Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 23 July 2024
Sec. Psychology of Aging

Openness to experience, a personality trait that reduces susceptibility to memory age-based stereotype threat

Badia Bouazzaoui
Badiâa Bouazzaoui*Sverine FaySéverine FayEmilie AlibranEmilie AlibranLa MartinezLéa MartinezFlorent PinardFlorent PinardNolwenn KerhardyNolwenn KerhardyTugba OnsekizTugba OnsekizLaurence TaconnatLaurence Taconnat
  • UMR CNRS 7295, Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition et l’Apprentissage, Université de Tours, Université de Poitiers, Tours, France

Introduction: Age-based stereotype threat (ABST), the concern of being judged according to a negative age stereotype may lead to underperformance in the stereotype domain. The present study aims to replicate the negative effect of ABST on episodic memory. Importantly, we further examine openness to experience as a potential buffer of the ABST effect as well as the role that different memory strategies may play in episodic memory performance.

Method: Seventy-five older adults were randomly assigned to the ABST condition or the control condition before taking a word-stem cued recall memory task. They learned word-lists with either a repetition strategy, low resource demanding but less efficient, or a mental imagery strategy, high resource demanding but more efficient. Openness was measured with the Big-5 personality questionnaire.

Results: ABST reduced memory performance and disrupted more the recall of words learned with the imagery strategy. The results also showed that openness predicted recall performance associated with the imagery strategy only in the threatened group.

Conclusion: These results indicated that a high level in openness may disrupt the negative effect of ABST by improving the capacity of threatened people to execute efficient, resource demanding memory strategies. This finding supports the idea that contextual factors as well as individual characteristics such as personality, need to be considered when assessing episodic memory in aging.

1 Introduction

A socially shared stereotype is that getting old means having a failing memory. Stereotypes can lead to the phenomenon of stereotype threat (Steele and Aronson, 1995), defined as the risk of applying a negative stereotype about one’s group to oneself. The negative impact of stereotype on performance has been studied through different fields such as racial, gender and age differences (review, Barber, 2017). There is now an abundant literature on age-based stereotype threat (ABST), especially with regard to memory. In these studies, older adults were subliminally primed with aging stereotype cues (Levy, 1996; Stein et al., 2002; Bouazzaoui et al., 2016, 2020; Lemaire et al., 2018) and/or an instruction manipulation in which the memory component of the task was emphasized (Rahhal et al., 2001; Stein et al., 2002; Hess et al., 2003, 2009; Desrichard and Kopetz, 2005; Hess, 2005; Hess and Hinson, 2006; Mazerolle et al., 2012; Bouazzaoui et al., 2016, 2020; Lemaire et al., 2018; for meta-analyses see Armstrong et al., 2017 and Lamont et al., 2015). A well-replicated finding is that older adults underperformed when they were stereotyped as being less efficient in performing a memory task. Stereotype threat not only affects performance on laboratory tasks, but also clinical assessment for predementia (Mazerolle et al., 2017). Thus, ABST may lead to overestimating the magnitude of the age-related effect on memory.

Studies examining the processes underlying the stereotype threat effect have identified several factors as determinants or mediators, supporting a multifactorial hypothesis (see Barber, 2017). Among these factors, a possible key variable is the ability to implement controlled processes when faced with stereotype threat. According to Schmader et al. (2008), these processes are difficult to implement when cognitive resources are depleted by coping with intrusive thoughts and the anxiety they produce. Mazerolle et al. (2012) confirmed that stereotype threat interferes with the working memory of older adults, reducing controlled processes and increasing automatic processes. It is now well-known that controlled processes support the implementation of efficient memory strategies (Bryan et al., 1999; Taconnat et al., 2006, 2007; Bouazzaoui et al., 2010; Burger et al., 2017; and see Lemaire, 2010 for a review). Therefore, one might reasonably expect that by reducing the ability to implement controlled processes, ABST would also affect the use of efficient strategies.

Efficient strategies are needed to optimize encoding and retrieval processes. Because older adults generally have reduced cognitive resources, they fail to initiate efficient strategies adopted spontaneously by younger adults, for example, increasing study time to cope with task difficulty (Murphy et al., 1981; Souchay and Isingrini, 2004; Froger et al., 2012) or using an imagery strategy (Froger et al., 2012). Hess et al. (2003) found that the effect of ABST on memory performance was mediated by strategy use, participants making less use of semantic clustering as a recall strategy when under threat. More recently, Lemaire et al. (2018) examined the ABST effect on the ability to select and execute two memory strategies, one low resource demanding (repetition), the other high resource demanding but more efficient (imagery) as it allows deeper encoding and leads to better memory performance (Craik and Lockhart, 1972; Paivio and Csapo, 1973; Froger et al., 2012; Hertzog et al., 2012; Burger et al., 2017). Lemaire et al. (2018) showed that ABST affected mainly the selection and execution of the most resource demanding strategy (i.e., imagery). Examining the level of processing at encoding through imagery and repetition strategies sheds light on the underlying mechanisms of the effect of ABST on episodic memory. The present study aimed to extend this finding with a new direction that was to investigate the role of personality in susceptibility to ABST. The variability observed in older adults’ memory performance might be explained in part by personality and although several personality traits may be associated with cognitive performance, the main one seems to be openness to experience (Simon et al., 2020), defined as the tendency to be intellectually curious, creative, unconventional and imaginative. In this study we were particularly interested in this personality trait because it appears to support both episodic memory performance and, more specifically, the implementation of strategies that are altered by ABST. The present study was then designed to explore a new and interesting topic, namely how open people might deal with ABST and whether openness might provide some protection against the negative effects of ABST on the execution of effortful encoding strategies.

