Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 14 August 2024
Sec. Evolutionary Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Hormones and Person Perception View all 5 articles

Do lifestyle and hormonal variables explain links between health and facial attractiveness?

  • 1Human Evolution Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Nipissing University, North Bay, ON, Canada
  • 2Department of Social Sciences, Canadore College, North Bay, ON, Canada

Introduction: Facial attractiveness has recently been considered an indicator of underlying immunocompetence. However, studies examining this relationship have yielded mixed findings. Previous research suggested that these discrepant findings could be due to the common influence of lifestyle factors upon both rated facial attractiveness and health.

Methods: Young men (N = 162) provided standardized facial photos with a neutral expression subsequently rated by eight women for overall attractiveness. Saliva was assayed for immunoglobulin A, testosterone (T) and cortisol (C), and body fat was measured using a skinfold caliper. Self-reports of poor health, and lifestyle factors that could influence health status (age, sleep habits, smoking, drinking alcohol, family stress, and exercising) were collected.

Results: Results showed that symptoms of poor health and skinfold negatively predicted facial attractiveness. There was a modest but statistically non-significant T x C interaction where higher T lower C men trended toward having more attractive faces. A sequential mediation model examining the influence of lifestyle showed support for an indirect effect on facial attractiveness. Specifically, skinfold and poor health symptoms mediated the links between exercise, stress, and facial attractiveness.

Discussion: These findings suggest links between facial attractiveness and immunocompetence could be linked to some common lifestyle and hormonal variables, but that more comprehensive research involving lifestyle indicators (such as nutrition) are necessary.

1 Introduction

Humans are remarkably consistent in their assessment of what constitutes an attractive face (Langlois et al., 1991, 2000; Cunningham et al., 1995). Symmetry, averageness, skin quality, and hormone-linked sexually dimorphic features together form a facial structure that may be considered along a spectrum of attractiveness to the opposite sex (Arnocky et al., 2014). Faces may be an important source of reproductively relevant information accessible within a small amount of space (see Arnocky et al., 2014 for review). Specifically, attractive faces are believed to serve as a cue to an individual’s genotypic quality (e.g., Hume and Montgomerie, 2001). Indeed, some previous research has reported positive links between facial attractiveness and health (see Arnocky et al., 2014; Jones et al., 2021 for review). From this perspective, ancestors who happened to prefer immunocompetence-linked morphological traits, such as those contributing to facial attractiveness, would have mated with partners who were better able to survive, accrue resources, and successfully rear offspring, and to have produced healthier offspring who themselves would be more likely to survive and reproduce.

In support of the facial attractiveness immunocompetence hypothesis, Shackelford and Larsen (1997) found that facial asymmetry was linked with poorer health among men and women. Hume and Montgomerie (2001) found that female facial attractiveness was tied to their body mass index (BMI) and health history, whereas facial attractiveness in men was linked to their childhood socioeconomic standing, which could indicate a role of environmental or lifestyle factors affecting facial development. There is also circumstantial evidence suggesting a putative link between facial attractiveness and immunocompetence. Humans tend to reliably rate more attractive faces as being healthier. For instance, Fink et al. (2006) found that female faces that were more symmetrical were also more attractive, but also were perceived as healthier. Foo et al. (2020) found that facial traits contributing to overall attractiveness, such as averageness, symmetry, skin yellowness, and adiposity in men, predicted raters’ perceptions of the health of those faces. Facial attractiveness is also tied to mating success in some studies: men with attractive faces have more short-term sex partners, and women with attractive faces start having sex at an earlier age and have more long-term sex partners (Rhodes et al., 2005). Some research suggests that there may be a sex difference in the link between facial attractiveness and immunocompetence. For example, men (Rantala et al., 2012), but not women (Rantala et al., 2013), with attractive faces have a stronger immune response to a hepatitis vaccine.

Still, other research has found null links between facial attractiveness and health. Kalick et al. (1998) examined the relationship between adult health and their rated facial attractiveness at late adolescence. They found no links across the lifespan. Nevertheless, raters inaccurately perceived attractive faces as being healthier within the sample. Similar findings were observed by Foo et al. (2020), where (as described earlier), attractive faces were viewed as healthier by raters, yet facial attractiveness was nevertheless unrelated to markers of immunocompetence including oxidative stress and lysozyme activity. Other research using a large (> 4,000 participant) sample found no links between longitudinal measures of childhood health and facial asymmetry (Pound et al., 2014). More recently, Cai et al. (2019) found that neither female facial attractiveness, sexual dimorphism, averageness, or coloration predicted self-reported health or salivary immunoglobulin-A (sIgA). Similarly, other work found that male facial attractiveness did not predict antibody levels following vaccination (Pátková et al., 2022).

1.1 Considering potentially important covariates

Jones et al. (2021) recently suggested that the discordant findings pertaining to the link between facial attractiveness and health might be due to covariates that could impact both variables. Specifically, they proposed that “rather than reflecting immunocompetence, facial attractiveness is instead more closely linked to aspects of lifestyle that produce health benefits” (pp. 3). The researchers argued that lifestyle factors, which can vary intra-individually over time, might explain changes in individuals’ facial attractiveness over time. Which lifestyle factors are relevant to facial attractiveness and health? Jones et al. (2021) focused on the examples of diet and body fat, which certainly have implications for health status and may have a stronger link to facial attractiveness than do markers of immunocompetence (Cai et al., 2019).

Rantala et al. (2013) found that body fat was curvilinearly related to facial attractiveness: Women with low or high body fat were rated as less attractive than those having intermediate body fat. Exercise also has well-established links to health (e.g., Akimoto et al., 2003; Murphy et al., 2009). Diets rich in highly processed and refined foods, typical of Western populations, have been linked to a range of physical and mental health problems (Cordain et al., 2005). Focusing on unhealthy dietary habits, Visine et al. (2024) found that consumption of food high in refined carbohydrates with a high glycemic load was associated with reduced facial attractiveness (rated by opposite-sex others) in both women and men. These effects remained after controlling for potential confounds, including age, sexual dimorphism, BMI, physical activity, smoking, and relationship status. Despite well-established links between exercise and health (e.g., Murphy et al., 2009), the link between exercise and facial attractiveness is less clear. Hönekopp et al. (2010) found that a composite measure of physical fitness predicted rated body but not facial attractiveness. Yet other research has shown that higher performance athletes are rated as being more facially attractive (e.g., Bagozzi et al., 2018). Moreover, men with stronger grip strength are rated as being more facially attractive (Fink et al., 2007).

