Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 29 August 2024
Sec. Environmental Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Climate Change Challenge: Adaptation to Climate Change View all 12 articles

Embodied earth kinship: interoceptive awareness and relational attachment personal factors predict nature connectedness in a structural model of nature connection

  • Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom

Previous research has found that nature connectedness, an experiential close connection to nature with cognitive, affective and physical benefits, profoundly impacts individual wellbeing and subsequently increases pro-environmental behaviors. However, little is known about the personal and contextual factors that predict nature connectedness. Testing theory derived from a qualitative interpretative phenomenological analysis study, this research addresses the lacuna in the literature. A structural equation model analysis finds that interoceptive awareness significantly predicts nature connection, that secure attachment to nature significantly explains this relationship, and that these inter-related constructs predict both pro-environmental behavior and wellbeing. This revised model of nature connection indicates important antecedents for the human-nature bond, illuminating in particular that the interpersonal relational processes foundational for close bonding with humans also occur in bonding with nature. Structural equation modeling indicates that emotional awareness is the dimension of interoceptive awareness that most significantly predicts nature connection, suggesting that the more aware a person is of the connection between inner bodily sensations and emotions, the more likely they can bond with nature. Given that interoceptive awareness indicates a coherent relationship with the self, including effective communication between body, mind and feelings, this process is therefore implicated in the capacity for humans to bond with nature. In sum, this present research points to the efficacy of an embodied, secure attachment with nature to help close both the human-nature disconnection chasm, and the environmental value-action gap. Theoretical and methodological implications for research and policy are discussed.

1 Introduction

Human-caused climate change, advanced through rapid industrialization over the past two centuries, has led to rampant biodiversity loss, extensive-reduction of wetlands and woodlands, and negative transformation of ice-free land and oceans with economic (Burke et al., 2015), social (Goldstein, 2016) and health (Smith and Myers, 2018) consequences. The global rate of nature's decline is unprecedented in human history and is directly linked to human actions, with one million species threatened with extinction and 69% of animal populations already having been lost since 1970 (Brondizio et al., 2019). This evidence suggests human complicity in the rampant loss and damage of nature, which also lays bare an ongoing coloniality, co-constitutive of processes of capitalism and imperialism (Sultana, 2022). Disconnection from nature is one of the fundamental root causes from which environmental change results (Redvers et al., 2023). The human-nature relationship, or the connectedness between humans and nature, a psychological term that captures various aspects of the human relationship to nature, has been severely damaged (Ives et al., 2017; Richardson, 2023). If the apparent dissolution of the relationship between humans and nature continues, societies will increasingly operate outside safe planetary boundaries, a concept introduced in 2009 to define the environmental limits in which humanity can safely operate (Steffen et al., 2015). Yet, the human-nature relationship is essential to mitigate climate collapse, illustrated by a recent meta-analysis of 147 correlational studies indicating that the strength of the human-nature relationship is critical for predicting resource management and sustainability (Barragan-Jason et al., 2022). The same meta-analysis also analyzed 59 experimental studies and found that environmental education leads to no effect on pro-nature behaviors, whereas people with higher nature connectedness have significantly more pro-nature behaviors, and are also significantly healthier. This evidence points to an interdigitated relationship between nature connection, pro-environmental behaviors and human health. Nature connection is linked to mindsets that value sustainability and behaviors that enhance it. Therefore, concerted effort is necessary to explicate both the root causes of this disconnection and discover leverage points for reconnection.

1.1 Nature connection

Empirical studies suggest nature connection can have immense benefits for humans, including improving individual wellbeing (Lambert et al., 2020), with mood (MacKerron and Mourato, 2013), cognitive (Berman et al., 2008), health (Frumkin, 2001), and longevity (Mitchell and Popham, 2008) benefits, indicative of nature's importance for overall optimal health (Aberson et al., 2000).

Recent epidemiological studies demonstrate the association between people's perceived health and availability of green space (de Vries et al., 2003; Maas et al., 2008), suggesting that the urban-rural health gap is mediated by a discrepancy in nature availability (Richardson et al., 2020). Several public health experimental studies have also discovered positive physiological benefits as a result of exposure to nature in diverse ways, including forest bathing, mindful exposure and even virtual contact (Song et al., 2016; Frost et al., 2022; Kotera et al., 2022).

Nature connection can also restore emotional and cognitive resources. In a meta-analysis of empirical research surveying 32 studies with a total of 2,356 participants, exposure to natural environments was associated with a moderate rise in positive affect and small, but consistent, decreases in negative affect (McMahan and Estes, 2015). The stress-reduction theory [SRT; (Ulrich et al., 1991)], and the satisfaction of the need to be connected to the natural world as theorized by the Biophilia hypothesis (Wilson and Kellert, 1993), could explain these results. While these findings are suggestive of nature's impact on emotional resourcing, the mechanism of emotional regulation is unclear.

Emotional regulation mediates the relationship between nature connection and happiness (Richardson and McEwan, 2018), and between nature connectedness and stress (Bakir-Demir et al., 2021), however more research is needed to understand the relationship between emotional regulation and nature connection. If nature connection is indeed primarily an emotional bond as Richardson and McEwan (2018) argues, then investigating personal factors that determine these emotional regulation capacities is potentially important.

The theoretical background of nature connection, or nature relatedness, draws on Wilson's Biophilia hypothesis, which theorizes that humans are born with an innate tendency to affiliate with nature (Wilson, 1984). Nature connection is different than exposure to greenspaces, defined as open, undeveloped land with natural vegetation (Twohig-Bennett and Jones, 2018), as well as outdoor learning environments (Harris, 2021). While simply being amongst nature suggests a form of passive engagement and potentially perpetuates the very objectification of nature that nature immersion attempts to correct, nature connection is generally concerned with the aesthetic, intellectual, cognitive, spiritual and emotional dimensions of connection, as put forward by Wilson (1984). However, the nature connection literature contains multiple and conflicting definitions of nature connection, which is also reflected in measurement.

In some cases, nature connection is defined cognitively as being part of nature or self in nature (Richardson et al., 2021; Barragan-Jason et al., 2022), while other scholars argue it is a capacity to feel a pleasant and secure connection to nature (Baxter and Pelletier, 2019). Still others suggest it is an emotional and psychological connection to nature (Richardson et al., 2021), and yet others as a function of love and deep caring for nature (Perkins, 2010). Nisbet et al. (2019) stress that nature relatedness is a multi-dimensional construct which captures several facets of human-nature relationships including cognition, affect and experience. Overall, nature connection scholars converge on agreeing that nature relatedness is distinct from a general sense of connection or environmental attitudes (Nisbet and Zelenski, 2013). Yet what exactly is nature connection?

Recent meta-analyses of the empirical nature connection studies (Tam, 2013; Whitburn et al., 2020) suggest that it is both inconclusive which measures properly capture the nature connection construct, and how divergent or convergent they are. The result is diffuse approaches to increasing so-called nature connectedness without consistency in what it is that is being targeted. Furthermore, while these definitions seem to capture the subjective sense of connectedness with nature and claim to operationalise a relationship between humans and nature, the mechanism of relationship between humans and nature remains unclear.

Empirical nature connection studies mostly test the efficacy of various intervention designs, including comparing dosage length and frequency (e.g. brief vs. extended), as well as different types of nature exposure (e.g. nature walks, noticing nature, virtual, various primes including active or passive attention, etc.), which all focus on how to increase nature connection. What remains missing is further explication on what this relationship comprises of.

In a study that sought to go beyond general nature contact knowledge and knowledge-based activities, specific routes to nature connectedness were examined, systematically structured around the nine values of the Biophilia hypothesis (Lumber et al., 2017). Contact, emotion, meaning, and compassion were identified as ‘routes' or pathways to increasing nature connection. Another study using data from a large national survey in the United Kingdom, revealed that noticing nature vs. spending time in nature, explained levels of nature connectedness (Richardson et al., 2022). In both of these examples, the ways to increase nature connection are more illuminated, and yet the mechanism of relationship between humans and nature remains muddled. If empirical research continues to only test how connected to nature people are but not what this relationship equates, important mechanisms of action as well as critical personal factors in predicting nature connectedness will go overlooked.

In addition, empirical studies suggest nature connection is not just important for wellbeing (Dean et al., 2018), but can even motivate pro-environmental behavior (Whitburn et al., 2020). In a meta-analysis of 37 independent samples from 26 studies of 13,237 individuals, a random-effects model demonstrated a positive and significant association between connection to nature and pro-environmental behaviors (Whitburn et al., 2020). This is the strongest empirical evidence in the nature connection science literature to date of the link between nature connection and pro-environmental behaviors. With public health demands increasing for research that can meaningfully address what is called the environmental value-action gap, or the gulf between one's understanding and care of nature and a willingness to act on behalf of nature (Barr, 2006), as well as to create new ways to work together for a just and sustainable future (Redvers et al., 2023), this evidence is promising.

1.2 Relational attachment to nature

Therefore, to examine a relationship with nature, then understanding both the human motivation for bonding with nature through the lens of relational attachment theory could be efficacious, in addition to isolating the core emotional regulation capacity. Secure relational attachment as first theorized by Bowlby (1969), provides a basis for healthy self-relating and the capacity for intimate relationships throughout the lifespan (Bowlby, 1969; Ainsworth, 1978; Mikulincer and Shaver, 2003). Schaller (2007) believed that secure attachment is important for such a diverse range of meaningful outcomes that, a “sense of secure attachment may be the psychological equivalent of a broadband antibiotic” (p. 191). Research by Mikulincer and Shaver (2003) suggests that when attachment security is made salient, it creates not just a sense of interpersonal attachment, but security in general.

