Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 01 July 2024
Sec. Organizational Psychology

Yogi or fireball – or both – a diary study on the interaction between mindfulness and vigor on job performance

  • Department of Work and Organizational Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, FernUniversität Hagen, Hagen, Germany

Introduction: Building upon the conservation of resources theory and the episodic process model of performance, this research addresses the gap in understanding how daily variations in two personal resources, particularly their interaction, affect job performance. Specifically, this study examines the influence of vigor and mindfulness on daily fluctuations in task performance considering the potential compensation effect between these personal resources in the workplace.

Methods: We conducted a five-day online diary study involving 192 participants (926 daily observations). At the conclusion of each workday, participants were asked to assess their level of mindfulness and vigor in the workplace using validated scales, as well as estimate their task performance.

Results: Multilevel analyses showed that both daily mindfulness and daily vigor positively predict self-reported task performance. The interaction between mindfulness and vigor was significant. The results suggest that high levels of mindfulness can compensate for low levels of vigor, and vice versa.

Discussion: Exploring the interplay of personal resources at work provides a valuable starting point for individual-tailored interventions that enable individuals to reach their full potential. Enhancing employees’ mindfulness may increase job performance directly and empowers workers to compensate for periods of low energy.

1 Introduction

The significance of individual resources in the workplace is increasingly acknowledged. This is evident in the numerous and valuable contributions of theories such as the job-demands-resources theory and the conservation of resources theory to the analysis of the origins of stress, well-being, performance, and other critical outcomes in the workplace. Each employee possesses a unique set of personal resources that contribute to their daily functioning at work, enabling them to achieve their work-related goals (Halbesleben et al., 2014). According to the substitution hypothesis of conservation of resources theory, resources can serve as substitutes for each other when they align with environmental demands (Hobfoll et al., 1990). However, existing literature primarily focuses on examining the individual impact of specific resources on work outcomes.

To comprehensively understand the composition of an ideal set of personal resources, it is essential not only to explore their direct associations with work outcomes but also to investigate their dynamic interrelationships (Vermooten et al., 2021). The substitution hypothesis raises the question of which resources have the potential to compensate for each other. Existing research has identified external resources that can compensate for lacking personal resources in the workplace. For example, Ott et al. (2019) demonstrated that perceived organizational support (POS) could compensate for low levels of vigor and self-efficacy in relation to day-level work engagement. Similarly, Venz and Sonnentag (2015) and Venz et al. (2018) found that the strategy of selective optimization with compensation (SOC) can help employees manage a lack of role clarity and insufficient recovery.

However, these studies have not yet addressed whether personal resources can compensate for each other in an entirely internal and dynamic process. Given the increasing demands and stressors in modern work environments, understanding the compensatory effects of personal resources is crucial for enhancing employee well-being and performance. To address this research gap, our study aims to examine the compensatory effect of two well-established personal predictors of job performance, namely mindfulness and energy at work, at the daily level.

For that, we refer to the episodic process model by Beal et al. (2005), which incorporates a dynamic nature of performance. The model proposes that the allocation of resources – especially self-regulatory resources - is the primary mechanism that explains why employees perform well on 1 day and below their average the other day. Few attempts to test the proposition have identified resources such as positive and negative affective states (Merlo et al., 2018), and energetic state (Vahle-Hinz et al., 2021) as predictors of dynamic performance.

Unfortunately, personal energy resources deplete during work as well as during off-time (Dettmers et al., 2020) and are influenced by factors like sleep quality and recovery experiences (Hülsheger, 2016). To counteract the negative impact of low energy states on daily performance, we aim to investigate a potential antidote through the compensatory effect of another personal resource that can be enhanced when needed.

As the potential compensator, we focus on mindfulness – a state of attentiveness and awareness of present-moment experiences (Bishop et al., 2004; Brown et al., 2007). Mindfulness qualifies as a potential moderator, because it has been repeatedly claimed to improve self-regulation (Good et al., 2016). Additionally, it fluctuates naturally within individuals but can also be induced through brief daily mindfulness practices (Hülsheger et al., 2015).

In this study, we propose that the two personal resources – mindfulness and energy - can compensate for each other in regard to job performance because they are both associated with self-regulation and kick off similar cognitive, behavioral, and emotional processes. To test this assumption, we conducted a diary study investigating the joint effects of mindfulness and energetic activation on daily performance.

Our study makes several contributions to the literature. Firstly, we address the largely overlooked interaction between personal resources in the work context, specifically linking mindfulness and energy at work. This is important as it provides insights into the underlying processes through their interactive effect. Secondly, we contribute to understanding the short-term fluctuations of job performance within individuals. To our knowledge, this is the first study to examine state mindfulness and state energy at work as predictors of daily job performance within a common research model. This expands the field of empirical studies on the episodic process model proposed by Beal et al. (2005) and integrates research on mindfulness into the model. These insights are essential for a deeper understanding of how and when employees can reach their full potential at work. Furthermore, examining the interaction between personal resources at work can serve as a valuable starting point for developing tailored strategies to prevent and address issues related to low performance. For instance, it could provide justification for companies to support short mindfulness practices during work hours.

2 Theory and hypotheses development

2.1 Performance variability and vigor

Job performance is defined as employees’ behaviors at work that support organizational goals (Motowildo et al., 1997). In this article, we focus more specifically on task performance, which refers to “behaviors that are recognized by formal reward systems and are part of the requirements as described in job descriptions” (Williams and Anderson, 1991). Factors that predominantly account for differences in static job performance are relatively stable constructs like abilities, knowledge, experience, and non-cognitive traits like personality (Sonnentag et al., 2008). In the analysis of job performance, within-person variance has largely been treated as measurement error (Dalal et al., 2014). However, as postulated by the theory of dynamic performance (Beal et al., 2005), job performance is determined by stable and transient factors, such as day-specific job demands, resources and personal energy levels. Indeed, within-person fluctuations can explain approximately 50% of the variation in job performance (Podsakoff et al., 2019; McCormick et al., 2020).