Other candidates include exposure to smoke and alcohol, which when used in excess are known to have widespread negative health consequences (see Hurley et al., 2012 for review). Prototype faces of identical twins who smoke are rated less attractive than the non-smoking twin images (Skinner et al., 2017). Likely mechanisms of smoking-related change in attractiveness include skin wrinkling, pale-yellow (i.e., sallow) complexion, and gaunt facial structure (reviewed in Doshi et al., 2007). Some studies show that acute alcohol use can increase others’ ratings of the drinker’s facial attractiveness (e.g., Van Den Abbeele et al., 2015). Nevertheless, excessive alcohol use can lead to psoriasis, eczema, and skin infections (Higgins and Du Vivier, 1992) as well as jaundice, hyperpigmentation, and vascular issues including spider telangiectasias and angiomas (Liu et al., 2010).

Stress has also been implicated in both features influencing facial attractiveness (such as skin quality; see Koizumi et al., 2023 for review) and a diverse range of negative health consequences (see Apanius, 1998). Finally, sleep might also affect both facial attractiveness and health. Individuals photographed following 2 days of sleep restriction were rated as less attractive than when they had appropriate sleep. The researchers reasoned that aversion to mating with a sleep disturbed partner could help avoid sleep-related health issues (Sundelin et al., 2017).

Besides the study by Visine et al. (2024) described above, one other study to consider lifestyle factors in relation to facial attractiveness and health was conducted by Mengelkoch et al. (2022). These researchers examined rated facial attractiveness and various markers of health along with covariates, including BMI, adult socioeconomic status (SES), exercise, smoking behavior, and recent stress. However, given that these variables were not the primary focal point of the study, only those that were significantly related to rated attractiveness (BMI and age) were retained in their models. Their findings suggested that facial attractiveness was related with higher rates of phagocytosis and lower rates of bacterial growth in plasma, along with lower neutrophil counts, together suggesting better anti-bacterial immunity, but not with cellular proliferation or cytokine production.

1.2 Hormones

Hormones play an important role in coordinating phenotypic development (Roney, 2016) and therefore might also serve as important covariates when examining links between facial attractiveness and health. In a sample of young Latvian women, Rantala et al. (2013) found that facial attractiveness (as rated by men) was unrelated to the production of anti-hepatitis B surface antigen following a hepatitis B vaccination. However, they did find that (in addition to the body fat finding described earlier), women with high cortisol (C) had faces that were rated as less attractive. The authors considered that perhaps facial attractiveness serves as a cue to one’s exposure to, or ability to cope with, life stressors, or that low C also signals health in humans. Other research has found either null links between women’s facial attractiveness and C (Gonzalez-Santoyo et al., 2015, Study 1) or mixed results whereby some samples rate women with low C as having either more attractive (US raters) or less attractive (Mexican raters) faces (Gonzalez-Santoyo et al., 2015). Meta-analysis shows that flatter diurnal Cortisol slopes are associated with diverse negative health markers (Adam et al., 2017; see also: Knack et al., 2013). Nevertheless, comparatively less work has considered the role between men’s cortisol and their facial attractiveness.

Testosterone (T) is another hormone that may be complicit in both men’s health and facial attractiveness. Some research has shown that men with higher T are rated by women as having more attractive faces (e.g., Roney et al., 2006; Rantala et al., 2012), and T also has implications for immune functioning. For example, T is positively associated with sIgA in men (Arnocky et al., 2018; Hodges-Simeon et al., 2020). Yet other studies have found null links between T and rated male facial attractiveness (e.g., Swaddle and Reierson, 2002; Neave et al., 2003; Penton-Voak and Chen, 2004), and others have found null links between T, C, and both facial attractiveness and other-rated perceptions of health (Kandrik et al., 2017). Some researchers have suggested that relying on baseline T or C levels may be insufficient, and that the dual hormone hypothesis involving an interaction between high T and low C might be complicit in phenotypic masculinization. Indeed, Rantala et al. (2012) found that high T low C men’s facial photos were rated as most attractive by women. However, other research has failed to observe these effects (Kordsmeyer et al., 2019). For example, T, C, and percentage of adipose tissue were unrelated to ratings of men’s facial attractiveness (Pátková et al., 2022). Thus, more research is needed regarding the potential impacts of immunocompetence by hormone interactions on the development of phenotypic characteristics and the perceived attractiveness of those traits (Davis and Arnocky, 2022).

2 The present study

The goal of this research was to examine whether individual lifestyle factors, as well as abdominal skinfold measurements, that are theoretically common to both facial attractiveness and health might eliminate these links when controlled for in a regression analysis. In so doing, this research also aimed to examine whether previously reported links between facial attractiveness and biological and self-report markers of health are broadly replicable, given previously inconsistent findings.

Two indices of health were examined in the current study: Self-reports of poor health symptom frequency and severity and, following Cai et al. (2019), salivary immunoglobulin-A (sIgA). sIgA is a potentially important marker of underlying immunocompetence that is produced by plasma B cells, comprising over 70% of our mucosal antibodies (Macpherson et al., 2008) that provide an initial defense against pathogens (Macpherson et al., 2008; Brandtzaeg, 2009). Low levels of sIgA have been linked to increased infection (Fahlman and Engels, 2005; Nakamura et al., 2006; Volkmann and Weekes, 2006) as well as to self-reported severity and frequency of poor health symptoms in otherwise healthy young adult university students (Arnocky et al., 2023) and, longitudinally, to death in older adults (Phillips et al., 2015). sIgA has previously been linked to other apparently sexually selected phenotypic traits that may serve as cues to underlying immunocompetence, including the deep male voice (Arnocky et al., 2018) and female breast morphology (Locke and Arnocky, 2021).

We expected an initial negative bivariate correlation between facial attractiveness and self-reported symptoms of poor health (Hypothesis 1A), and a positive correlation between facial attractiveness and sIgA (Hypothesis 1B). We then examined whether controlling for age (in years), lifestyle variables (sleep, familial stress, alcohol and tobacco use, exercise) and skinfold, along with hormones that have been linked to both health and facial attractiveness (T, C, and a T x C interaction), would weaken any observed links between symptoms of poor health, sIgA, and facial attractiveness (Hypothesis 2). Finally, we considered a sequential mediation model whereby unhealthy lifestyle habits have an indirect effect on facial attractiveness. Specifically, we expected that unhealthy lifestyle variables would predict a thicker abdominal skinfold, which has been identified as a predictor of future health problems in previous research (Loh et al., 2018). Therefore, in our model, skinfold was entered as a predictor of poor health symptoms, which in turn would predict lower rated facial attractiveness (Hypothesis 3, see Figure 1).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Path model for indirect effects of lifestyle variables upon facial attractiveness via skinfold and symptoms of poor health. Solid lines depict statistically significant paths, dashed lines depict statistically non-significant paths. Standardized coefficients shown. * = p < .05; ** = p < 0.01; *** = p < 0.001.

