assessment involves integrating distance units along the path separating representations of the test dots and that the adaptation process increases the size of the distance unit such that the path for a given test-dot pair encompasses fewer instances of that unit.

Similarly, a pair of dots is evaluated as being closer following short (120 s) bouts of coordinated two-point stimulation at eccentricities that are optimal and values of separation that match rather than mismatch those of the test dots. The researchers who observed these results attributed them to the strengthening of lateral connections between distance units in V1, with the number of units between the representations of two points being the basis of the assessed separation between the points (Song et al., 2017). Such strengthening was suggested as a possible mechanism underlying the effects that Hisakata et al. (2016) observed.

The forgoing effects of adaptation and learning on accuracy were not associated with changes in precision. Consistent with this, Chambers et al. (2018) showed that adaptation to a textured annulus (a stimulus similar to those producing the forgoing effects of adaptation and learning) produces perceived compression in the region surrounded by the annulus but no change in crowding within that region. More recently, however, changes in precision have been observed that may reflect similar mechanisms as do the forgoing adaptation and learning effects. Precision in discriminating the mean separation from smaller and larger members in a critical set of separations was higher in a group of participants for whom additional levels of separation were interleaved between the critical levels. The critical separation levels, the average separation level, and the range of separation levels were held constant across the Interleaved group and a control group for whom additional separation levels were not interleaved (Dopkins and McIntire, 2022). Building on the account of Hisakata et al. the researchers proposed (1) that the separation between two points is assessed by integrating the distance units between representations of the two points, and (2) that the size of the distance units decreases as the separation levels tested are more closely spaced. The researchers noted the difficulty of explaining their results under a direct account, asking why, under such an account, assessing the separation between a pair of points should depend on the assessment of separation levels interleaved between the separation level of the points? A rival account of these results, however, is that the interleaved levels allowed more precise learning of the response criterion. In argument against this rival account, a similar effect of interleaved levels was observed in a follow-up task that required discriminating each of the separation levels tested in the original experiment from several smaller and larger levels. Because the Interleaved group in this follow-up task had to learn more response criteria than the Control group, the higher precision in the Interleaved group probably did not reflect better criterion learning (Dopkins, 2024).

Further support for the indirect view has come from tasks requiring assessment of vertical and horizontal separation (Dopkins and Galyer, 2020). Pairs of points have for years been known to be assessed as further apart when oriented on the vertical as opposed to the horizontal axis. The precision of vertical and horizontal separation assessment has not been examined, however. Using a new technique in which vertical and horizontal assessments are both made against the same implicit standard, Dopkins and Galyer demonstrated that, when the two sorts of assessment are made in the same context, vertical assessments are more precise than horizontal assessments. Noting the difficulty of explaining their results under a direct account Dopkins and Galyer proposed (1) that the separation between two points is assessed by integrating the distance units between the two points, and (2) that, when vertical and horizontal separation are assessed in the same context the distance units have smaller vertical than horizontal extent, with the result that vertical assessments are more precise than horizontal assessments.

Finally, if we grant that the size assessment is related to separation assessment, we find that certain peripheral aspects of size assessment are consistent with the indirect view. An object is perceived as larger when its image falls on a compressed retina and smaller when its image falls on a stretched retina (Duke-Elder, 1934; Critchley, 1953; Winn et al., 1988). Further, an object is perceived as smaller when it is observed peripherally than when it is observed more foveally (Newsome, 1972; Schneider et al., 1978; Thompson and Fowler, 1980).