Building on the half-century of research since Bowlby's attachment theory was put forward, attachment security in relation to nature connectedness is worth investigating as an avenue to get closer to the processual relationship between humans and nature. In an empirical study to investigate the psychological determinants of place attachment, which is defined as “the bonds that humans share with particular settings” (Nisa et al., 2020), attachment security was induced, which was found to increase the strength of place attachment, particularly in individuals with insecure attachment styles (Nisa et al., 2020). In the first study to examine if a sense of connectedness deriving from secure attachment could indeed extend to external environments, Nisa et al found that the ability for humans to connect with humans is crucial to understanding how humans bond with place. Over four studies, results indicate that attachment style is associated with the strength of place attachment (Nisa et al., 2020). While Nisa et al.'s research suggests a link between attachment security and attachment to place, more research is needed on attachment security and attachment to nature, specifically on the role of secure attachment to mediate the relationship between potential personal factors and nature connectedness.

To further this point, a review on personal and social influences on environmental concern indicated that personal identification with a place is a critical predictor of environmental concern and behavior (Gifford and Nilsson, 2014), therefore suggesting that the role of bonding takes a part in explaining individuals' responses to environmental problems. Therefore, it is feasible to investigate a motivational framework toward connecting with nature that is partially satisfying of the interpersonal need to attach, drawing upon the belongingness hypothesis (Baumeister and Leary, 1995), and expressed via individual differences in attachment style.

Since the bonds humans develop with other humans are consequential to how humans bond with place, this present research seeks to understand if human attachment is likewise related to the processual mechanism of bonding with nature.

The literature on nature connection supports the importance of interpersonal processes within the human-nature bond, exploring in multitudinous ways that a close relationship with nature is a basic human psychological need from cognitive, emotional and physiological dimensions, and one that must be filled in order to experience increased wellbeing (Baxter and Pelletier, 2019; Richardson et al., 2021). Baxter and Pelletier (2019) argue that this psychological need is only satisfied by in vivo immersive experiences in nature that are pleasant.

However, the nature connection literature often defines nature connectedness as nature immersion and nature exposure (Baxter and Pelletier, 2019), ignoring interpersonal relational process mechanisms. More specificity is needed to understand the interpersonal processes within the human-nature bond.

One phenomenological study explored participants' experiences with the natural world and found that the natural world was indeed experienced as a primary attachment figure, a secure base, and as embodied (Schweitzer et al., 2018). These results support the need for more research into relational attachment and embodied factors of nature relatedness. In sum, explicating the human-nature relationship as a function of a secure attachment to nature, and examining it as a personal factor of nature connectedness, would fill the gap in the nature connection science literature related to the mechanism of human-nature bonding.

1.3 Interoceptive awareness as emotional regulation mechanism

Considering that emotional regulation is important in maintaining secure human relational attachment (Ferraro and Taylor, 2021), emotional regulation could be likewise critical in the human-nature attachment. Health science and biomedical literature offers a framework to understand the role of the body in emotional regulation, through the construct of interoceptive awareness (body awareness). Interoceptive awareness refers to the processing and central representation of afferent internal bodily signals (Critchley and Garfinkel, 2017). Interoceptive awareness is based on an interoceptive predictive processing framework in which emotional feeling states arise from physiological changes in the body. Increased body awareness improves accurate detection of emotional states and boosts regulation of them (Critchley and Garfinkel, 2017; Quadt et al., 2018).

Interoceptive awareness is derived from peripheral theories of emotion (James, 1884), and has been found to have special efficacy to reducing anxiety (Dunn et al., 2010) through improving emotional regulation (Füstös et al., 2013; Dunne et al., 2021), is integral to higher-order cognition (Khalsa et al., 2017), and is thought to facilitate regulation of an integrated sense of self by decreasing distress will be (Price and Hooven, 2018). According to Price and Hooven (2018), when applying reappraisal as an emotion regulation strategy, interoceptive awareness facilitates the downregulation of affect-related arousal.

These findings suggest that the more aware a person is of ongoing bodily processes, the more successful this person's emotion regulation in response to negative affect. A correlational study found that interoceptive awareness and dispositional mindfulness, which is thought to encourage insight into the relationship between mind and body, are tightly interwoven and partly overlapping constructs, and that both are independently linked to psychological wellbeing (Hanley et al., 2017). Yet while interoception has also been seen to improve emotional regulation amongst autistic populations (Nord and Garfinkel, 2022), and a putative target for novel interventions to address neural activation in specific psychiatric disorders (Nord et al., 2021), there has been no published research into the potential interconnection between body awareness and nature connection.

Even though nature connection studies have investigated the relationship between physiological health and nature connection, body-based awareness has been measured only as physiological health indicators. For example, the benefits of “forest bathing” (therapeutic restorative-effects of physiological relaxation in a forest or natural environment) have been found to improve physical and mental health, defined as a decrease in the most prevalent mental health disorders (e.g., depression, anxiety and stress) (Song et al., 2016; Kotera et al., 2022). Forest bathing is a type of nature connection practice dependent on immersing oneself in nature using the senses (Song et al., 2016; Timko Olson et al., 2020), which could be argued increases somatosensory areas and interoceptive pathways (Medeiros et al., 2023). However, the engagement of one's senses in these interventions is via an external focus vis à vis paying attention to internal bodily sensations. In a recent meta-analysis of the human-nature relationship as a pathway to sustainability, 59 experimental studies were analyzed that attempted to increase nature connectedness (Barragan-Jason et al., 2022). Out of the six types of experimental designs identified, none primed participants to pay attention to inner bodily sensations.

Given that body awareness is connected to emotional regulation (Khalsa et al., 2017), and emotional regulation has been cited as important for nature connection (Korpela et al., 2018; Richardson and McEwan, 2018), the role of body awareness in the human-nature relationship deserves attention. Richardson and McEwan (2018) linked emotional regulation to the wellbeing benefits of nature connectedness, highlighting the role of affect regulation. Yet mechanisms that predict emotional regulation are not captured in existing nature connection studies. Common predictors in nature connection studies include demographic information, nature-exposure condition, and individual differences like mind-attribution, the big five personality traits (Tam, 2015), spiritual and religious attitudes (Preston and Shin, 2022), personal intentions (Sparks et al., 2014), and eudaimonic values (Shin et al., 2022). Empirical studies in nature connection therefore investigate what can increase nature connectedness, but not necessarily what personal factors are essential for nature connectedness to occur.

1.4 Preliminary study development

The theoretical framework of this present study was informed by the results of an interpretative phenomenological analysis (IPA) study. The methodology of analysis was followed step-by-step as outlined by Smith et al. (2022). In-depth interviews lasting 1.5 h were conducted with six environmental activists to better understand the phenomenology of their human-nature bond. Considering these people were already utilizing their professional lives to address climate collapse, understanding their relationships with nature could be efficacious to illuminating the experience of the human-nature bond, revealing gaps and areas for further research. In addition, the majority of nature connection research is conducted amongst mainly Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich and Democratic nations [WEIRD; (Henrich et al., 2010)]. I sought to correct for this in part by orienting this research first in the perspectives of Black, Indigenous and People of Color in an interpretative phenomenological analysis study.

The ramifications of the separation between humans and nature have wrought cumulative harms to biodiversity, lands and waters, intertangled with the interlocking oppressions of race, class, gender and other axes (Sultana, 2022). The IPA study asked: what is an equitable relationship between humans and nature that can challenge the normalization of climate breakdown and meaningfully address it?

Four group experience themes emerged:

• Body awareness: the experience of the body is a vehicle of sensory connection and communication with nature.

• Relational attachment: the emotional and intimate close bond with nature occurs through the attachment mechanism.

• Entangled identity and sacred cosmology: widening individual identities beyond the locus of the individual “self,” into entanglement is precipitated on anthropomorphic and animistic tendencies.

• Intersectional environmentalism: a close bond with nature motivates intersectional justice as an experiential act, rooted in reciprocal relationship.

Since interpretative phenomenological analysis studies are based on a discrete number of cases and not meant for generalizability (Smith et al., 2022), it was necessary to examine these dimensions with other methods suited for understanding individual differences in line with a broader research aim. The following study can be seen as inspired by these IPA results. Two dimensions, body awareness and relational attachment, were taken forward in the following study. The entangled identity theme was not taken forward since there is already ample evidence in the existing literature to the importance of an interdependent self-construal in a bond with nature. For example, the widely-used Inclusion of Nature in the Self Scale (INS) developed by Schultz (2002). A meta-analysis found that INS had a small but significant effect size (r = 0.0.25) in predicting pro-environmental behavior (Whitburn et al., 2020). The fourth theme, intersectional environmentalism, requires developing new scale items to reflect these behaviors, which are not currently captured in pro-environmental behavior or sustainability behavior scales. This author has done so and that scale is in development.

1.5 This present research

The statement of purpose for this research is as follows: are body awareness and relational attachment critical personal factors that explicate the human-nature relationship? Furthermore, does a relationship with nature benefit not only human health but influence pro-environmental behaviors and human wellbeing?

This present research seeks to utilize structural equation modeling to investigate support for the following redefinition of nature connectedness: an embodied, secure relationship with nature with positive personal and collective wellbeing consequences.

An online survey research design was chosen as a first step to validate the above definition of nature connectedness given its robustness in estimating inter-relationships between constructs.

One way to bridge the disconnection between humans and nature is through restoring an equal-status relationship between humans and nature. The science of nature connectedness gets close to this aim, but this present research argues, does not yet go far enough.

This research agenda is split into the following hypotheses:

• Interoceptive awareness predicts nature connectedness.

• Interoceptive awareness predicts nature connection even after controlling for covariates.

• A revised theoretical model of nature connection indicates support for a reciprocal, embodied, secure relationship with nature.

• Relational attachment mediates the relationship between interoceptive awareness and nature connection.

The proposed structural model under investigation in this study is below (see Figure 1). Based on previous studies, a relationship is expected between nature connection and pro-environmental behavior (Mackay and Schmitt, 2019), as well as from nature connection to wellbeing (Martin et al., 2020). Interoceptive awareness is expected to predict nature connection (Hypothesis 1), even when controlling for confounds (Hypothesis 2) but that not all dimensions of interoceptive awareness will be significant. The revised structural model of nature connection (Hypothesis 3) will enhance theoretical precision of the human-nature bond and its impacts and support this study's definition of nature connectedness. Relational attachment will mediate the relationship between interoceptive awareness and nature connection (Hypothesis 4). This data is correlational and so causality claims should be considered with caution.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Hypothesized relationships between constructs. This figure theorizes how the personal factors of Interoceptive Awareness and Relational Attachment predict Nature Connection and subsequent wellbeing and pro-environmental behavior which build on previous literature (Martin et al., 2020).