Openness is positively correlated with engagement in a variety of cognitively stimulating activities (Kraaykamp and van Eijck, 2005) and also to high cognitive performance including episodic memory (e.g., Soubelet and Salthouse, 2010; Simon et al., 2020; Karsazi et al., 2021). Studies examining the role of openness in older adults have also demonstrated a beneficial effect of openness on episodic memory (Martinez et al., 2021; Taconnat et al., 2022a). In addition to supporting memory functioning, openness also appears to be a factor of cognitive reserve protecting against the effects of age (Franchow et al., 2013; Luchetti et al., 2016; Jackson et al., 2020; Simon et al., 2020; Karsazi et al., 2021). Some studies have also hypothesized that the beneficial effect of openness on performance may depend on the experimental context. Using the generation effect paradigm (Martinez et al., 2021; Taconnat et al., 2022a) or the prior task success paradigm (Taconnat et al., 2022a) to vary the level of difficulty of the task in terms of the cognitive resources required, the authors found that openness helps to improve memory performance mainly when the task is resource-dependent. This beneficial effect can be explained by the fact that openness leads to cognitive stimulation, thereby facilitating the acquisition of crystallized knowledge and fluid intelligence, which are therefore less affected by age (Salthouse et al., 2002; Moutafi et al., 2006; Jopp and Hertzog, 2007; Zimprich et al., 2009), and are essential resources for memory functioning (Bouazzaoui et al., 2013, 2014; Guerrero-Sastoque et al., 2020). Considering the relationship between openness and the implementation of memory strategies, Talpain and Soubelet (2020) observed that openness was positively associated with the use of strategies supporting memory performance, and that strategy use mediated the positive effect of openness on episodic memory functioning. This mediation effect was more significant when the strategy involved imagery rather than repetition, indicating that openness mainly affects the most resource-dependent memory strategy.

Thus, in contrast to ABST, openness to experience positively supports memory performance mainly in effortful cognitive tasks, due in part to the exposure to cognitive-stimulating life experiences which increase the ability to implement efficient strategies. To examine the interaction between ABST and openness in older adults’ episodic memory performance, the present study took a dual approach, extending findings that ABST has a negative impact on the use of memory strategies (Hess et al., 2003; Lemaire et al., 2018), and those that observed a beneficial effect of openness on memory performance and strategic abilities (Talpain and Soubelet, 2020; Martinez et al., 2021; Taconnat et al., 2022a). Older adults were randomly assigned to the ABST condition or the control condition before carrying out a word-stem cued-recall memory task. Two strategies were proposed at encoding: repetition for half of the words and mental imagery for the other half. Openness was measured using the Big Five Inventory (John et al., 1991; Plaisant et al., 2010). In line with Lemaire et al. (2018), given that stereotype threat consumes attentional resources and that the imagery strategy is highly resource demanding, we first expected that ABST would reduce memory performance, and more so in the imagery condition than in the repetition condition. Second, we expected stereotype threat to interact with strategy and openness. If stereotype endorsement depends on personality, we can expect that individuals with higher levels of openness would be less susceptible to stereotype threat than those who are less open to experience. Furthermore, if openness supports memory performance in the more resource-dependent situations, it would be mainly related to memory performance in the threatened group by counteracting the negative effect of stereotype threat and hence improving the ability to execute more effortful but also more efficient strategies.

2 Method

2.1 Participants

The size of the target sample was determined using an a priori power analysis using R software with the pwr.f2.test function (R Core Team, 2021). In line with Lamont et al. (2015) and previous studies using a similar experimental design, we used a d of 0.52. Our mixed design could achieve 80% power with a total of 59 participants. We decided to exceed this criterion and recruit 80 participants, 40 per group to anticipate the risk of having to exclude participants. Finally, the preliminary analyses have led to the exclusion of 5 participants, one in the control condition and four in the threatened condition because their MMSE score was less than 27 (see below).