3 Materials and methods

3.1 Participants and procedure

As a part of a larger study on immune function and phenotypic development, males from a small Canadian University and the community were recruited through the institutional research participation system and posters in local businesses around town. A sample size calculation was performed using G*Power (3.1.9.7) with an expected medium effect size (F2 = 0.15), 80% power, α = 0.05, and 12 predictors, which yielded a sample size of 127. The total sample in the existing data set was 162 young adult men, aged 18–39 years (Mage = 22.7, SD = 4.7; 91.4% were students). The ethnic composition of the sample was Caucasian (90%), Black (4%), East Asian (3%), South Asian (2%), and Indigenous/First peoples (1%). Participants received either $50 CAD remuneration or partial course credit and $10. This research received approval by the Nipissing University Research Ethics Board (protocol # 100770–26,667).

3.2 Measures

3.2.1 Hormones and immunoglobulin-a

Participants were asked not to eat, drink (except water), brush their teeth, or exercise 2 hrs prior to their testing session, and were rescheduled if they reported any current or recent acute symptoms of illness during their telephone screener prior to their session. Saliva samples were collected in 5 mL polystyrene culture tubes and stored at −80°C until assayed in duplicate via enzyme immunoassay kits (DRG International, NJ, United States) in the Principal Investigator’s lab. Sample provision time ranged between 8:30 AM and 5:00 PM. For sIgA, intra- and inter-assay CVs were below 6%. For T (pg/mL), intra-assay CV was below 4% and the inter-assay CV was below 8%. For C (ng/mL), the intra-assay CV was below 2% and the inter-assay CV was below 11%. To account for the typical non-normal distribution of these markers, the average of the duplicates was log-transformed. Given that salivary flow rate affects sIgA levels, we corrected the concentration value to reflect a flow (mL/s)-corrected μg/mL score (log-transformed). Sample provision time was related to C, and participant age was related to both C (r = −0.21, p = 0.007) and T (r = −0.24, p = 0.002), whereas sIgA was unrelated to either age or sample provision time.

3.2.2 Unhealthy lifestyle

As part of a self-reported health screener, participants then completed items which addressed unhealthy lifestyle factors, including alcohol consumption (“How many alcoholic beverages do you drink [on average] per week?”), smoking/tobacco exposure (“Do you smoke?” [binary]), life stress (“Have you or your family recently experienced any life changes or unusual psychological stress?” [binary]), exercise (“Do you exercise regularly?” [binary]), along with sleep (“How many hours do you sleep on average at night?” [continuous]). A second indicator of unhealthy lifestyle was individuals’ skinfold measurement obtained using a digital body fat caliper. Thicker skinfold is associated with unhealthy eating habits from an early age (e.g., Dalrymple et al., 2019), including being linked to consumption of ultra-processed foods (Rohatgi et al., 2017), and is associated with a host of cardiometabolic risks in adulthood (González-Torres et al., 2023). Accordingly, skinfold has been used by researchers as an indicator of nutritional status (e.g., Bernstein et al., 2002). Body fat is highly correlated with facial adiposity (see Sierra-Johnson and Johnson, 2004) and plays an important role in determining male facial attractiveness (Windhager et al., 2011). The participant’s suprailliac skinfold (approximately one inch about the right hipbone) was measured three times and then averaged, (α = 0.99, 95% LLCI = 0.991, 95% ULCI = 0.995).

3.2.3 Self-reported health

Self-reported health was assessed using The Health Symptoms Survey (Knack et al., 2011, 2012), which records both the frequency and severity of physical health problems. The measure demonstrates good construct validity, correlating with health-linked personality factors and behavioral issues (Knack et al., 2012), altered hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis functioning (Knack et al., 2011), and sIgA as a biological marker of immunocompetence (Arnocky et al., 2023). The measure includes 56 items ranging from 1 (Not at All/Does not Hurt at All) to 4 (All the Time/Unbearable Pain) to determine the frequency and severity (28 items each) of symptoms, including stomach aches, flu, mouth sores, fatigue, chest pain, diarrhea, muscle aches and pains, headache or migraine, coughing, and fever experienced over the past year. A mean score was created with the measure demonstrating good internal consistency (α = 0.91, 95% LLCI = 0.77, 95% ULCI = 0.94).

3.2.4 Facial attractiveness

Each male participant provided a standardized color photograph with a neutral facial expression. Photos were taken from a stationary camera (Canon EOS Rebel T6) in a well-lit room with no windows. Photos were in color and were 4,608 pixels wide by 3,456 pixels high. The facial stimuli took up most of the photo area, with only a small portion of the neck and shoulders visible. Photos were not edited in any manner, with the intention of having the ratings being made on naturalistic stimuli. These photos were rated by eight Caucasian women (Mage = 21, SD = 1.70) who were asked to report the level of facial attractiveness of each photo, presented in random order, using a Likert-type scale ranging from 1 = Very unattractive, to 10 = Very attractive. The raters were reliably consistent in their ratings for facial attractiveness (α = 0.82, 95% LLCI = 0.77, 95% ULCI = 0.86). Previous studies have demonstrated that researchers can obtain reliable attractiveness ratings using a small number of raters (e.g., Buss and Shackelford, 2008; Kordsmeyer et al., 2019).

4 Results

Descriptive statistics are presented in Table 1. Analyses were preformed using SPSS (29.0.1.0; IBM Corp, 2023). First, a bivariate correlation analysis (Table 2) was conducted to determine whether facial attractiveness correlated with the control variables (age, lifestyle factors, skinfold, and hormones) and the two health indicators (symptoms of poor health, sIgA). Age was negatively correlated with T, C, and their interaction, but was otherwise unrelated to lifestyle, health, and facial attractiveness. T and C were positively correlated. C was correlated negatively with exercise and positively with smoking, whereas T was unrelated to all lifestyle variables. Results showed that those with more attractive faces had lower abdominal skin fold values, fewer health problems, exercised more, and were modestly higher in sIgA.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for study variable.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Bivariate correlations for variables.