Brain bases

What are the brain bases of the indirect separation assessment process that Hisakata et al. (2016), Song et al. (2017), Dopkins and Galyer (2020), and Dopkins and McIntire (2022) propose? The distance units are probably represented early in the visual system. Consistent with this view, V1 has been linked with judgments of Vernier acuity, which have been localized at the same level of processing as judgments of separation (Duncan and Boynton, 2003; Vesker and Wilson, 2013). Similarly, V1 has been linked with judgments of size, which are related to judgments of separation. The critical results were observed under conditions in which two stimuli encompassed the same visual angle, one stimulus seemed farther away than the other, and the far stimulus was perceived as being larger than the near stimulus (e.g., in the Ponzo illusion). In this situation, the far stimulus, which was perceived as larger, activated a larger area in V1 than the near stimulus, which was perceived as smaller (Murray et al., 2006; Fang et al., 2008; see also Pooresmaeili et al., 2013). Thus, perceived size was positively correlated with the amount of V1 activated. As a possible account of these results MacEvoy and Fitzpatrick (2006) proposed that, in this situation, neurons in V1 shift their receptive fields toward the center of the stimulus so that the boundaries of the stimulus encompass a larger number of neurons. Under this account the shift has a functional basis in that the perceived size of a stimulus depends on the number of V1 neurons involved in representing the stimulus. Neurophysiological and fMRI evidence supports the claim of receptive field shift. Early reports suggested that the diameters of some human and monkey V1 receptive fields increase with decreases in viewing distance (Marg and Adams, 1970; Smith and Marg, 1975). Later reports suggested that receptive fields are tuned to the ‘perceived’ as opposed to the retinal size of the stimuli (Ni et al., 2014; He et al., 2015). More generally, judged size is correlated with V1 population receptive field size (Moutsiana et al., 2016). On the basis of this research V1 has been accorded an important role in judgments of size. Notice also that the forgoing results are consistent with the indirect account of separation assessment in that they suggest a relationship between perceived size and the amount of V1 activation.

Other results support the account further. The magnitude of several size illusions is negatively correlated with surface area of V1 (Schwarzkopf et al., 2011; Schwarzkopf and Rees, 2013). These results suggest the relevance of lateral V1 connections for size judgments because the impact of these connections is reduced when V1 surface area is larger (Schwarzkopf, 2015). Such lateral connections have been proposed as a mechanism for the integration of distance units (Song et al., 2017). Finally, V1 has been said to have the structure of a scale-space, comprising fields of Gaussian ‘samplers,’ of differing grains, with samplers at more refined grains being sampled by samplers of coarser grains, such that samplers of all grains at a given spatial location are correlated (Koenderink, 2019). Such a multi-scale representation is consistent with proposals that the distance units underlying separation assessment can vary in size (Dopkins and Galyer, 2020; Dopkins and McIntire, 2022).

It makes sense that V1 would be involved in separation and size assessment. First, visual representation is more refined here than elsewhere in the brain (Srinivasan et al., 2015). Second, V1 represents visual space very consistently. The cortical magnification factor (CMF), the number of neurons dedicated to processing a part of the visual field, decreases with increases in distance from the center of the visual field (eccentricity). At the same time the size of the population receptive field (pRF) increases with eccentricity. As a result of the aforementioned two relationships, the population point image size, the product of CMP and pRF size, is nearly constant as a function of eccentricity. By implication, the point image size may be constant as a function of eccentricity (Harvey and Dumoulin, 2011).

Of course, other brain areas in addition to V1 are presumably involved in separation and size assessment. Mechanisms that integrate distance units are probably housed later in the system, possibly serving multiple modalities (Hisakata and Kaneko, 2021). TMS, lesion, and imaging results suggest that the contextual information (e.g., depth information) needed for size scaling is incorporated by higher visual areas such as the lateral occipital cortex (Frassinetti et al., 1999; Servos, 2006; Plewan et al., 2012; Zeng et al., 2020). Imaging data imply that the right superior parietal cortex supports the actual evaluation process (Plewan et al., 2012). Explicit values of separation and size may even be represented cortically. An area of the parietal cortex has been reported to support topographically organized tuning for visual object size (Harvey et al., 2015).

Of relevance to the brain bases of separation and size assessment are proposals that length, size, duration, numerosity are assessed in terms of a generalized magnitude representation (Dormal and Pesenti, 2007; Walsh, 2003; Lourenco and Longo, 2011; Bonn and Cantlon, 2017; Martin et al., 2017). Magnitude assessment in terms of this representation is often conceived in Bayesian terms as process of accumulation (Martin et al., 2017). Supporting this view are studies showing involvement of overlapping parietal areas with magnitude judgments of various sorts (Bueti and Walsh, 2009; Dormal and Pesenti, 2009, 2012; Dormal et al., 2012; Cona et al., 2021) and behavioral studies showing cross-dimensional interactions in magnitude judgment (Lambrechts et al., 2013). The view of separation as being assessed in terms of a generalized magnitude representation comports with the indirect view of separation assessment.