In their model of dynamic performance, Beal et al. (2005) propose that resource allocation to the task and the direction of one’s attention to the task are central for momentary performance. This requires self-regulatory resources that are not stable but may fluctuate over time. Studies have argued that internal states comprising energy indicate that self-regulatory resources are available at a specific time, which leads to better performance (e.g., Vahle-Hinz et al., 2021). Energy is a sense of positive affective arousal, which includes an eagerness and capability to act (Quinn and Dutton, 2005). It is a concept involved in many motivational theories (Schippers and Hogenes, 2011), as research has shown that highly energized employees tend to be more motivated (Butt et al., 2020) and get work done – resulting in higher productivity (Chen et al., 2016).

To capture employees’ energetic resources, we focus on vigor as a much-studied concept, that captures the energy specifically directed at work and that may vary from episode to episode like job performance (Reis et al., 2016). Vigor is characterized by “high levels of energy and mental resilience while working, the willingness to invest effort in one’s work, and persistence even in the face of difficulties” (Schaufeli et al., 2006). It is one of three constitutive aspects of work engagement, reflecting a positive, fulfilling affective-motivational state of work-related well-being (Schaufeli et al., 2002).

Various studies have demonstrated the fundamental role of the current level of vigor for daily job performance. On a between-person level, Carmeli et al. (2009) have shown that vigorous employees tend to perform better according to supervisor ratings. At a within-person level, this relationship has not been studied decisively. However, related constructs indicating the energetic state have been found to predict task performance at a within-person level, for example, recovery state (Binnewies et al., 2009), fatigue (Dettmers et al., 2020), or the hourly measured energy level (Vahle-Hinz et al., 2021). We therefore propose Hypothesis 1:

Hypothesis 1: Daily vigor is positively related to daily self-reported task performance.

2.2 Mindfulness

Considering personal energy as a critical, limited resource that positively influences daily job performance and can become depleted (Parker et al., 2021), it is essential to explore the incremental impact of other internal states on job performance. Specifically, understanding the interaction between vigor and other internal states and how job performance is affected when vigor is low, is crucial. We focused on mindfulness as an internal state whose key feature is the self-regulation of attention (Good et al., 2016) and leads to higher functioning at work (Glomb et al., 2011). Therefore, we argue that it might be capable of buffering detrimental influences of low energetic states on momentary performance.

Mindfulness is a state of non-judgmental attentiveness and awareness of what is taking place in the present moment, internally and externally (Brown et al., 2007). One way to view mindfulness is as a personal resource that assists people in managing demands by directing their attention on the present moment rather than issues outside of their control (Grover et al., 2017). It has gained increasing attention in the context of work and organizational psychology (van Dam et al., 2018) as research on mindfulness has shown beneficial effects for employee health and performance (Lomas et al., 2017; Mesmer-Magnus et al., 2017). Workplace mindfulness has mainly been studied as a stable personality trait (e.g., Lyddy et al., 2021) or as a skill that can be trained through interventions (for a review, see Creswell, 2017). However, it has been repeatedly argued that mindfulness is at its core a state that naturally fluctuates from moment to moment within persons (state or daily mindfulness; Forjan et al., 2020).

Past studies have employed different operationalizations of mindfulness. However, most conceptualizations distinguish between two central components of mindfulness: awareness and acceptance (Bishop et al., 2004; Sauer et al., 2013). Nonetheless, Brown et al. (2007) argue that clear awareness of internal and external present-moment experiences is the first and foremost aspect of mindfulness. The authors define awareness as being aware of all physical senses as well as emotional and mental activities. In the context of organizational functioning, being aware seems especially valuable as it is a vital prerequisite for staying attentive and present while being engaged in a (work) task (Good et al., 2016). Accordingly, we want to follow most studies on mindfulness at work that measured mindfulness unidimensionally in the form of awareness – particularly when assessing state mindfulness in the workplace (Hülsheger et al., 2013, 2014; Haun et al., 2018).

In general, there is good evidence for mindfulness to play an essential role in performance. Mesmer-Magnus et al. (2017) reported meta-analytical evidence that employees high in trait mindfulness may work harder and perform better than employees low in mindfulness. However, understanding the state mindfulness’ role for organizational outcomes is insufficient (Forjan et al., 2020). Studies on naturally occurring short-term fluctuations of workplace mindfulness and how they relate directly to short-term performance variability are limited. To our knowledge, only Forjan et al. (2020) tested the intraindividual link at the day-level and found that on days when employees experienced heightened mindful awareness, they also reported stronger daily task performance. Similarly, Haun et al. (2020) reported a positive within-person correlation of awareness and goal attainment at work. We want to examine this relationship further and propose the following hypothesis based on these recent study results:

Hypothesis 2: Daily mindfulness is positively related to daily self-reported task performance.

2.3 Interaction of mindfulness and vigor on performance

In this study, we examine state mindfulness and state vigor as separate predictors of daily fluctuations in job performance. However, our main hypothesis suggests an interactive effect between these personal resources, beyond their individual impacts. Specifically, we propose the possibility of a negative interaction, whereby low energetic states can be compensated by high levels of mindfulness. This assumption is grounded in the shared underlying mechanisms through which both influence performance.

The positive effect of vigor on performance may be easily explainable by its energetic function: feeling vigorous, employees can approach their work tasks with more energy (Owens et al., 2016). Energy states indicate how much effort an employee is willing and ready to contribute (Schiuma et al., 2007). Energy is also involved in other motivational processes such as directing one’s effort to a work task and regulating the persistence in carrying it out (Owens et al., 2016). All in all, the capacity for action is increased in vigorous individuals so that work goals are achieved more easily (Owens et al., 2016). However, the linking mechanism is more complex: vigor is a positive experience per se as it is one aspect of positive affect (Shirom, 2011). Thus, notions about the association of positive affect with task performance are applicable.

