Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 25 April 2024
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

Exposure to workplace bullying: the incremental effect of gelotophobia beyond the big five

  • 1Department of Psychology, Palacký University Olomouc, Olomouc, Czechia
  • 2School of Psychology, University of Sunderland, Sunderland, United Kingdom

The role of Big Five personality traits in exposure to workplace bullying has been a focus of numerous studies. Yet less is known about the incremental validity of narrower personality constructs. The aim of the present study was to investigate the incremental effect of gelotophobia (the fear of being laughed at) in predicting exposure to workplace bullying beyond the Big Five personality domains. The sample comprised 328 employees (77% females) from different regions of the Czech Republic. Correlational analysis showed that negative emotionality and gelotophobia were related to workplace bullying in theoretically expected ways. Results from a multiple regression indicated that gelotophobia had an incremental effect in predicting exposure to workplace bullying over and above the personality domains. Overall, this study provides new insights and extends previous investigations concerning the role of gelotophobia in workplace bullying. We also discuss the limitations of our study and provide suggestions for future research.

Introduction

Two main perspectives, namely the work environment hypothesis and the individual dispositions hypothesis, are typically used to explain the antecedents of workplace bullying [Nielsen and Knardahl, 2015; also see Leymann (1996), Zapf and Einarsen (2001), and Balducci et al. (2021)]. From the work environment perspective, factors such as job design, organizational climate and culture, and leadership are usually seen as crucial for the occurrence of workplace bullying (Gamian-Wilk et al., 2022), while on the other hand, the individual dispositions hypothesis (also known as the vulnerability hypothesis) focuses on individual characteristics which might increase the risk of being a target or a victim of bullying [Nielsen and Knardahl, 2015, see also Gamian-Wilk et al. (2022)].

In the present study, we follow the vulnerability hypothesis – keeping in mind that the perspectives should be seen as complementary, but not opposite – and investigate the role of the Big Five domains (Soto and John, 2017a) and the incremental validity of gelotophobia (Ruch and Proyer, 2008a) in predicting workplace bullying, understood as “the persistent exposure to interpersonal aggression and mistreatment from colleagues, superiors or subordinates” (Einarsen et al., 2009, p. 24). In particular, we consider workplace bullying as operationalized with the Negative Acts Questionnaire-Revised (NAQ-R) (Einarsen et al., 2009), which includes three different forms of exposure to bullying (work-related, person-related, and physically intimidating). Moreover, we introduce a dimension labeled as humor-related bullying. The role of the personality dimensions and gelotophobia in predicting self-labeled victimization from bullying is one of the interests of this study as well.

Personality and workplace bullying

In a meta-analysis, investigating the relationship between the Five-Factor Model of personality (McCrae and Costa, 1987) and exposure to harassment (a higher-ordered construct including different forms of psychological mistreatment), Nielsen et al. (2017) reported that being exposed to harassment was positively associated with neuroticism, and negatively related to extraversion, agreeableness, and conscientiousness. The theoretical relations between the personality dimensions and harassment were built upon the four mechanisms explaining the relationship between bullying and individual dispositions (Nielsen and Knardahl, 2015).

In sum, Nielsen and Knardahl (2015) propose that bullying and individual dispositions relationship can be explained by (1) the no-relationship mechanism; indicating no association, (2) the target-behavior mechanism; implying that employees with certain dispositions not only fail to meet expectations but also irritate others, possibly by violating the usual norms of polite and friendly interaction, which can lead to others responding with aggressive behaviors, (3) the negative perceptions mechanism; suggesting that specific individual dispositions are related with a lowered threshold for interpreting behaviors as harassing, and therefore, employees with such dispositions have a higher risk than others for labeling negative events at the workplace as bullying, and (4) the reverse causality mechanism; individual dispositions are viewed as outcomes rather than precursors of workplace bullying.

Nielsen et al. (2017) highlight that the findings regarding the association between bullying and personality should not be used to conclude whether dispositional characteristics among those harassed are causes or consequences of harassment, as their study was cross-sectional [see Bowling et al. (2010), Nielsen and Knardahl (2015), and Podsiadly and Gamian-Wilk (2017) for longitudinal research findings]. As our study is also cross-sectional in nature, we firstly focus on replicating the above findings in a Czech cultural setting. Moreover, we utilize a more recent operationalization of the Big Five dimensions [see Soto and John (2017a,b)]. Soto and John (2017a) used the following labels for the Big Five personality dimensions: Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Negative Emotionality (also known as Neuroticism; see, e.g., McCrae and Costa, 2008), and Open-Mindedness [also known as Openness to Experience, Intellect, or Imagination; see Goldberg (1993), John et al. (2008), and McCrae and Costa (2008)]. The Big Five Inventory-2 (BFI-2) operationalizes these domains using a total of 60 items, and it was shown that it has a robust hierarchical structure, controls for individual differences in acquiescence, and has conceptual breadth, specificity, and predictive power (Soto and John, 2017a). In the present study, we employ the abbreviated 30 items version of the scale, i.e., the BFI-2-S (Soto and John, 2017b).