Summary

In sum, recent results support a view under which visual separation is assessed through the integration of distance units. These units may be housed in V1, with integrative processes occurring in later areas of the visual system. Little support has emerged for the rival view under which visual separation is assessed through the comparison of position values. These conclusions regarding visual separation are paralleled by similar conclusions regarding the somatosensory system. Here, position is apparently coded in population terms, with the differences in the profile of neural activity across fibers in a nerve increasing with the distance between the pairs of positions stimulated (Ray and Doetsch, 1990). Brushing the skin produces patterns of activity that are distributed across surprisingly large regions of the cortex (Tommerdahl et al., 1993). Tactile separation is said to be assessed by ‘counting’ the number of neurons between the somatotopic map locations for the points at which stimulation is occurring (Longo, 2006). Distortions and anisotropies in the assessment of tactile separation have been explained in terms of this account.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

SD: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Bonn, C. D., and Cantlon, J. F. (2017). Spontaneous, modality-general abstraction of a ratio scale. Cognition 169, 36–45. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.07.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bueti, D., and Walsh, V. (2009). The parietal cortex and the representation of time, space, number and other magnitudes. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 364, 1831–1840. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0028

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Burbeck, C. A., and Hadden, S. (1993). Scaled position integration areas: accounting for Weber's law for separation. J. Optical Soc. Am. A Optics Image Sci. 10, 5–15. doi: 10.1364/JOSAA.10.000005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Burbeck, C. A., and Pizer, S. M. (1995). Object representation by cores: identifying and representing primitive spatial regions. Vis. Res. 35, 1917–1930. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)00286-U

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Burbeck, C. A., Pizer, S. M., Morse, B. S., Ariely, D., Zauberman, G. S., and Rolland, J. P. (1996). Linking object boundaries at scale: a common mechanism for size and shape judgments. Vis. Res. 36, 361–372. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00106-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Burbeck, C. A., and Yap, Y. L. (1990). Spatiotemporal limitations in bisection and separation discrimination. Vis. Res. 30, 1573–1586. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(90)90145-B

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chambers, A., Johnston, A., and Roach, N. W. (2018). Visual crowding is unaffected by adaptation-induced spatial compression. J. Vis. 18:12. doi: 10.1167/18.3.12

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cona, G., Wiener, M., and Scarpazza, C. (2021). From ATOM to GradiATOM: cortical gradients support time and space processing as revealed by a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. NeuroImage 224:117407. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117407

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Critchley, M. (1953). The parietal lobes. London: Edward Arnold.

Google Scholar

Dopkins, S. (2005). Access to dimensional values can be unselective during early perceptual processing. Percept. Psychophys. 67, 513–530. doi: 10.3758/BF03193328

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dopkins, S. (2024). Criterion learning cannot account for increased precision with the interleaving additional levels. In preparation.

Google Scholar

Dopkins, S., and Collier, G. (2024). Separation and position do not show covarying patterns of bias. In preparation.

Google Scholar

Dopkins, S., and Galyer, D. (2020). Horizontal–vertical anisotropy with respect to bias and sensitivity. J. Vis. 20:1. doi: 10.1167/jov.20.7.1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dopkins, S., and Hoyer, D. (2015). Dimensional interaction in distance judgment. Perception 44, 490–510. doi: 10.1068/p7723

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dopkins, S., and Hoyer, D. (2018). Analytic processing of distance. Acta Psychol. 182, 129–137. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2017.11.016