In more detail, vigor may facilitate task performance through improved self-regulation and different mechanisms on cognitive, behavioral, and emotional levels: concerning self-regulation, some researchers consider energy as the accessibility of glucose in the blood, which determines the momentary capacity to self-regulate behavior (e.g., Gailliot et al., 2007). Following the model of Beal et al. (2005), self-regulation is the crucial factor for dynamic performance. Aside from self-regulation, Shirom (2003) argues that vigor generates a particular thought-action repertoire that expands activity and broadens the range of options. Furthermore, the broaden-and-build theory (Fredrickson, 2001) states that positive affects broaden the range of attention and cognition. Consequently, employees in a positive mood tend to see the big picture, take a holistic perspective, and show more approach tendencies (Diener et al., 2020). They also tend to have expanded cognitive repertoires, which leads to more problem-solving confidence (Forjan et al., 2020), efficient decision-making (Chuang, 2007), and increased creativity (Baas et al., 2008) – all potential facilitators of task performance.

Referring to mindfulness theories (e.g., Glomb et al., 2011; Good et al., 2016), we propose that mindfulness will trigger highly parallel mechanisms positively impacting job performance. Self-regulation of thoughts, emotions, and behavior is considered the central outcome of mindfulness with beneficial effects on performance (Glomb et al., 2011). This assumption is underlined by findings that electrophysiological features of mindfulness are associated with the self-regulation of attention (Verdonk et al., 2020) and the importance of self-regulation of attention to adequately concentrate on a task despite off-task concerns (Beal et al., 2005). Good et al. (2016) also emphasize improvements in stability, control, and efficiency of attention as the linkage between mindfulness and various other improvements in cognitive, emotional, and behavioral domains of functioning. Dane (2011) argues that broad attentional breadth held in the present moment is the key characteristic of mindfulness that influences task performance. In such a broad attentional state, employees are able to focus with due attention to the current task, but without losing their situational awareness (van Gordon et al., 2014). Additionally, mindfulness has been linked to cognitive capacity – especially in terms of improved working memory (Roeser et al., 2013) –, cognitive flexibility (Moore and Malinowski, 2009), and creativity (Baas et al., 2014). Glomb et al. (2011) argue that mindfulness fosters reduced automaticity of mental processes, which creates a mental gap between stimulus and behavioral response. This gives individuals response flexibility, which allows them to consciously choose behavioral responses to attain more adaptive outcomes (Good et al., 2016). In this way, the range and quality of possible reactions increases (Glomb et al., 2011). On a behavioral level, Weinstein et al. (2009) also found that mindful individuals confronted with stressful demands reported more approach than avoidance coping strategies. Furthermore, studies on mindfulness suggest that it leads to more positive and less negative affective reactions and quicker recovery from adverse work events (Glomb et al., 2011). It enables mindful people to avoid automatic, maladaptive emotional reactions and negative affective states by reflecting experiences from a meta-perspective (Malinowski and Lim, 2015). At this point, further beneficial effects of positive affective states may occur.

Considering all this, it becomes salient that mindfulness and vigor share many of the mechanisms in which they influence performance. First, both internal states influence job performance because they improve the capacity to self-regulate. The feeling of vigor indicates that self-regulatory resources are available, whereas mindfulness simplifies the act of self-regulation. This makes it easier for the employee to stay attuned to the current task. Second, vigor and mindfulness are accompanied by similar changes in cognition, behavior, and emotion (Good et al., 2016; Diener et al., 2020). Cognitively, both are associated with flexibility and breadth of mental processes. Behaviorally, both promote approach rather than avoidance strategies. Emotionally, both are either in themselves positive experiences or foster them.

According to the substitution hypothesis of conservation of resources theory (Hobfoll et al., 1990), personal resources are substitutable for each other. Both high state mindfulness and high state vigor can create a mindset that enhances employees’ performance. When this mindset is activated by one of these resources, improvements in performance may not arise from the other resource. However, if an employee is lacking either mindfulness or vigor, it is hypothesized that the other resource can compensate for the deficit and mitigate the decline in performance. Therefore, we assume a compensation effect:

Hypothesis 3: Day-level mindfulness and day-level vigor interact negatively on day-level self-reported task performance, in the form that one compensates for low levels of the other.

3 Materials and methods

To test our hypotheses, we conducted a diary study to capture the daily fluctuating states of vigor and mindfulness at work and related it to self-rated daily job performance. The diary study was conducted over five consecutive workdays. Each day, the participants had to complete short online questionnaires. The study variables were measured after work was finished. The participants received a link to the survey via e-mail according to the individualized schedule provided before the start of the study. After the five workdays, the participants completed a general survey that assessed sociodemographic data.

3.1 Sample

The criteria for study participation included regular employment with a minimum of 30 h per week. As this study was part of a major research project, another inclusion criterion was responsibility for at least one child aged 0–12 years living in the same household. This criterion was unrelated to our specific research question. We employed convenience sampling methods: Potential participants were contacted by social networks, such as XING and Facebook, and via personal contacts. To increase the motivation for participation, we promised to provide general feedback regarding the study results and an invitation to a workshop concerning the reconciliation of work and family duties. Furthermore, the participants received information about data protection regulations and the voluntariness of participation. They were asked to provide a formal informed consent to participate.

In total, 192 participants started the diary surveys resulting in 926 after-work surveys during the 5 days (34 after-work surveys missed). We further analyzed whether the actual measurement points corresponded with the predetermined survey schedule or not (e.g., a participant completing his after-work survey at night or next morning). In the latter case, we set the values of the respective study variables of that measurement point to “missing value.” In total, the dataset contained 848 after-work surveys of 189 participants completed at the correct time. In total, 52% of the participants were females. The mean age was 40 years (SD = 7.5), and 53% had a university degree. The mean working time indicated by the participants was 32 h per week (SD = 12). At the day-level, participants indicated on average 7.1 h.

