Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 15 July 2024
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Students' Social and Emotional Skills in Educational Settings View all 6 articles

Declines in social–emotional skills in college students during the COVID-19 pandemic

  • 1Department of Psychological Science, University of Vermont, Burlington, VT, United States
  • 2Department of Psychology, Middlebury College, Middlebury, VT, United States
  • 3Vermont Psychological Services, Burlington, VT, United States

Introduction: The present study investigated whether social–emotional skills in first year college students differed before and after the coronavirus disease (COVID-19) lockdowns.

Methods: Participants (N = 1,685) consisted of first year college students (mean age 18.53 years) selected from a broader cohort enrolled in a longitudinal study on college mental health at liberal arts colleges in the United States. In a cohort-sequential design, participants completed an online survey assessing social–emotional skills in January of 2018, 2019, 2020, and 2022. Using analysis of covariance, we examined mean differences in social–emotional skills between students who were first years before (January 2018–2020) and after the lockdowns (January 2022), controlling for sociodemographic variables.

Results: The post-lockdown group scored significantly lower on emotional control and expressivity and marginally higher on social sensitivity compared to the pre-lockdown group. No group differences in social/emotional expressivity or social control were detected.

Discussion: These findings indicate that the COVID-19 lockdowns impaired some, but not all, social–emotional skills in first year college students. Addressing social–emotional skills in college may help to reduce the COVID-19 mental health burden.

1 Introduction

Many schools, from K-12 to college, shifted to remote instruction in March of 2020 in response to the rapid escalation of the coronavirus disease (COVID-19) pandemic. For most students, this shift was a dramatic change in their educational and social experiences. In the United States, the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) strongly recommended social distancing, the avoidance of crowded indoor spaces, and the use of a mask, all of which added both logistical and subjective barriers to social contact (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2020). Regarding educational outcomes, the shift to remote instruction at home has been shown to have a negative and long-lasting impact on higher education students’ critical thinking skills (Lv et al., 2022). In addition to no longer attending classes in person, many of the everyday social interactions students had with friends, peers, and teachers were reduced or eliminated. This led to less in-person contact with peers and an increased sense of emotional distance from social contacts (Dotson et al., 2022). Making up for the loss of in-person contact, use of digital media for socializing increased (Pandya and Lodha, 2021). Despite increasingly connecting with peers via social media, rates of loneliness significantly increased for college students during the COVID-19 pandemic (Labrague et al., 2021). Like most others, students experienced the COVID-19 lockdowns as physically and psychologically stressful (Birmingham et al., 2021; Camacho-Zuñiga et al., 2021).

For late high school and early college students, this change in socialization came at a critical time. Emerging adulthood is a period of important exploration (Arnett, 2014), identity development (Chickering and Reisser, 1993), and the growth of mature interpersonal relationships (Martin, 2000; Foubert and Urbanski, 2006). At the same time, development of key social–emotional skills continues (Wood et al., 2018; Bos et al., 2020). The ongoing development of the social and emotional skills of reading emotion in others and regulating one’s own social and emotional states to match social and contextual demands may have been hindered by the context shift from face-to-face to screen-mediated interactions (Isohätälä et al., 2021). Considering the importance of schools as a context for social–emotional development (Roeser et al., 2000; Eccles and Roeser, 2011), this impact may be both substantial and long-lasting (Center for Reinventing Public Education, 2021). In addition to direct social effects, the impaired development of social–emotional skills may have a deleterious effect on students’ mental health (Payton et al., 2000; Greenberg et al., 2017).

1.1 Social–emotional skills and mental health

Even in the decade prior to the COVID-19 pandemic, there was substantial concern about the high rates of psychological distress among college students. College student mental health centers (Gallagher, 2015) and the American College Health Association’s National College Health Assessment (American College Health Association, 2018) reported that rates of mental health challenges were already rising significantly well before the 2020 pandemic. Duffy et al. (2019) note that between 2007 and 2018, data from both the National College Health Assessment and the Healthy Minds Study indicated increased rates of depression and anxiety among college students. Eisenberg (2019) offers a series of possible explanations for the rise in mental health issues among college students including suggesting that an increase in social media leads to impaired sleep (Hysing et al., 2015), which in turn leads to increases in depression and anxiety (Eisenberg et al., 2018).

Thus, the pandemic and the lockdowns associated with it made things worse, with rates of depression, anxiety and stress all increasing (Jones et al., 2021). Adolescents’ experiences of loneliness also increased during the pandemic (Branje and Morris, 2021). Labrague et al. (2021) note that while loneliness was prevalent among college students, differences in how students experienced the pandemic were likely influenced by their use of coping behaviors and social supports. Other researchers have found that the COVID-19 pandemic and related social restrictions had a significant impact on the mental health of adolescents, particularly in terms of their access to social support and disruptions to their daily routines (Knowles et al., 2022). The mental health of young adults has been affected by the social isolation brought about by COVID-19, and early studies on this topic have highlighted the need to understand how the pandemic has affected the social–emotional skills that are crucial for helping individuals manage difficult circumstances. In addition to the direct effect of the loss of skills, this impairment is harmful to the mental health of emerging adults. For example, using the Trait Meta Mood scale of emotional intelligence (Salovey et al., 1995), a cross-sectional analysis of a subset of data from the present study found emotional attention, clarity, and repair all predicted college students’ depression, anxiety, and stress scores (Moeller et al., 2020).

1.2 The wide-ranging sequelae of social–emotional skills

Social and emotional skills are an integral component of Emotional Intelligence as initially theorized by Mayer and Salovey (1997) and subsequently revised by Mayer et al. (2004). This theory is structured around a four-component model of ability, including: perception of emotions, utilization of emotional information for cognition, understanding of emotions, and the ability to regulate and manage one’s emotions (Mayer et al., 2004).

