Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 04 July 2024
Sec. Developmental Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Early Development of Sound Processing in the Service of Speech and Music Perception View all 11 articles

The influence of temperament and perinatal factors on language development: a longitudinal study

Andrea Balzs,&#x;Andrea Balázs1,2Krisztina Lakatos
&#x;Krisztina Lakatos2*Veronika Harmati-PapVeronika Harmati-Pap1Ildik TthIldikó Tóth2Bence Kas,Bence Kas1,3
  • 1Institute for General and Hungarian Linguistics, HUN-REN Hungarian Research Centre for Linguistics, Budapest, Hungary
  • 2Sound and Speech Perception Research Group, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Psychology, HUN-REN Research Centre for Natural Sciences, Budapest, Hungary
  • 3MTA-ELTE Language-Learning Disorders Research Group, Eötvös Loránd University, Bárczi Gusztáv Faculty of Special Needs Education, Budapest, Hungary

Early language development is characterized by large individual variation. Several factors were proposed to contribute to individual pathways of language acquisition in infancy and childhood. One of the biologically based explaining factors is temperament, however, the exact contributions and the timing of the effects merits further research. Pre-term status, infant sex, and environmental factors such as maternal education and maternal language are also involved. Our study aimed to investigate the longitudinal relationship between infant temperament and early language development, also considering infant gender, gestational age, and birthweight. Early temperament was assessed at 6, 9, 18, 24, and 30 months with the Very Short Form of Infant Behavior Questionnaire (IBQ-R) and the Very Short Form of Early Childhood Behavior Questionnaire (ECBQ). Early nonverbal communication skills, receptive and expressive vocabulary were evaluated with the Hungarian version of The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory (HCDI). Our study adds further evidence to the contribution of infant temperament to early language development. Temperament, infant gender, and gestational age were associated with language development in infancy. Infants and toddlers with higher Surgency might enter communicative situations more readily and show more engagement with adult social partners, which is favorable for communication development. Gestational age was previously identified as a predictor for language in preterm infants. Our results extend this association to the later and narrower gestational age time window of term deliveries. Infants born after longer gestation develop better expressive vocabulary in toddlerhood. Gestational age may mark prenatal developmental processes that may exert influence on the development of verbal communication at later ages.

1 Introduction

Early language development shows great individual variation both in the extension and the expansion of receptive and expressive vocabulary. The range of a 12-month-old, typically developing child’s receptive vocabulary may span from 25 to more than 200 words, and similar variation can be observed in expressive vocabulary (Fenson et al., 2007). Some children utter their first words by the age of 12 months while others only after 18 months. The rate of development begins to even out by the third year of life (Fenson et al., 2007). Previous studies indicated several biological factors influencing the dynamics of linguistic development in early childhood. Gestational age (Barre et al., 2011), birthweight (Stolt et al., 2009), infant sex (Law et al., 2019) and infant temperament (Ishikawa-Omori et al., 2022) were among these factors, however, social class, family history, certain environmental characteristics (AlHammadi, 2017) seem to play a role.

Concerning gestational age, studies suggest that children born very preterm and extremely preterm exhibit delayed language skills compared to full-term children (Foster-Cohen et al., 2007, 2010). In their earlier work, Foster-Cohen et al. (2007) studied 90 preterm children (N = 36 extremely preterm gestational age < 28 weeks, and N = 54 very preterm, gestational age 28–33) and 102 full-term children (gestational age 38–41). The MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory: Words and Sentences (CDI-WS) at 2 years of corrected age was associated with gestational age at birth. Vocabulary size, word use quality, morphological and syntactic complexity were related to longer gestation before birth. An association between gestational age and language outcomes persisted after the authors controlled for child and family factors otherwise related to gestational age. At 4 years (Foster-Cohen et al., 2010), the association between language development and very preterm birth was replicated. These children had significantly poorer linguistic outcomes even after excluding children with neurosensory impairment and statistical control for the effect of social risk. By contrast, Pérez-Pereira et al. (2016) studied language performance at 30 months with the Galician version of the CDI. Comparing low-risk preterm (mean gestational age, GA: 32.60 weeks) and full-term children (GA: 39.84 weeks), they found no significant differences in the language outcomes: word production, MLU and sentence complexity between groups.