3.2 Measures

In the daily after-work questionnaire, we assessed task performance, mindfulness state at work, and vigor. All items were assessed on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).

3.2.1 Mindfulness

We measured the mindfulness state at the day-level with four items from the Mindful Attention and Awareness Scale [MAAS; Brown and Ryan, 2003; German translation by Michalak et al. (2008)]. A sample item is “I rushed through activities without being really attentive to them” (reverse scored). Reliability estimates of the daily measures based on Cronbach’s alpha ranged between α = 0.84 and α = 0.88.

3.2.2 Vigor

We measured vigor with the three items vigor subscale of the UWES-9 (Schaufeli et al., 2006). A sample item is: “When I got up this morning, I felt like going to work.” Cronbach’s alpha ranged between α = 0.75 and α = 0.85 over the 5 days.

3.2.3 Task performance

To assess daily task performance, we used four items from Williams and Anderson (1991; e.g., “Today, I adequately completed assigned duties”) to which the participants responded after work (t3). Cronbach’s alpha ranged between α = 0.89 and α = 0.92 over the 5 days.

3.2.4 Control variables

We assessed daily work hours at the end of the workday as daily control variables because they potentially influence job performance (Ng and Feldman, 2008). Employees indicated how many hours they spent at work during the present day (M = 7.08; SD = 1.44). Table 1 displays the means and standard deviations of all study variables at the between and within-person level.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Mean, standard deviations, and intercorrelations of the study variables.

3.3 Preliminary analysis

To examine whether the day-level measures of mindfulness, vigor and task performance were empirically distinct, we conducted Multilevel Confirmatory Factor Analysis (MCFA) via MPlus 8.4 (Muthén and Muthén, 1998–2017). The 3-factor day-level solution displayed acceptable fit to the data, χ2 = 247.99, df = 84, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.95, TLI = 0.93, RMSEA = 0.04, SRMR = 0.04 (within level) and 0.13 (between level).

4 Results

We tested our hypotheses with hierarchical multilevel analysis using R. This study analyzed the relationships among the variables for which data were collected at multiple time points. We were primarily interested in within-person processes, i.e., the relationship in day-specific variation in vigor, mindfulness, and task performance. The share of within-person and between-person variation of the day-level variables derived from intercept-only models was investigated. The variance explained by the day-level (within-person variation) ranged from 46% (mindfulness) to 65% for task performance, indicating that a multilevel approach is justified. We centered the predictor variables vigor and mindfulness at the individual mean (person-mean centering).

To test our hypotheses, we compared several nested models. We tested the improvement of each model over the previous one by using the difference between the respective likelihoods. This difference follows a chi-square distribution, with the degrees of freedom corresponding to the number of parameters added to the model. Furthermore, all null models were tested for autocorrelation and heteroscedasticity. For all hypotheses, the null model included only the intercept controlling for effects of heteroscedasticity using the R package nlme (Pinheiro et al., 2021). For hypotheses H1, H2, H3, we computed a hierarchical regression model. Table 2 displays the results of the analysis.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results of multilevel analysis with task performance as outcome.

The results support hypothesis 1 and hypothesis 2 as vigor (γ = 0.29, p < 0.00) and mindfulness (γ = 0.31, p < 0.00) exhibited a significant within-person effect on task performance. Furthermore, the analysis revealed a significant negative interaction of vigor and mindfulness.

For a more straightforward interpretation of the interaction effect, we conducted simple slope analyses. The results of the simple slope tests (Aiken and West, 1991) indicated that the slopes of vigor on task performance varied between days that participants experienced high mindfulness and days that participants experienced low mindfulness from 0.35 (unstandardized), p < 0.00 (days with low mindfulness) to 0.23 (unstandardized), p = 0.00 (days with high mindfulness). Conversely, the slopes of mindfulness on task performance varied between days where participants experienced high vigor and days where participants experienced low vigor from 0.38 (unstandardized), p < 0.00 (days with low vigor) to 0.24 (unstandardized), p = 0.00 (days with high vigor). Figures 1, 2 illustrate the differences in the simple slopes.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Simple slopes illustrating the vigor-mindfulness interaction on task performance.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Simple slopes illustrating the mindfulness-vigor interaction on task performance.

5 Discussion

This diary study sought to enhance our understanding of the influence of personal resources and their interplay in relation to fluctuating job performance. The present study supports that fluctuating levels of vigor and mindfulness are important internal predictors of daily performance at work. Consistent with hypothesis 1, the participants who reported feeling more vigorous rated their daily performance as better. That is particularly in line with Vahle-Hinz et al. (2021), who demonstrated that episodic energy levels were related to higher levels of self-reported episodic performance throughout one working day. Likewise, consistent with hypothesis 2, participants who experienced more mindfulness evaluated their day-specific performance higher. Thus, we replicate the results of Forjan et al. (2020), who also found a positive relationship of daily mindfulness and task performance. Those relationships had been argued theoretically and found empirically before on trait-level for vigor (Carmeli et al., 2009; Shirom, 2011) and mindfulness (Good et al., 2016; Lomas et al., 2017). However, we identified a shortage of empirical evidence regarding those relationships on the state level. This gap is unfortunate because the direction, shape, and size of a relationship on the state-level can differ from the same relationship on the trait-level (Dalal et al., 2014).

However, the core goal of this study was to test an interactive effect of vigor and mindfulness on task performance. Results suggest that the two personal resources interact, leading to a compensatory effect on performance. More precisely, on days when workers experience lower levels of vigor, the influence of mindfulness on task performance becomes more significant, while on days characterized by lower mindfulness, the impact of vigor on task performance gains greater importance. Put differently, when an employee experiences exhaustion and mindlessness on a particular day, their performance is likely to be negatively affected. Nevertheless, the presence of either vigor or mindfulness alone can compensate for the other, mitigating the current hindrance in state of mind. Consequently, this result reinforces our notion that the mechanisms of how vigor and mindfulness influence performance on a daily level may share important aspects on cognitive, affective and behavioral levels.