The sample consisted of 75 native French speakers aged between 60 and 78, recruited from the general community by advertising and word of mouth. They were randomly assigned to either the control (n = 39) or the threatened condition (n = 36). The characteristics of participants in each group are summarized in Table 1. The groups did not differ significantly in age, educational level, proportion of females, vocabulary (Mill Hill vocabulary test, Deltour, 1993) or in openness to experience (John et al., 1991; Plaisant et al., 2010). None of the participants had a history of brain injury, cardiovascular disease, psychiatric disease or alcoholism, and all had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. None of them were taking medication known to affect the central nervous system. All the participants scored below the cut-off of 11 on the two subscales of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS, Zigmond and Snaith, 1983), with no significant difference between groups. They scored at or above the cut-off of 27 on the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE, Folstein et al., 1975), suggesting that they were not suffering from cognitive impairment. The study was approved by the local ethics committee of the University of Tours, and all participants were volunteers and signed consent forms.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of participants per group (means and standard deviations).

2.2 Material and procedure

During an initial meeting with the experimenter, participants completed the screening measures (Mill Hill vocabulary test, HADS, MMSE). They also completed the French version of the Big Five Inventory (John et al., 1991; Plaisant et al., 2010). This self-report questionnaire rates participants on five personality traits (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.74): Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Neuroticism and Openness. For the purposes of the present study, we analyzed only the scores for “Openness to experience,” which has 10 items rated on a five-point Likert scale, from “Strongly disapprove” to “Strongly approve.” A high score indicates a high level of openness to experience.

In the second session, as previous studies (Bouazzaoui et al., 2016; Lemaire et al., 2018). Participants in the threatened group were primed with a questionnaire to activate the aging stereotype. The material, inspired by previous studies (Levy, 1996; Stein et al., 2002), consisted of 14 adjectives describing older people, 7 positive and 7 negative. Participants had to indicate orally the degree to which each adjective was relevant to an older adult, from “not at all relevant” to “very relevant.” Next, all the participants performed the episodic memory task. The stereotype threat was manipulated through the information given to the participants (Bouazzaoui et al., 2016, 2020; Lemaire et al., 2018). For the threatened group, the memory component of the task was emphasized as follows: “You are participating in a study on the effects of aging on memory. More specifically, we want to know whether, as shown by other studies in psychology, memory performance tends to decline with age. We will compare your memory performance to that of a group of young adults.” For the control group, the instructions deemphasized the memory component of the test: “You are participating in a study on vocabulary knowledge. This is why you are now going to carry out tasks based on vocabulary understanding.”

Participants performed a word-stem cued-recall task comprising six study-test blocks (Angel et al., 2010; Guerrero et al., 2022). The material consisted of 270 words divided into six lists (one list per block) of 45 words, matched for mean number of letters and mean frequency of occurrence.

In the encoding phase (see Figure 1) 30 items were presented one by one in uppercase for 500 msec. Participants were told that they would see words on the screen that they would have to learn. For half the blocks, they were told they could repeat the words as many times as they liked. For the other half, they were invited to create a mental image of the words. The order of the strategies was counterbalanced across blocks; half the participants started with the imagery strategy and the other half started with the repetition strategy. We chose to use three blocks per condition to obtain a large number of events per strategy condition and per group and therefore a sensitive memory measure.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Visual depiction of the sequence of events in the encoding and cued-recall memory phases.

Each cued-recall phase (see Figure 1) comprised 45 word-stems displayed one by one for 500 msec, 30 from the study list and 15 not presented during the encoding phase. These word stems consist of the three first letters of a word and were all different (i.e., the words chosen to make the word lists all begin with different three-letter stems). Participants were instructed to complete each stem with a studied word, or failing that, with another suitable word, and then indicating whether the completion corresponded to a studied (old) or an unstudied (new) word. Participants had 4.5 s to answer orally.

At the beginning of each of the six experimental blocks, the instructions manipulating the stereotype threat were repeated in order to maintain the effect. The six word lists were counterbalanced across strategy conditions and across participants, the sublist used for distractors was counterbalanced across participants, and the words in each list were presented in random order. Before starting the test phase, participants first completed a familiarization phase to understand the instructions and the procedure. During this phase, they encoded a list of 6 words using the repetition strategy followed by a cued-recall task, and then they also encoded a list of 6 words using the imagery strategy followed by a cued-recall task. These two lists were also counterbalanced in order of presentation.

2.3 Data processing

Three memory scores were calculated per strategy condition: (1) a hit score (number of stems completed by a studied word and correctly judged as old), (2) a false alarm score (number of stems completed by an unstudied word and falsely judged as old), (3) a corrected hit rate corresponding to the hit score minus the false alarm score divided by the total number of targets. It should be noted that, in addition to counterbalancing the order of the strategies across blocks at the encoding stage, we also ensured that there were no effects of the order of the strategies or interactions with the variables of interest, and that it therefore made sense to group the three imagery strategy blocks on the one hand and the three repetition strategy blocks on the other (i.e., summing the memory scores for the three lists for each strategy).