The Riggio (1986, 2005) social skills inventory measures critical aspects of this theoretical model. Riggio’s social skills inventory assesses social–emotional skills within the domains of verbal-social and nonverbal-emotional (Riggio, 1986). Within each of the two domains, the measure captures three types of skills—encoding/expressivity skills, decoding/sensitivity skills and regulation/control skills (Riggio, 2005)—for a total of six constructs measured. The original 90-item measure (Riggio, 1986) has since been successfully abridged and tested with fewer items (Riggio and Carney, 2003; Segrin et al., 2007; Umphrey and Sherblom, 2014). Moeller and Seehuus (2019b) used this abridged inventory to determine that the social skills of expressivity (i.e., encoding communication), sensitivity (i.e., decoding communication), and control (i.e., regulating behaviors during communication) individually predict loneliness, depression, and anxiety in college students. Similarly, the social skills inventory has been found to be significantly associated with positive relations with others and overall psychological well-being (Suresh and Sandhu, 2012). Emotional control, the ability to recognize and regulate one’s social–emotional responses, has been found to be negatively associated with a sense of being a burden, thwarted belongingness, and suicidal ideation (Umphrey et al., 2021).

Emotional sensitivity, social expressivity and social control have all been documented to be associated with undergraduates’ reported levels of happiness, while social expressivity and social control were associated with self-esteem (Panchal and Joshi, 2013). In another study of university students, the social skills inventory was negatively correlated with loneliness and positively correlated with life satisfaction (Ozben, 2013). Umphrey and Sherblom (2014) found that the social skills inventory was positively correlated with hope, self-compassion, and life satisfaction.

Additionally, social–emotional skills are important for academic performance. Iqbal et al. (2021) note that emotional intelligence is an important factor in predicting academic performance, specifically in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic. Others including Koc and Turan (2018) note all six of the subscales of the social skills inventory were associated with college students’ cultural intelligence. This is particularly important as part of students’ ability to navigate the social world of colleges is to engage in the multicultural dynamics of a college.

1.3 The present study

Given the critical role that social–emotional skills play in the lives of emerging adults, the question of the impact of the COVID-19 lockdowns on the development of social–emotional skills looms large. Mayer and Salovey (1997)’s Emotional Intelligence theory provides a framework for how emotional capabilities and cognition underpin social–emotional skills, highlighting how understanding and managing emotions enhance everyday social interactions. Research has consistently shown that cognitive and affective processes are deeply intertwined, such that cognitive processes can up- or down-regulate emotional states and vice versa (Campos et al., 2004; Ochsner and Gross, 2005; Kray et al., 2020). Cognitive and affective networks function together to enhance the use of social–emotional skills in everyday life through attentional control, cognitive flexibility, emotion regulation, and empathy (Beauchamp and Anderson, 2010). Among young adults, the COVID-19 pandemic lockdowns and resulting social isolation have negatively impacted cognitive domains, including executive functioning, and affective domains, including emotion recognition and decreased positive emotions (Carbone et al., 2021; Bland et al., 2022; Panteli et al., 2022; Murtaza et al., 2023).

The COVID-19 pandemic lockdowns’ impact on cognitive and affective processes has important implications for the development and refinement of social–emotional skills. Based on Emotional Intelligence Theory (Mayer and Salovey, 1997), heightened stress, uncertainty, and anxiety during the COVID-19 pandemic lockdowns (Varma et al., 2021) may have overwhelmed young adults’ ability to manage emotions effectively, leading to maladaptive emotion regulation strategies and unhelpful social interactions. Evidence also suggests that foundational components of social–emotional skills, such as emotion recognition (Carbon and Serrano, 2021; Pazhoohi et al., 2021; Grahlow et al., 2022), assessment of emotional intensity (Tsantani et al., 2022), and emotional mimicry (Kastendieck et al., 2022), were all impaired by the facial coverings common during the pandemic. Moreover, Bandura’s Social Cognitive Theory posits that individuals’ social–emotional skills develop through observing, modeling, and reinforcing behaviors in social interactions (Bandura, 1997; Glanz et al., 2015). Social distancing and remote learning during the pandemic may have further hindered the development and refinement of these skills through limited face-to-face interactions and decreased social interactions (Wu et al., 2022), leading to impaired mental health specifically among college students (Thompson et al., 2021; Olson et al., 2023). Collectively, these theoretical perspectives suggest that the COVID-19 pandemic lockdowns may be associated with impairment in social–emotional skills through multiple mechanisms, including disruptions in emotional and cognitive abilities and social learning opportunities.

However, it remains unclear how social–emotional skills were specifically impacted by the COVID-19 pandemic lockdowns. To identify the effect of COVID-19 on the development of social–emotional skills in college students, we compared pre- and post-lockdown measurements of social–emotional skills in first year college students from two liberal arts colleges using data from a five-year longitudinal study of college student mental health spanning from 2018 to 2022. We focused on first year students for several reasons. First, by focusing on first year students we can assess their incoming social–emotional skills and mental health before their college experiences have (potentially) altered their psychosocial skills. Second, the transition to college occurs during the transition from late adolescence to emerging adulthood, a key developmental period in psychosocial adjustment (Schulenberg et al., 2004). Thus, an assessment of first year students would capture the disruption in the normal developmental trajectory more clearly than if older students were included. Finally, these compound, complex, and simultaneous adjustments—transitioning physically, emotionally, and intellectually a college environment—are notoriously difficult and associated with a significant dropout rate (Schneider, 2010; Aulck et al., 2019). By limiting our analysis to first year students, we are thus more likely to capture the breadth of college student experience. Further, we excluded data collected in January 2021 from analysis. In January 2021, the COVID-19 lockdown was in effect for some students and not others; thus, a sample collected in January of 2021 would have been drawn from a qualitatively distinct population, limiting interpretability.

Based on Emotional Intelligence Theory and Social Cognitive Theory, we hypothesized that the COVID-19-related lockdowns would be associated with impaired social–emotional skills across all six social and emotional skill subdomains analyzed, and that this effect would be evident when comparing first year students post-lockdown (January 2022) to first year students pre-lockdown (January 2018–January 2020).