Second, regression analysis was conducted with specific lifestyle indicators, skinfold, age, and hormones, entered as predictors of facial attractiveness simultaneously (Table 3) using Model 1 of the PROCESS macro for SPSS (Hayes, 2013). Results showed that average skinfold and symptoms of poor health1 were the only statistically significant predictors of facial attractiveness, such that poorer health and more body fat was linked to lower facial attractiveness. Although T, C, and the T x C interaction were not statistically significant predictors of facial attractiveness, these variables trended toward being statistically significant (e.g., p’s < 0.10).2 Visual examination of the interaction suggests a trend toward men with high T and low C being rated as more facially attractive (Figure 2).3

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Bootstrapped regression analyses (Model 1, PROCESS Macro) for lifestyle factors, skinfold, sIgA, testosterone and cortisol as predictors of men’s facial attractiveness (as rated by women).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Visual depiction of the T x C interaction in a model predicting facial attractiveness.

Third, we considered the possibility that lifestyle factors might instead have an indirect effect on facial attractiveness, specifically via increased skinfold and associated health problems. To test this prediction, we used AMOS (version 29; Arbuckle, 2019) to create an observed variable path model, with facial attractiveness entered as the dependent variable, unhealthy lifestyle variables (family stress, smoking, drinking alcohol, exercise, and sleep) as the predictors, and abdominal skinfold and poor health symptoms as the sequential mediators. The chi-square test of significance (relative χ2 index values <3.00), Comparative Fit Index (CFI; values >0.90), and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA; values <0.08, Kline, 2016) were used to determine model fit. Indirect (mediation) effects were examined using 1,000 bootstrap samples and bias-corrected 95% confidence intervals. Results showed that, of the unhealthy lifestyle habits, life stress (B = 5.33, SE = 1.56, β = 0.25, p < 0.001) and exercise (B = −3.49, SE = 1.74, β = −0.16, p = 0.045) predicted abdominal skin fold, whereas smoking (B = −0.84, SE = 2.91, β = 0.02, p = 0.77), drinking alcohol (B = 0.04, SE = 0.12, β = −0.03, p = 0.74), and sleep (B = −0.06, SE = 0.66, β = −0.01, p = 0.93) did not. Skinfold, in turn, directly predicted the severity and frequency of poor health symptoms (B = 0.01, SE = 0.002, β = 0.34, p < 0.001). Poor health symptoms, in turn, predicted lower facial attractiveness (B = −1.05, SE = 0.41, β = −0.20, p = 0.01). Skinfold directly negatively predicted facial attractiveness (B = −0.03, SE = 0.01, β = −0.21, p = 0.007), and had an indirect effect through the mediator of poor health symptoms (B = −0.01, SE = 0.004, p = 0.015, 95% LLCI = −0.02, 95% ULCI = −0.003). Exercise (B = 0.15, SE = 0.09, p = 0.03, 95% LLCI = 0.02, 95% ULCI = 0.36) and life stress (B = −0.23, SE = 0.10, p = 0.02, 95% LLCI = −0.46, 95% ULCI = −0.06) also showed statistically significant indirect effects through the sequentially mediated pathway of skinfold and poor health symptoms upon facial attractiveness. The sequential mediation model fit the data well, relative χ2 index = 1.31 (df = 19, p = 0.16), RMSEA = 0.01 (95% CI = 0.00–0.08), CFI = 0.91 (Figure 1).

5 Discussion

Tests of the links between facial attractiveness and health have yielded mixed results, with some researchers suggesting that lifestyle factors common to both facial attractiveness and health might account for these links (Jones et al., 2021). Accordingly, the present study examined whether indicators of immunocompetence (self-reported poor health symptoms and sIgA), unhealthy lifestyle (smoking, alcohol consumption, poor sleep, lack of exercise, and family stress), age, along with skinfold (as an index of body fat) and hormones (testosterone and cortisol) predicted facial attractiveness. Initially, results showed a significant bivariate link between facial attractiveness and self-reported health symptoms, and this relationship remained statistically significant when including age, unhealthy lifestyle habits, along with skinfold, T, C, and the T x C interaction in the model. However, none of the lifestyle factors themselves predicted facial attractiveness, whereas skinfold did. Previous work has shown that abdominal skinfold is a strong predictor of both facial adiposity and overall facial attractiveness (Sierra-Johnson and Johnson, 2004; Windhager et al., 2011). Similar findings have been observed in women, where BMI predicts facial attractiveness (Han et al., 2016). These links are likely due to related changes to facial morphology that are associated with visceral body fat (Lee and Kim, 2014). Abdominal skinfold is strongly associated with diverse indices of poor health (see Lee and Kim, 2014 for review) and future all-cause mortality in white males (Loh et al., 2018). Skinfold is influenced by lifestyle factors, including nutrition and exercise (e.g., Kwak et al., 2010). We therefore considered whether lifestyle factors instead had an indirect effect upon facial attractiveness via abdominal skinfold and subsequent poor health. Results of a mediation analysis supported this for two of the lifestyle variables: Exercise and life stress. This finding suggests that exercise and life stress have an indirect effect on facial attractiveness via changes to body fat and related poor health symptoms. Some of the lifestyle factors examined here do not necessarily increase body fat. For example, although smoking has been linked to long-term weight gain (Carrasquilla et al., 2024), it may have more meaningful effects in young adulthood upon skin quality and specific health problems (e.g., lung disease). Given that only 6% of our sample smoked, we were likely unable to appropriately assess the potential indirect effects of smoking on facial attractiveness. Future research using a broader community-based sample could address this limitation.

There was also a modest positive correlation between female-rated facial attractiveness and men’s sIgA, but this effect was eliminated in the regression equation that included the control variables. This finding corresponds with that of Cai et al. (2019) who found that sIgA was broadly unrelated to female facial appearance. Unlike other sexually dimorphic features that have been linked to sIgA, such as male voice pitch (Arnocky et al., 2018; Hodges-Simeon et al., 2024) and female breast symmetry (Locke and Arnocky, 2021), this null finding could mean that links between health and facial attractiveness are weaker than with other attractive secondary sex characteristics, or perhaps are more strongly driven by lifestyle influences. Future work involving a broader range of immunological markers in relation to facial appearance is therefore encouraged.