The role of gelotophobia

Gelotophobia is defined as the pathological fear of being an object of laughter or appearing to others as a ridiculous object (Ruch and Proyer, 2008b; Titze, 2009). Individuals scoring high on gelotophobia do not perceive humor and laughter as relaxing and joyful social experiences (Titze, 2009) and they fail to discriminate between ridicule and good-humored teasing (Platt, 2008). Clinically observed behaviors described by Titze (1997, 2009) was developed into a model of the causes and consequences of gelotophobia (Ruch and Proyer, 2008a), which later was updated to include putative causes and the moderating factors (Ruch et al., 2014). Ruch and Stahlmann (2020) advanced the framework to a dynamic model of gelotophobia, providing an updated definition that is anchored in the construct of vulnerability. Gelotophobia is understood “as a distinguishable pattern of lacking resources (i.e., misinterpretation of joy and laughter) that can result in negative consequences (e.g., reduced well-being and performance) if individuals have no access to further resources (e.g., social support) or are exposed to severe stressors (e.g., workplace bullying)” (Ruch and Stahlmann, 2020, p. 16,369).

By far, a few studies have investigated the association between gelotophobia and (workplace) bullying. Some of these previous investigations focused on children and/or adolescents (Führ, 2010; Proyer et al., 2012, 2013) and reported that higher gelotophobia was related to feelings of being a victim of bullying. Therefore, the importance of gelotophobia in school therapy practice has been already put forward (Bledsoe and Baskin, 2014; Platt et al., 2016). Considering workplace bullying – although the role of gelotophobia in workplace bullying has been theoretically discussed (e.g., Hofmann et al., 2017) – to our knowledge there are the only three empirical studies to date.

In a sample of adults, Platt (2008) found that participants who reported that were victims of bullying had higher gelotophobia scores (in comparison to individuals who did not disclose such an experience). It should be noted that it was not considered whether being a victim of bullying was specifically in the workplace. Gelotophobes [for cut-off points indicating slight pronounced, and extreme expression of gelotophobia, see Ruch and Proyer (2008b)] also did not discriminate between scenarios of ridicule (a form of bullying) and friendly teasing; individuals with extreme gelotophobia had same emotional reactions (i.e., disgust, surprise, and shame) to both types of interactions. Platt et al. (2009) confirmed and extended these findings, in particular by showing that being a victim of bullying was best predicted by high gelotophobia scores and by low happiness scores concerning playful teasing situations. Although some of the participants were recruited from an anti-workplace bullying support network group, the victim of bullying status was more broadly defined as in the previous study. The most recent panel study by Ruch and Stahlmann (2020) focused, among other things, on the relationship between gelotophobia and workplace bullying (workplace bullying was operationalized with the four-item Workplace Incivility Scale; Cortina et al., 2001), and found that there was a positive correlation between gelotophobia and workplace bullying in all of the six measurement intervals (waves) in their research.

Finally, it should be stressed that previous investigations have related gelotophobia to personality dimensions (Ruch et al., 2008, 2013; Hřebíčková et al., 2009; Ruch and Proyer, 2009; Proyer and Ruch, 2010; Ďurka and Ruch, 2015). Utilizing a Czech sample, Hřebíčková et al. (2009) reported that gelotophobia was associated with higher neuroticism, and lower extraversion, agreeableness, and openness to experience (neuroticism and extraversion showed the most robust relations); and concluded that personality dimensions play a significant role in whether individuals cope with ridicule easily, or whether they find it difficult. In general, gelotophobes can be described as introverted neurotics with a lower inclination to openness (Ruch et al., 2013). While personality characteristics have been studied in relation to gelotophobia, there is no previous research taking into account the joint consideration of personality and gelotophobia, and their relation to workplace bullying.

Aim of the present study

The current study aimed to investigate the relationship between personality dimensions, gelotophobia, and exposure to workplace bullying. Furthermore, our specific aim was to explore whether gelotophobia has an incremental validity in predicting exposure to workplace bullying beyond the Big Five dimensions of personality. We expect that gelotophobia will have an incremental effect over and above personality dimensions in predicting exposure to workplace bullying (and especially humor-related bullying).

Two research gaps are considered in this study. First, the joint investigation of the Big Five dimensions and gelotophobia – and their relation to workplace bullying – has not been undertaken yet. Secondly, previous studies (relating gelotophobia with bullying) have either focused on children and/or adolescents (Führ, 2010; Proyer et al., 2012, 2013) or used only self-labeled victimization from bullying (Platt, 2008; Platt et al., 2009; cf., Ruch and Stahlmann, 2020). In general, we aim to extend previous findings by including a well-established behavioral type measure of workplace bullying (the Negative Acts Questionnaire-Revised – NAQ-R; Einarsen et al., 2009) and consider a humor-related bullying dimension (a specific sub-factor of person-related bullying) as well.

Materials and methods

Participants and procedure

Data was gathered online using software for online assessment (MindMap Diagnostic Methods). On the first page of the study link, participants were informed about the purpose of the study and it was stated that by continuing to fill in the questionnaires they provide an informed consent. In addition, it was stated that they should participate in the research if they are more than 6 months employed. The data collection was conducted from January 2023 to June 2023, and participants were recruited by social media posts on Facebook and LinkedIn. No monetary incentives were offered for participation. An email from one of the authors of this study was provided in case of further questions regarding the research. However, no participant utilized this option.