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Procedure

The study procedure received approval from the Institutional Review Board (IRB) at the corresponding author’s institution. The current study is a secondary analysis of a cross-sequential study exploring psychosocial skills and mental health among undergraduate college students at two liberal arts colleges located in the Northeast United States. Data collection for the parent study occurred in five separate waves during January of 2018, 2019, 2020, 2021, and 2022. At each wave, all currently enrolled undergraduate students at both colleges (ranging from N = 4,348 to 4,679) received an email inviting students to participate in a study seeking to explore how students’ experiences with stress and other mental health factors change over time. The email contained a unique link to an online Qualtrics survey where students completed study measures. Students who consented and completed the survey were offered the chance to enter a raffle to win a $50 or $100 Amazon gift card. Each participant could complete a survey every year. Across the five waves, N = 6,348 unique students provided a total of 8,966 responses, a total study response rate across 5 years of data collection of 40%.

2.2 Participant characteristics

Of the 8,966 survey responses, surveys not from first year students and those that did not respond to the question of what year in college they were (n = 6,597) were excluded. Participants (n = 48) were then dropped if their responses did not pass one or more data quality test (e.g., participants were excluded if they had straight line response pattern across more than one measure, or inconsistent data within a single data collection wave), and/or if they did not report age or reported an age equal to or beyond 28 years, the known highest value for this population. We further removed responses (n = 157) who did not complete all 24 survey questions from the abridged version of the Social Skills Inventory (SSI) (Riggio, 1986), our primary variable of interest. Preliminary analyses determined there were no significant differences between participants who completed and did not complete SSI questions on age, gender, race, and socioeconomic status. Finally, as described above, participants from the January 2021 wave (n = 479) were excluded from the current study, resulting in a total analytic sample of 1,685 participants. Among the final analytic sample (N = 1,685), there were 533 participants in 2018 (31.6%), 551 in 2019 (32.7%), 174 in 2020 (10.3%), and 427 in 2022 (25.3%) waves.

To determine whether SSI scores differed before and after the COVID-19 lockdowns, we collapsed waves 2018–2020 to create a pre-lockdown group (n = 1,258). Pairwise comparisons were run to identify any significant differences in demographics or SSI measures between 2018, 2019, and 2020 waves, with no significant differences found. The post-lockdown group (n = 427) included participants from 2022. Of note, data collection occurred in January of each academic year and thus 2020 data was collected prior to the lockdowns beginning in March 2020.

2.3 Measures

2.3.1 Sociodemographics

During the online survey, participants were asked to self-report their age, gender, race, and perceived socioeconomic status. Age was modeled as a continuous variable, while gender, race, and socioeconomic status were dummy-coded.

2.3.2 Social–emotional skills

Social–emotional skills were measured via an abridged version of the 90 Social Skills Inventory (SSI) (Riggio, 1986). The abridged version of the SSI, which has been validated in prior work (Oldmeadow et al., 2013), includes a total of 24 questions capturing two domains of social ability: emotional (related to non-verbal skills) and social (related to verbal skills). The emotional and social domains are each broken down into the following three subdomains:

1. Expressivity: emotional expressivity (EE) and social expressivity (SE)

2. Sensitivity: emotional sensitivity (ES) and social sensitivity (SS)

3. Control: emotional control (EC) and social control (SC)

Each subdomain assesses non-verbal (emotional) and verbal (social) aspects of social ability, respectively, and yields a total of six social skills measures (SSI measures). Each SSI measure includes four items on a five-point scale ranging from “Not at all like me” (1) to “Exactly like me” (5). Expressivity focuses on one’s ability to communicate non-verbally (EE; e.g., “I rarely show my feelings or emotions”) and verbally (SE; e.g., “At parties I enjoy talking to a lot of different people”). The sensitivity subdomain captures one’s ability to mediate communication non-verbally (ES; e.g., “I always seem to know what peoples’ true feelings are no matter how hard they try to conceal them”) and verbally (SS; e.g., “It is very important that other people like me”). Lastly, the control subdomain measures the ability to control and regulate behaviors non-verbally (EC; e.g., “I am rarely able to hide a strong emotion”) and verbally (SC; e.g., “I am usually very good at leading group discussion”). Higher scores on all SSI measures, excluding SS, indicate more adaptive social–emotional abilities. Higher scores on SS reflect poorer adaptation, as higher scores may signal hypervigilance regarding how others perceive oneself and sensitivity to criticism.

2.4 Analysis

Data were examined for normality using skewness and kurtosis, and Levene’s tests were used to test the homogeneity of variance assumption. Descriptive statistics were generated for sociodemographic variables, and we compared differences in sociodemographics between pre- and post-lockdown groups using t-tests and chi-square tests for continuous and categorical variables, respectively. We conducted a total of six one-way analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) to assess whether mean scores of SSI measures were significantly different between the pre-lockdown and post-lockdown groups, controlling for gender, race, and socioeconomic status. We included these variables as covariates given prior research showing that social and emotional skills vary by gender, race, and socioeconomic status (de Ridder et al., 2012; Kuo et al., 2020; Gruijters et al., 2024). To estimate the SSI measures’ internal consistencies, Cronbach’s alpha coefficients were generated for pre- and post-lockdown groups. We further performed sensitivity analyses to examine if any potentially significant results related to emotional expressivity (EE) were driven by items related to concerns of touching others following the wake of COVID-19. Specifically, EE contained two items related to touch, for example “I usually feel uncomfortable touching other people.” We created a sum score of the two EE items that were unrelated to touch (i.e., “Sometimes I have trouble making my friends and family realize how angry or upset I am with them,” and “I rarely show my feelings or emotions”). As a robustness check, we ran an ANCOVA, controlling for gender, race, and socioeconomic status, to examine if there was a significant difference on this two-item EE sum score between the pre- and post-lockdown groups. All analyses were performed in R version 3.6 (R Core Team, 2022).