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dopkins, S., and McIntire, G. (2022). Greater sensitivity in separation discrimination with closer spacing of separation levels tested. J. Vis. 22:5. doi: 10.1167/jov.22.9.5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dopkins, S., and Sargent, J. (2014). Analyzing distances in a frontal plane. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 76, 420–437. doi: 10.3758/s13414-013-0576-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dormal, V., Andres, M., and Pesenti, M. (2012). Contribution of the right intraparietal sulcus to numerosity and length processing: an fMRI-guided TMS study. Cortex 48, 623–629. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.05.019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dormal, V., and Pesenti, M. (2007). Numerosity-length interference: a Stroop experiment. Exp. Psychol. 54, 289–297. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.54.4.289

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dormal, V., and Pesenti, M. (2009). Common and specific contributions of the intraparietal sulci to numerosity and length processing. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 2466–2476. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20677

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dormal, V., and Pesenti, M. (2012). “Processing magnitudes within the parietal cortex” in Horizons in neuroscience research. eds. A. Costaand and E. Villalba, vol. 8 (New York, NY: Nova Science Publishers), 107–140.

Google Scholar

Duke-Elder, W. S. (1934). Textbook of ophthalmology, vol. 1. St Louis, MO: Mosby.

Google Scholar

Duncan, R. O., and Boynton, G. M. (2003). Cortical magnification within human primary visual cortex correlates with acuity thresholds. Neuron 38, 659–671. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00265-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fang, F., Boyaci, H., Kersten, D., and Murray, S. O. (2008). Attention-dependent representation of a size illusion in human V1. Curr. Biol. 18, 1707–1712. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.09.025

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Frassinetti, F., Nichelli, P., and Di Pellegrino, G. (1999). Selective horizontal dysmetropsia following prestriate lesion. Brain 122, 339–350. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.2.339

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillam, B., and Chambers, D. (1985). Size and position are incongruous: measurements on the Müller-Lyer figure. Percept. Psychophys. 37, 549–556. doi: 10.3758/BF03204921

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harvey, B. M., and Dumoulin, S. O. (2011). The relationship between cortical magnification factor and population receptive field size in human visual cortex: constancies in cortical architecture. J. Neurosci. 31, 13604–13612. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2572-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harvey, B. M., Fracasso, A., Petridou, N., and Dumoulin, S. O. (2015). Topographic representations of object size and relationships with numerosity reveal generalized quantity processing in human parietal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 112, 13525–13530. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1515414112

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

He, D., Mo, C., Wang, Y., and Fang, F. (2015). Position shifts of fMRI-based population receptive fields in human visual cortex induced by Ponzo illusion. Exp. Brain Res. 233, 3535–3541. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4425-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, R. F., and Badcock, D. R. (1995). Metric for separation discrimination by the human visual system. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 12, 3–16. doi: 10.1364/JOSAA.12.000003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hisakata, R., and Kaneko, H. (2021). Temporal enhancement of cross-adaptation between density and size perception based on the theory of magnitude. J. Vis. 21:11. doi: 10.1167/jov.21.11.11

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hisakata, R., Nishida, S., and Johnston, A. (2016). An adaptable metric shapes perceptual space. Curr. Biol. 26, 1911–1915. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.05.047

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jovanovic, L., McGraw, P. V., Roach, N. W., and Johnston, A. (2022). The spatial properties of adaptation-induced distance compression. J. Vis. 22:7. doi: 10.1167/jov.22.11.7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kay, K. N., Weiner, K. S., and Grill-Spector, K. (2015). Attention reduces spatial uncertainty in human ventral temporal cortex. Curr. Biol. 25, 595–600. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.12.050

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koenderink, J. J. (1990). The brain a geometry engine. Psychol. Res. 52, 122–127. doi: 10.1007/BF00877519

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koenderink, J. J. (2019). Vision, an optical user interface. Perception 48, 545–601. doi: 10.1177/0301006619853758

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kohly, R. P., and Regan, D. (2000). Coincidence detectors: visual processing of a pair of lines and implications for shape discrimination. Vis. Res. 40, 2291–2306. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(00)00090-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lambrechts, A., Walsh, V., and van Wassenhove, V. (2013). Evidence accumulation in the magnitude system. PLoS One 8:e82122. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0082122

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lappin, J. S., and Craft, W. D. (2000). Foundations of spatial vision: from retinal images to perceived shapes. Psychol. Rev. 107, 6–38. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.107.1.6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Levi, D. M., Klein, S. A., and Yap, Y. L. (1988). Weber's law' for position: unconfounding the role of separation and eccentricity. Vis. Res. 28, 597–603. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(88)90109-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Longo, M. R. (2006). Distorted body representations in healthy cognition. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 70, 378–388. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1143956

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lourenco, S., and Longo, M R. (2011). “Origins and development of generalized magnitude representation” in Space, time and number in the brain. eds. S. Dehaene and E. Brannon. International Symposium on Attention and Performance (24th: 2010 : Cernay-la-Ville, France.