Theoretically, those findings add to the theory of dynamic performance (Beal et al., 2005) and its recent empirical tests by Merlo et al. (2018) and Vahle-Hinz et al. (2021). The presence of resources, particularly self-regulatory resources, determines an individual’s current work performance relative to their baseline level. Up to now the availability of self-regulatory resources in the sense of Beal’s model has been measured either by the valence component of affect (positive/negative affect; Merlo et al., 2018) or by its arousal component (energy; Vahle-Hinz et al., 2021). With mindfulness, we introduce a construct to the model that is not primarily an affective state - used as an indicator of self-regulatory resources – but one of which self-regulation is the central outcome (Glomb et al., 2011). This insight opens a new focus on variables to examine in the context of dynamic performance, as various constructs and conditions influence the current ability to self-regulate and therefore perform at one’s best.

Additionally, examining the potential compensatory effect of mindfulness on the relationship between low levels of vigor and task performance is a new research path. Only very limited research has explored same-level interactive effects, let alone compensatory effects, on dynamic performance. As an exception, Vahle-Hinz et al. (2021) found that task significance, as an environmental factor, can compensate for low energy levels in relation to task performance. Building upon this result, we contribute the insight that the detrimental effects of low energy on dynamic task performance can also be compensated for by another personal resource, namely mindfulness. Thus, this finding also supports the resource substitution hypothesis by Hobfoll et al. (1990).

5.1 Limitations and directions for future research

From a methodological point of view, our study has several limitations. First, all our variables were assessed with self-report measures and at the same measurement occasions (end of the working day). Therefore, common method bias may limit the validity of our results (Podsakoff et al., 2003). However, vigor and mindfulness are internal states and, as such, inherently subjective. Thus, they can hardly be assessed with other than self-report measures because of observability issues. To lower the concerns of common method bias that our findings are due to inter-individual traits, we centered the day-level predictor variables vigor and mindfulness at the individual mean (person-mean centering) so that all person-level variance was removed (Sonnentag and Niessen, 2008). Furthermore, the mean correlations of about 0.3 and the interaction effect give us confidence that our findings are not due to common method bias (Siemsen et al., 2010). For job performance, however, other measures exist (e.g., supervisor or co-worker ratings of performance, objective indicators), which are significantly less prone to social desirability in comparison to self-reports (Carpenter et al., 2014). However, in this study, we compare the daily performance rating of a person with their own performance ratings on other days so that self-presentational biases should be less of an issue (Beal et al., 2005). Moreover, employees usually know their daily tasks and objectives best in order to assess how far they have reached their goals, especially in such a short timeframe as 1 day (Kampf et al., 2021). Nevertheless, future research should use diverse measuring sources and separate and multiply measurement points throughout the working day (e.g., vigor and mindfulness in the morning and at noon).

Second, due to our correlational study design, causal conclusions cannot be drawn. Therefore, it remains unclear if vigor and mindfulness indeed increase job performance, as the relationship may also be reciprocal or reversed. For example, employees who observe significant achievements may experience increased vigor, while mindfulness may heighten awareness of minor tasks completed. Future studies could manipulate mindfulness (e.g., by mindfulness interventions before the workday) to investigate if the same results occur.

5.2 Practical implications

There is growing support for the notion that the comprehension of short-timeframe within-person performance variability has important implications regarding personnel development interventions (Dalal et al., 2020). In particular, it could help prevent short-term minimum performance called troughs (Dalal et al., 2014; Good et al., 2016). The results linking variability in both vigor and mindfulness to fluctuations in job performance offer potential avenues for addressing this issue. Both state mindfulness and state vigor independently impact performance, suggesting two approaches to enhance task performance. First, interventions could target the reduction of vigorless days. As day-level recovery is an important predictor of day-level work engagement (Sonnentag et al., 2010), recovery seems to be a fruitful approach for that. Therefore, job design should provide sufficient job resources to counterbalance job demands, reduce strain, and facilitate recovery (Demerouti et al., 2009). While this may raise mean levels of vigor and the frequency of invigorated days, there will always be less energetic days. In this case, mindfulness could function as a compensator. Again, there are possibilities to design jobs for mindful work (see Lawrie et al., 2018) and mindfulness trainings to raise trait mindfulness (Vonderlin et al., 2020). However, in light of our results, promoting the use of mindfulness tools when needed, such as mindfulness meditation practices (Hülsheger et al., 2013) can avert a working day marked by troughs. Just-in-time adaptive interventions could be a means for that (Nahum-Shani et al., 2018).

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found in the article. https://doi.org/10.23668/PSYCHARCHIVES.4974.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Ethics Committee of the Medical School Hamburg. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

JB: Writing – original draft, Formal analysis. JD: Writing – review & editing, Supervision, Project administration, Funding acquisition, Formal analysis, Conceptualization.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This research was supported by grants from the German Federal Ministry of Research and Education (BMBF; Grant-ID: 01FA15119).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Aiken, L. S., and West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: SAGE.