3 Results

Among the final analytic sample (N = 1,685), there was no missing data on age, gender, or socioeconomic status; however, 86 observations (5.10% of the sample) were missing on race. Demographic details are shown in Table 1. In the full analytic sample, the mean age of first year students was 18.53 years, approximately 56.8% of the sample identified as female, and 69.5% identified as white/non-Hispanic. There were significant differences in the distributions of gender, race, and socioeconomic status between pre- and post-lockdown groups (see Table 1). Age was not significantly different between pre- and post-lockdown groups and was therefore removed as a covariate in the ANCOVAs.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographics of full sample and pre-/post-COVID lockdown groups.

The Cronbach’s alphas for emotional sensitivity (ES), emotional control (EC), social expressivity (SE), social sensitivity (SS), and social control (SC) were found to be highly reliable in both pre- and post-lockdown groups (4 items each; Cronbach’s α ranged from.78–.92). However, Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for the 4-item emotional expressivity (EE) subdomain in the pre-lockdown and post-lockdown group were.57 and.60, respectively, suggesting a lower level of reliability for the EE. We found that social–emotional skill scores were approximately normally distributed with skewness and kurtosis values within +/− 1.00 for both pre- and post-lockdown groups. Levene’s tests indicated no significant differences (all ps > .05) in the variance of social–emotional skills between pre- and post-lockdown groups, suggesting the assumption of homogeneity of variance was met.

ANCOVAs were conducted on the dataset using complete cases, which involved the removal of 86 observations with missing data on race. As a result, the final sample size for the ANCOVAs consisted of 1,599 participants (pre-lockdown group: n = 1,199; post-lockdown group: n = 400). Results from the six ANCOVAs are presented in Tables 2, 3 for emotional-focused (nonverbal) and social-focused (verbal) skill subdomains, respectively. All ANCOVAs controlled for the following covariates: gender, race, and socioeconomic status. Our hypothesis was partially supported: mean SSI scores significantly differed between pre- and post-lockdown groups in two out of six subdomains. Controlling for covariates, the post-lockdown group (M = 12.49, SD = 3.13) had significantly lower scores on EE than the pre-lockdown group (M = 12.97, SD = 3.03), F(1, 1,586) = 5.30, p = .021, η2 = .003, suggesting a poorer ability to communicate emotions non-verbally after COVID-19 lockdowns. Additionally, the post-lockdown group (M = 13.3, SD = 3.65) scored significantly lower on EC than the pre-lockdown group (M = 14.48, SD = 3.44), F(1, 1,586) = 5.71, p = .017, η2 = .004, suggesting that, after the COVID-19 lockdown, students were less able to control and regulate their emotions than before the lockdowns. Lastly, the post-lockdown group (M = 14.38, SD = 3.63) had higher scores on SS than the pre-lockdown group (M = 13.95, SD = 3.70), F(1, 1,586) = 3.76, p = .052, η2 = .002, although this difference was only marginally significant. Given that higher scores on this subscale indicate more sensitivity to criticism and concern regarding how one is perceived by others, this result suggests poorer adaptation following the COVID-19 lockdowns. There were no significant differences between pre- and post-lockdown groups on ES, SE, or SC measures.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. ANCOVAs comparing pre- and post-COVID lockdown groups on emotional skill subdomains (N = 1,599).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. ANCOVAs comparing pre- and post-COVID lockdown groups on social skill subdomains (N = 1,599).

Sensitivity analysis revealed that both of the two EE items unrelated to touch were significantly, positively correlated with the original EE measure (r = .59, p < .01; r = .69, p < .01). After controlling for covariates, the difference between the pre-lockdown group (M = 6.99, SD = 1.80) and the post-lockdown group (M = 6.74, SD = 1.88) on the summed two-item EE variable unrelated to touch was marginally significant, F(1, 1,586) = 3.66, p = .056.

The main effects of covariates on emotional and social skill subdomains are presented in Tables 4, 5, respectively. Only gender was significantly related to all three emotional skill subdomains. For social skill subdomains, all covariates were significantly related to SS and SC. Lastly, only race and socioeconomic status were significantly related to SE.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Main effects of covariates on emotional skill subdomains (N = 1,599).

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Main effects of covariates on social skill subdomains (N = 1,599).

4 Discussion

Social–emotional skills play an important role in students’ academic achievement, mental health, and other life outcomes (Greenberg et al., 2017; Alzahrani et al., 2019). The current study investigated whether social–emotional skills differed between first year college students in the United States before and after the lockdowns related to the COVID-19 pandemic. To our knowledge, this study is the first to assess the impact of the COVID-19 lockdowns on the development of social–emotional skills in college students. Our hypothesis that students’ social–emotional skills would decline following COVID-19 lockdowns was partially supported. The COVID-19 pandemic lockdowns had a negative impact on some of the social and emotional skills of incoming college students, but other domains were unaffected. Compared to pre-pandemic first year college students, first year college students assessed in 2022 had lower emotional control and expressivity skills, and marginally higher social sensitivity skills, after controlling for gender, race, and socioeconomic status. That suggests that post-lockdown, students were coming out of high school with more poorly developed skills to communicate emotions non-verbally (EE, emotional expressivity) and to control and regulate their emotional displays (EC, emotional control). Further, social sensitivity (SS), or the ability to navigate social norms, was higher. Higher SS scores are theorized to be associated with increased social anxiety and self-consciousness (Riggio, 1986). Differences were not observed in emotional sensitivity (ES), or the ability to perceive non-verbal affective communications, social expressivity (SE), which is the ability to communicate emotional states verbally, and social control (SC), which is the ability to manage self-presentation in social settings.

Although we hypothesized that all domains of social and emotional skills would be impaired, these results paint a more nuanced picture of the effect of the lockdowns. The results describe a cohort reaching for more emotional connection while self-consciously aware of their own impaired social skills; a cohort that lacks the real-world experience that could reduce the intensity of that self-consciousness with repeated exposures.