Google Scholar

MacEvoy, S. P., and Fitzpatrick, D. (2006). Visual physiology: perceived size looms large. Curr. Biol. 16, R330–R332. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.076

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marg, E., and Adams, J. E. (1970). Evidence for a neurological zoom system in vision from angular changes in some receptive fields of single neurons with changes in fixation distance in the human visual cortex. Experientia 26, 270–271. doi: 10.1007/BF01900088

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, B., Wiener, M., and van Wassenhove, V. (2017). A Bayesian perspective on accumulation in the magnitude system. Sci. Rep. 7:630. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-00680-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Matin, L. (1972). “Eye movements and perceived visual direction” in Visual psychophysics. eds. D. Jameson and L. M. Hurvich (Berlin: Springer).

Google Scholar

McGraw, P. V., Roach, N. W., Badcock, D. R., and Whitaker, D. (2012). Size-induced distortions in perceptual maps of visual space. J. Vis. 12:8. doi: 10.1167/12.4.8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McGraw, P. V., and Whitaker, D. (1999). Perceptual distortions in the neural representation of visual space. Exp. Brain Res. 125, 122–128. doi: 10.1007/s002210050667

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morgan, M. J., and Regan, D. (1987). Opponent model for line interval discrimination: interval and Vernier performance compared. Vis. Res. 27, 107–118. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(87)90147-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moutsiana, C., de Haas, B., Papageorgiou, A., van Dijk, J. A., Balraj, A., Greenwood, J. A., et al. (2016). Cortical idiosyncrasies predict the perception of object size. Nat. Commun. 7:12110. doi: 10.1038/ncomms12110

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, S. O., Boyaci, H., and Kersten, D. (2006). The representation of perceived angular size in human primary visual cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 429–434. doi: 10.1038/nn1641

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Newsome, L. R. (1972). Visual angle and apparent size of objects in peripheral vision. Percept. Psychophys. 12, 300–304. doi: 10.3758/BF03207209

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ni, A. M., Murray, S. O., and Horwitz, G. D. (2014). Object-centered shifts of receptive field positions in monkey primary visual cortex. Curr. Biol. 24, 1653–1658. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.06.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrizzo, I., Anobile, G., and Arrighi, R. (2020). Motor adaptation distorts visual space. Vis. Res. 171, 31–35. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2020.04.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Plewan, T., Weidner, R., Eickhoff, S. B., and Fink, G. R. (2012). Ventral and dorsal stream interactions during the perception of the Müller-Lyer illusion: evidence derived from fMRI and dynamic causal modeling. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 2015–2029. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00258

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pooresmaeili, A., Arrighi, R., Biagi, L., and Morrone, M. C. (2013). Blood oxygen level-dependent activation of the primary visual cortex predicts size adaptation illusion. J. Neurosci. 33, 15999–16008. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1770-13.2013

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ray, R. H., and Doetsch, G. S. (1990). Coding of stimulus location and intensity in populations of mechanosensitive nerve fibers of the raccoon: II. Across-fiber response patterns. Brain Res. Bull. 25, 533–550. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(90)90110-L

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rose, D. (1999). The historical roots of the theories of local signs and labelled lines. Perception 28, 675–685. doi: 10.1068/p2881

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, B., Ehrlich, D. J., Stein, R., Flaum, M., and Mangel, S. (1978). Changes in the apparent lengths of lines as a function of degree of retinal eccentricity. Perception 7, 215–223. doi: 10.1068/p070215