Google Scholar

Baas, M., De Dreu, C. K. W., and Nijstad, B. A. (2008). A meta-analysis of 25 years of mood-creativity research: hedonic tone, activation, or regulatory focus? Psychol. Bull. 134, 779–806. doi: 10.1037/a0012815

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baas, M., Nevicka, B., and Ten Velden, F. S. (2014). Specific mindfulness skills differentially predict creative performance. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 40, 1092–1106. doi: 10.1177/0146167214535813

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Beal, D. J., Weiss, H. M., Barros, E., and MacDermid, S. M. (2005). An episodic process model of affective influences on performance. J. Appl. Psychol. 90, 1054–1068. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.90.6.1054

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Binnewies, C., Sonnentag, S., and Mojza, E. J. (2009). Daily performance at work. Feeling recovered in the morning as a predictor of day-level job performance. J. Organiz. Behav. 30, 67–93. doi: 10.1002/job.541

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., et al. (2004). Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 11, 230–241. doi: 10.1093/clipsy/bph077

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, K. W., and Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 822–848. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., and Creswell, J. D. (2007). Mindfulness: theoretical foundations and evidence for its salutary effects. Psychol. Inq. 18, 211–237. doi: 10.1080/10478400701598298

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Butt, T. H., Abid, G., Arya, B., and Farooqi, S. (2020). Employee energy and subjective well-being: a moderated mediation model. Serv. Ind. J. 40, 133–157. doi: 10.1080/02642069.2018.1563072

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carmeli, A., Ben-Hador, B., Waldman, D. A., and Rupp, D. E. (2009). How leaders cultivate social capital and nurture employee vigor: implications for job performance. J. Appl. Psychol. 94, 1553–1561. doi: 10.1037/a0016429

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carpenter, N. C., Berry, C. M., and Houston, L. (2014). A meta-analytic comparison of self-reported and other-reported organizational citizenship behavior. J. Organiz. Behav. 35, 547–574. doi: 10.1002/job.1909

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, H., Cheng, H., Zhu, D., and Long, R. (2016). Dimensions of employee energy and their differences: evidence from Chinese insurance companies. Hum. Factors Man. 26, 740–753. doi: 10.1002/hfm.20677

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chuang, S.-C. (2007). Sadder but wiser or happier and smarter? A demonstration of judgment and decision making. J. Psychol. 141, 63–76. doi: 10.3200/JRLP.141.1.63-76

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Creswell, J. D. (2017). Mindfulness interventions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 68, 491–516. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-042716-051139

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalal, R. S., Alaybek, B., and Lievens, F. (2020). Within-person job performance variability over short timeframes: theory, empirical research, and practice. Annu. Rev. Organ. Psychol. Organ. Behav. 7, 421–449. doi: 10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-012119-045350

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalal, R. S., Bhave, D. P., and Fiset, J. (2014). Within-person variability in job performance. J. Manag. 40, 1396–1436. doi: 10.1177/0149206314532691

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dane, E. (2011). Paying attention to mindfulness and its effects on task performance in the workplace. J. Manag. 37, 997–1018. doi: 10.1177/0149206310367948

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Demerouti, E., Bakker, A. B., Geurts, S. A., and Taris, T. W. (2009). “Daily recovery from work-related effort during non-work time” in Current perspectives on job-stress recovery. eds. S. Sonnentag, P. L. Perrewé, and D. C. Ganster (Bingley: Emerald Group Publishing Limited), 85–123.

Google Scholar

Dettmers, J., Wendt, C., and Biemelt, J. (2020). Already exhausted when arriving at work? A diary study of morning demands, start-of-work-day fatigue and job performance and the buffering role of temporal flexibility. Eur. J. Work Organ. Psy. 29, 809–821. doi: 10.1080/1359432X.2020.1810666

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E., Thapa, S., and Tay, L. (2020). Positive emotions at work. Annu. Rev. Organ. Psychol. Organ. Behav. 7, 451–477. doi: 10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-012119-044908

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Forjan, D. N., Tuckey, M. R., and Li, Y. (2020). Problem solving and affect as mechanisms linking daily mindfulness to task performance and job satisfaction. Stress. Health 36, 338–349. doi: 10.1002/smi.2931

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology. The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Am. Psychol. 56, 218–226. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.56.3.218

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gailliot, M. T., Baumeister, R. F., DeWall, C. N., Maner, J. K., Plant, E. A., Tice, D. M., et al. (2007). Self-control relies on glucose as a limited energy source: willpower is more than a metaphor. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 92, 325–336. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.2.325

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Glomb, T. M., Duffy, M. K., Bono, J. E., and Yang, T. (2011). Mindfulness at work. Res. Pers. Hum. Resour. Manag. 30, 115–157. doi: 10.1108/S0742-7301(2011)0000030005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Good, D. J., Lyddy, C. J., Glomb, T. M., Bono, J. E., Brown, K. W., Duffy, M. K., et al. (2016). Contemplating mindfulness at work. J. Manag. 42, 114–142. doi: 10.1177/0149206315617003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grover, S. L., Teo, S. T. T., Pick, D., and Roche, M. (2017). Mindfulness as a personal resource to reduce work stress in the job demands-resources model. Stress Health 33, 426–436. doi: 10.1002/smi.2726

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Halbesleben, J. R. B., Neveu, J.-P., Paustian-Underdahl, S. C., and Westman, M. (2014). Getting to the “COR”. J. Manag. 40, 1334–1364. doi: 10.1177/0149206314527130

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haun, V. C., Nübold, A., and Bauer, A. G. (2018). Being mindful at work and at home: buffering effects in the stressor-detachment model. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 91, 385–410. doi: 10.1111/joop.12200

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haun, V. C., Nübold, A., and Rigotti, T. (2020). Being mindful at work and at home: a diary study on predictors and consequences of domain-specific mindfulness. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 25, 315–329. doi: 10.1037/ocp0000263

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hobfoll, S. E., Freedy, J., Lane, C., and Geller, P. (1990). Conservation of social resources: social support resource theory. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 7, 465–478. doi: 10.1177/0265407590074004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hülsheger, U. R. (2016). From dawn till dusk: shedding light on the recovery process by investigating daily change patterns in fatigue. J. Appl. Psychol. 101, 905–914. doi: 10.1037/apl0000104

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hülsheger, U. R., Alberts, H. J. E. M., Feinholdt, A., and Lang, J. W. B. (2013). Benefits of mindfulness at work: the role of mindfulness in emotion regulation, emotional exhaustion, and job satisfaction. J. Appl. Psychol. 98, 310–325. doi: 10.1037/a0031313