However, the third trimester is characterized by important developmental changes in the brain. Shortened gestation, even within the normal term delivery range (greater than 37 weeks), had long-lasting effects on neural development in a healthy, low-risk population (Davis et al., 2011) with lower gray matter density detected by magnetic resonance imaging. These structural differences may lead to variation in later cognitive development as well. Can et al. (2013) identified several brain regions with early white matter and gray-matter concentrations in association with infants’ receptive language ability and expressive language at 12 months. The indicated cerebellum, PLIC/cerebral peduncle, and hippocampus are suggested to be associated with early language development. These brain developmental processes may contribute to the underlying mechanism connecting higher gestational age with better receptive language at 24 months of age in a sample of toddlers born after 32 weeks of gestation (Snijders et al., 2020). The Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study found that children born both early-term and late-preterm had an increased risk for communication impairment at 18 months and for expressive language impairments at 36 months (Stene-Larsen et al., 2014). Thus, we hypothesize that the linguistic performance of a full-term child may also be related to gestational age in a middle-class, term infant sample.

The contribution of birthweight to variations in language development tends to be confounded by preterm status (Stolt et al., 2009; Barre et al., 2011). No effect of birthweight on language outcomes was detected in a sample of Hungarian children on the Hungarian version of CDI-III (Dale et al., 2001; Fenson et al., 2007; Kas et al., 2022) at 2–4 years of age. The sample of 1,424 term children included 9.3% low-birthweight (<2,500 g) children. CDI scores were predicted by children’s age, gender, and parents’ education level, whereas other factors including birthweight, birth problems, number of siblings, birth order, multilingualism, familial net income, and children’s chronic illness did not have significant effects. Individual differences within normal birthweight (>2,500 g) have not yet been linked to language development, however, Full-scale IQ performance was positively associated with birthweight within the normal range (Matte et al., 2001). Marinopoulou et al. (2021) found that the number of words used by children at age 2.5 years was associated with deficits in intellectual functioning at age 7 years. Children who used 50 words or fewer at age 2.5 years had lower scores of Full-scale IQ, verbal comprehension, working memory, and perceptual reasoning at age 7 years. Given the contradictory results and the potential association via IQ, further investigation of the role of birthweight is needed.

Although there is a growing body of research on the role of infant temperament (Ishikawa-Omori et al., 2022), the results are inconclusive. Studies differ in the definitions of temperament, the stage of language development investigated, the age range of the children, the length of data collection, and the set of other variables included in the analyses. The diversity of these parameters makes it difficult to compare the results. Major theories agree that temperament is inherently present at an early age and influences the expression of behaviors related to activity, affectivity, and self-regulation (Goldsmith et al., 1987; Shiner et al., 2012). However, different approaches to temperament use divergent operational definitions and thus operate within somewhat different frameworks. According to Rothbart and Derryberry (1981) and Rothbart (2007), whose approach was applied in the present study, temperament is constitutionally based, can be measured from infancy, and shows a relatively stable pattern extending over the lifetime (Hampson and Goldberg, 2006; Putnam et al., 2008; Kopala-Sibley et al., 2018; Tang et al., 2020). It can be defined as individual differences in reactivity and self-regulation that manifest in emotions, activity, and attention. Temperament is described by 3 major, distinct factors: Positive Emotionality/Surgency, Negative Affectivity and Regulatory Capacity/Effortful Control (see Table 1 for example items assessing the three factors). Buss and Plomin’s (1984) approach shares some of the concepts and behaviors observed, Thomas and Chess (1977) defined rather different temperament types based on nine dimensions of temperament that captured patterns relevant to clinical practice. While these theories consider emotions and affectivity as components of temperament, Goldsmith (1996) sees temperament as the expression and regulation of emotions. Thus, instruments based on one theory or the other may capture different aspects of temperament.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Example items assessing the 3 factors of temperament (effortful control, Surgency, negative affectivity).