To test our hypotheses, we conducted a mixed measures ANOVA with two factors: condition (Control vs. ABST, between) and strategy use (repetition vs. imagery, within). Openness was used as a linear covariate to test the interaction with condition and strategy use. In this analysis, we focused first on the main effect of condition and second on the higher order interaction involving condition. The dependent variable was the corrected hit rates.

3 Results

The results of the ANOVA analysis (see Table 2) showed that the condition effect was significant when entered alone in the model, indicating that memory performance was significantly higher in the control group than in the threatened group. When openness was included in the model, the analyses showed that the effect of condition was no longer significant, but that it interacted with strategy, with the effect of condition being more important in the imagery condition than in the repetition condition, and with strategy and openness in a 3-way interaction. Regression analyses were performed to disentangle this high-level interaction (see Figure 2). These showed that participants in the ABST condition who scored higher on openness showed better memory performance when using the imagery strategy (β = 0.61, p = 0.000) compared to the repetition strategy (β = 0.12, p = 0.48). In the control condition, openness was not related to memory performance regardless of the strategy used (imagery: β = 0.25, p = 0.11; repetition: β = 0.18, p = 0.25).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results of ANOVA.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Regression slopes showing the relationship between openness scores and corrected hit rate as a function of the strategy and the condition.

4 Discussion

The aim of this research was to improve our understanding of the effect of ABST on episodic memory by exploring whether it affects the implementation of memory strategies at encoding. We also investigated the potential protective role of openness, hypothesizing that a high level of this personality trait would reduce the stereotype threat effect and help the implementation of a more resource-dependent memory strategy.

The results confirm previous findings that older participants recall fewer words when under stereotype threat (Lamont et al., 2015; Armstrong et al., 2017; Barber, 2017). As reported in the literature, imagery, by generating deep encoding, leads to better recall than repetition (Craik and Lockhart, 1972; Paivio and Csapo, 1973; Froger et al., 2012; Hertzog et al., 2012; Burger et al., 2017). We observed that threat mainly affected memory performance in the imagery condition, partly confirming the findings of Lemaire et al. (2018) who found a significant effect of ABST only on words encoded with imagery, with no effect on words encoded with the repetition, whereas we found an effect on both strategies, but to a greater extent for imagery. This discrepancy could be explained by differences in the experimental design, as we used repeated measures for strategy execution, whereas Lemaire et al. (2018) used independent groups. Moreover, we used several experimental blocks to obtain a number of trials for each strategy, which may have allowed an effect to emerge statistically even on the repetition strategy. In any case, ABST mainly effects the ability to implement the strategy that is most resource-demanding but also the most effective. Consequently, participants under threat did not benefit from the effect of imagery strategy, in contrast to those in the control group whose memory performance improved through use of the imagery strategy. When under threat, participants have to manage intrusive thoughts and stress; consequently, their cognitive resources are less available to perform a task, especially when this requires high cognitive resources, notably for older adults whose cognitive resources are reduced due to aging (Rabinowitz et al., 1982).

As expected, openness was associated with memory performance, the most open-minded individuals achieving the best performance. This result is in accordance with previous research investigating the role of openness in episodic memory performance (Soubelet and Salthouse, 2010; Simon et al., 2020; Karsazi et al., 2021; Martinez et al., 2021; Taconnat et al., 2022a). The beneficial effect of openness can be explained by the fact that individuals with high levels of openness tend to engage in a wide range of activities, providing strong cognitive stimulation that promotes the development of crystallized and fluid intelligence (Chamorro-Premuzic and Furnham, 2004; Soubelet and Salthouse, 2010; von Stumm et al., 2011); both these types of intelligence support memory functioning in aging (Bouazzaoui et al., 2013, 2014; Taconnat et al., 2022b). This result is also in accordance with recent study showing that high level of openness is associated with the preservation of the brain’s memory network and then with episodic memory performance and may be considered as a protective factor against aging (Stolz et al., 2023). As expected, we found that openness impacted differently strategies implementation, and that high levels of openness were positively associated with the recall of words encoded with an imagery strategy, but not with those encoded with a repetition strategy. Examining the role of openness on strategy use, Talpain and Soubelet (2020) found a positive association with both strategies, but that people with high levels of openness used imagery more than repetition. They also found that the relationship between openness and memory performance was mediated more by imagery than by repetition. Two major conclusions can be drawn from these findings. First, by facilitating efficient strategy use, openness provides executive support for memory functioning. This is in line with previous findings of a relationship between openness and executive functioning (DeYoung et al., 2005), and of the role of executive control in supporting episodic memory functioning and strategy use (Bouazzaoui et al., 2010, 2013, 2014; Bryan et al., 1999; Taconnat et al., 2006, 2007, 2022b, and see Lemaire, 2010 for a review). Secondly, the observation that openness was correlated only with the recall of words learned with the imagery strategy confirms previous studies and supports our hypothesis that openness is involved in the most effortful memory conditions (Talpain and Soubelet, 2020; Martinez et al., 2021; Taconnat et al., 2022a). By stimulating cognitive resources, openness acts as a cognitive reserve factor supporting memory performance, particularly in difficult tasks, by facilitating the use of a high resource-demanding strategy (Franchow et al., 2013; Luchetti et al., 2016; Jackson et al., 2020; Simon et al., 2020; Karsazi et al., 2021). The main and novel result of this study is that openness was only related to the efficiency of the imagery strategy in the ABST group. Being stereotyped as old makes older adults vulnerable, depleting their resources. However, high openness seems to be a protective factor, as it reduces the negative effects of ABST and helps to implement the more resource-dependent memory strategy. Our stereotype threat condition can thus be likened to the reading condition in the generation effect paradigm (Slamecka and Graf, 1978) and not performing a prior task in the prior-task-success paradigm (Taconnat et al., 2022a), which were considered as low environmental support conditions (Craik, 1986; Taconnat and Isingrini, 2004). In fact, the threat situation can also be considered as an experimental condition, which is very costly in terms of processing resources and requires more support to cope with the difficulty. Our results confirm that openness enhances memory performance and efficient strategy use in resource dependent conditions, such as when confronted by stereotype threat.