The study sample size was a priori estimated with power analysis on previous correlations of gelotophobia and workplace bullying (from Ruch and Stahlmann, 2020), where the mean zero-order correlation was r = 0.145. Using 85% power, a standard α = 0.05, and a one-sided test, the needed total sample would be 339 participants (performed with package pwr, Champely et al., 2020). We maximized our collection possibilities; in total, 482 participants opened the test battery, with 137 not completing all of the questionnaires. Therefore, only 345 individuals completed the test battery. The final sample consisted of 328 participants (77% females) as 17 participants were flagged as outliers (and removed from the dataset) using the Mahalanobis Distance procedure in the careless package [v1.1.3; Yentes and Wilhelm, 2018; see also Meade and Craig (2012) for identifying careless responding by using Mahalanobis Distances]. Data cleaning was conducted on the answers provided on the separate scales on the Big Five Inventory-2 (BFI-2-S; see Instruments section). Participants were from the Czech Republic and their age was between 20 and 66 years (M = 37.95, SD = 9.66). The sociodemographic characteristics of the sample are provided in the Supplementary Table S1.

Instruments

Workplace bullying

Exposure to workplace bullying was assessed with the Czech version (Cakirpaloglu et al., 2017) of the NAQ-R (Einarsen et al., 2009). The questionnaire is comprised of 22 behavioral items measuring three factors (work-related bullying, person-related bullying, and physically intimidating bullying). For the purpose of this study, a humor-related bullying factor comprised of three items involving behaviors related to humor was calculated. The included items were “Being humiliated or ridiculed in connection with your work” (item 2); “Practical jokes carried out by people you do not get along with” (item 15) and, “Being the subject of excessive teasing and sarcasm” (item 20). The NAQ-R also includes self-labeled victimization from bullying during the last 6 months assessed with a single-item measure (item 23: Have you ever been bullied at work?) following a definition of workplace bullying (Einarsen and Skogstad, 1996; see also Supplementary Table S3). Participants choose one of the five alternatives on the behavioral items (“Never,” “Now and then,” “Monthly,” “Weekly,” and “Daily”) and the single-item measure [“no,” “yes, sometimes (rarely),” “yes, several times per month,” “yes, several times per week” and “yes, almost daily”]. Internal consistency in the present sample was high for all of the bullying dimensions as well as the total NAQ-R score, namely McDonald’s Omegas were 0.86 (work-related bullying), 0.95 (person-related bullying), 0.78 (physically intimidating bullying), 0.79 (humor-related bullying), and 0.96 (NAQ-R total score).

Personality domains

To assess the Big Five personality domains, we used the short form of the Big Five Inventory-2 (BFI-2-S) (Soto and John, 2017b; Hřebíčková et al., 2020). The scale is composed of 30 items, which have a common item stem (“I am someone who…”) and short descriptive phrases (e.g., “Tends to be quiet,” “Is temperamental, gets emotional easily”). A 5-point scale (ranging from disagree strongly to agree strongly) is utilized for the rating of each item by the participants. In this study, McDonald’s Omegas were 0.74 (extraversion), 0.74 (agreeableness), 0.77 (conscientiousness), 0.84 (negative emotionality), and 0.74 (open-mindedness).

Gelotophobia

Gelotophobia was measured using the Czech language version of the GELOPH<15> (Ruch and Proyer, 2008a; Hřebíčková et al., 2009), which is the psychometrically valid 15-item Czech language self-report instrument used for assessment of gelotophobia (e.g., “When others make joking remarks about me I feel being paralyzed”). Participants answer each item on a four-point scale (1 = “strongly disagree,” 4 = “strongly agree”). McDonald’s Omega was 0.93.

Results

Descriptive statistics

The descriptive statistics (mean, standard deviation, skewness, and kurtosis) of the measures are depicted in Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the measures used.

Table 1 shows that skewness and kurtosis indicated substantial non-normality for the NAQ-R total score and the bullying dimensions (i.e., values for the skewness and/or kurtosis were greater than +2). For the rest of the measures, the values of the skewness and kurtosis could be considered acceptable [see Hair et al. (2022)].

Considering the prevalence of workplace bullying, in our sample it was found that 27.1% of the participants were exposed to workplace bullying when Leymann’s criterion (Leymann, 1996) was applied (i.e., facing at least one of the 22 negative acts on a weekly/daily basis during a minimum of 6 months). When a more strict criterion was used, or two acts on a week/daily basis (Mikkelsen and Einarsen, 2001), 12% of the participants could be classified as victims of workplace bullying. The prevalence of self-reported bullying was 9%. The percentage of endorsed behavioral items and the self-labeled victimization from bullying are given in the Supplementary Tables S2, S3.

Intercorrelations

Pearson product–moment correlations between the Big Five domains, gelotophobia and exposure to workplace bullying are given in Table 2.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Pearson product–moment correlations between the study variables.