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hülsheger, U. R., Feinholdt, A., and Nübold, A. (2015). A low-dose mindfulness intervention and recovery from work: effects on psychological detachment, sleep quality, and sleep duration. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 88, 464–489. doi: 10.1111/joop.12115

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hülsheger, U. R., Lang, J. W. B., Depenbrock, F., Fehrmann, C., Zijlstra, F. R. H., and Alberts, H. J. E. M. (2014). The power of presence: the role of mindfulness at work for daily levels and change trajectories of psychological detachment and sleep quality. J. Appl. Psychol. 99, 1113–1128. doi: 10.1037/a0037702

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kampf, P. H., Hernández, A., and González-Romá, V. (2021). Antecedents and consequences of workplace mood variability over time: a weekly study over a three-month period. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 94, 160–186. doi: 10.1111/joop.12329

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawrie, E. J., Tuckey, M. R., and Dollard, M. F. (2018). Job design for mindful work: the boosting effect of psychosocial safety climate. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 23, 483–495. doi: 10.1037/ocp0000102

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lomas, T., Medina, J. C., Ivtzan, I., Rupprecht, S., Hart, R., and Eiroa-Orosa, F. J. (2017). The impact of mindfulness on well-being and performance in the workplace: an inclusive systematic review of the empirical literature. Eur. J. Work Organ. Psy. 26, 492–513. doi: 10.1080/1359432X.2017.1308924

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyddy, C. J., Good, D. J., Bolino, M. C., Thompson, P. S., and Stephens, J. P. (2021). The costs of mindfulness at work: the moderating role of mindfulness in surface acting, self-control depletion, and performance outcomes. J. Appl. Psychol. 106, 1921–1938. doi: 10.1037/apl0000863

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Malinowski, P., and Lim, H. J. (2015). Mindfulness at work: positive affect, hope, and optimism mediate the relationship between dispositional mindfulness, work engagement, and well-being. Mindfulness 6, 1250–1262. doi: 10.1007/s12671-015-0388-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McCormick, B. W., Reeves, C. J., Downes, P. E., Li, N., and Ilies, R. (2020). Scientific contributions of within-person research in management: making the juice worth the squeeze. J. Manag. 46, 321–350. doi: 10.1177/0149206318788435

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Merlo, K. L., Shaughnessy, S. P., and Weiss, H. M. (2018). Affective influences on within-person changes in work performance as mediated by attentional focus. Eur. J. Work Organ. Psy. 27, 126–139. doi: 10.1080/1359432X.2017.1417258

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mesmer-Magnus, J., Manapragada, A., Viswesvaran, C., and Allen, J. W. (2017). Trait mindfulness at work: a meta-analysis of the personal and professional correlates of trait mindfulness. Hum. Perform. 30, 79–98. doi: 10.1080/08959285.2017.1307842

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Michalak, J., Heidenreich, T., Ströhle, G., and Nachtigall, C. (2008). Die deutsche Version der Mindful Attention and Awareness Scale (MAAS). Psychometrische Befunde zu einem Achtsamkeitsfragebogen. Z. Klin. Psychol. Psychother. 37, 200–208. doi: 10.1026/1616-3443.37.3.200

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, A., and Malinowski, P. (2009). Meditation, mindfulness and cognitive flexibility. Conscious. Cogn. 18, 176–186. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.12.008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Motowildo, S. J., Borman, W. C., and Schmit, M. J. (1997). A theory of individual differences in task and contextual performance. Hum. Perform. 10, 71–83. doi: 10.1207/s15327043hup1002_1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Muthén, L. K., and Muthén, B. O. (1998–2017). Mplus User’s Guide. 8th Edn. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

Google Scholar

Nahum-Shani, I., Smith, S. N., Spring, B. J., Collins, L. M., Witkiewitz, K., Tewari, A., et al. (2018). Just-in-time adaptive interventions (JITAIs) in mobile health. Key components and design principles for ongoing health behavior support. Ann. Behav. Med. 52, 446–462. doi: 10.1007/s12160-016-9830-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ng, T. W. H., and Feldman, D. C. (2008). Long work hours: a social identity perspective on meta-analysis data. J. Organiz. Behav. 29, 853–880. doi: 10.1002/job.536

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ott, A. R., Haun, V. C., and Binnewies, C. (2019). Negative work reflection, personal resources, and work engagement: the moderating role of perceived organizational support. Eur. J. Work Organ. Psy. 28, 110–123. doi: 10.1080/1359432X.2018.1550076

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Owens, B. P., Baker, W. E., Sumpter, D. M., and Cameron, K. S. (2016). Relational energy at work: implications for job engagement and job performance. J. Appl. Psychol. 101, 35–49. doi: 10.1037/apl0000032

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, S. L., Dawson, N., van den Broeck, A., Sonnentag, S., and Neal, A. (2021). Employee motivation profiles, energy levels, and approaches to sustaining energy: a two-wave latent-profile analysis. J. Vocat. Behav. 131:103659. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2021.103659

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S., and Sarkar, D.R Core Team (2021). Nlme: linear and nonlinear mixed effects models. R package version 3.1–152. Available at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme

Google Scholar

Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J.-Y., and Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 88, 879–903. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Podsakoff, N. P., Spoelma, T. M., Chawla, N., and Gabriel, A. S. (2019). What predicts within-person variance in applied psychology constructs? An empirical examination. J. Appl. Psychol. 104, 727–754. doi: 10.1037/apl0000374

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Quinn, R. W., and Dutton, J. E. (2005). Coordination as energy-in-conversation. AMR 30, 36–57. doi: 10.5465/amr.2005.15281422

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reis, D., Arndt, C., Lischetzke, T., and Hoppe, A. (2016). State work engagement and state affect: similar yet distinct concepts. J. Vocat. Behav. 93, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2015.12.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roeser, R. W., Schonert-Reichl, K. A., Jha, A., Cullen, M., Wallace, L., Wilensky, R., et al. (2013). Mindfulness training and reductions in teacher stress and burnout: results from two randomized, waitlist-control field trials. J. Educ. Psychol. 105, 787–804. doi: 10.1037/a0032093