The pandemic lockdowns were associated with a reduction in at least the variety if not quantity of in-person interactions. We speculate that it was that change that led to the observed reduction in non-verbal communication skills, measured by a lower EE score. Further, we suggest that the stress and emotional intensity of the pandemic, while paired with a reduction in access to the adaptive social coping opportunities normally present, has left a cohort with reduced abilities to manage their own emotional displays (whether through lack of practice or because a video-mediated communication alters the normal non-verbal communication methods; see Schaarschmidt and Koehler (2021) for discussion) and hyperaware of their own social presentation. These data suggest that lockdowns did not impair attention and sympathy toward the emotional states of others, and neither did they alter verbal communication or social presentation skills.

There are important limitations to these results. Most notably, students from small liberal arts colleges have important differences when compared to other college students, or to emerging adults who did not pursue college, including differences in race, ethnicity, educational attainment, and socioeconomic status (Blanco et al., 2008; Han et al., 2016; Baldwin et al., 2017). Collectively these factors may limit the generalizability of our findings. These results are unable to speak to how those differences may have an effect on these results or conclusions, and caution is warranted in generalizing these conclusions. Further, although self-report is the most common way of assessing social–emotional skills, all self-report methods have inherent self-presentation limitations that should be considered (Althubaiti, 2016). There may have been a selection bias, which influenced which students chose to participate in the study and those students who did not. It is possible that our sample included students who had more interest in mental health subject matter, or had higher levels of social–emotional skills. Additionally, while our analytic approach allowed us to address an emerging and time-sensitive question on changes in social–emotional skills associated with the COVID-19 pandemic lockdowns, our results may be subject to measurement error. Future research should test more complex models, such as those incorporating latent variables to account for measurement error, to further investigate the trajectories of social–emotional skills throughout the COVID-19 pandemic.

Lastly, the COVID-19 lockdowns were not experienced uniformly across the United States. Although the significant effects detected were robust to key sociodemographic covariates, other unanticipated and thus unmeasured factors likely influenced the effect of the lockdowns on students’ social–emotional skills. Future research is warranted to explore how factors such as home life, alternative remote learning methods, and the severity of state-level lockdown restrictions impacted students’ social–emotional skills development.

This is not the only interpretation of this pattern of results, and further work will be necessary to confirm our speculative causal mechanism and compare its strength with other factors. There is some urgency to understanding this process because these findings suggest that the COVID-19 associated lockdowns had deleterious effects on the social–emotional skills of incoming college students. Given the mental health implications of social–emotional skills, particularly amongst college students (Payton et al., 2000; Greenberg et al., 2017), these changes may be part of the mechanism connecting the lockdowns to the observed decline in college student mental health (Conrad et al., 2021; Copeland et al., 2021). If so, they are potentially a target for intervention. Combined with the evidence that the mental health of college students has been declining steadily over the past decade (Kruisselbrink Flatt, 2013; Pedrelli et al., 2015; Moeller and Seehuus, 2019b), these results both suggest that, absent some kind of intervention, the emotional impact of the lockdowns is likely to further accelerate the trend toward increased psychological distress amongst college students.

Although social skills are widely assessed and implemented, particularly in primary school settings (la Greca and Santogrossi, 1980; Ogilvy, 1994), that is less true for the general college population (Brown, 2013; Moeller and Seehuus, 2019a; Kul and Gönültaş, 2022). It is likely that explicitly addressing social–emotional skills amongst rising and current college students would be of benefit in a number of domains; given the impact of the pandemic lockdowns on those social–emotional skills, teaching these social and emotional skills may be an important step in mitigating the impact of the lockdowns. Future research should explore such interventions.

Data availability statement

The data analyzed in this study is subject to the following licenses/restrictions: the data that support the findings of this study will be made available in the National Data Archive upon publication. Requests to access these datasets should be directed to RM, the corresponding author at rmoeller@middlebury.edu.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Institutional Review Board, Middlebury College. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

JC: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Methodology, Formal analysis, Conceptualization. KB: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Methodology, Formal analysis, Conceptualization. RM: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Methodology, Funding acquisition, Data curation, Conceptualization. MS: Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Methodology, Data curation, Conceptualization.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Research reported in this publication was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health of the National Institutes of Health under award number 1R15MH125373–01.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Althubaiti, A. (2016). Information bias in health research: definition, pitfalls, and adjustment methods. J. Multidiscip. Healthc. 9, 211–217. doi: 10.2147/JMDH.S104807

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Alzahrani, M., Alharbi, M., and Alodwani, A. (2019). The effect of social-emotional competence on children academic achievement and behavioral development. Int. Educ. Stud. 12, 141–149. doi: 10.5539/ies.v12n12p141

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

American College Health Association (2018). American college health association-National College Health Assessment II: Reference group executive summary fall 2017. Hanover, MD: American College Health Association.

Google Scholar

Arnett, J. J. (2014). Emerging adulthood: The winding road from the late teens through the twenties. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Aulck, L., Nambi, D., Velagapudi, N., Blumenstock, J., and West, J. (2019). Mining university registrar records to predict first-year undergraduate attrition, In International conference on educational data mining (EDM) Montreal, Canada

Google Scholar

Baldwin, D. R., Towler, K., Oliver, M. D., and Datta, S. (2017). An examination of college student wellness: a research and liberal arts perspective. Health Psychol. Open 4:205510291771956. doi: 10.1177/2055102917719563

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York, NY: Freeman.