Both T and C were also uncorrelated with men’s facial attractiveness. However, the regression equation controlling for other variables led to a modestly significant positive link between the T x C interaction and facial attractiveness. Specifically, men with higher T and lower C were rated as most attractive, but this effect did not reach the conventional benchmark for statistical significance. However, it is noteworthy that this finding does conform to that of Rantala et al. (2012), who found the same effect. The overall weak association between hormones and facial attractiveness diverges from a study of women which showed a link between high C and lower facial attractiveness (Rantala et al., 2013), but corresponds with others of male facial attractiveness showing no links with either hormone (Swaddle and Reierson, 2002; Neave et al., 2003; Penton-Voak and Chen, 2004; Kandrik et al., 2017; Kordsmeyer et al., 2019). It has long been assumed in evolutionary psychology that male facial attractiveness is an honest cue of an individual’s health and immunocompetence (see Jones et al., 2021 for discussion). Some work does support links between certain immune markers (e.g., high functioning natural killer cells) being associated with female perceptions of male facial attractiveness (Mengelkoch et al., 2022).

T and C did not correlate with symptoms of poor health or sIgA. These findings contrast with previous work on similar samples of young adult men from Northern Ontario that have shown positive links between single samples of sIgA and T (Arnocky et al., 2018). There is a need for more comprehensive assessments of hormonal markers in relation with health variables, perhaps by assessing ‘trait’ levels of these hormones across multiple timepoints and days (discussed by Davis and Arnocky, 2022).

5.1 Limitations

One limitation of this work is the use of a homogenous sample of young, primarily Caucasian, undergraduates. This segment of the population tends to be particularly healthy, relative to the broader population. This likely limited variability in lifestyle, which might partly account for the relatively weak predictive role of most lifestyle factors. For instance, College graduates eat healthier, smoke less, and exercise more (see Lawrence, 2017 for review). Similarly, the brief measurement of each lifestyle factor was also limiting. There exist longer form measures of diet quality (Warren-Findlow et al., 2017), drinking behavior (The Drinking Styles Questionnaire DSQ; Smith et al., 1995), tobacco use (e.g., Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence FTND; Heatherton et al., 1991), physical activity (Healey et al., 2020), and sleep quality (Yi et al., 2006). The measures used in the present study also asked participants to self-report their own health symptoms and behaviors, and it is important to consider the various sources of self-report bias that can influence this kind of data (e.g., recall bias; Van den Bergh and Walentynowicz, 2016). Moreover, young adult men were the target population in the current study. Therefore, we cannot say that the same results would apply to different age groups, such as older adult men (e.g., Ponholzer et al., 2005). Although this study was sufficiently powered, the sample size was also a limitation, with some researchers suggesting that stability of estimates requires a larger sample than what was achieved in this study (Schönbrodt and Perugini, 2013). Future work should therefore consider these links in larger and more heterogenous samples. Reliance on statistical non-significance may be limited when examining control variables in a regression model to determine whether health remains a meaningful predictor of facial attractiveness. Another limitation lies in the reliance upon assessments of overall facial attractiveness. Although ecologically valid, this measure does not identify specific phenotypic structures of the face that might be tied to either immunocompetence or the effects of an unhealthy lifestyle. Using explicitly facial-oriented variables (e.g., geometric morphometric analyses, GMM) could help to determine how specific facial features contribute proportionally to explained variance in attractiveness, health, and lifestyle. For example, GMM has recently been used to examine facial features in relation with men’s and women’s sociosexual orientation (Antar and Stephen, 2021).

Future research could examine the impact of both lifestyle factors and hormones during development (adolescence) on adult facial attractiveness. Indeed, some aspects of facial attractiveness are relatively changeable (e.g., such as those affected by current health), whereas other aspects are more stable, such as facial masculinity, which is heavily influenced by steroids and some aspects of immune function during early adolescence (see Foo et al., 2020). Measuring these relationships during adolescence and again during adulthood might help to clarify their unique contributions to facial attractiveness. Finally, it may be useful for future work to consider including a measure of lean muscle mass, such as flexed bicep circumference (see, e.g., Holzleitner et al., 2014) as it may be related both to lifestyle and hormonal factors and has been tied to women’s ratings of men’s attractiveness via modified facial stimuli (e.g., Lei et al., 2019).

6 Conclusion

Mixed findings characterize the research on the links between facial attractiveness, health, and immunocompetence in men (Jones et al., 2021), which has significant implications for evolutionary theories dealing with the purported ultimate explanations for attractive phenotypic traits (e.g., immunocompetence handicap hypothesis; Nowak et al., 2018). We add to this growing literature to help make sense of the equivocal findings by considering lifestyle and hormonal factors that might influence the links between facial attractiveness and immune function. As suggested by previous authors (Jones et al., 2021), we did find evidence that lifestyle habits (indirectly) and hormones appear to matter when studying the relations between facial attractiveness and immunocompetence. These insights help to advance our understanding of why certain phenotypic traits (e.g., facial characteristics) are regarded as attractive and what kind of information these attractive traits communicate to others, such as health status and lifestyle habits.

Data availability statement

Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found here: https://osf.io/adxj3.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Nipissing University Research Ethics Board. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

SA: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Supervision, Methodology, Funding acquisition, Formal analysis, Conceptualization. AD: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Formal analysis.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Funding for this research provided by a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) Discovery Grant (DG) (file # RGPIN-2019-05988) awarded to SA.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

1. ^Given past research linking facial attractiveness to less respiratory illness, less antibiotic use, but not fewer stomach bugs in male (Boothroyd et al., 2013) and female participants (Gray and Boothroyd, 2012). We reran the model with only respiratory symptoms comprising the health problems variable. Results did not meaningfully change from those reported.

2. ^Including saliva sample time of day as a covariate did not meaningfully alter the results reported herein.

3. ^Excluding the participant case with an outlying (very high) T concentration did not meaningfully alter the results of the interaction results reported herein.

References

Adam, E. K., Quinn, M. E., Tavernier, R., McQuillan, M. T., Dahlke, K. A., and Gilbert, K. E. (2017). Diurnal cortisol slopes and mental and physical health outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology 83, 25–41. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.05.018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Akimoto, T., Kumai, Y., Akama, T., Hayashi, E., Murakami, H., Soma, R., et al. (2003). Effects of 12 months of exercise training on salivary secretory IgA levels in elderly subjects. Br. J. Sports Med. 37, 76–79. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.37.1.76

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Antar, J. C., and Stephen, I. D. (2021). Facial shape provides a valid cue to sociosexuality in men but not women. Evol. Hum. Behav. 42, 361–370. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2021.02.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Apanius, V. (1998). “Stress and immune response” in Stress and behavior. eds. A. P. Møller, M. Milinski, and P. J. B. Slater (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 133–154.

Google Scholar

Arbuckle, J. L. (2019). Amos 26.0 User’s Guide. Chicago: IBM SPSS.