Future research could address the limitations of this study. The effect of stereotype threat on memory performance led us to believe that participants were stereotype threatened by the manipulation we used. However, it would be interesting to check whether participants actually felt threatened by questioning them at the end of the experiment, as other studies have done (Chasteen et al., 2005). Also, openness to experience is not the only personality trait associated with memory functioning. Taking a holistic view of personality, it would be interesting to test the role of other dimensions (and their possible interactions) in the relationship between memory, strategies and stereotype threat. Another research perspective would be to test our hypothesis using a memory task that is even more demanding task than cued recall. Indeed, several studies have used free recall, a task that requires important cognitive resources. The choice of task may have minimized the effects of stereotype threat, and also the relationship between strategies implementation and openness, even in the control group. Future research could also test the relationship between openness and strategy selection. Our study shows that openness affects the execution of memory strategies, but we did not test whether this is the case for strategy selection, which is also known to be affected by age-related stereotype threat. It would therefore be interesting to see whether openness plays a moderating role in the effect of stereotype threat on strategy selection. Finally, the literature on the moderators of the threat effect is now extensive and several hypotheses have emerged in recent years. In particular, there are psychosocial factors that may interact with personality, such as motivation and task commitment. Future studies could therefore investigate these relationships.

In sum, older adults under threat have difficulty using efficient strategies, resulting in impaired episodic memory performance. However, our results show that individual characteristics, such as a high level of openness, can counteract the negative effect of ABST by facilitating the implementation of efficient but resource-demanding strategies. Then, personality, and more specifically openness to experience, seems to be a variable to consider when assessing older people’s episodic memory, especially in a context that generates a sense of threat and stress. Further research is needed to replicate these results and also to clarify certain discrepancies observed in different studies, for example whether ABST affects the use of both repetition and imagery strategies, as found in our study, or only imagery (Lemaire et al., 2018), and whether openness is related to memory performance with both strategies (Talpain and Soubelet, 2020) or only with the imagery strategy (our study). These discrepancies could be due to differences in experimental design.

Data availability statement

The data are available on the Open Science Framework depository here: https://osf.io/q6tjv/?view_only=6fa491a65d984b76a8261e22c6bae0df.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Comité d’Ethique pour la Recherche, Tours-Poitiers. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

BB: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Validation, Supervision, Software, Methodology, Investigation, Formal analysis, Data curation, Conceptualization. SF: Writing – review & editing, Validation, Methodology, Conceptualization. EA: Writing – review & editing, Investigation. LM: Writing – review & editing, Investigation. FP: Writing – review & editing, Methodology, Investigation. NK: Writing – review & editing, Investigation. TO: Writing – review & editing, Investigation. LT: Writing – review & editing, Validation, Project administration, Methodology, Funding acquisition, Conceptualization.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This study was funded by the “Agence Française de la Recherche,” grant ANR-17 CE28–0003-02.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Jordan Mille for his help in processing the data in R.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Angel, L., Fay, S., Bouazzaoui, B., and Isingrini, M. (2010). More data-driven processing at retrieval reduces age-related memory deficits. Can. J. Psychol. 64, 117–123. doi: 10.1037/a0018570

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Armstrong, B., Gallant, S. N., Li, L., Patel, K., and Wong, B. I. (2017). Stereotype threat effects on older adults’ episodic and working memory: a meta-analysis. Gerontologist 57, S193–S205. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnx056