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants and procedure

Participants included a convenience sample from the United Kingdom and the United States recruited on Prolific, an online research platform, who passed two attention checks and gave informed signed consent (N = 299). The final sample (61.20% female) ranged from 19 to 93 years old (M = 39.75, SD = 15.41) with varying levels of education; high school (15.41%), undergraduate (75.90%) and graduate (8.70%). Participants were mostly White (73.6%), and some rated religion as extremely important (28.1%).

For those who had childhood contact with nature, 25.75% rated it as extremely important (M = 3.6, SD = 1.12).

For full demographic summary statistics see Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic summary statistics of sample.

2.2 Measures

All outcome measures, including means and standard deviations, are listed in Table 2. Independent variable measures are listed in Table 3 including subdimensions. Confound variables including means and standard deviations are in Table 4. The measures are listed below including Cronbach alpha reliability statistics.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Overview of all items used as outcome variables, with descriptive statistics.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Overview of all items used for interoceptive awareness including subdimensions with descriptive statistics.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Overview of all items used as confound variables with descriptive statistics.

2.2.1 Interoceptive awareness

Interoceptive sensibility is defined as the self-perceived tendency to focus on interoceptive stimuli (Desmedt et al., 2022). According to Khalsa et al. (2017), this construct is well-captured by the 37-item scale, Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA) (Mehling et al., 2012, 2018). The MAIA includes the following eight subdimensions; (1) Noticing (e.g. “I notice where in my body I am comfortable”), (2) Not-distracting (e.g. “I distract myself from sensations of discomfort”), (3) Not-worrying (e.g. “When I feel physical pain I become upset”), (4) Attention regulation (e.g. “I can return awareness to my body if I become distracted”), (5) Emotional awareness (e.g. “I notice how my body changes when I am angry”), (6) Self-regulation (e.g. “I can use my breath to reduce tension”), (7) Body listening, (e.g. “I listen to my body to inform me about what to do”), (8) Trusting (e.g. “I am at home in my body”). Responses included nine reverse-key items and were rated on a scale from 1—Never to 5—Always (α = 0.91).

2.2.2 Nature connection

The 15-item Love and Care for Nature Scale (LCN) was chosen due to the explicitly emotional dimensions of love and deep caring it captures which is in line with the theoretical proposition of this study (e.g. “I feel a deep love for nature; Perkins, 2010). The LCN scale has been used consistently throughout the nature connection literature (Tam, 2013). Responses included no reverse-key items and were rated on a scale from 1—Strongly disagree to 7—Strongly agree (α = 0.97).

2.2.3 Wellbeing

Eudaimonic wellbeing is the most consistently enhanced form of wellbeing according to the nature connection literature (Pritchard et al., 2020). Among the various dimensions of eudaimonic wellbeing (such as positive affect and life satisfaction), vitality is most strongly associated with nature connectedness (Capaldi et al., 2014). Therefore, the 6-item Vitality Scale was used to measure eudaimonic wellbeing (“I feel alive and vital”; Ryan and Frederick, 1997). Responses included no reverse-key items and were rated on a scale from 1—Not at all true to 7—Very true (α = 0.90).

2.2.4 Pro-environmental behavior

An 8-item scale was used to assess both personal and public sphere PEBs, according to a coherent theory on pro-environmental behavior (Stern, 2000), with questions from Martin et al. (2020). Personal sphere pro-environmental behaviors included items related to recycling, conserving gasoline, buying ethical fashion, plastic use and more. Public sphere PEBs included items related to lobbying, voting, veganism and more. The scale was kept as one dimension instead of two, due to low reliability if kept separate. Responses included no reverse-key items and were rated on a scale from 1—Never to 7—Always (α = 0.81).

2.2.5 Relational attachment humans

The proposed mediator is both human-human and human-nature relational attachment. The 9-item Experiences in Close Relationships measure was utilized (Wei et al., 2007), which is considered to have the greatest precision and validity in measuring relational attachment (Fraley et al., 2000). The scale investigates approach-avoidance (e.g. “I don”t feel comfortable opening up to others”), approach-anxiety (e.g. “I”m afraid that other people may abandon me”), and security in human-human relationships (e.g. “It helps to turn to people in times of need”). The first four items are reverse-keyed and were rated on a scale from 1—Strong disagree to 7—Strongly agree (α = 0.86).

2.2.6 Relational attachment nature

In addition, per the authors admonition, I adapted the relational attachment scale to substitute words of nature in order to measure attachment to nature which became a secondary measure. The scale investigates human-nature approach-avoidance (e.g. “I don”t feel comfortable getting close to nature”), human-nature approach-anxiety (e.g. “I”m afraid that nature may abandon me”), and human-nature relational security (e.g. “I am supported by nature”). The first four items are reverse-keyed and were rated on a scale from 1—Strong disagree to 7—Strongly agree (α =0.82).

2.2.7 Covariates

Lastly, to control for potential confounds, I included measures of traits and experiences suggested by previous literature; childhood contact with nature (Capaldi et al., 2014), certain psychedelic use including psilocybin (Lyons and Carhart-Harris, 2018; Forstmann et al., 2023), previous mystical experience (Paterniti et al., 2022) and trait openness to experience from the Big 5 personality test (Tam, 2013). These covariates are all predictive of nature connectedness. Accounting for the potential shared variance of these measures allows better isolation of the role of interoceptive awareness in predicting nature connection.

Openness to experience (McCrae and Sutin, 2009) was reliable (α = 0.84) and included 10-items rated on a scale from 1—Strong disagree, to 5—Strongly agree. See Table 4 for descriptive statistics of the confounds.

2.3 Analytic approach

Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) allows estimation between a large number of independent variables and more than one dependent variable at the same time, a superior technique compared to traditional mediation analysis, and useful for understanding inter-related structural models (Mehmetoglu and Jakobsen, 2017, p. 294). Further, due to SEM being correlative and suggestive of frameworks, not causality, it fits the overall research paradigm of this study.

3 Results

3.1 Validation and measurement quality

Before beginning the SEM analysis, Principle Component Factor Analysis (PCF), part of Factor Analysis (FA), was performed to verify the measurement quality of the constructs for the eventual SEM model (Mehmetoglu and Jakobsen, 2017). This was done as an exploratory first step which built to the eventual Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA). This step verifies the validity of the latent constructs before using them as independent or dependent variables in the model.

The Interoceptive Awareness (IA) latent variable was analyzed in Stata (Version 18), which indicated eight principle components. An extraction method based on principal component analysis and the promax rotation method with Kaiser normalization was executed, since the factors should be correlated. See Table 5 for generated factor scores from taking the average of the variables expressing each factor for the eight factors, as well as alpha coefficients to indicate the reliability of the summated subdimension scales (Mehmetoglu and Jakobsen, 2017, p. 287). This corroborated the literature which argues for eight subdimensions of the Interoceptive Awareness scale (Mehling et al., 2018). By running the pcf command on the remaining constructs, one factor was indicated for each scale, according to Kaiser criterion, which fits the theory of those constructs.

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Generated factor scores and alpha coefficients for each dimension of interoceptive awareness factors.

Secondly, the suitability of the dataset was determined including the value of the determinant using the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measure of sampling adequacy. KMO values between 0.8 and 1.0 indicate the sampling is adequate (Shrestha, 2021). The Interoceptive Awareness scale had a KMO of 0.89, indicating adequacy. Thirdly, appropriateness of the data set was tested for a functioning factor analysis with the Bartlett's test of sphericity. If the Bartlett's test of Sphericity is highly significant at p < 0.001, this indicates that that the correlation matrix has significant correlations among at least some of the variables (Shrestha, 2021).

The results of KMO tests for all latent constructs are in Table 6. The Bartlett's test of sphericity was statistically significant for all latent constructs, with a p = 0.00. Therefore, each passed the KMO and Bartlett's test of sphericity, indicating validity for use as latent constructs in SEM.

Table 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. KMO statistics for latent constructs.

3.2 Confirmatory factor analysis

A Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) was then performed for each proposed latent variable in order to investigate the hypothesized underlying structure of the data and if it fit with the theorized latent variable measurement model. A structural equation model was estimated using maximum likelihood on each potential latent variable. Model fit evaluation and satisfactory results indicated model fit for all latent variables. All factor loadings, which show strong linear combinations of underlying indicators with the latent variables, are reported in full, along with goodness of fit statistics of χ2, RMSEA, Comparative Fit Index (CFI), and Tucker-Lewis Index (Tucker and Lewis, 1973) in Table 7. Although no precise standards exist for what value of indices equate to good fit, typical guidelines are that TFI and CFI should exceed 0.90. RMSEA values above.10 indicate poor model fit (Mehmetoglu and Jakobsen, 2017, p. 308). All the latent variables passed these fit indices.

Table 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Factor loadings and goodness of fit statistics including Chi-squared, RMSEA, Comparative Fit Index, Tucker- Lewis and convergent/divergent validity AVE and Raykov.

Internal consistency was assessed by Cronbach's alpha, also reported in Table 7. Each measure passed the adequate threshold for Cronbach's alpha of > 0.7, indicating adequate to excellent internal consistency and reliability (Shrestha, 2021). According to Kline (2016), convergent validity refers to a set of indicators designed to measure a construct, which can be tested using Average Variance Extracted (AVE). A high AVE (> 0.50) indicates a high convergent validity, therefore AVE for each construct should be at least 0.50. Since a recent paper suggests removing constructs below 0.40 (Haji-Othman and Yusuff, 2022), the pro-environmental behavior variable was retained. To indicate divergent validity, Raykov's factor reliability coefficient is provided, which computes reliability coefficients for factors with and without correlated errors (Mehmetoglu and Jakobsen, 2017, p. 287). Raykov coefficients > 0.70 are considered divergent and all latent variables passed this test which are reported in Table 7.