Google Scholar

Arnocky, S., Bird, B. M., and Perilloux, C. (2014). “An evolutionary perspective on characteristics of physical attractiveness in humans” in Psychology of interpersonal perception and relationships. ed. A. Rennolds (New York: NOVA publishers), 115–155.

Google Scholar

Arnocky, S., Denomme, B., Hodges-Simeon, C. R., Hlay, J. K., Davis, A. C., and Brennan, H. (2023). Self-perceived mate value is predicted by biological and self-reported indices of health in young adults. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 9, 54–71. doi: 10.1007/s40750-022-00209-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnocky, S., Hodges-Simeon, C., Ouellette, D., and Albert, G. (2018). Do men with more masculine voices have better immunocompetence? Evol. Hum. Behav. 39, 602–610. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.06.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bagozzi, R. P., Verbeke, W. J. M. I., Belschak, F., and van Poele, M. (2018). Facial attractiveness as a function of athletic prowess. Evol. Psychol. 16:147470491880136. doi: 10.1177/1474704918801369

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernstein, M. A., Tucker, K. L., Ryan, N. D., O’Neill, E. F., Clements, K. M., Nelson, M. E., et al. (2002). Higher dietary variety is associated with better nutritional status in frail elderly people. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 102, 1096–1104. doi: 10.1016/S0002-8223(02)90246-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boothroyd, L. G., Scott, I., Gray, A. W., Coombes, C. I., and Pound, N. (2013). Male facial masculinity as a cue to health outcomes. Evol. Psychol. 11, 1044–1058. doi: 10.1177/147470491301100508

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brandtzaeg, P. (2009). Mucosal immunity: Induction, dissemination, and effector functions. Scand. J. Immunol. 70, 505–515. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3083.2009.02319.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Buss, D. M., and Shackelford, T. K. (2008). Attractive women want it all: good genes, economic investment, parenting proclivities, and emotional commitment. Evol. Psychol. 6:600. doi: 10.1177/147470490800600116

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cai, Z., Hahn, A. C., Zhang, W., Holzleitner, I. J., Lee, A. J., DeBruine, L. M., et al. (2019). No evidence that facial attractiveness, femininity, averageness, or coloration are cues to susceptibility to infectious illnesses in a university sample of young adult women. Evol. Hum. Behav. 40, 156–159. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.10.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrasquilla, G. D., García-Ureña, M., Romero-Lado, M. J., and Kilpeläinen, T. O. (2024). Estimating causality between smoking and abdominal obesity by Mendelian randomization. Addiction 119, 1024–1034. doi: 10.1111/add.16454

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cordain, L., Eaton, S. B., Sebastian, A., Mann, N., Lindeberg, S., Watkins, B. A., et al. (2005). Origins and evolution of the Western diet: health implications for the 21st century. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 81, 341–354. doi: 10.1093/ajcn.81.2.341

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cunningham, M. R., Roberts, A. R., Barbee, A. P., Druen, P. B., and Wu, C. (1995). “Their ideas of beauty are, on the whole, the same as ours”: consistency and variability in the cross-cultural perception of female physical attractiveness. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 68, 261–279. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.68.2.261

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalrymple, K. V., Flynn, A. C., Seed, P. T., Briley, A. L., O’Keeffe, M., Godfrey, K. M., et al. (2019). Associations between dietary patterns, eating behaviours and body composition and adiposity in 3-year old children of mothers with obesity. Pediatric. Obesity 15:e12608. doi: 10.1111/ijpo.12608

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, A. C., and Arnocky, S. (2022). Darwin versus Wallace: esthetic evolution and preferential mate choice. Front. Psychol. 13:385. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.862385

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Doshi, D. N., Hanneman, K. K., and Cooper, K. D. (2007). Smoking and skin aging in identical twins. Arch. Dermatol. 143, 1543–1546. doi: 10.1001/archderm.143.12.1543

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fahlman, M. M., and Engels, H. J. (2005). Mucosal IgA and URTI in American college football players: A year longitudinal study. Med Sci Sports Exerc, 37, 374–380. doi: 10.1249/01.mss.0000155432.67020.88

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fink, B., Neave, N., Manning, J. T., and Grammer, K. (2006). Facial symmetry and judgements of attractiveness, health and personality. Personal. Individ. Differ. 41, 491–499. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.01.017

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fink, B., Neave, N., and Seydel, H. (2007). Male facial appearance signals physical strength to women. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 19, 82–87. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.20583

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Foo, Y. Z., Simmons, L. W., Perrett, D. I., Holt, P. G., Eastwood, P. R., and Rhodes, G. (2020). Immune function during early adolescence positively predicts adult facial sexual dimorphism in both men and women. Evol. Hum. Behav. 41, 199–209. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.02.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzalez-Santoyo, I., Wheatley, J. R., Welling, L. L. M., Cárdenas, R. A., Jimenez-Trejo, F., Dawood, K., et al. (2015). The face of female dominance: women with dominant faces have lower cortisol. Horm. Behav. 71, 16–21. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.03.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

González-Torres, S., Anaya-Esparza, L. M., Trigueros del Valle, G. F., Rivera-León, E. A., Villagrán, Z., and Sánchez-Enríquez, S. (2023). Skinfold thickness as a cardiometabolic risk predictor in sedentary and active adult populations. J. Pers. Med. 13:1326. doi: 10.3390/jpm13091326

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, A. W., and Boothroyd, L. G. (2012). Female facial appearance and health. Evol. Psychol. 10, 66–77. doi: 10.1177/147470491201000108

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, C., Hahn, A. C., Fisher, C. I., Debruine, L. M., and Jones, B. C. (2016). Women's facial attractiveness is related to their body mass index but not their salivary cortisol. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 28, 352–355. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.22792

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis. A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Healey, E. L., Allen, K. D., Bennell, K., Bowden, J. L., Quicke, J. G., and Smith, R. (2020). Self-report measures of physical activity. Arthritis Care Res. 72, 717–730. doi: 10.1002/acr.24211

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Heatherton, T. F., Kozlowski, L. T., Frecker, R. C., and Fagerström, K. O. (1991). The Fagerström test for nicotine dependence: a revision of the Fagerström tolerance questionnaire. Br. J. Addict. 86, 1119–1127. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1991.tb01879.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, E. U., and Du Vivier, W. P. (1992). Invited review: alcohol and the skin. Alcohol Alcohol. 27, 595–602. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.alcalc.a045308