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barber, S. J. (2017). An examination of age-based stereotype threat about cognitive decline: implications for stereotype-threat research and theory development. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12, 62–90. doi: 10.1177/1745691616656345

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouazzaoui, B., Angel, L., Fay, S., Taconnat, L., Froger, C., and Isingrini, M. (2014). Does the greater involvement of executive control in memory with age act as a compensatory mechanism? Can. J. Psychol. 68, 59–66. doi: 10.1037/cep0000005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouazzaoui, B., Fay, S., Guerrero-Sastoque, L., Semaine, M., Isingrini, M., and Taconnat, L. (2020). Memory age-based stereotype threat: role of locus of control and anxiety. Exp. Aging Res. 46, 39–51. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2019.1693009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouazzaoui, B., Fay, S., Taconnat, L., Angel, L., Vanneste, S., and Isingrini, M. (2013). Differential involvement of executive functioning and crystallized knowledge in episodic memory performance in young and older adults. Can. J. Psychol. 67, 100–107. doi: 10.1037/a0028517

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouazzaoui, B., Follenfant, A., Ric, F., Fay, S., Croizet, J. C., Atzeni, T., et al. (2016). Aging-related stereotypes in memory: when the beliefs come true. Memory 24, 659–668. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2015.1040802

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouazzaoui, B., Isingrini, M., Fay, S., Angel, L., Vanneste, S., Clarys, D., et al. (2010). Aging, executive functioning and self-reported memory strategy use. Acta Psychol. 135, 59–66. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.05.007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bryan, J., Luszcz, M. A., and Pointer, S. (1999). Executive function and processing resources as predictors of adult age differences in the implementation of encoding strategies. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 6, 273–287. doi: 10.1076/1382-5585(199912)06:04;1-B;FT273

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Burger, L., Uittenhove, K., Lemaire, P., and Taconnat, L. (2017). Strategy difficulty effects in young and older adults' episodic memory are modulated by inter-stimulus interval and executive control processes. Acta Psychol. 175:688. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2017.02.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chamorro-Premuzic, T., and Furnham, A. (2004). A possible model for understanding the personality-intelligence interface. Br. J. Psychol. 95, 249–264. doi: 10.1348/000712604773952458

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chasteen, A. L., Bhattacharyya, S., Horhota, M., Tam, R., and Hasher, L. (2005). How feelings of stereotype threat influence older Adults' memory performance. Exp. Aging Res. 31, 235–260. doi: 10.1080/03610730590948177

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Craik, F. I. M. (1986). “A functional account of age differences in memory” in Human memory and cognitive capabilities: Mechanisms and performances. eds. F. Klix and H. Hagendorf (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 409–422.

Google Scholar

Craik, F. I. M., and Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: a framework for memory research. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 11, 671–684. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(72)80001-X

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Deltour, J. J. (1993). Echelle de vocabulaire de Mill Hill de JC Raven. Adaptation française et normes comparées du Mill Hill et du Standard Progressive Matrice (PM 38). Braine-le-Château: L'application des techniques modernes.

Google Scholar

Desrichard, O., and Kopetz, C. (2005). A threat in the elder: the impact of task instructions, self-efficacy and performance expectations on memory performance in the elderly. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 35, 537–552. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.249

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

DeYoung, C. G., Peterson, J. B., and Higgins, D. M. (2005). Sources of openness/intellect: cognitive and neuropsychological correlates of the fifth factor of personality. J. Pers. 73, 825–858. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00330.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-mental state: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Franchow, E. I., Suchy, Y., Thorgusen, S. R., and Williams, P. (2013). More than education openness to experience contributes to cognitive reserve in older adulthood. J. Aging Res. 1, 10–4172. doi: 10.4172/2329-8847.1000109y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Froger, C., Bouazzaoui, B., Isingrini, M., and Taconnat, L. (2012). Study time allocation deficit of older adults: the role of environmental support at encoding? Psychol. Aging 27, 577–588. doi: 10.1037/a0026358

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Guerrero, L., Bouazzaoui, B., Alibran, É., Isingrini, M., and Angel, L. (2022). Is episodic memory change in aging associated with level of control processes and rate of control change? A 4-year longitudinal study. Ann. Psychol. 122, 567–587. doi: 10.3917/anpsy1.224.0567

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Guerrero-Sastoque, L., Bouazzaoui, B., Angel, L., Fay, S., Gombart, S., and Isingrini, M. (2020). Differential protective role of control and representation against age-related memory decline. Can. J. Psychol. 74, 44–55. doi: 10.1037/cep0000191

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hertzog, C., Price, J., and Dunlosky, J. (2012). Age differences in the effects of experimenter-instructed versus self-generated strategy use. Exp. Aging Res. 38, 42–62. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2012.637005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, T. M. (2005). Memory and aging in context. Psychol. Bull. 131, 383–406. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.3.383