3.3 Correlations

Next, Pearson correlation coefficients were calculated to assess the effect size and statistical significance of the univariate relationship between all of the variables under consideration and are reported in full in Table 8. As expected, Interoceptive Awareness is correlated with nature connection, but only on certain dimensions. Nature connection is strongly positively correlated with Emotional Awareness (r = 0.51), moderately positively correlated with Self-Regulation (r = 0.46), Body Listening (r = 0.39), and Body Trust (r = 0.30), and maintains a small positive correlation with Noticing (r = 0.26), Attention Regulation (r = 0.23), and a small negative correlation with Non-Distracting (r = −0.20).

Table 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Correlations between all latent variables (N = 299).

Therefore, Emotional Awareness, followed by Self-Regulation, Body Listening and Body Trust, seem to be the most important dimensions of Interoceptive Awareness in connection with a bond to nature.

Vitality follows the same pattern and is highly positively correlated with Self-Regulation (r = 0.52), and Body Trust (r = 0.57), and is positively moderately correlated with Emotional Awareness (r = 0.32) and Attention Regulation (r = 0.38).

Lastly, Relational Attachment Humans is moderately positively correlated to Body Trust (r = 0.24), Self-Regulation (r = 0.20), and Emotional Awareness (r = 0.23). Relational attachment to nature is highly positively correlated to Emotional Awareness (r = 0.43), Self-Regulation (r = 0.39) and Body Listening (r = 0.34), moderately positively correlated to Body Trust (r = 0.24) and Noticing (r = 0.24), and moderately negatively correlated to Non-Distracting (r = −0.24).

Overall, this suggests that the dimensions of Emotional Awareness, Self-Regulation, Body Listening, Body Trust and Non-Distracting are perhaps the most important dimensions of interoceptive awareness in relationship to nature connection. Therefore, since the other dimensions are not correlated to nature connection and they did not pass the AVE threshold test, they will be excluded from the path analysis and deemed insignificant to nature connection.

Relational Attachment Humans and Relational Attachment Nature have a weak significant correlation (r = 0.18). This could suggest that these constructs are tapping different aspects of human bonding and that patterns of attachment in human-human bonds vs. human-nature bonds have their own unique expressions. Below are the results per research hypothesis.

Hypothesis 1: Interoceptive awareness predicts nature connectedness.

I ran a structural model using maximum likelihood. The following interoceptive awareness dimensions are significantly predictive of nature connection in this structural model: Non-Distracting is significantly negatively predictive (b = −0.33, SE = 0.11, p = 0.00, 95% [−0.55, −0.12]), meaning the more distracted one is with their bodily sensations, the less connected to nature they will be, whereas Emotional Awareness (b = 0.67, SE = 0.13, p = 0.00, 95% [0.45, −0.94]), and Self-Regulation (b = 0.36, SE = 0.12, p = 0.00, 95% [0.13, 0.56]) are positively significantly predictive of nature connection. This indicates that Emotional Awareness and Self-Regulation increase the likelihood of being nature connected. Body Listening is nearly positively significantly predictive of nature connection (b = 0.12, SE = 0.07, p = 0.06, 95% [0.13, 0.56]). This indicates that the various dimensions do not function similarly in their relationship to nature connection, and not all interoceptive awareness dimensions are significantly predictive.

Hypothesis 2: Interoceptive awareness predicts nature connection even after controlling for covariates.

A series of linear regressions indicated that certain dimensions of interoceptive awareness do significantly predict nature connection even when controlling for previous childhood nature contact, previous psychedelic use and mystical experiences. These are as follows; Emotional Awareness (b = 0.39, SE = 0.05, p = 0.00, 95% [0.30, 0.49]), Self-Regulation (b= 0.36, SE = 0.05, p = 0.00, 95% [0.27, 0.45]), Body Listening (b = 0.28, SE = 0.05, p = 0.00, 95% [0.19, 0.38]), and Body Trust (b = 0.22, SE = 0.05, p = 0.00, 95% [0.12, 0.32]). Non-distracting was significantly negatively predictive of nature connection (b = 0.16, SE = 0.05, p = 0.00, 95% [−0.26, −0.60]). Emotional awareness accounted for 30% of the variance in nature connection. Other dimensions ranged from 16% variance (non-distracting), 18.5% (body trust), to 28% (self-regulation), indicating the importance of these dimensions on nature connection. These subdimensions of Interoceptive Awareness follow the same pattern as in hypothesis 1.

What this indicates is that people who possess an ability to be emotionally aware, self-regulate, listen to inner bodily sensations and trust them, while being undistracted from painful and disturbing sensations, are all likely to be more connected to nature and to develop a close bond, even when controlling for the above confounds. Further, Emotional Awareness accounts for the most variance, indicating that the ability to notice, identify and locate one's emotions is highly predictive of nature connectedness. This evidence points to the importance of a coherent bodily self to motivate a relationship with nature.

Hypothesis 3: A revised theoretical model of nature connection indicates support for a reciprocal, embodied, secure relationship with nature.

A saturated, non-recursive structural model using maximum likelihood estimation and the latent variables under investigation was utilized. The model is below (see Figure 2).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Proposed structural model of interoceptive awareness, nature connection, pro-environmental behaviors and wellbeing. Each latent variable passed validity tests. The estimated structural equation model (SEM) specifies five predictor latent variables, chosen because these Interoceptive Awareness dimensions were significant in Hypothesis 1.

Table 9 reports the standardized regression weights for the model. The model identifies a significant positive relationship between the dimensions of Non-Distracting, Emotional Awareness and Self-Regulation on nature connection, which in turn significantly predicts pro-environmental behaviors and increased wellbeing. Modifications were made to the model during model fit testing and the final model used all available 299 observations and indicated good model fit (χ2 = 2,000.379, p > 0.05, CFI = 0.92, TLI = 0.91). The goodness-of-fit indices calculated for the SEM indicate the model estimated provides a good fit to the data. For the final model, see Figure 3.

Table 9
www.frontiersin.org

Table 9. Structural equation model of interoceptive awareness dimensions on nature connection, pro-environmental behaviors and wellbeing.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Final structural model of interoceptive awareness dimensions on nature connection, pro-environmental behaviors and wellbeing. Standardized coefficients are reported.

A second model was fitted that investigated the latent variable interoceptive awareness as the predictor instead of splitting this latent variable into its theoretical subdimensions. Table 10 reports the standardized regression weights for the model. Interoceptive Awareness as a latent variable is significantly predictive of nature connection, which predicts wellbeing and flourishing. Modifications were made to the model during model fit testing and the final model indicated good model fit (χ2 = 6,220.277, p > 0.05, CFI = 0.91, TLI = 0.91) (see Figure 4). Therefore, the results indicate that overall, interoceptive awareness is predictive of nature connection and subsequently increased pro-environmental behaviors and increased wellbeing, and that parsing further, the subdimensions of emotional awareness, self-regulation and non-distracting are responsible for this relationship.

Table 10
www.frontiersin.org

Table 10. Structural equation model of interoceptive awareness, nature connection, pro-environmental behaviors and wellbeing.

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Structural model of interoceptive awareness as a single latent construct on nature connection, pro-environmental behaviors and wellbeing. Standardized coefficients are reported.

Hypothesis 4: Relational attachment mediates the relationship between interoceptive awareness and nature connection.

A path analysis model was estimated using maximum likelihood, testing the mediating effect of relational attachment with nature in the relationship between interoceptive awareness and nature connection. The model had good fit (χ2 = 442.383, p > 0.05, CD = 0.249) and used all available 299 observations. Results point to a statistically significant relationship between interoceptive awareness and relational attachment (see Figure 5); and respectively, between interoceptive awareness and nature connection, as well as relational attachment and nature connection. Results also indicate that the proportion of total effect mediated by relational attachment to nature was large at 76%. This indicates that the relationship between interoceptive awareness and nature connection is mostly explained by the closeness of the relational attachment with nature. The mediating effect of relational attachment with nature in the relationship between interoceptive awareness and nature connection was non-significant and so only the relational attachment to nature construct is in the mediation model.

Figure 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Direct, indirect and total effects of interoceptive awareness on nature connection, mediated by relational attachment to nature. Mediation diagram; a, b, c and c' are path coefficients representing unstandardized regression weights and standard errors (in parentheses). The c path coefficient represents the total effect of interoceptive awareness on nature connection. The c-prime path coefficient refers to the direct effect of the interoceptive awareness on nature connection. ab path is the indirect mediated effect of interoceptive awareness on nature connection via relational attachment to nature. Significant level, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

These results indicate that secure relational attachment to nature could be more important than interoceptive awareness in predicting nature connection, or at minimum, that there is a parsimonious relationship between these constructs.

4 Discussion

Overall, the results lend support to the initially proposed structural theoretical model (Figure 1). Interoceptive awareness, which has not yet been investigated in the nature connection literature, indeed predicts nature connection, increased wellbeing and increased pro-environmental behaviors. Further, human-nature relational attachment plays a significant mediating role between interoceptive awareness and nature connection, indicating the importance of relational security, and lending credence to a bond with nature that is at least partially satisfying of the human need to attach and belong, occurring through the attachment-system, and built on body awareness capacities. A relationship with nature could function similarly to interpersonal relationships, built through an attachment-system that downregulates stress with the parallel benefits of positive emotions and increased wellbeing.

Finally, it is important to note that although these findings through structural and mediation models are suggestive of strong relationships, this study does not reveal causality. Instead, conceptualizing these findings as strong associations is appropriate.

The potential impact of this research includes the following; (1) to provide actionable knowledge that points to key areas that can help ameliorate the human-nature disconnection crisis; and (2) provide recommendations for nature connection interventions that could improve the growing disconnection between humans and nature and motivate environmental sustainability behaviors, thereby also addressing the environmental value-action gap.