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodges-Simeon, C. R., Albert, G., McHale, T., Gaulin, S. J. C., Gurven, M., Landry, N., et al. (2024). Masculine voice is associated with better mucosal immune defense in adolescent and adult males. Evol. Hum. Behav. 45:106590. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.05.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodges-Simeon, C. R., Grail, G. P. O., Albert, G., Landry, N., Ortiz, T. L., Carré, J. M., et al. (2020). Testosterone, cortisol, and secretory immunoglobulin-a within a single day and across two sequential days among trans- and cis-gender men. Steroids 160:108640. doi: 10.1016/j.steroids.2020.108640

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Holzleitner, I. J., Hunter, D. W., Tiddeman, B. P., Seck, A., Re, D. E., and Perrett, D. I. (2014). Men’s facial masculinity: when (body) size matters. Perception 43, 1191–1202. doi: 10.1068/p7673

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hönekopp, J., Rudolph, U., Beier, L., Liebert, A., and Müller, C. (2010). Physical attractiveness of face and body as indicators of physical fitness in men. Evol. Hum. Behav. 28, 106–111. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.09.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hume, D. K., and Montgomerie, R. (2001). Facial attractiveness signals different aspects of “quality” in women and men. Evol. Hum. Behav. 22, 93–112. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(00)00065-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hurley, L. L., Taylor, R. E., and Tizabi, Y. (2012). Positive and negative effects of alcohol and nicotine and their interactions: a mechanistic review. Neurotox. Res. 21, 57–69. doi: 10.1007/s12640-011-9275-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

IBM Corp (2023). IBM SPSS statistics for windows, version 29.0.1.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.

Google Scholar

Jones, B. C., Holzleitner, I. J., and Shiramizu, V. (2021). Does facial attractiveness really signal immunocompetence? Trends Cogn. Sci. 25, 1018–1020. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2021.09.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalick, S. M., Zebrowitz, L. A., Langlois, J. H., and Johnson, R. M. (1998). Does human facial attractiveness honestly advertise health? Longitudinal data on an evolutionary question. Psychol. Sci. 9, 8–13. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kandrik, M., Hahn, A. C., Han, C., Wincenciak, J., Fisher, C. I., DeBruine, L. M., et al. (2017). Does the interaction between cortisol and testosterone predict men’s facial attractiveness? Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 3, 275–281. doi: 10.1007/s40750-017-0064-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kline, R. B. (2016). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. 4th Edn. New York: Guilford Publications.

Google Scholar

Knack, J. M., Arnocky, S., Lawton, C., Wallace, M., and Sussman, J. (2013). “School bullying and health problems: a developmental examination of predictive and protective factors and coping strategies” in School bullying: Predictive factors, coping strategies, and effects on mental health. eds. K. Dekker and M. Dijkstra (New York: NOVA Publishers), 147–167.

Google Scholar

Knack, J. M., Iyer, P. A., and Jensen-Ccampbell, L. A. (2012). Not simply "in their heads": individual differences associated with victimization and health1. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 42, 1625–1650. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2012.00898.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Knack, J. M., Jensen-Campbell, L. A., and Baum, A. (2011). Worse than sticks and stones? Bullying is associated with altered HPA axis functioning and poorer health. Brain Cogn. 77, 183–190. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.06.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koizumi, K., Hirao, N., Yamanami, H., and Ohira, H. (2023). Effects of mild psychological stress on facial impressions. Front. Psychol. 14:1186046. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1186046

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kordsmeyer, T. L., Lohöfener, M., and Penke, L. (2019). Male facial attractiveness, dominance, and health and the interaction between cortisol and testosterone. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 5, 1–12. doi: 10.1007/s40750-018-0098-z

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kwak, L., Kremers, S. P. J., Candel, M. J. J. M., Visscher, T. L. S., Brug, J., and van Baak, M. A. (2010). Changes in skinfold thickness and waist circumference after 12 and 24 months resulting from the NHF-NRG in balance-project. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 7:26. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-7-26

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Langlois, J. H., Kalakanis, L., Rubenstein, A. J., Larson, A., Hallam, M., and Smoot, M. (2000). Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychol. Bull. 126, 390–423. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.3.390

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Langlois, J. H., Ritter, J. M., Roggman, L. A., and Vaughn, L. S. (1991). Facial diversity and infant preferences for attractive faces. Dev. Psychol. 27, 79–84. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.27.1.79

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawrence, E. M. (2017). Why do College graduates behave more healthfully than those who are less educated? J. Health Soc. Behav. 58, 291–306. doi: 10.1177/0022146517715671

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, B. J., and Kim, J. Y. (2014). Predicting visceral obesity based on facial characteristics. BMC Complement. Altern. Med. 14:248. doi: 10.1186/1472-6882-14-248

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lei, X., Holzleitner, I. J., and Perrett, D. I. (2019). The influence of body composition effects on male facial masculinity and attractiveness. Front. Psychol. 9:2658. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02658

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, S. W., Lien, M. H., and Fenske, N. A. (2010). The effects of alcohol and drug abuse on the skin. Clin. Dermatol. 28, 391–399. doi: 10.1016/j.clindermatol.2010.03.024

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Locke, A., and Arnocky, S. (2021). Breast symmetry, but not size or volume, predicts salivary immunoglobulin-a (sIgA) in women. Evol. Hum. Behav. 42, 517–523. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2021.05.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Loh, W. J., Johnston, D. G., Oliver, N., and Godsland, I. F. (2018). Skinfold thickness measurements and mortality in white males during 27.7 years of follow-up. Int. J. Obes. 42, 1939–1945. doi: 10.1038/s41366-018-0034-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Macpherson, A. J., McCoy, K. D., Johansen, F. E., and Brandtzaeg, P. (2008). The immune geography of IgA induction and function. Mucosal Immunol. 1, 11–22. doi: 10.1038/mi.2007.6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mengelkoch, S., Gassen, J., Prokosch, M. L., Boehm, G. W., and Hill, S. E. (2022). More than just a pretty face? The relationship between immune function and perceived facial attractiveness. Proc. R. Soc. B 289:20212476. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2021.2476

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, M. H., Blair, S. N., and Murtagh, E. M. (2009). Accumulated versus continuous exercise for health benefit. Sports Med. 39, 29–43. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200939010-00003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakamura, D., Akimoto, T., Suzuki, S., and Kono, I. (2006). Daily changes of salivary secretory immunoglobulin A and appearance of upper respiratory symptoms during physical training. J Sports Med Phys Fitness. 46, 152–157. Available at: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/16596115/

Google Scholar

Neave, N., Laing, S., Fink, B., and Manning, J. T. (2003). Second to fourth digit ratio, testosterone and perceived male dominance. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 270, 2167–2172. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2003.2502