Google Scholar

Beauchamp, M. H., and Anderson, V. (2010). SOCIAL: an integrative framework for the development of social skills. Psychol. Bull. 136, 39–64. doi: 10.1037/a0017768

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Birmingham, W. C., Wadsworth, L. L., Lassetter, J. H., Graff, T. C., Lauren, E., and Hung, M. (2021). COVID-19 lockdown: impact on college students' lives. J. Am. Coll. Heal. 71, 879–893. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2021.1909041

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanco, C., Okuda, M., Wright, C., Hasin, D. S., Grant, B. F., Liu, S.-M., et al. (2008). Mental health of college students and their non–college-attending peers: results from the national epidemiologic study on alcohol and related conditions. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 65, 1429–1437. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.12.1429

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bland, A. R., Roiser, J. P., Mehta, M. A., Sahakian, B. J., Robbins, T. W., and Elliott, R. (2022). The impact of COVID-19 social isolation on aspects of emotional and social cognition. Cogn. Emot. 36, 49–58. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2021.1892593

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bos, D. J., Dreyfuss, M., Tottenham, N., Hare, T. A., Galván, A., Casey, B. J., et al. (2020). Distinct and similar patterns of emotional development in adolescents and young adults. Dev. Psychobiol. 62, 591–599. doi: 10.1002/dev.21942

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Branje, S., and Morris, A. S. (2021). The impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on adolescent emotional, social, and academic adjustment. J. Res. Adolesc. 31, 486–499. doi: 10.1111/jora.12668

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, C. (2013). Are we becoming more socially awkward? An analysis of the relationship between technological communication use and social skills in college students. Honors Paper, Conneticut College.

Google Scholar

Camacho-Zuñiga, C., Pego, L., Escamilla, J., and Hosseini, S. (2021). The impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on students' feelings at high school, undergraduate, and postgraduate levels. Heliyon 7:e06465. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2021.e06465

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Campos, J. J., Frankel, C. B., and Camras, L. (2004). On the nature of emotion regulation. Child Dev. 75, 377–394. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00681.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carbon, C. C., and Serrano, M. (2021). The impact of face masks on the emotional reading abilities of children-a lesson from a joint school-university project. I-Perception 12:20416695211038265. doi: 10.1177/20416695211038265

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carbone, E., Palumbo, R., Sella, E., Lenti, G., Di Domenico, A., and Borella, E. (2021). Emotional, psychological, and cognitive changes throughout the COVID-19 pandemic in Italy: is there an advantage of being an older adult? Front. Aging Neurosci. 13:712369. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2021.712369

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Center for Reinventing Public Education (2021). How has the pandemic affected students’ social-emotional well-being? A review of the evidence to date. Available at: https://crpe.org/how-has-the-pandemic-affected-students-social-emotional-well-being-a-review-of-the-evidence-to-date/ (Accessed December 30, 2022)

Google Scholar

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2020). Social distancing, quarantine, and isolation. Available at: https://www.cdc.gov/coronavirus/2019-ncov/prevent-getting-sick/social-distancing.html (Accessed March 30, 2020)

Google Scholar

Chickering, A. W., and Reisser, L. (1993). Education and identity. Second Edn. The Jossey-Bass Higher and Adult Education Series. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass Inc.

Google Scholar

Conrad, R. C., Hahm, H. C., Koire, A., Pinder-Amaker, S., and Liu, C. H. (2021). College student mental health risks during the COVID-19 pandemic: implications of campus relocation. J. Psychiatr. Res. 136, 117–126. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.01.054

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Copeland, W. E., McGinnis, E., Bai, Y., Adams, Z., Nardone, H., Devadanam, V., et al. (2021). Impact of COVID-19 pandemic on college student mental health and wellness. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 60, 134–141.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2020.08.466

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

de Ridder, D. T., Lensvelt-Mulders, G., Finkenauer, C., Stok, F. M., and Baumeister, R. F. (2012). Taking stock of self-control: a meta-analysis of how trait self-control relates to a wide range of behaviors. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 16, 76–99. doi: 10.1177/1088868311418749

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dotson, M. P., Castro, E. M., Magid, N. T., Hoyt, L. T., Suleiman, A. B., and Cohen, A. K. (2022). "emotional distancing": change and strain in U.S. young adult college students' relationships during COVID-19. Emerg. Adulthood 10, 546–557. doi: 10.1177/21676968211065531

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Duffy, M. E., Twenge, J. M., and Joiner, T. E. (2019). Trends in mood and anxiety symptoms and suicide-related outcomes among U.S. undergraduates, 2007-2018: evidence from two national surveys. J. Adolesc. Health 65, 590–598. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2019.04.033

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eccles, J. S., and Roeser, R. W. (2011). Schools as developmental contexts during adolescence. J. Res. Adolesc. 21, 225–241. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00725.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberg, D. (2019). Countering the troubling increase in mental health symptoms among U.S. college students. J. Adolesc. Health 65, 573–574. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2019.08.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberg, D., Lipson, S. K., Ceglarek, P., Kern, A., and Phillips, M. V. (2018). “College student mental health: the national landscape” in Promoting behavioral health and reducing risk among college students. eds. M. Dolores Cimini and E. M. Rivero (New York, NY: Routledge), 75–86.

Google Scholar

Foubert, J. D., and Urbanski, L. A. (2006). Effects of involvement in clubs and organizations on the psychosocial development of first-year and senior college students. J. Stud. Aff. Res. Pract. 43, 166–182. doi: 10.2202/1949-6605.1576

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallagher, R. P. (2015). National survey of college counseling centers 2014 : The International Association of Counseling Services Inc.