As Botanist and Indigenous scholar Robin Wall Kimmerer writes,

Knowing that you love the earth changes you, activates you to defend and protect and celebrate. But when you feel that the earth loves you in return, that feeling transforms the relationship from a one-way street into a sacred bond (Kimmerer, 2013).

4.1 Body awareness is the foundation of a relationship with the earth

The cross-sectional study indicates that interoceptive awareness significantly predicts increased nature connectedness, which can be explained by its emotional regulatory capacity. Interoceptive awareness is the processing and central representation of afferent internal bodily signals (Critchley and Garfinkel, 2017), which leads to a coherent relationship with the self, defined as effective communication between the body, mind and emotions (Price and Hooven, 2018). People who are able to identify, assess and appraise internal bodily signals likewise experience improvement in emotional and sensory awareness, a decrease in distress, and an improvement in emotional regulation (Price and Hooven, 2018).

Of the eight dimensions of interoceptive awareness (Mehling et al., 2012), only several significantly predict nature connection; (1) non-distraction, (2) emotional awareness, and (3) self-regulation, and to a lesser degree, body trust and body listening. Perhaps these dimensions address the core capacities of emotional literacy and regulation given that they are mid-level constructs in the overall interoceptive awareness progressively-built model. Considering the nature connection literature has already well-established that the most important predictor of nature connection is an emotionally-driven bond (Richardson, 2023, p. 58), and that nature connection predicts wellbeing and pro-environmental behaviors (Whitburn et al., 2020), these findings point to a key antecedent of this bond: interoceptive awareness. Thus, the results suggest that interoceptive awareness offers something akin to a set of building blocks to nature connectedness; by not being distracted by painful sensations, possessing emotional awareness, and having the ability to regulate sensations, bonding with nature is more likely.

Emotional awareness and self-regulation were the two most highly significant dimension out of the eight, as well as holding the most variance, suggesting the dual role of both simply noticing changes within one's inner state (e.g. “I notice that my body feels different after a peaceful experience”), as well as regulating them (e.g. “I can use my breath to reduce tension”), can increase the likelihood of connecting to nature. This could be due to the dimensions' similarity to mindfulness capacities (enhancing present moment awareness).

In the effort by many researchers and interdisciplinary fields of study to understand how to reconnect humans and nature, this research therefore suggests that focusing on individual capacities for interoceptive awareness is a currently overlooked, yet critical factor. Therefore, interoceptive awareness can be understood as an entry point to a shared sensory language with more-than-human life that increases emotional regulation and subsequent bonding with nature.

To underscore the importance of interoceptive awareness as a personal factor of nature connectedness, confounds which have been previously investigated as important explainers of individual differences in nature connection, did not erase the significant relationship between interoceptive awareness on nature connection. These are as follows: previous mystical experience, psychedelic experience, childhood contact with nature and openness.

Returning to this research's philosophically phenomenological roots, embodied sensory awareness is synonymous with philosopher Merleau-Ponty (1974) conceptualization of sensation in his Phenomenology of Perception. Becoming reacquainted with the breathing, sensing body opens the perceptual capacity to frequent the sensorial dimension of experience in which humans are corporeally embedded. Inhabiting the body's language of sensations in an ontological manner serves to bridge the mind-body split by reconnecting humans and nature not as a matter of utility, but as a relationship in an ontological necessity. Such a framing binds embodiment, nature and experience together in a shared reality.

4.2 A relationship with nature can function as a secure relational attachment

The human-nature bond parallels human relational attachment and security-based bonding processes. Secure attachment with nature mediated the relationship between interoceptive awareness and nature connectedness, and significantly explained that relationship, indicating the importance of relational attachment with nature as theorized by Bowlby (1979). Further, the model predicted subsequent wellbeing and pro-environmental behaviors, thus indicating the importance of a secure attachment to nature in the overall wellbeing and altruism model.

These findings suggest that the relational attachment-system, which has been studied copiously in human-human relationships (Mikulincer and Shaver, 2003; Shaver et al., 2016), attenuating threat through the availability of an “attachment figure,” creating a “safe haven,” and a “secure base,” also could function similarly between humans and nature. Perhaps if people repeatedly experience nature as available in secure and safe, positive encounters, further proximity-seeking is motivated (which serves to reduce threat), deepening the bond around a secure attachment system (e.g., a cognitive-affective structure; Bowlby, 1988).

The relational attachment process reflects a comprehensive framework for understanding how emotional bonds and relationships develop through patterns of attachment between children and caregivers developed by Ainsworth (1978). Given that interactions with a safe attachment figure is theorized to be incorporated into working models of the self, and since a person tends to assimilate any new bonds, whether with people, or in the case of this present research, with nature, into an existing model, it is feasible to suggest that the human-nature relationship is similarly incorporated into an existing model of the self (Bowlby, 1979). The result is a relational schema that operates automatically and could drastically shape a person's experience in bonding with both nature and others throughout the lifespan.

The cross-sectional study found that secure attachment with nature significantly mediates the relationship between interoceptive awareness and nature connection, thus suggesting that “nature” has the potential to be an attachment figure, serving to facilitate bonding via a secure attachment system and becoming embedded into a relational schema. Given that the cross-sectional study found that human-nature attachment does not covary with human-human attachment, the attachment system between humans and nature is unique, yet built on the same threat attenuation system as human-human bonding.

No research on nature connection has adapted the relational attachment scale to reflect human-nature bonding, and so these findings open up new avenues of exploration. If nature can be a secure attachment figure, and if proximity-seeking to nature is motivated by threat attenuation, and further, if that relationship can be integrated into a working model of the self, a type of human-nature bond is revealed that outpaces the tertiary benefits of connecting to nature, and rather points to a fundamental bond inseparable from models of the self.

This present research thus finds that a core psychological process, one's relational attachment system (Bowlby, 1969) is activated in human-nature bonding. Previous research on place attachment has shown support for the development of place attachment through facilitating a sense of connectedness and a positive bond between individuals and places (Nisa et al., 2020). However, secure attachment to nature is different than place attachment; the former based on inter-personal relational processes, the later on the strength of identification with an externalized other.

Interoceptive awareness has also been shown to have a strong link with relational attachment, with suggestive evidence that early developmental attachment teaches the basics of how to know and trust inner bodily signals and sensations just like attachment cues (Oldroyd et al., 2019). As this research suggests, from this foundation of a coherent bodily self , relational attachment to nature is more likely. Yet due to the correlational nature of the cross-sectional study, directional causality between these constructs is inconclusive, yet their inter-relatedness is apparent.

4.3 A relationship with nature is primarily an emotional bond

A relationship with nature is principally an emotional bond, evidenced by interoceptive awareness (an emotional regulation mechanism) predicting higher levels of nature connectedness, mediated by secure relational attachment (requiring emotional regulation to increase secure attachment). To underscore this finding, out of the eight interoceptive dimensions, emotional awareness and self-regulation were the most highly significantly predictive of nature connectedness. The emotional awareness dimension is defined as the ability to notice how emotions translate as inner bodily signals (e.g., I notice how my body changes when I feel happy/joyful). As stated earlier in the discussion, the capacity to mindfully notice these emotions, in addition to self-regulating them, leads to a greater likelihood of secure bonding with nature.

The importance of affect in human-nature bonding is also reflected in nature connection studies. Richardson et al. (2021) and Lumber et al. (2017), building on myriad empirical nature connection studies, claim that nature connectedness is primarily an emotional bond, vs. an information or knowledge-based connection. In addition, the Love and Care for Nature scale created by Perkins (2010) seeks to psychometrically capture the construct of love and deep caring for nature as an expression of an explicitly emotional relationship with nature. Used throughout the literature on nature connection, and seen to significantly predict nature connection (Zylstra, 2014), the Love and Care for Nature scale operationalizes this affective domain and supports the importance of emotion in bonding with nature.

This present research underscores this claim, while adding new findings which indicate that awareness of emotions plus bodily states together create a mechanism that initiates a spiral of positive emotions stemming from relational security with nature. Therefore, this research parses a difference between merely possessing affective feelings toward nature, and the mechanism of generating those emotions. The results suggest that the human-nature bond is an interpersonal relational process in which a relational schema that impacts one's cognition, affect and behavior is continuously updated vis à vis positive emotional experiences with nature, which motivate further proximity-seeking with nature, resulting in both secure attachment with nature and subsequent behavior that reflects the desire to protect this bond. In sum, ongoing, positive interactions with nature over time will not only increase wellbeing, but motivate further bonding with nature, built on affective and embodied interpersonal relational mechanisms.

4.4 A relationship with nature is mutually-beneficial

One way the human-nature relationship is mutually beneficial, and even reciprocal, occurs through increasing both wellbeing and flourishing for humans, while simultaneously motivating pro-environmental behaviors on behalf of nature. The cross-sectional study found that nature connectedness significantly predicts wellbeing (measured as eudaimonic happiness), and that those with higher interoceptive awareness and secure attachment to nature were more likely to have increased wellbeing. Wellbeing, measured as eudaimonia (life purpose), is the form of wellbeing most strongly related to nature connection, as indicated in both nature connection empirical studies and a meta-analysis (Nisbet et al., 1998; Pritchard et al., 2020; Shin et al., 2022). Further, this study found that those with higher wellbeing also exhibited significantly higher pro-environmental behaviors. Therefore, this research offers a more precise way to understand a mutually-beneficial relationship.

Mutual benefit can be understood as a gratitude-driven altruism framework (Tam, 2022). The focus on mutual benefit differentiates this present research's definition of nature connection from current nature connection studies, pointing to the importance of collective altruism intrinsic to a mutually, reciprocal relationship with nature. Tam (2022) suggests that a gratitude-driven altruism trait might come from the following: it is associated with how frequently the person has contact with nature, how strongly one feels entitled to nature's benefits, and to what extent one perceives nature as humanlike (p. 11).