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, T. M., Auman, C., Colcombe, S. J., and Rahhal, T. A. (2003). The impact of stereotype threat on age differences in memory performance. J. Gerontol. Psychol. Sci. 58, P3–P11. doi: 10.1093/geronb/58.1.P3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, T. M., and Hinson, J. T. (2006). Age-related variation in the influences of aging stereotypes on memory in adulthood. Psychol. Aging 21, 621–625. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.3.621

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, T. M., Hinson, J. T., and Hodges, E. A. (2009). Moderators of and mechanisms underlying stereotype threat effects on older adults’memory performance. Exp. Aging Res. 35, 153–177. doi: 10.1080/03610730802716413

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, J. J., Hill, P. L., Payne, B. R., Parisi, J. M., and Stine-Morrow, E. A. (2020). Linking openness to cognitive ability in older adulthood: the role of activity diversity. Aging Ment. Health 24, 1079–1087. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2019.1655705

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

John, O. P., Donahue, E. M., and Kentle, R. L. (1991). The big five inventory-versions 4a and 54. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Institute of Personality and Social Research, University of California.

Google Scholar

Jopp, D., and Hertzog, C. (2007). Activities, self-referent memory beliefs, and cognitive performance: evidence for direct and mediated relations. Psychol. Aging 22, 811–825. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.22.4.811

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Karsazi, H., Rezapour, T., Kormi-Nouri, R., Mottaghi, A., Abdekhodaie, E., and Hatami, J. (2021). The moderating effect of neuroticism and openness in the relationship between age and memory: implications for cognitive reserve. Pers. Individ. Differ. 176:110773. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.110773

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kraaykamp, G., and van Eijck, K. (2005). Personality, media preferences, and cultural participation. Pers. Individ. Differ. 38, 1675–1688. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2004.11.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamont, R. A., Swift, H. J., and Abrams, D. (2015). A review and meta-analysis of age-based stereotype threat: negative stereotypes, not facts, do the damage. Psychol. Aging 30, 180–193. doi: 10.1037/a0038586

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemaire, P. (2010). Variations stratégiques et vieillissement. Rev. Neuropsychol. 2, 124–132. doi: 10.3917/rne.022.0124

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemaire, P., Brun, F., and Regner, I. (2018). Negative aging stereotypes disrupt both the selection and execution of strategies in older adults. Gerontology 64, 373–381. doi: 10.1159/000486756

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Levy, B. (1996). Improving memory in old age through implicit selfstereotyping. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 71, 1092–1107. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.71.6.1092

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Luchetti, M., Terracciano, A., Stephan, Y., and Sutin, A. R. (2016). Personality and cognitive decline in older adults: data from a longitudinal sample and Meta-analysis. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 71, 591–601. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbu184

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martinez, L., Fay, S., Onsekiz, T., Bouazzaoui, B., and Taconnat, L. (2021). Episodic memory and aging: the role of cognitive reserve and cognitive resources according to task difficulty. Geriatr. Psychol. Neuropsychiatr. Vieil. 19, 219–228. doi: 10.1684/pnv.2021.0999

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazerolle, M., Régner, I., Barber, S. J., Paccalin, M., Miazola, A. C., Huguet, P., et al. (2017). Negative aging stereotypes impair performance on brief cognitive tests used to screen for Predementia. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 72, gbw083–gbw936. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbw083

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazerolle, M., Régner, I., Morisset, P., Rigalleau, F., and Huguet, P. (2012). Stereotype threat strengthens automatic recall and undermines controlled processes in older adults. Psychol. Sci. 23, 723–727. doi: 10.1177/0956797612437607

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moutafi, J., Furnham, A., and Crump, J. (2006). What facets of openness and conscientiousness predict fluid intelligence score? Learn. Individ. Differ. 16, 31–42. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2005.06.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, M. D., Sanders, R. E., Gabriesheski, A. S., and Schmitt, F. A. (1981). Metamemory in the aged. J. Gerontol. 36, 185–193. doi: 10.1093/geronj/36.2.185

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Paivio, A., and Csapo, K. (1973). Picture superiority in free recall: imagery or dual coding? Cogn. Psychol. 5, 176–206. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(73)90032-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Plaisant, O., Courtois, R., Réveillère, C., Mendelsohn, G. A., and John, O. P. (2010). Validation par analyse factorielle du big five inventory français (BFIFr). Analyse convergente avec le NEOPIR [validation by factorial analysis of the French big five inventory (BFIFr). Convergent analysis with NEOPIR]. Ann. Med. Psychol. 168, 97–106. doi: 10.1016/j.amp.2009.09.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2021). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Google Scholar

Rabinowitz, J. C., Craik, F. I., and Ackerman, B. P. (1982). A processing resource account of age differences in recall. Can. J. Psychol. 36, 325–344. doi: 10.1037/h0080643