Google Scholar

Glanz, K., Rimer, B. K., and Viswanath, K. (2015). Health behavior: Theory, research, and practice. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Google Scholar

Grahlow, M., Rupp, C. I., and Derntl, B. (2022). The impact of face masks on emotion recognition performance and perception of threat. PLoS One 17:e0262840. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0262840

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenberg, M. T., Domitrovich, C. E., Weissberg, R. P., and Durlak, J. A. (2017). Social and emotional learning as a public health approach to education. Futur. Child. 27, 13–32. doi: 10.1353/foc.2017.0001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gruijters, R. J., Raabe, I. J., and Hübner, N. (2024). Socio-emotional skills and the socioeconomic achievement gap. Sociol. Educ. 97, 120–147. doi: 10.1177/00380407231216424

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, B., Compton, W. M., Eisenberg, D., Milazzo-Sayre, L., McKeon, R., and Hughes, A. (2016). Prevalence and mental health treatment of suicidal ideation and behavior among college students aged 18-25 years and their non-college-attending peers in the United States. J. Clin. Psychiatry 77, 815–824. doi: 10.4088/JCP.15m09929

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hysing, M., Pallesen, S., Stormark, K. M., Jakobsen, R., Lundervold, A. J., and Sivertsen, B. (2015). Sleep and use of electronic devices in adolescence: results from a large population-based study. BMJ Open 5:e006748. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2014-006748

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Iqbal, J., Ashraf, M. A., Perveen, S., Qureshi, N., Imran, Z., and Jin, N. (2021). How emotional intelligence influences cognitive outcomes among university students: the mediating role of relational engagement during the Covid-19 pandemic. Front. Psych. 12, 1–11.doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.711439

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Isohätälä, J., Näykki, P., Järvelä, S., Baker, M. J., and Lund, K. (2021). Social sensitivity: a manifesto for CSCL research. Int. J. Comput.-Support. Collab. Learn. 16, 289–299. doi: 10.1007/s11412-021-09344-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, E. A. K., Mitra, A. K., and Bhuiyan, A. R. (2021). Impact of COVID-19 on mental health in adolescents: a systematic review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:2470. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18052470

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kastendieck, T., Zillmer, S., and Hess, U. (2022). (un)mask yourself! Effects of face masks on facial mimicry and emotion perception during the COVID-19 pandemic. Cogn. Emot. 36, 59–69. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2021.1950639

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Knowles, G., Gayer-Anderson, C., Turner, A., Dorn, L., Lam, J., Davis, S., et al. (2022). Covid-19, social restrictions, and mental distress among young people: a UK longitudinal, population-based study. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 63, 1392–1404. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13586

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koc, K., and Turan, M. B. (2018). The impact of cultural intelligence on social skills among university students. EduLearn 7, 241–249. doi: 10.5539/jel.v7n6p241

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kray, J., Ritter, H., and Müller, L. (2020). The interplay between cognitive control and emotional processing in children and adolescents. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 193:104795. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104795

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kruisselbrink Flatt, A. (2013). A suffering generation: six factors contributing to the mental health crisis in north American higher education. Coll. Q. 16.

Google Scholar

Kul, A., and Gönültaş, M. (2022). Impacts of a cognitive behavioral approach based social skills training on the social skills of freshman students. Psycho-Educ. Res. Rev. 11, 382–391. doi: 10.52963/PERR_Biruni_V11.N1.24

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuo, Y.-L., Casillas, A., Walton, K. E., Way, J. D., and Moore, J. L. (2020). The intersectionality of race/ethnicity and socioeconomic status on social and emotional skills. J. Res. Pers. 84:103905. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2019.103905

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

la Greca, A. M., and Santogrossi, D. A. (1980). Social skills training with elementary school students: a behavioral group approach. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 48, 220–227. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.48.2.220

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Labrague, L. J., De Los Santos, J. A. A., and Falguera, C. C. (2021). Social and emotional loneliness among college students during the COVID-19 pandemic: the predictive role of coping behaviors, social support, and personal resilience. Perspect. Psychiatr. Care 57, 1578–1584. doi: 10.1111/ppc.12721

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lv, X., Ma, J., Brinthaupt, T. M., Zhao, S., and Ren, X. (2022). Impacts of university lockdown during the coronavirus pandemic on college students' academic achievement and critical thinking: a longitudinal study. Front. Psych. 13:995784. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.995784

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, L. M. (2000). The relationship of college experiences to psychosocial outcomes in students. J. Coll. Stud. Dev. 41, 292–301.

Google Scholar

Mayer, J. D., and Salovey, P. (1997). “What is emotional intelligence?” in Emotional development and emotional intelligence: Educational implications (New York, NY, US: Basic Books), 3–34.

Google Scholar

Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., and Caruso, D. R. (2004). Emotional intelligence: theory, findings, and implications. Psychol. Inq. 15, 197–215. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli1503_02

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moeller, R.W., and Seehuus, M. (2019a). Effect of social skills on depression and anxiety mediated by loneliness in college students, In international convention of psychological science. Paris, France

Google Scholar

Moeller, R. W., and Seehuus, M. (2019b). Loneliness as a mediator for college students' social skills and experiences of depression and anxiety. J. Adolesc. 73, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2019.03.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moeller, R. W., Seehuus, M., and Peisch, V. (2020). Emotional intelligence, belongingness, and mental health in college students. Front. Psych. 11:93. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00093

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Murtaza, G., Sultana, R., Abualait, T., Fatima, M., and Bashir, S. (2023). Social isolation during the COVID-19 pandemic is associated with the decline in cognitive functioning in young adults. PeerJ 11:e16532. doi: 10.7717/peerj.16532

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ochsner, K. N., and Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive control of emotion. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 242–249. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogilvy, C. M. (1994). Social skills training with children and adolescents: a review of the evidence on effectiveness. J. Educ. Psychol. 14, 73–83. doi: 10.1080/0144341940140105

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Oldmeadow, J. A., Quinn, S., and Kowert, R. (2013). Attachment style, social skills, and Facebook use amongst adults. Comput. Hum. Behav. 29, 1142–1149. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2012.10.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Olson, R., Fryz, R., Essemiah, J., Crawford, M., King, A., and Fateye, B. (2023). Mental health impacts of COVID-19 lockdown on US college students: results of a photoelicitation project. J. Am. Coll. Heal. 71, 411–421. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2021.1891921

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ozben, S. (2013). Social skills, life satisfaction, and loneliness in Turkish university students. Soc. Behav. Pers. 41, 203–213. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2013.41.2.203

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Panchal, S., and Joshi, H. L. (2013). Happiness in relation to social skills and self-esteem among youths. IJHW 4, 34–37. doi: 10.1037/e670282012-157