Table 2 shows that negative emotionality was positively related to the NAQ-R total score, and to each of the bullying dimensions, as expected. However, there was not a statistically significant relation between negative emotionality and self-labeled victimization from bullying. The rest of the Big Five traits were not related to any of the bullying measures, with an exception of the significant negative association between extraversion and humor-related bullying. In line with the expectations, gelotophobia correlated positively with the NAQ-R total score, the separate bullying dimensions, and the self-labeled victimization from bullying. The relation between personality and gelotophobia also corroborated previous findings, as gelotophobia was related to each of the personality domains, and the most robust correlations were found with extraversion and negative emotionality. Finally, self-reported victimization from bullying was strongly related to the NAQ-R total score and its dimensions.

In order to correct for the substantial non-normality of the exposure to bullying measures, we also calculated Spearman rank order correlations (see Supplementary Table S4). Each of the above mentioned findings were replicated (except for the extraversion relation to humor-related bullying, which cease to be significant), albeit the correlations were generally weaker.

Regression analysis

We conducted two hierarchical multiple regression models and used the NAQ-R total score as a dependent variable. Model 1 included the personality domains (Big Five), whereas Model 2 incorporated both personality domains and gelotophobia to assess its incremental effect on bullying. A summary of both models can be found in Table 3. Assumptions of linear regression were mostly met, however residuals had a minor deviation from normal distribution.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Linear regression models for exposure to workplace bullying (NAQ-R total score).

In the first model, negative emotionality was the only statistically significant predictor of the NAQ-R total score; beta = 0.16 [95% CI (0.03, 0.29), t(322) = 2.43, p = 0.016]. However, in the second model, gelotophobia had the only significant effect [beta = 0.37, 95% CI (0.24, 0.50), t(321) = 5.61, p < 0.001], and the difference in R2 between Models 1 and 2 is ΔR2 = 0.086.

Next, humor-related bullying was used as a dependent variable. Similar results occurred (see Table 4), where only gelotophobia was statistically significant in the second model [beta = 0.39, 95% CI (0.26, 0.52), t(321) = 5.98, p < 0.001].

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Linear regression models for exposure to humor-related bullying.

Discussion

The current study aimed to explore and advance knowledge pertaining to the relation between personality dimensions, gelotophobia, and exposure to workplace bullying. The specific goal was to investigate the incremental validity of gelotophobia in predicting workplace bullying beyond the personality dimensions. We firstly focus on the prevalence of workplace bullying in our sample, and afterwards discuss the main findings of our study.

The percentage of individuals who were classified as victims of workplace bullying (using both the looser and stricter criterion) via the behavioral items are comparable to previous studies. For example, the study by Cakirpaloglu et al. (2017), which was also done in a Czech context utilizing a sample of 7,103 employees, reported that 24.78% (using the looser criterion) and 14.84% (using the stricter criterion) of the participants in their study could be classified as victims of bullying. Furthermore, we have also found a drop of the percentage when the self-labeling approach was applied, and the percentage of individuals who self-labeled as victims of bullying in our study is similar to previous investigations (e.g., Einarsen et al., 2009; Tsuno et al., 2010; Dujo López et al., 2020).

The correlational findings indicated that negative emotionality was related to exposure to workplace bullying (i.e., experienced negative acts). This is in line with the meta-analytic investigation by Nielsen et al. (2017) which concluded that neuroticism is the strongest and most consistent correlate of exposure to harassment. It should be noted that each of the three mechanisms indicating an association between individual dispositions and bullying (Nielsen and Knardahl, 2015) can be applied to explain this relation [see Nielsen et al. (2017); see also Djurkovic et al. (2006) and Bowling et al. (2010)]. The present results confirmed previous investigations and extend to a different cultural context. Moreover, we have utilized a more recent operationalization of the neuroticism (i.e., negative emotionality) construct – which was not considered in Nielsen et al. (2017) – comprised of the facets of anxiety, depression, and emotional volatility (Soto and John, 2017b).

It is interesting to note that we have not confirmed the results considering the relation between the other personality dimensions (i.e., extraversion, agreeableness, and conscientiousness) and workplace bullying. However, this can be explained by the fact that one of the moderators for this association was the geographical region [see Nielsen et al. (2017)]. In particular, the relation between agreeableness and conscientiousness, and exposure to workplace harassment was moderated by the geographical region, that is higher estimates were found in studies from the United States compared to Europe (for agreeableness), and studies from the USA and Asia/Oceania compared to Europe (for conscientiousness). As the Czech Republic represents a central European context, our findings confirm this trend. However, the non-significant association between extraversion and workplace bullying cannot be explained by the moderator analyses reported by Nielsen et al. (2017), and needs further consideration.

Gelotophobia can be placed on introverted neurotic personality dimensions with a lower inclination to openness (Ruch et al., 2013). While personality characteristics have been studied in relation to gelotophobia, this is the first study that accounts for the joint consideration of personality and gelotophobia, and their relation to workplace bullying. Focusing specifically on the gelotophobia and exposure to workplace bullying relation, the results indicated that gelotophobia was positively related to both the behavioral measures of bullying and the self-labeled victimization. Furthermore, it was a stronger correlate to workplace bullying than neuroticism, and in line with theoretical considerations, the most robust relation was found with humor-related bullying. In general, the association between gelotophobia and workplace bullying can also be theoretically explained using the mechanisms proposed by Nielsen and Knardahl (2015), as analogous to the personality dimensions – bullying relation.