Reciprocity is currently discussed in decolonisation and Indigenous literature, but is missing from the nature connection studies. Nature connection empirical studies rely on Western notions of “giving back,” including certain behaviors that are determined “pro-environmental” or “pro-nature conservation behavior” (e.g., recycling, planting pollinator-friendly plants, voting for certain policies; Mackay and Schmitt, 2019; Barbett et al., 2020).

While this study measured giving back within the above domains, further research should expand measurement of pro-environmental behaviors to include measures of intersectional environmentalism. Climate justice is intersectionally interconnected with other areas of injustice. In line with this, reciprocal ethics, which can be understood as a gratitude-driven altruism, roots the mutual benefit of the human-nature bond in relational qualities. The value of such a mutually-beneficial relationship is reflected in Indigenous scholarship on the importance of the relational tipping point vs. single lens focus on the ecological tipping point. Whyte (2020) argues that relational qualities like trust, reciprocity and accountability are critical for climate justice, above and beyond individual public or private sphere pro-environmental behaviors.

Such a perspective shifts the focus from research concerned with how to motivate individual pro-environmental behaviors to prevent an ecological tipping point, to knowledge and practices that cultivate the relational qualities necessary to prevent a relational tipping point. The theoretical proposition is that the human-nature relationship, which motivates reciprocal altruism, could be a primary mechanism to mitigate human-driven climate change and human-driven biodiversity loss, via relational qualities, and not just individual behavior. While not mutually exclusive, these paths chart vastly different priorities, epistemologies, ontologies and praxis.

4.5 Limitations and further exploration

There are several limitations to this exploratory study. The biomedical science literature indicates that interoceptive awareness is best measured with the heartbeat count or perception task (Garfinkel et al., 2015), not a self-report measure, which was outside the scope of this study. Therefore, future research could seek to replicate these findings by using physiological measures, include a larger sample, and investigate cultural differences. While sample size was sufficient for a correlational study, larger, more diverse, population-level studies would validate and extend these findings and prevent sampling bias.

Since previous research has investigated the efficacy for interoceptive awareness to reduce anxiety, future research could test the efficacy of interoceptive awareness to reduce climate and eco-anxiety in comparison to general anxiety disorder and tease apart the inter-relationships. Future research could also parse into the inter-relatedness of interoceptive awareness dimensions and secure attachment, including relational attachment dimensions of approach-avoidance and approach-anxiety. Future work could test if relational attachment predicts nature connection mediated by interoceptive awareness, instead of relational attachment being the mediator, to better understand causality. Theories of body-emotion and self-other bonding could also be brought to bear to better understand the mechanism of body-mind-nature relationships.

Although the sample was drawn from the crowd sourcing platform Prolific Academic, which has their merits compared to Amazon Mechanical Turk (Peer et al., 2017), including transparency about the sample population, options for longitudinal studies, and participant payment rights, they were still mainly derived from Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich and Democratic nations [WEIRD; (Henrich et al., 2010)]. I sought to correct for this in part by founding this entire research in the perspective of Black, Indigenous and People of Color in the IPA study, but the correlational study did not reflect this population sample diversity. The results of this research are meant to be suggestive and are not causal, nor are they generalizable.

Future research should seek to replicate these results within diverse contexts and to be culturally-specific. In so doing, meaningful intervention design and best-practices can reflect context and culture. In addition, further research could utilize a larger scale structured interview survey study design on a one-on-one or group basis instead of online surveys.

In respect to diversity and equity regarding nature connection research, at least four major strands of research are needed according to Frumkin et al. (2017): (a) patterns of disproportionate exposure; (b) cultural and contextual factors that affect nature preferences and the experience of nature; (c) differing patterns of benefit across different populations; and (d) the possibility that improved access to nature may have unintended negative consequences on vulnerable populations. This research sought to address the cultural and contextual factors that affect the experience of nature, and further research should address the other major strands (Frumkin et al., 2017). There is too little emphasis on culturally and contextually-specific nature connection studies, and much more work is needed to center the perspectives of those whose cultural values already reflect sustainability and kinship with nature, as well as those who are disproportionately affected by climate change. A widespread commitment in nature connection science to diversity, equity and inclusion is essential to protect against perpetuating harm through colonial ideologies.

Regarding interventions to increase nature connectedness, future work could seek to prime interoceptive awareness in a field setting to increase ecological validity. Interventions that pair interoceptive awareness and nature bonding vs. just interoceptive awareness could help to tease apart the role of the body vs. the role of interpersonal bonding in the human-nature relationship. Considering this research points to interpersonal processes of human-nature closeness, future interventions could include primes for animism and anthropomorphism in comparison to just secure relational attachment, to further illuminate how the nature-human bond functions. While this research points to emotion regulation capacities, more work is needed to understand if the mechanism is primarily one of buffering to stressors, in terms of threat-reduction, or is perhaps one of increasing capacity to experience self-transcendent emotions.

In order to examine the collective impacts of interdependent identity shifts occurring in parallel with increased nature connection, future work could seek to understand not just individual responses to an intervention, but how these individuals make up social networks that can reinforce social norms of intersectional environmentalism to one another. Researchers could investigate how inter and intra personal factors fit together with the larger, more fluid and dynamic structural forces that influence environmental justice. To change both individuals and environments requires capturing this reciprocal process. Employing social network analysis would allow researchers to watch the effects of an interoceptive awareness intervention expand and multiply to other members in the network.

Lastly, future research should replicate these findings in partnership with Indigenous scholars using Indigenous research methodologies, including participatory action research and appreciative inquiry, which would increase social and transformational change outcomes (Chilisa, 2020, p. 181).

4.6 Conclusion

Human-driven climate change, biodiversity loss and the rapid increase of climate-related disasters indicates that the human-nature relationship is failing. However, this research points to a way to reconnect humans and nature via embodied and secure relational attachment processes. Body awareness creates a foundation for a secure attachment with nature, resulting in positive emotions and behavioral displays of climate care. Thus, the human-nature bond is an emotional regulation strategy, satisfying one's need for attachment security. Body awareness is the starting point for a close relationship with nature: the whole endeavor begins in the senses.

As a lived life process, the body experiences the phenomenal world moment-to-moment, ordering experience and enabling an intimate, felt relationship with the natural world. Given that the human body and the “body” of the Earth are so interconnected, the essentialism of the body points to an ontological rendering of the body as a shared phenomenal reality, and therefore reduces the separatism and individualism undergirding extractive behaviors. Nature and humans are intra-bodied. Humans and nature are kin. Attempts to reduce nature connection to anything less than this fails to account for the immensity of this bond.

This research builds on the literature of the myriad benefits of both nature connection and adult relationships, while addressing the environmental value-action gap through revealing the critical personal factor of interoceptive awareness and highlighting the necessary mechanism of the interpersonal processes occurring in the human-nature relationship. Thus, while advocating for individual increases in nature connection that motivate pro-environmental behaviors is a starting point, it does not go far enough. This research broadens that perspective to include evidence of an embodied, reciprocal, secure relationship with nature that could impact communal, structural and societal futures, repealing ideologies that justify domination over nature instead of inter-relatedness with nature. To foster this kind of a relationship with nature, which predicts how committed one is to pro-environmental behaviors in-line with that relationship, requires approaches that center both increasing body awareness and opportunities to interpersonally attach to nature.

With climate collapse and species extinction increasing the urgency of new approaches to address the environmental value-action gap, this research points to the core aspects of body awareness and the closeness of the interpersonal, emotional bond with nature, as essential to shifting extractive and damaging human-driven climate change behaviors. The findings of this research should be taken by policy makers, global health practitioners and educators, to design, implement and rapidly scale interventions to increase body awareness in parallel with creating equitable access for the public to experience immersive, emotional encounters with nature. This is a global, low-cost, readily-available solution. Climate change and nature connection interventions should shift from prioritizing exposure to nature or knowledge about nature, to a laser focus on facilitating the close, emotional, relational development of a bond with nature. The result could be an interconnected network of human-nature relational fascia, a broadband inoculation to the demands of global imperialism in favor of the values of living in sustainable relational harmony with the Earth. In sum, the strength of the human-nature bond, which is predicated on one's body awareness, is an essential factor in predicting future behavior that either protects or destroys the planet, and so every effort should be made to facilitate it for people everywhere.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by University of Cambridge Department of Psychology Research Ethics Committee. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

LB: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This study was made possible due to the Cambridge Trust scholarship to LB. The funding for this study was provided by the University of Cambridge Department of Psychology.

Acknowledgments

I would like to thank my supervisor Dr. David Good and my academic advisor Dr. Eolene Boyd-MacMillan for providing co-supervision and invaluable ideas and feedback. Thank you to the Cambridge Trust and the University of Cambridge for funding this research as part of my doctoral studies. A special thank you to the participants in this research, especially those who participated in the in-depth interviews. Their wisdom, perspective, and shared life-experience is the foundation of this undertaking. I acknowledge Indigenous communities who have long-held sacred the human-nature relationship and environmental land defenders, protectors, advocates, and artists around the world, who refuse to abdicate responsibility and care of the planet. A very deep thank you to the collective ontological body that stretches far further than skin or geography. Thank you to the trees, who inspired this research and to whom it is dedicated.

Conflict of interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Aberson, C. L., Healy, M., and Romero, V. (2000). Ingroup bias and self-esteem: a meta-analysis. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 4, 157–173. doi: 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0402_04

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1978). The Bowlby-Ainsworth attachment theory. Behav. Brain Sci. 1, 436–438. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00075828

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakir-Demir, T., Kazak Berument, S., and Akkaya, S. (2021). Nature connectedness boosts the bright side of emotion regulation, which in turn reduces stress. J. Environ. Psychol. 76:101642. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2021.101642

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barbett, L., Stupple, J., Sweet, M., Schofield, M., and Richardson, M. (2020). Measuring actions for nature-development and validation of a pro-nature conservation behaviour scale. Sustainability 12:4885. doi: 10.3390/su12124885

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barr, S. (2006). Environmental Action in the Home: Investigating the “Value-Action” Gap. Geography, 91, 43–54. Retrieved from: https://www.jstor.org/stable/40574132

Google Scholar

Barragan-Jason, G., de Mazancourt, C., Parmesan, C., Singer, M. C., and Loreau, M. (2022). Human–nature connectedness as a pathway to sustainability: a global meta-analysis. Conservation Lett. 15, 1–7. doi: 10.1111/conl.12852

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., and Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychol. Bull. 117, 497–529. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baxter, D. E., and Pelletier, L. G. (2019). Is nature relatedness a basic human psychological need? A critical examination of the extant literature. Can. Psychol. 60, 21–34. doi: 10.1037/cap0000145

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Berman, M. G., Jonides, J., and Kaplan, S. (2008). The cognitive benefits of interacting with nature. Psychol. Sci. 19, 1207–1212. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02225.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowlby, J. (1979). The Making and Breaking of Affectional Bonds. London: Tavistock.