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rahhal, T. A., Hasher, L., and Colcombe, S. J. (2001). Instructional manipulations and age differences in memory: now you see them, now you don’t. Psychol. Aging 16, 697–706. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.16.4.697

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A., Berish, D. E., and Miles, J. D. (2002). The role of cognitive stimulation on the relations between age and cognitive functioning. Psychol. Aging 17, 548–557. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.4.548

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmader, T., Johns, M., and Forbes, C. (2008). An integrated process model of stereotype threat effects on performance. Psychol. Rev. 115, 336–356. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.336

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Simon, S. S., Lee, S., and Stern, Y. (2020). Personality-cognition associations across the adult lifespan and potential moderators: results from two cohorts. J. Pers. 88, 1025–1039. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12548

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Slamecka, N. J., and Graf, P. (1978). The generation effect: delineation of a phenomenon. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Learn. Mem. 4, 592–604. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.4.6.592

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Soubelet, A., and Salthouse, T. A. (2010). The role of activity engagement in the relations between openness/intellect and cognition. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 49, 896–901. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.07.026

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Souchay, C., and Isingrini, M. (2004). Age-related differences in metacognitive control: role of executive functioning. Brain Cogn. 56, 89–99. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.06.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Steele, C. M., and Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 69, 797–811. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, R., Blanchard-Fields, F., and Hertzog, C. (2002). The effects of age-stereotype priming on the memory performance of older adults. Exp. Aging Res. 28, 169–181. doi: 10.1080/03610730252800184

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stolz, C., Bulla, A., Soch, J., Schott, B. H., and Richter, A. (2023). Openness to experience is associated with neural and performance measures of memory in older adults. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 18:nsad041. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsad041

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Taconnat, L., Baudouin, A., Fay, S., Clarys, D., Vanneste, S., Tournelle, L., et al. (2006). Aging and implementation of encoding strategies in the generation of rhymes: the role of executive functions. Neuropsychology 20, 658–665. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.20.6.658

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Taconnat, L., Bouazzaoui, B., Bouquet, C., Larigauderie, P., Witt, A., and Blaye, A. (2022b). Cognitive mechanisms underlying free recall in episodic memory performance across the lifespan: testing the control/representation model. Psychol. Res. 87, 1370–1388. doi: 10.1007/s00426-022-01736-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Taconnat, L., Clarys, D., Vanneste, S., Bouazzaoui, B., and Isingrini, M. (2007). Aging and strategic retrieval in memory: the role of executive functions. Brain Cogn. 64, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2006.09.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Taconnat, L., and Isingrini, M. (2004). Cognitive operations in the generation effect on a recall test: role of aging and divided attention. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 30, 827–837. doi: 10.1037/02787393.30.4.827

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Taconnat, L., Pinard, F., Vanneste, S., Bouazzaoui, B., Fay, S., Martinez, L., et al. (2022a). Personality traits affect older adults’ memory differently depending on the environmental support provided at encoding. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 191:111572. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2022.111572

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Talpain, E., and Soubelet, A. (2020). Strategy use mediates the relation between openness to experience and episodic memory in younger and older adults. Psychol. Rep. 125, 358–374. doi: 10.1177/0033294120981938

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

von Stumm, S., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., and Ackerman, P. L. (2011). Re-visiting intelligence-personality associations: vindicating intellectual investment. In T. Chamorro-Premuzic, S. Stummvon, and A. Furnham (Eds.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of individual differences. Oxford: Wiley Blackwell. 217–241).

Google Scholar

Zigmond, A. S., and Snaith, R. P. (1983). The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 67, 361–370. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983.tb09716.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimprich, D., Allemand, M., and Dellenbach, M. (2009). Openness to experience, fluid intelligence, and crystallized intelligence in middle-aged and old adults. J. Res. Pers. 43, 444–454. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2009.01.018

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: stereotype threat, aging, episodic memory, strategy, openness trait personality

Citation: Bouazzaoui B, Fay S, Alibran E, Martinez L, Pinard F, Kerhardy N, Onsekiz T and Taconnat L (2024) Openness to experience, a personality trait that reduces susceptibility to memory age-based stereotype threat. Front. Psychol. 15:1399131. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1399131

Received: 11 March 2024; Accepted: 08 July 2024;
Published: 23 July 2024.

Edited by:

Magdalena Leszko, University of Szczecin, Poland

Reviewed by:

Stephen Cheong Yu Chan, Saint Francis University, Hong Kong SAR, China
Maria Clara P. De Paula Couto, Friedrich Schiller University Jena, Germany

Copyright © 2024 Bouazzaoui, Fay, Alibran, Martinez, Pinard, Kerhardy, Onsekiz and Taconnat. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Badiâa Bouazzaoui, bouazzaoui@univ-tours.fr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.