Gelotophobia overlaps with neuroticism/negative emotionality in regard to behaviors related to nervousness and insecurity (e.g., indicators of gelotophobia are difficulty to hold eye contact and stiffness) and these behaviors could be seen by others as annoying. Thus, making gelotophobes both provocative and easy targets of bullying. This is consistent with the explanation applying the target behavior mechanism for the neuroticism–harassment relation (Milam et al., 2009; Nielsen et al., 2017).

Next, following the negative perception mechanism, individuals with high gelotophobia scores might have a lowered threshold for interpreting behaviors as harassing/bullying, especially behaviors related to humor. Moreover, the perceived bullying could constitute a “false alarm,” or in reality, there is a lack of objective proof for it (Platt et al., 2009; Ruch, 2009; Hofmann et al., 2017). In other words, gelotophobes might misinterpret the good-humored teasing from their colleagues, superiors, or subordinates, as being ridiculed/bullied (Platt, 2008).

Finally, workplace bullying might in fact be an antecedent of gelotophobia, as proposed by the reverse causality mechanism. This especially could be true in workplaces with extreme humor culture (Plester, 2016; Plester et al., 2022) and/or negative humor climate (Cann et al., 2014), and is in accordance with the proposed causes of gelotophobia. According to Ruch et al. (2014), in adulthood, intense traumatic experiences of being laughed at or ridiculed is one of the causes of gelotophobia. Therefore, individuals can become gelotophobic as a consequence of working in an extreme ‘fun culture.’

The incremental validity of gelotophobia

To the best of our knowledge, our study is the first to provide evidence for the incremental validity of gelotophobia beyond the personality domains. Although negative emotionality and gelotophobia overlap to a certain degree, the results from the regression analysis indicated that gelotophobia predicted exposure to workplace bullying (operationalized with the total score on the NAQ-R) over and above negative emotionality. More interestingly, gelotophobia was the only significant predictor of humor-related bullying, which is in line with theoretical considerations (e.g., Hofmann et al., 2017). In sum, we provide initial evidence that the construct of gelotophobia should be considered, over and above the personality dimensions, in order to understand exposure to workplace bullying.

Limitations and future directions

Besides the general limitations common to cross-sectional design studies, and utilizing convenience sampling, there are also some further limitations to our study. First, we have not focused on investigating whether there are different homogenous groups, which may vary based on the nature and extent of their exposure to bullying (Notelaers et al., 2006; Einarsen et al., 2009). Therefore, future studies might employ latent class cluster analysis [e.g., Magidson and Vermunt, 2004; see also Reknes et al. (2021)] and also investigate how the identified groups differ regarding the personality dimensions and gelotophobia. Second, recent methodological considerations propose that incremental validity should be tested using structural equation modeling (SEM) (Wang and Eastwick, 2020; Feng and Hancock, 2022). For example, Wang and Eastwick (2020) warn that standard multiple regression inflates Type 1 error. Therefore, our results need to be replicated using SEM. It should be stressed that such research would require large sample sizes; possibly in the thousands, in order to reach a desirable level of power (Wang and Eastwick, 2020). Large sample sizes are also required if one is utilizing the latent class cluster approach [see Nylund-Gibson and Choi (2018)].

Furthermore, we did not explore the contribution of the facets of personality dimensions, and future investigations could focus on the relation between specific facets of personality and their relation to exposure to workplace bullying. Moreover, we used the short version of the BFI-2, while future research should employ the full version (Soto and John, 2017a); especially when the interest is on the facets of personality. Research is needed that will focus particularly on the humor-related bullying and its relation to gelotophobia. Platt (2021) expressed the need to include samples which include greater levels of the higher ranges on the distribution of gelotophobia on the continuum of fear – from no fear to extreme fear of being laughed at. This would be an important consideration of further studies, given the reported prevalence of gelotophobia in Hřebíčková’s et al. (2009) study that indicated the low percentage of gelotophobes in the Czech Republic [e.g., slight (5.24 %) marked (1.05 %)] and no reported extreme gelotophobes being identified [for comparable countries see Platt and Forabosco (2012)]. As Platt (2021) reports, it is only at the marked and extreme levels that strong pathological effects of gelotophobia are observed. These investigations should also consider the humor culture/climate in the specific organizations, which could include qualitative focus groups statements that would identify any mis-interpretation of pro-social humorous interaction as bullying.