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sauer, S., Walach, H., Schmidt, S., Hinterberger, T., Lynch, S., Büssing, A., et al. (2013). Assessment of mindfulness. Review on state of the art. Mindfulness 4, 3–17. doi: 10.1007/s12671-012-0122-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaufeli, W. B., Bakker, A. B., and Salanova, M. (2006). The measurement of work engagement with a short questionnaire. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 66, 701–716. doi: 10.1177/0013164405282471

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaufeli, W. B., Salanova, M., Gonzalez Roma, V., and Bakker, A. B. (2002). The measurement of engagement and burnout: a two sample confirmatory factor analytic approach. J. Happiness Stud. 3, 71–92. doi: 10.1023/A:1015630930326

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schippers, M. C., and Hogenes, R. (2011). Energy management of people in organizations: a review and research agenda. J. Bus. Psychol. 26, 193–203. doi: 10.1007/s10869-011-9217-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schiuma, G., Mason, S., and Kennerley, M. (2007). Assessing energy within organisations. Meas. Bus. Excell. 11, 69–78. doi: 10.1108/13683040710820764

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shirom, A. (2003). “Feeling vigorous at work? The construct of vigor and the study of positive affect in organizations” in Emotional and physiological processes and positive intervention strategies. eds. P. L. Perrewé and D. C. Ganster (Bingley: Emerald Group Publishing Limited), 135–164.

Google Scholar

Shirom, A. (2011). Vigor as a positive affect at work: conceptualizing vigor, its relations with related constructs, and its antecedents and consequences. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 15, 50–64. doi: 10.1037/a0021853

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Siemsen, E., Roth, A., and Oliveira, P. (2010). Common method Bias in regression models with linear, quadratic, and interaction effects. Organ. Res. Methods 13, 456–476. doi: 10.1177/1094428109351241

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sonnentag, S., Dormann, C., and Demerouti, E. (2010). “Not all days are created equal: the concept of state work engagement” in Work engagement: a handbook of essential theory and research. eds. A. B. Bakker and M. P. Leiter (New York: Psychology Press), 25–38.

Google Scholar

Sonnentag, S., and Niessen, C. (2008). Staying vigorous until work is over. The role of trait vigour, day-specific work experiences and recovery. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 81, 435–458. doi: 10.1348/096317908X310256

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sonnentag, S., Volmer, J., and Spychala, A. (2008). “Job performance” in The SAGE handbook of organizational behavior: volume I-micro approaches (London: SAGE Publications Ltd.), 427–448.

Google Scholar

Vahle-Hinz, T., Bloom, J.De, Syrek, C., and Kühnel, J. (2021). Putting the episodic process model to the test: explaining intraindividual fluctuations in job performance across the working day. J. Bus. Psychol. 36, 71–84. doi: 10.1007/s10869-019-09672-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van Dam, N. T., van Vugt, M. K., Vago, D. R., Schmalzl, L., Saron, C. D., Olendzki, A., et al. (2018). Mind the hype: a critical evaluation and prescriptive agenda for research on mindfulness and meditation. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 13, 36–61. doi: 10.1177/1745691617709589

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van Gordon, W., Shonin, E., Zangeneh, M., and Griffiths, M. D. (2014). Work-related mental health and job performance: can mindfulness help? Int. J. Ment. Health Addiction 80, 129–137. doi: 10.1007/s11469-014-9484-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Venz, L., Pundt, A., and Sonnentag, S. (2018). What matters for work engagement? A diary study on resources and the benefits of selective optimization with compensation for state work engagement. J. Organiz. Behav. 39, 26–38. doi: 10.1002/job.2207

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Venz, L., and Sonnentag, S. (2015). Being engaged when resources are low: a multi-source study of selective optimization with compensation at work. J. Vocat. Behav. 91, 97–105. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2015.09.008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Verdonk, C., Trousselard, M., Canini, F., Vialatte, F., and Ramdani, C. (2020). Toward a refined mindfulness model related to consciousness and based on event-related potentials. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 15, 1095–1112. doi: 10.1177/1745691620906444

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vermooten, N., Malan, J., Kidd, M., and Boonazier, B. (2021). Relational dynamics amongst personal resources: consequences for employee engagement. SA J. Hum. Resour. Manag. 19:1310. doi: 10.4102/sajhrm.v19i0.1310

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vonderlin, R., Biermann, M., Bohus, M., and Lyssenko, L. (2020). Mindfulness-based programs in the workplace: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Mindfulness 11, 1579–1598. doi: 10.1007/s12671-020-01328-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinstein, N., Brown, K. W., and Ryan, R. M. (2009). A multi-method examination of the effects of mindfulness on stress attribution, coping, and emotional well-being. J. Res. Pers. 43, 374–385. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2008.12.008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, L. J., and Anderson, S. E. (1991). Job satisfaction and organizational commitment as predictors of organizational citizenship and in-role behaviors. J. Manag. 17, 601–617. doi: 10.1177/014920639101700305

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: dynamic performance, within-person analysis, vigor, mindfulness, human energy, personal resources, dairy study

Citation: Blume JB and Dettmers J (2024) Yogi or fireball – or both – a diary study on the interaction between mindfulness and vigor on job performance. Front. Psychol. 15:1385674. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1385674

Received: 13 February 2024; Accepted: 10 June 2024;
Published: 01 July 2024.

Edited by:

Kok Ban Teoh, ViTrox College, Malaysia

Reviewed by:

Zi Jian Oh, Kolej Vokasional Batu Lanchang, Malaysia
Kia Hui Gan, INTI International College Penang, Malaysia
Kwang Shean Tan, INTI International College Penang, Malaysia

Copyright © 2024 Blume and Dettmers. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Johanna Barbara Blume, Johanna.blume@fernuni-hagen.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.