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pandya, A., and Lodha, P. (2021). Social connectedness, excessive screen time during COVID-19 and mental health: a review of current evidence. Front. Hum. Dyn. 3:684137. doi: 10.3389/fhumd.2021.684137

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Panteli, M., Papantoniou, A., Vaiouli, P., Leonidou, C., and Panayiotou, G. (2022). Feeling down in lockdown: effects of COVID-19 pandemic on emotionally vulnerable individuals. Couns. Psychol. 50, 335–358. doi: 10.1177/00110000211064905

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Payton, J. W., Wardlaw, D. M., Graczyk, P. A., Bloodworth, M. R., Tompsett, C. J., and Weissberg, R. P. (2000). Social and emotional learning: a framework for promoting mental health and reducing risk behavior in children and youth. J. Sch. Health 70, 179–185. doi: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.2000.tb06468.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pazhoohi, F., Forby, L., and Kingstone, A. (2021). Facial masks affect emotion recognition in the general population and individuals with autistic traits. PLoS One 16:e0257740. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0257740

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pedrelli, P., Nyer, M., Yeung, A., Zulauf, C., and Wilens, T. (2015). College students: mental health problems and treatment considerations. Acad. Psychiatry 39, 503–511. doi: 10.1007/s40596-014-0205-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2022). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Google Scholar

Riggio, R. E. (1986). Assessment of basic social skills. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 51, 649–660. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.3.649

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Riggio, R. E. (2005). “The social skills inventory (SSI): measuring nonverbal and social skills” in The sourcebook of nonverbal measures: Going beyond words. ed. V. Manusov (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers), 25–33.

Google Scholar

Riggio, R. E., and Carney, D. R. (2003). The social skills inventory manual. Mountain View, CA: Mind Garden.

Google Scholar

Roeser, R. W., Eccles, J. S., and Sameroff, A. J. (2000). School as a context of early adolescents' academic and social-emotional development: a summary of research findings. Elem. Sch. J. 100, 443–471. doi: 10.1086/499650

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Salovey, P., Mayer, J. D., Goldman, S. L., Turvey, C., and Palfai, T. P. (1995). “Emotional attention, clarity, and repair: exploring emotional intelligence using the trait Meta-mood scale” in Emotion, disclosure, & health. ed. J. W. Pennebaker (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 125–154.

Google Scholar

Schaarschmidt, N., and Koehler, T. (2021). Experiencing emotions in video-mediated psychological counselling versus to face-to-face settings. Societies 11:20. doi: 10.3390/soc11010020

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, M. (2010). Finishing the first lap: The cost of first year student attrition in America's four year colleges and universities. Washington, DC: American Institutes for Research.

Google Scholar

Schulenberg, J. E., Sameroff, A. J., and Cicchetti, D. (2004). The transition to adulthood as a critical juncture in the course of psychopathology and mental health. Dev. Psychopathol. 16, 799–806. doi: 10.1017/s0954579404040015

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Segrin, C., Hanzal, A., Donnerstein, C., Taylor, M., and Domschke, T. J. (2007). Social skills, psychological well-being, and the mediating role of perceived stress. Anxiety Stress Coping 20, 321–329. doi: 10.1080/10615800701282252

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Suresh, S., and Sandhu, D. (2012). Social skills and well-being: the mediating role of positive relations with others. Indian J. Posit. Psychol. 3, 71–74.

Google Scholar

Thompson, C., Mancebo, M. C., and Moitra, E. (2021). Changes in social anxiety symptoms and loneliness after increased isolation during the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychiatry Res. 298:113834. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2021.113834

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsantani, M., Podgajecka, V., Gray, K. L. H., and Cook, R. (2022). How does the presence of a surgical face mask impair the perceived intensity of facial emotions? PLoS One 17:e0262344. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0262344

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Umphrey, L. R., and Sherblom, J. C. (2014). The relationship of hope to self-compassion, relational social skill, communication apprehension, and life satisfaction. Int. J. Wellbeing 4, 1–18. doi: 10.5502/ijw.v4i2.1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Umphrey, L. R., Sherblom, J. C., and Swiatkowski, P. (2021). Relationship of self-compassion, hope, and emotional control to perceived burdensomeness, thwarted belongingness, and suicidal ideation. Crisis 42, 121–127. doi: 10.1027/0227-5910/a000697

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Varma, P., Junge, M., Meaklim, H., and Jackson, M. L. (2021). Younger people are more vulnerable to stress, anxiety and depression during COVID-19 pandemic: a global cross-sectional survey. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 109:110236. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2020.110236

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, D., Crapnell, T., Lau, L., Bennett, A., Lotstein, D., Ferris, M., et al. (2018). “Emerging adulthood as a critical stage in the life course” in Handbook of life course health development. eds. N. Halfon, C. B. Forrest, R. M. Lerner, and E. M. Faustman (New York, NY: Springer), 123–143.

Google Scholar

Wu, C. Y., Mattek, N., Wild, K., Miller, L. M., Kaye, J. A., Silbert, L. C., et al. (2022). Can changes in social contact (frequency and mode) mitigate low mood before and during the COVID-19 pandemic? The I-CONECT project. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 70, 669–676. doi: 10.1111/jgs.17607

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: college students, COVID-19, social–emotional, social–emotional skills, mental health

Citation: Cerutti J, Burt KB, Moeller RW and Seehuus M (2024) Declines in social–emotional skills in college students during the COVID-19 pandemic. Front. Psychol. 15:1392058. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1392058

Received: 26 February 2024; Accepted: 28 June 2024;
Published: 15 July 2024.

Edited by:

Jing Zhang, East China Normal University, China

Reviewed by:

Maura Pilotti, Prince Mohammad Bin Fahd University, Saudi Arabia
Zvjezdan Penezić, University of Zadar, Croatia

Copyright © 2024 Cerutti, Burt, Moeller and Seehuus. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Robert W. Moeller, rmoeller@middlebury.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.