Conclusion

Bullying behavior is detrimental for the bully victim. This is further impacted on in a workplace which can have serious financial as well as emotional consequences. This study has supported the link between workplace bullying and gelotophobia from the framework of personality and providing initial incremental validity for this relationship.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

Ethical approval was not required for the studies involving humans because the study adhered to ethical principles outlined by APA. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

FS: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. KP: Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft. JG: Conceptualization, Data curation, Writing – original draft. TP: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. MS: Data curation, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. The funding for the present publication was provided by the Czech Ministry of Education, Youth and Sports for specific research (IGA_FF_2023_025).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The handling editor WR declared a past co-authorship with the author TP.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1400940/full#supplementary-material

References

Balducci, C., Conway, P. M., and van Heugten, K. (2021). “The contribution of organizational factors to workplace bullying, emotional abuse and harassment” in Pathways of job-related negative behaviour. Handbooks of workplace bullying, emotional abuse and harassment. eds. P. D’Cruz, E. Noronha, E. Baillien, B. Catley, K. Harlos, and A. Høgh, et al., vol. 2 (Springer), 3–28.

Google Scholar

Bledsoe, T. S., and Baskin, J. J. (2014). Recognizing student fear: the elephant in the classroom. Coll. Teach. 62, 32–41. doi: 10.1080/87567555.2013.831022

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowling, N. A., Beehr, T. A., Bennett, M. M., and Watson, C. P. (2010). Target personality and workplace victimization: a prospective analysis. Work Stress 24, 140–158. doi: 10.1080/02678373.2010.489635

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cakirpaloglu, P., Šmahaj, J., Cakirpaloglu, S. D., and Zielina, M. (2017). Šikana na pracovišti: Reliabilita a validita českého překladu revidované verze dotazníku negativních aktů—NAQ-R [bullying at work: reliability and validity of the Czech version of the negative acts questionnaire—revised]. Československá Psychol. Časopis Pro Psychol. Teorii Praxi 61, 546–558.

Google Scholar

Cann, A., Watson, A. J., and Bridgewater, E. A. (2014). Assessing humor at work: the humor climate questionnaire. Humor Int. J. Humor Res. 27, 307–323. doi: 10.1515/humor-2014-0019

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Champely, S., Ekstrom, C., and Dalgaard, P. (2020). Pwr: basic functions for power analysis. R package version 1.3-0 [computer software]. Available at: https://github.com/heliosdrm/pwr

Google Scholar

Cortina, L. M., Magley, V. J., Williams, J. H., and Langhout, R. D. (2001). Incivility in the workplace: incidence and impact. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 6, 64–80. doi: 10.1037/1076-8998.6.1.64

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Djurkovic, N., McCormack, D., and Casimir, G. (2006). Neuroticism and the psychosomatic model of workplace bullying. J. Manag. Psychol. 21, 73–88. doi: 10.1108/02683940610643224

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dujo López, V., González Trijueque, D., Graña Gómez, J. L., and Andreu Rodríguez, J. M. (2020). A psychometric study of a Spanish version of the negative acts questionnaire-revised: confirmatory factor analysis. Front. Psychol. 11:1856. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01856

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ďurka, R., and Ruch, W. (2015). The location of three dispositions towards ridicule in the five-factor personality model in the population of Slovak adults. Personal. Individ. Differ. 72, 177–181. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.08.045

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Einarsen, S., Hoel, H., and Notelaers, G. (2009). Measuring exposure to bullying and harassment at work: validity, factor structure and psychometric properties of the negative acts questionnaire-revised. Work Stress 23, 24–44. doi: 10.1080/02678370902815673

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Einarsen, S., and Skogstad, A. (1996). Bullying at work: epidemiological findings in public and private organizations. Eur. J. Work Organ. Psy. 5, 185–201. doi: 10.1080/13594329608414854

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Feng, Y., and Hancock, G. R. (2022). Model-based incremental validity. Psychol. Methods 27, 1039–1060. doi: 10.1037/met0000342

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Führ, M. (2010). The applicability of the GELOPH< 15 > in children and adolescents: first evaluation in a large sample of Danish pupils. Psychol. Test Assess. Model. 52, 60–76.

Google Scholar

Gamian-Wilk, M., Bjorkelo, B., Mikkelsen, E. G., D'Cruz, P., and Madeja-Bien, K. (2022). Workplace bullying: individual hostility, poor work environment or both? Exploring competing explanatory models in a single longitudinal study. Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health 95, 1955–1969. doi: 10.1007/s00420-022-01896-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldberg, L. R. (1993). The structure of phenotypic personality traits. Am. Psychol. 48, 26–34. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.48.1.26

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hair, J., Hult, G. T. M., Ringle, C. M., and Sarstedt, M. (2022). A primer on partial least squares structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM). 3rd Edn. Thousand Oaks: SAGE Publications.

Google Scholar

Hofmann, J., Ruch, W., Proyer, R. T., Platt, T., and Gander, F. (2017). Assessing dispositions toward ridicule and laughter in the workplace: adapting and validating the PhoPhiKat-9 questionnaire. Front. Psychol. 8:714. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00714

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hřebíčková, M., Ficková, E., Klementová, M., Ruch, W., and Proyer, R. T. (2009). Strach ze zesměšněni: Česká a slovenská verze dotazníku GELOPH (15) pro zjištování gelotofobie [the fear of being laughed at: Czech and Slovak version of a questionnaire GELOPH (15) for gelotophobia]. Československá Psychol. Časopis Pro Psychol. Teorii Praxi 53, 468–479. doi: 10.5167/uzh-23696

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hřebíčková, M., Jelínek, M., Květon, P., Benkovič, A., Botek, M., Sudzina, F., et al. (2020). Pětifaktorový dotazník BFI-2: Hierarchický model s 15 subškálami [Big Five Inventory 2 (BFI-2): Hierarchial model]. Československá Psychol. Časopis Pro Psychol. Teorii Praxi 64, 437–460.