Google Scholar

Bowlby, J. (1988). A Secure Base: Clinical Applications of Attachment Theory. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Brondizio, E. S., Settele, J., Díaz, S., and Ngo, H. T (2019). Global Assessment Report on Biodiversity and Ecosystem Services of the Intergovernmental Science-Policy Platform on Biodiversity and Ecosystem Services. IPBES.

Google Scholar

Burke, M., Hsiang, S. M., and Miguel, E. (2015). Global non-linear effect of temperature on economic production. Nature 527, 235–239. doi: 10.1038/nature15725

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Capaldi, C. A., Dopko, R. L., and Zelenski, J. M. (2014). The relationship between nature connectedness and happiness: a meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 5:976. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00976

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chilisa, B. (2020). Indigenous Research Methodologies. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Google Scholar

Critchley, H. D., and Garfinkel, S. N. (2017). Interoception and emotion. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 17, 7–14. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.04.020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

de Vries, S., Verheij, R. A., and Spreeuwenberg, P. (2003). Natural environments - Healthy environments? An exploratory analysis of the relationship between greenspace and health. Environm. Plann. A. 35, 1717–1731. doi: 10.1068/a35111

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dean, J. H., Shanahan, D. F., Bush, R., Gaston, K. J., Lin, B. B., Barber, E., et al. (2018). Is nature relatedness associated with better mental and physical health? Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 15, 9–11. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15071371

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Desmedt, O., Heeren, A., Corneille, O., and Luminet, O. (2022). What do measures of self-report interoception measure? Insights from a systematic review, latent factor analysis, and network approach. Biol. Psychol. 169:108289. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2022.108289

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, B. D., Stefanovitch, I., Evans, D., Oliver, C., Hawkins, A., and Dalgleish, T. (2010). Can you feel the beat? Interoceptive awareness is an interactive function of anxiety- and depression-specific symptom dimensions. Behav. Res. Ther. 48, 1133–1138. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.07.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunne, J., Flores, M., Gawande, R., and Schuman-Olivier, Z. (2021). Losing trust in body sensations: Interoceptive awareness and depression symptom severity among primary care patients. J. Affect. Disord. 282, 1210–1219. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.12.092

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferraro, I. K., and Taylor, A. M. (2021). Adult attachment styles and emotional regulation: The role of interoceptive awareness and alexithymia. Pers. Individ. Differ. 173:110641. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.110641

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Forstmann, M., Kettner, H. S., Sagioglou, C., Irvine, A., Gandy, S., Carhart-Harris, R. L., et al. (2023). Among psychedelic-experienced users, only past use of psilocybin reliably predicts nature relatedness. J. Psychopharmacol. 37, 93–106. doi: 10.1177/02698811221146356

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fraley, R. C., Waller, N. G., and Brennan, K. A. (2000). An item response theory analysis of self-report measures of adult Attachment 78, 350–365. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.78.2.350

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Frost, S., Dymand, L., Schaffer, V., Millear, P., Allen, A., Stallman, H., et al. (2022). Virtual immersion in nature and psychological well-being: a systematic literature review. J. Environ. Psychol. 80:101765. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2022.101765

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Frumkin, H. (2001). Beyond toxicity: human health and the natural environment. Am. J. Prev. Med. 20, 234–240. doi: 10.1016/S0749-3797(00)00317-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Frumkin, H., Bratman, G. N., Breslow, S. J., Cochran B., P. H. K. Jr, Lawler, J. J, et al. (2017). Nature contact and human health: a research agenda. Environ. Health Perspect. 125, 1–18. doi: 10.1289/EHP1663

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Füstös, J., Gramann, K., Herbert, B. M., and Pollatos, O. (2013). On the embodiment of emotion regulation: interoceptive awareness facilitates reappraisal. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 911–917. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss089

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Garfinkel, S. N., Seth, A. K., Barrett, A. B., Suzuki, K., and Critchley, H. D. (2015). Knowing your own heart: Distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biol. Psychol. 104, 65–74. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.11.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gifford, R., and Nilsson, A. (2014). Personal and social factors that influence pro-environmental concern and behaviour: a review. Int. J. Psychol. 49, 141–157. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12034

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldstein, J. S. (2016). Climate change as a global security issue. J. Global Secu. Stud. 1, 95–98. doi: 10.1093/jogss/ogv010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haji-Othman, Y., and Yusuff, M. S. S. (2022). Assessing reliability and validity of attitude construct using partial least squares structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM). Int. J. Acad. Res. Busin. Soc. Sci. 12, 378–385. doi: 10.6007/IJARBSS/v12-i5/13289

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanley, A. W., Mehling, W. E., and Garland, E. L. (2017). Holding the body in mind: Interoceptive awareness, dispositional mindfulness and psychological well-being. J. Psychosom. Res. 99, 13–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2017.05.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, F. (2021). Developing a relationship with nature and place: the potential role of forest school. Environm. Educ. Res. 27, 1214–1228. doi: 10.1080/13504622.2021.1896679

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., and Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Nature 466, 29–29. doi: 10.1038/466029a

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ives, C. D., Glusti, M., Fischer, J., Abson, D., Klanwiecki, K., Dorninger, C., et al. (2017). Human–nature connection: a multidisciplinary review. Curr.Opin. Environm. Sustainab. 26–27, 106–113. doi: 10.1016/j.cosust.2017.05.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

James, W. (1884). What is an emotion?. Mind 19, 188–205. doi: 10.1093/mind/os-IX.34.188

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Khalsa, S. S., Adolphs, R., Cameron, O. G., Critchley, H. D., Davenport, P. W., Feinstein, J. S., et al. (2017). Interoception and mental health: a roadmap. Biol. Psychiat. 3, 501–513. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.04.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kimmerer, R. W. (2013). Returning the Gift, Minding Nature.

Google Scholar

Kline, R. B. (2016). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. New York: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Korpela, K. M., Pasanen, T., Repo, V., Hartig, T., Staats, H., Mason, M., et al. (2018). Environmental strategies of affect regulation and their associations with subjective well-being. Front. Psychol. 9:562. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00562

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kotera, Y., Richardson, M., and Sheffield, D. (2022). Effects of Shinrin-Yoku (Forest Bathing) and nature therapy on mental health: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Mental Health Addict. 20, 337–361. doi: 10.1007/s11469-020-00363-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lambert, L., Lomas, T., van de Weijer, M. P., Passmore, H. A., Joshanloo, M., Harter, J., et al. (2020). Towards a greater global understanding of wellbeing: a proposal for a more inclusive measure. Int. J. Wellbeing 10, 1–18. doi: 10.5502/ijw.v10i2.1037

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lumber, R., Richardson, M., and Sheffield, D. (2017). Beyond knowing nature: Contact, emotion, compassion, meaning, and beauty are pathways to nature connection. PLoS ONE 12, 1–24. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0177186

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyons, T., and Carhart-Harris, R. L. (2018). Increased nature relatedness and decreased authoritarian political views after psilocybin for treatment-resistant depression. J. Psychopharmacol. 32, 811–819. doi: 10.1177/0269881117748902

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Maas, J., Verheij, R. A., Spreeuwenberg, P., and Groenewegen, P. P. (2008). Physical activity as a possible mechanism behind the relationship between green space and health: a multilevel analysis. BMC Public Health 8, 1–13. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-8-206

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mackay, C. M. L., and Schmitt, M. T. (2019). Do people who feel connected to nature do more to protect it? A meta-analysis. J. Environm. Psychol. 65:101323. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.101323

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

MacKerron, G., and Mourato, S. (2013). Happiness is greater in natural environments. Global Environm. Change 23, 992–1000. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2013.03.010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, L., White, M., Hunt, A., Richardson, M., Pahl, S., and Burt, J. (2020). Nature contact, nature connectedness and associations with health, wellbeing and pro-environmental behaviours. J. Environm. Psychol. 68:101389. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101389

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McCrae, R. R., and Sutin, A. R. (2009). “Openness to experience. in Leary,” in Handbook of Individual Differences in Social Behavior, eds. M. R. and Hoyle, R. H. (New York: The Guilford Press), 257–273.

Google Scholar

McMahan, E. A., and Estes, D. (2015). The effect of contact with natural environments on positive and negative affect: a meta-analysis. J. Posit. Psychol. 10, 507–519. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2014.994224

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Medeiros, S., Langer, Á. I., and Stolzenbach, S. (2023). “Contemplation of nature to promote mental health and prevent depression in youth,” in Prevention and Early Treatment of Depression Through the Life Course. Depression and Personality, eds. V. Martínez, C. Miranda-Castillo (Cham: Springer).

Google Scholar

Mehling, W. E., Acree, M., Stewart, A., Silas, J., and Jones, A. (2018). The multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness, version 2 (MAIA-2). PLoS ONE 13:e0208034. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0208034

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mehling, W. E., Price, C., Daubenmier, J. J., Acree, M., Bartmess, E., and Stewart, A. (2012). The multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness (MAIA). PLoS ONE 7:48230. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048230

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mehmetoglu, M., and Jakobsentor, G. (2017). Applied Statistics Stata. London: Sage Publications.

Google Scholar

Merleau-Ponty, M. (1974). Phenomenology of Perception. London: Humanities Press.