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nowak, J., Pawłowski, B., Borkowska, B., Augustyniak, D., and Drulis-Kawa, Z. (2018). No evidence for the immunocompetence handicap hypothesis in male humans. Sci. Rep. 8:7392. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-25694-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pátková, Ž., Schwambergová, D., Třebická Fialová, J., Třebický, V., Stella, D., Kleisner, K., et al. (2022). Attractive and healthy-looking male faces do not show higher immunoreactivity. Sci. Rep. 12:18432. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-22866-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Penton-Voak, I. S., and Chen, J. Y. (2004). High salivary testosterone is linked to masculine male facial appearance in humans. Evol. Hum. Behav. 25, 229–241. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.04.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillips, A. C., Carroll, D., Drayson, M. T., and Der, G. (2015). Salivary immunoglobulin a secretion rate is negatively associated with cancer mortality: The West of Scotland Twenty-07 Study. PLoS ONE, 10, Article e0145083. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0145083

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ponholzer, A., Plas, E., Schatzl, G., Struhal, G., Brössner, C., Mock, K., et al. (2005). Relationship between testosterone serum levels and lifestyle in aging men. Aging Male 8, 190–193. doi: 10.1080/13685530500298154

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pound, N., Lawson, D. W., Toma, A. M., Richmond, S., Zhurov, A. I., and Penton-Voak, I. S. (2014). Facial fluctuating asymmetry is not associated with childhood ill-health in a large British cohort study. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 281:20141639. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.1639

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rantala, M. J., Coetzee, V., Moore, F. R., Skrinda, I., Kecko, S., Krama, T., et al. (2013). Facial attractiveness is related to women's cortisol and body fat, but not with immune responsiveness. Biol. Lett. 9:20130255. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2013.0255

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rantala, M. J., Moore, F. R., Skrinda, I., Krama, T., Kivleniece, I., Kecko, S., et al. (2012). Evidence for the stress-linked immunocompetence handicap hypothesis in humans. Nature. Communications 3:694. doi: 10.1038/ncomms1696

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rhodes, G., Simmons, L. W., and Peters, M. (2005). Attractiveness and sexual behavior: Does attractiveness enhance mating success? Evol Hum Behav. 26, 186–201. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.08.014

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rohatgi, K. W., Tinius, R. A., Cade, W. T., Martínez Steele, E., Cahill, A. G., and Parra, D. C. (2017). Relationships between consumption of ultra-processed foods, gestational weight gain and neonatal outcomes in a sample of US pregnant women. PeerJ 5:e4091. doi: 10.7717/peerj.4091

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roney, J. R. (2016). Theoretical frameworks for human behavioral endocrinology. Horm. Behav. 84, 97–110. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2016.06.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roney, J. R., Hanson, K. N., Durante, K. M., and Maestripieri, D. (2006). Reading men’s faces: Women’s mate attractiveness judgments track men’s testosterone and interest in infants. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 273, 2169–2175. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2006.3569

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schönbrodt, F. D., and Perugini, M. (2013). At what sample size do correlations stabilize? J. Res. Pers. 47, 609–612. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2013.05.009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shackelford, T. K., and Larsen, R. J. (1997). Facial asymmetry as an indicator of psychological, emotional, and physiological distress. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 72, 456–466. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.72.2.456

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sierra-Johnson, J., and Johnson, B. D. (2004). Facial fat and its relationship to abdominal fat: a marker for insulin resistance? Med. Hypotheses 63, 783–786. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2004.06.020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Skinner, A. L., Woods, A., Stone, C. J., Penton-Voak, I., and Munafò, M. R. (2017). Smoking status and attractiveness among exemplar and prototypical identical twins discordant for smoking. Royal Society open. Science 4:1076. doi: 10.1098/rsos.161076

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, G. T., McCarthy, D. M., and Goldman, M. S. (1995). Self-reported drinking and alcohol-related problems among early adolescents: dimensionality and validity over 24 months. J. Stud. Alcohol 56, 383–394. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1995.56.383

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sundelin, T., Lekander, M., Sorjonen, K., and Axelsson, J. (2017). Negative effects of restricted sleep on facial appearance and social appeal. R. Soc. Open Sci. 4:918. doi: 10.1098/rsos.160918

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Swaddle, J. P., and Reierson, G. W. (2002). Testosterone increases perceived dominance but not attractiveness in human males. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 269, 2285–2289. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2002.2165

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Den Abbeele, J., Penton-Voak, I. S., Attwood, A. S., Stephen, I. D., and Munafò, M. R. (2015). Increased facial attractiveness following moderate, but not high, alcohol consumption. Alcohol Alcohol. 50, 296–301. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agv010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van den Bergh, O., and Walentynowicz, M. (2016). Accuracy and bias in retrospective symptom reporting. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 29, 302–308. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000267

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Visine, A., Durand, V., Guillou, L., Raymond, M., and Berticat, C. (2024). Chronic and immediate refined carbohydrate consumption and facial attractiveness. PLoS One 19:e0298984. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0298984

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Volkmann, E. R., and Weekes, N. Y. (2006). Basal SIgA and cortisol levels predict stress‐related health outcomes.Stress and Health: Journal of the International Society for the Investigation of Stress. 22: 11–23. doi: 10.1002/smi.1077

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Warren-Findlow, J., Reeve, C. L., and Racine, E. F. (2017). Psychometric validation of a brief self-report measure of diet quality: the DASH-Q. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 49, 92–99.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2016.09.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Windhager, S., Schaefer, K., and Fink, B. (2011). Geometric morphometrics of male facial shape in relation to physical strength and perceived attractiveness, dominance, and masculinity. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 23, 805–814. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.21219

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yi, H., Shin, K., and Shin, C. (2006). Development of the sleep quality scale. J. Sleep Res. 15, 309–316. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2006.00544.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: facial attractiveness, immunocompetence, good genes sexual selection, unhealthy lifestyle, skinfold, secondary sexual characteristics

Citation: Arnocky S and Davis AC (2024) Do lifestyle and hormonal variables explain links between health and facial attractiveness? Front. Psychol. 15:1404387. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1404387

Received: 21 March 2024; Accepted: 09 July 2024;
Published: 14 August 2024.

Edited by:

Lisa L. M. Welling, Oakland University, United States

Reviewed by:

Neil Robert Caton, The University of Queensland, Australia
Christopher Watkins, Abertay University, United Kingdom
James Rutter, Durham University, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2024 Arnocky and Davis. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Steven Arnocky, c3RldmVuYUBuaXBpc3Npbmd1LmNh

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.