Google Scholar

John, O. P., Naumann, L. P., and Soto, C. J. (2008). “Paradigm shift to the integrative big-five trait taxonomy: history, measurement, and conceptual issues” in Handbook of personality: theory and research. eds. O. P. John, R. W. Robins, and L. A. Pervin. 3rd ed (Guilford), 114–158.

Google Scholar

Leymann, H. (1996). The content and development of mobbing at work. Eur. J. Work Organ. Psy. 5, 165–184. doi: 10.1080/13594329608414853

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Magidson, J., and Vermunt, J. K. (2004). “Latent class models” in The Sage handbook for quantitative methodology. ed. D. Kaplan (Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications), 175–198.

Google Scholar

McCrae, R. R., and Costa, P. T. (1987). Validation of the five-factor model of personality across instruments and observers. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 52, 81–90. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.52.1.81

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McCrae, R. R., and Costa, P. T. (2008). “The five-factor theory of personality” in Handbook of personality: Theory and research. eds. O. P. John, R. W. Robins, and L. A. Pervin. 3rd ed (Guilford), 159–181.

Google Scholar

Meade, A. W., and Craig, S. B. (2012). Identifying careless responses in survey data. Psychol. Methods 17, 437–455. doi: 10.1037/a0028085

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mikkelsen, E. G., and Einarsen, S. (2001). Bullying in Danish work-life: prevalence and health correlates. Eur. J. Work Organ. Psy. 10, 393–413. doi: 10.1080/13594320143000816

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Milam, A. C., Spitzmueller, C., and Penney, L. M. (2009). Investigating individual differences among targets of workplace incivility. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 14, 58–69. doi: 10.1037/a0012683

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nielsen, M. B., Glasø, L., and Einarsen, S. (2017). Exposure to workplace harassment and the five factor model of personality: a meta-analysis. Personal. Individ. Differ. 104, 195–206. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.08.015

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nielsen, M. B., and Knardahl, S. (2015). Is workplace bullying related to the personality traits of victims? A two-year prospective study. Work Stress 29, 128–149. doi: 10.1080/02678373.2015.1032383

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Notelaers, G., Einarsen, S., De Witte, H., and Vermunt, J. K. (2006). Measuring exposure to bullying at work: the validity and advantages of the latent class cluster approach. Work Stress 20, 289–302. doi: 10.1080/02678370601071594

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nylund-Gibson, K., and Choi, A. Y. (2018). Ten frequently asked questions about latent class analysis. Trans. Issues Psychol. Sci. 4, 440–461. doi: 10.1037/tps0000176

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Platt, T. (2008). Emotional responses to ridicule and teasing: should gelotophobes react differently? Humor Int. J. Humor Res. 21, 105–128. doi: 10.1515/HUMOR.2008.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Platt, T. (2021). Extreme gelotophobia: affective and physical responses to ridicule and teasing. Curr. Psychol. J. Div. Perspect. Div. Psychol. Issues 40, 6076–6084. doi: 10.1007/s12144-019-00510-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Platt, T., and Forabosco, G. (2012). “Gelotophobia: the fear of being laughed at” in Humor and health promotion. ed. P. Gremigni (Hauppauge, NY: Nova Biomedical Books), 229–252.

Google Scholar

Platt, T., Proyer, R. T., Hofmann, J., and Ventis, W. L. (2016). Gelotophobia in practice and the implications of ignoring it. Eur. J. Humour Res. 4, 46–56. doi: 10.7592/EJHR2016.4.2.platt

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Platt, T., Proyer, R. T., and Ruch, W. (2009). Gelotophobia and bullying: the assessment of the fear of being laughed at and its application among bullying victims. Psychol. Sci. Q. 51, 135–147. doi: 10.5167/uzh-19396

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Plester, B. (2016). The complexity of workplace humour: laughter, jokers and the dark side of humour. Cham: Springer International Publishing.

Google Scholar

Plester, B., Bentley, T., and Brewer, E. (2022). “It only hurts when I laugh”: tolerating bullying humour in order to belong at work. Eur. J. Humour Res. 10, 116–134. doi: 10.7592/EJHR.2022.10.2.645

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Podsiadly, A., and Gamian-Wilk, M. (2017). Personality traits as predictors or outcomes of being exposed to bullying in the workplace. Personal. Individ. Differ. 115, 43–49. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.08.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Proyer, R. T., Meier, L. E., Platt, T., and Ruch, W. (2013). Dealing with laughter and ridicule in adolescence: relations with bullying and emotional responses. Soc. Psychol. Educ. Int. J. 16, 399–420. doi: 10.1007/s11218-013-9221-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Proyer, R. T., Neukom, M., Platt, T., and Ruch, W. (2012). Assessing gelotophobia, gelotophilia, and katagelasticism in children: an initial study on how six to nine-year-olds deal with laughter and ridicule and how this relates to bullying and victimization. Child Indic. Res. 5, 297–316. doi: 10.1007/s12187-011-9127-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Proyer, R. T., and Ruch, W. (2010). Enjoying and fearing laughter: personality characteristics of gelotophobes, gelotophiles, and katagelasticists. Psychol. Test Assess. Model. 52, 148–160.