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwarzkopf, D. S. (2015). Where is size in the brain of the beholder? Multisens. Res. 28, 285–296. doi: 10.1163/22134808-00002474

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwarzkopf, D. S., and Rees, G. (2013). Subjective size perception depends on central visual cortical magnification in human V1. PLoS One 8:e60550. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0060550

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwarzkopf, D. S., Song, C., and Rees, G. (2011). The surface area of human V1 predicts the subjective experience of object size. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 28–30. doi: 10.1038/nn.2706

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sereno, A. B., and Lehky, S. R. (2011). Population coding of visual space: comparison of spatial representations in dorsal and ventral pathways. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 4:159. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2010.00159

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Servos, P. (2006). Preservation of Emmert’s law in a visual form agnosic. Neurocase 12, 122–126. doi: 10.1080/13554790600586241

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, J. D., and Marg, E. (1975). Zoom neurons in visual cortex: receptive field enlargements with near fixation in monkeys. Experientia 31, 323–326. doi: 10.1007/BF01922564

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, C., Haun, A. M., and Tononi, G. (2017). Plasticity in the structure of visual space. ENeuro 4, ENEURO.0080–ENEU17.2017. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0080-17.2017

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Srinivasan, S., Carlo, C. N., and Stevens, C. F. (2015). Predicting visual acuity from the structure of visual cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 112, 7815–7820. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1509282112

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stuart, G., Bossomaier, T. R. J., and Johnson, S. (1993). Preattentive processing of object size: implications for theories of size perception. Perception 22, 1175–1193. doi: 10.1068/p221175

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Suzuki, S., and Cavanagh, P. (1997). Focused attention distorts visual space: an attentional repulsion effect. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 23, 443–463

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Thompson, J. G., and Fowler, K. A. (1980). The effects of retinal eccentricity and orientation on perceived length. J. Gen. Psychol. 103, 227–232. doi: 10.1080/00221309.1980.9921003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tommerdahl, M., Favorov, O. V., Whitsel, B. L., Nakhle, B., and Gonchar, Y. A. (1993). Minicolumnar activation patterns in cat and monkey SI cortex. Cereb. Cortex 3, 399–411. doi: 10.1093/cercor/3.5.399

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsal, Y., and Shalev, L. (1996). Inattention magnifies perceived length: the attentional receptive field hypothesis. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 22, 233–243

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Vesker, M., and Wilson, H. R. (2013). Face context advantage explained by Vernier and separation discrimination acuity. Front. Psychol. 3:617. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00617

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 483–488. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.09.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Watt, R. J. (1992). Visual analysis and the representation of spatial relation. In G. W. Humphreys (Ed.) Understanding vision (pp 19–38). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.

Google Scholar

Whitney, D., and Cavanagh, P. (2000). Motion distorts visual space: shifting the perceived position of remote stationary objects. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 954–959. doi: 10.1038/78878

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Winn, B., Ackerley, R. G., Brown, C. A., Murray, F. K., Prais, J., and St. John, M. F. (1988). Reduced aniseikonia in axial anisometropia with contact lens correction. Ophthalmic Physiol. Optics 8, 341–344,

Google Scholar

Wolfe, H. K. (1923). On the estimation of the middle of lines. Am. J. Psychol. 34, 313–358. doi: 10.2307/1413954

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeng, H., Fink, G. R., and Weidner, R. (2020). Visual size processing in early visual cortex follows lateral occipital cortex involvement. J. Neurosci. 40, 4410–4417. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2437-19.2020

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmermann, E., Fink, G., and Cavanagh, P. (2013). Perifoveal spatial compression. J. Vis. 13:21. doi: 10.1167/13.5.21

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: separation, position, distance perception, local sign, frontal plane, distance unit, size perception, counting distance units

Citation: Dopkins S (2024) How is visual separation assessed? By counting distance units. Front. Psychol. 15:1410297. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1410297

Received: 31 March 2024; Accepted: 14 May 2024;
Published: 30 May 2024.

Edited by:

Srimant Prasad Tripathy, University of Bradford, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

David Whitaker, Cardiff University, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2024 Dopkins. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Stephen Dopkins, dopkins@gwu.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.