Google Scholar

Mikulincer, M., and Shaver, P. R. (2003). The attachment behavioral system in adulthood: activation, psychodynamics, and interpersonal processes. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 35, 53–152. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(03)01002-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, R., and Popham, F. (2008). Effect of exposure to natural environment on health inequalities: an observational population study. Lancet 372, 1655–1660. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)61689-X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nisa, C. F., Bélanger, J. J., and Schumpe, B. M. (2020). On solid ground: Secure attachment promotes place attachment. J. Environ. Psychol. 70, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101463

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nisbet, E. K., Zelenski, J., and Murphy, S. (1998). Happiness is in our nature: exploring nature relatedness as a contributor to subjective well-being. J. Happiness Stud. 12, 303–322. doi: 10.1007/s10902-010-9197-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nisbet, E. K., and Zelenski, J. M. (2013). The NR-6: a new brief measure of nature relatedness. Front. Psychol. 4:813. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00813

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nisbet, E. K., Zelenski, J. M., and Grandpierre, Z. (2019). Mindfulness in nature enhances connectedness and mood. Ecopsychology 11, 81–91. doi: 10.1089/ECO.2018.0061

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nord, C. L., and Garfinkel, S. N. (2022). Interoceptive pathways to understand and treat mental health conditions'. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2022.03.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nord, C. L., Lawson, R. P., and Dalgleish, T. (2021). Disrupted Dorsal Mid-Insula Activation During Interoception Across Psychiatric Disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 178, 761–770. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.20091340

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Oldroyd, K., Pasupathi, M., and Wainryb, C. (2019). Social antecedents to the development of interoception: Attachment related processes are associated with interoception. Front. Psychol. 10, 1–13. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00712

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Paterniti, K., Bright, S., and Gringart, E. (2022). The relationship between psychedelic use, mystical experiences, and pro-environmental behaviors. J. Human. Psychol. 1–29. doi: 10.1177/00221678221111024

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Peer, E., Brandimarte, L., Saamat, S., and Acquisti, A. (2017). Beyond the Turk: alternative platforms for crowdsourcing behavioral research. J. Environm. Psychol. 70, 153–163. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2017.01.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Perkins, H. E. (2010). Measuring love and care for nature. J. Environm. Psychol. 30, 455–463. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2010.05.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Preston, J. L., and Shin, F. (2022). Opposing effects of spirituality and religious fundamentalism on environmental attitudes. J. Environm. Psychol. 80:101772. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2022.101772

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Price, C. J., and Hooven, C. (2018). Interoceptive awareness skills for emotion regulation: Theory and approach of mindful awareness in body-oriented therapy (MABT). Front. Psychol. 9:798. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00798

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pritchard, A., Richardson, M., Sheffield, D., and McEwan, K. (2020). The relationship between nature connectedness and Eudaimonic well-being: a meta-analysis. J. Happiness Stud. 21, 1145–1167. doi: 10.1007/s10902-019-00118-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Quadt, L., Critchley, H. D., and Garfinkel, S. N. (2018). The neurobiology of interoception in health and disease. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1428, 112–128. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13915

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Redvers, N., Faerron Guzmán, C. A., and Parkes, M. W. (2023). Towards an educational praxis for planetary health: a call for transformative, inclusive, and integrative approaches for learning and relearning in the Anthropocene. Lancet Planet. Health 7, e77–e85. doi: 10.1016/S2542-5196(22)00332-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, M. (2023). Reconnection: Fixing Our Broken Relationship with Nature. London: Pelagic Publishing.

Google Scholar

Richardson, M., Hamlin, I., Butlier, C. W., Thomas, R., and Hunt, A. (2022). Actively noticing nature (not just time in nature) helps promote nature connectedness. Ecopsychology 14, 8–16. doi: 10.1089/eco.2021.0023

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, M., Maspero, M., Golightiy, D., and Sheffield, D. (2020). “Nature: a new paradigm for well-being and ergonomics,” in New Paradigms in Ergonomics. London: Routledge, 142–155. doi: 10.4324/9781351208956-12

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, M., and McEwan, K. (2018). 30 days wild and the relationships between engagement with nature's beauty, nature connectedness and well-being. Front. Psychol. 9, 1–9. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01500

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, M., Passmore, H.-A., Lumber, R., Thomas, R., and Hunt, A. (2021). Moments, not minutes: the nature-wellbeing relationship. Int. J. Wellbeing 11, 8–33. doi: 10.5502/ijw.v11i1.1267

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Frederick, C. (1997). On energy, personality, and health: subjective vitality as a dynamic reflection of well-being. J. Pers. 65, 529–565. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1997.tb00326.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaller, M. (2007). Is secure attachment the antidote to everything that ails us? Psychol. Inq. 18, 191–193. doi: 10.1080/10478400701512802

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, P. W. (2002). “Inclusion with nature: the psychology of human-nature relations,” in Psychology of Sustainable Development, 61-78. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4615-0995-0_4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schweitzer, R. D., Glab, H., and Brymer, E. (2018). The human-nature experience: a phenomenological-psychoanalytic perspective. Front. Psychol. 9:969. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00969

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaver, P. R., Mikulincer, M., Sahdra, B., and Gross, J. (2016). “Attachment security as a foundation for kindness toward self and others,” in The Oxford Handbook of Hypo-egoic Phenomena, eds. K. W. Brown, and M. R. Leary (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 1–29.

Google Scholar

Shin, S., van Riper, C. J., Stewart, W., and Suski, C. (2022). The value of eudaimonia for understanding relationships among values and pro-environmental behavior. J. Environm. Psychol. 80:101778. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2022.101778

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shrestha, N. (2021). Factor analysis as a tool for survey analysis. Am. J. Appl. Mathem. Statist. 9, 4–11. doi: 10.12691/ajams-9-1-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, J. A., Flowers, P., and Larkin, M. (2022). Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis: Theory, Method and Research. London: SAGE.

Google Scholar

Smith, M. R., and Myers, S. S. (2018). Impact of anthropogenic CO2 emissions on global human nutrition. Nat. Clim. Change 8, 834–839. doi: 10.1038/s41558-018-0253-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, C., Ikei, H., and Miyazaki, Y. (2016). Physiological effects of nature therapy: a review of the research in Japan. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 13:781. doi: 10.3390/ijerph13080781

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sparks, P., Hinds, J., Curnock, S., and Pavey, L. (2014). Connectedness and its consequences: a study of relationships with the natural environment. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 44, 166–174. doi: 10.1111/jasp.12206

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Steffen, W., Richardson, K., Rockström, J., Cornell, S., Fetzer, I., Bennett, E., et al. (2015). Planetary boundaries: guiding human development on a changing planet. Science 347:1259855. doi: 10.1126/science.1259855

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, P. C. (2000). Toward a coherent theory of environmentally significant behavior. J. Social Issues 56, 407–424. doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.00175

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sultana, F. (2022). The unbearable heaviness of climate coloniality. Polit. Geogr. 99:102638. doi: 10.1016/j.polgeo.2022.102638

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tam, K. P. (2013). Concepts and measures related to connection to nature: similarities and differences. J. Environm. Psychol. 34, 64–78. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.01.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tam, K. P. (2015). Mind attribution to nature and proenvironmental behavior. Ecopsychology 7, 87–95. doi: 10.1089/eco.2014.0054

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tam, K. P. (2022). Gratitude to nature: presenting a theory of its conceptualization, measurement, and effects on pro-environmental behavior. J. Environm. Psychol. 79:101754. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2021.101754

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Timko Olson, E. R., Hansen, M. M., and Vermeesch, A. (2020). Mindfulness and shinrin-yoku: Potential for physiological and psychological interventions during uncertain times. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17, 1–13. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17249340

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tucker, L., and Lewis, C. (1973). A reliability coefficient for maximum likelihood factor analysis. Psychometrika 38, 421–422. doi: 10.1007/BF02291170

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Twohig-Bennett, C., and Jones, A. (2018). The health benefits of the great outdoors: a systematic review and meta-analysis of greenspace exposure and health outcomes. Environm. Res. 2018, 628–637. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2018.06.030

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulrich, R. S., Simons, R., Losito, B., Florito, E., Miles, M., and Zelson, M. (1991). Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 11, 201–230. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(05)80184-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wei, M., Russell, D., Mallinckrodt, B., and Vogel, D. (2007). The experiences in close relationship scale (ecr)-short form: reliability, validity, and factor structure. J. Pers. Assess. 88, 187–204. doi: 10.1080/00223890701268041

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitburn, J., Linklater, W., and Abrahamse, W. (2020). Meta-analysis of human connection to nature and proenvironmental behavior. Conserv. Biol. 34, 180–193. doi: 10.1111/cobi.13381

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Whyte, K. (2020). Too late for indigenous climate justice: ecological and relational tipping points. Wiley Interdiscipl. Rev. Climate Change 11, 1–7. doi: 10.1002/wcc.603

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, E. O. (1984). Biophilia. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Wilson, E. O., and Kellert, S. R. (1993). Biophilia Hypothesis. Washington, DC: Island Press.

Google Scholar

Zylstra, M. J. (2014). Exploring Meaningful Nature Experience, Connectedness with Nature and the Revitalization of Transformative Education for Sustainability (Doctoral dissertation). Stellenbosch University.

Google Scholar

Keywords: nature connection, body awareness, relational attachment, interoceptive awareness, climate change, social and ecological justice, connectedness, climate resilience

Citation: Branham L (2024) Embodied earth kinship: interoceptive awareness and relational attachment personal factors predict nature connectedness in a structural model of nature connection. Front. Psychol. 15:1400655. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1400655

Received: 13 March 2024; Accepted: 19 July 2024;
Published: 29 August 2024.

Edited by:

Giuseppe Carrus, Roma Tre University, Italy

Reviewed by:

Marcin Fabjański, University of Silesia in Katowice, Poland
Sam Gandy, Independent Researcher, Market Harborough, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2024 Branham. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lindsay Branham, Llb32@cam.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.