Google Scholar

Reknes, I., Notelaers, G., Iliescu, D., and Einarsen, S. V. (2021). The influence of target personality in the development of workplace bullying. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 26, 291–303. doi: 10.1037/ocp0000272

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruch, W. (2009). Fearing humor? Gelotophobia: the fear of being laughed at: introduction and overview [editorial]. Humor: Int. J. Humor Res. 22, 1–25. doi: 10.1515/HUMR.2009.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruch, W., Harzer, C., and Proyer, R. T. (2013). Beyond being timid, witty, and cynical: big five personality characteristics of gelotophobes, gelotophiles, and katagelasticists. Int. Stud. Humour 2, 24–42. doi: 10.5167/uzh-81159

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruch, W., Hofmann, J., Platt, T., and Proyer, R. (2014). The state-of-the art in gelotophobia research: a review and some theoretical extensions. Humor Int. J. Humor Res. 27, 23–45. doi: 10.1515/humor-2013-0046

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruch, W., and Proyer, R. T. (2008a). The fear of being laughed at: individual and group differences in gelotophobia. Humor Int. J. Humor Res. 21, 47–67. doi: 10.1515/HUMOR.2008.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruch, W., and Proyer, R. T. (2008b). Who is gelotophobic? Assessment criteria for the fear of being laughed at. Swiss J. Psychol. 67, 19–27. doi: 10.1024/1421-0185.67.1.19

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruch, W., and Proyer, R. T. (2009). Who fears being laughed at? The location of gelotophobia in the Eysenckian PEN-model of personality. Personal. Individ. Differ. 46, 627–630. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.01.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruch, W., Proyer, R. T., and Popa, D. E. (2008). The fear of being laughed at (gelotophobia) and personality. Anuarul Institutului de Istorie “G. Baritiu” din Cluj-Napoca (Ed.), Series Humanistica (VI, 53–68). Cluj-Napoca: Romania.

Google Scholar

Ruch, W., and Stahlmann, A. G. (2020). Toward a dynamic model of Gelotophobia: social support, workplace bullying and stress are connected with diverging trajectories of life and job satisfaction among Gelotophobes. Curr. Psychol. 42, 16368–16380. doi: 10.1007/s12144-020-01046-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Soto, C. J., and John, O. P. (2017a). The next big five inventory (BFI-2): developing and assessing a hierarchical model with 15 facets to enhance bandwidth, fidelity, and predictive power. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 113, 117–143. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000096

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Soto, C. J., and John, O. P. (2017b). Short and extra-short forms of the big five inventory–2: the BFI-2-S and BFI-2-XS. J. Res. Pers. 68, 69–81. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2017.02.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Titze, M. (1997). “Das Komische als schamauslösende Bedingung [the funny as an elicitor of shame]” in Scham – ein menschliches Gefühl [Shame – A human emotion]. eds. R. Kühn, M. Raub, and M. Titze (Opladen, Germany: Westdeutscher Verlag), 169–178.

Google Scholar

Titze, M. (2009). Gelotophobia: the fear of being laughed at. Humor Int. J. Humor Res. 22, 27–48. doi: 10.1515/HUMR.2009.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsuno, K., Kawakami, N., Inoue, A., and Abe, K. (2010). Measuring workplace bullying: reliability and validity of the Japanese version of the negative acts questionnaire. J. Occup. Health 52, 216–226. doi: 10.1539/joh.l10036

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y. A., and Eastwick, P. W. (2020). Solutions to the problems of incremental validity testing in relationship science. Pers. Relat. 27, 156–175. doi: 10.1111/pere.12309

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yentes, R., and Wilhelm, F. (2018). Careless (version 1.1.3) [computer software]. Available at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/careless

Google Scholar

Zapf, D., and Einarsen, S. (2001). Bullying in the workplace: recent trends in research and practice− an introduction. Eur. J. Work Organ. Psy. 10, 369–373. doi: 10.1080/13594320143000807

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: workplace bullying, NAQ-R, big five, gelotophobia, incremental validity

Citation: Sulejmanov F, Petr K, Gambová J, Platt T and Seitl M (2024) Exposure to workplace bullying: the incremental effect of gelotophobia beyond the big five. Front. Psychol. 15:1400940. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1400940

Received: 14 March 2024; Accepted: 11 April 2024;
Published: 25 April 2024.

Edited by:

Willibald Ruch, University of Zürich, Switzerland

Reviewed by:

Robert Durka, Catholic University in Ružomberok, Slovakia
Chloe Lau, Western University, Canada

Copyright © 2024 Sulejmanov, Petr, Gambová, Platt and Seitl. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Filip Sulejmanov, ZmlsaXAuc3VsZWptYW5vdkB1cG9sLmN6

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.