Based on Rothbart’s concept, longitudinal positive associations were found between temperament and expressive language skills. Children’s expressive vocabulary and length of utterance at 24 months were associated with Approach and Perceptual Sensitivity measured at 8 and 12 months of age (Davison et al., 2019). The scales of Approach and Perceptual Sensitivity, along with others, contribute to the Surgency factor (Gartstein and Rothbart, 2003). Laake and Bridgett (2014) also reported that a higher Surgency score measured at 10 months was predictive of improved expressive but not receptive language at 14 months. This relationship might be related to higher infant Surgency predicting higher levels of toddler Effortful Control (Putnam et al., 2008), and in turn, Effortful Control was reported to be associated with expressive language (Bruce et al., 2022). Also, as Positive Anticipation contributes to the Surgency factor, the general learning enhancing aspect or/and the social aspect of positive affect might be considered here as well. Kort et al. (2001) reported that positive affect enhanced students’ learning behavior. Yang et al. (2013) found that positive affect was related to better working memory and had a weaker relationship with short-term memory. They suggest that positive affect facilitates controlled cognitive processing, leading to improved learning ability. We may assume that improved learning ability may support language learning as well. Language learning is greatly facilitated by interactions with social partners. Dixon and Smith (2000) claim that individual differences in positive or negative emotionality might moderate the willingness of social partners to enter social dialogs in the first place, thus influencing exposure to language. Ishikawa-Omori et al. (2022) studied receptive and expressive vocabulary at 40 months. They found that two scales contributing to the Negative Affectivity factor, Motor Activation and Perceptual Sensitivity at 18 months predicted language skills at 40 months, however, the associations pointed in opposite directions. Higher scores on Perceptual Sensitivity were related to larger expressive and receptive vocabulary at 40 months, while higher scores on Motor Activation were related to poorer receptive and expressive vocabulary. Garello et al. (2012) also found concurrent negative correlations between Motor Activation and language development in 24- and 30-month-old children.

Early attentional control and the capacity for self-regulation, which consistently loaded on the Effortful Control factor, were associated positively with language development in infancy and early childhood as well. Dixon and Shore (1997) and Dixon and Smith (2000) reported that attentional control, positive affect and emotional stability measured at 13 months predicted the efficiency of language acquisition, including the time of appearance of first words and the time and speed of vocabulary expansion at 20–21 months. Dixon and Smith (2000) explained this pattern of longitudinal association by Rothbart and Bates’s (2007) theory of an early attentional control system, which corresponds to the maturation of the anterior attentional system at the end of the first year. This early attentional control system allows children to voluntarily direct and maintain attention and allows flexibility in awareness. In fact, emergent control of attention indicated by increases in the duration of orientation from 7 to 10 months was found to be associated with advanced language production at 20 months.

In summary, higher Positive Affect and Effortful Control at the end of the first year and the beginning of the second year are associated with better language performance between 1 and 2 years of age. Conversely, a higher score on the Negative Affectivity between 18 and 30 months is associated with poorer language performance between 24 and 40 months. Additionally, Negative Affectivity may influence the rate of expressive language development around the age of 2 and beyond due to a lower likelihood of engaging in social interactions.

The present study focused on examining the role of perinatal variables in addition to temperament in language development and assessing concurrent and longitudinal relationships in a longitudinal design. Both language development and temperament were evaluated repeatedly, allowing for capturing the potentially changing patterns of associations between temperament and language skills. In addition to expressive language and receptive vocabulary, gestural communication was measured. According to the design of CDI, the latter two were assessed up to 18 months (Frank et al., 2021). Regression models were used to determine the effect of temperament, infant gender and perinatal factors.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants

A longitudinal project on early language development was carried out by recruiting 186 families. The inclusion criteria for the present study were that the child was born on time (gestational age > 37 and birthweight >2,500) and was taken to the baby lab at least once. All infants included in the present investigation were of low social risk and were the first-born children of their mothers. As is common in developmental studies with voluntary participants, mothers with higher education were overrepresented, with 75% having college or university degrees. All participants came from metropolitan (Budapest) or agglomeration areas, all children were monolingual. No hearing problems were reported. The sample was ethnically homogeneous Caucasians of Hungarian origin. Families were recruited at the infant’s birth, 4, 9, and 18 months of age (see Table 2). All families received detailed information on the study, and informed consent was obtained. The first wave included 74 middle-class mothers recruited in the HONVED PMC hospital’s maternity ward. Data were collected up to 18 months in this phase. An additional recruitment at 18 months was planned to increase the sample size continuing to the second phase, however, as the dropout rate was higher than previously expected due to the COVID-19 epidemic, additional recruitment of 4- and 9-month-old infants was carried out (see Table 2). The sex ratio and infant characteristics in the participating and the dropout families did not differ significantly. The present data set includes varying numbers of infants at different ages due to the disruption caused by the pandemic breaking out during data collection and preventing families from visiting the child laboratory. The exact numbers of available data at each age are presented in Table 2.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Number of participants at each data collection point in cohorts recruited at different ages.

2.2 Procedures and instruments

According to the original protocol, mothers were to fill in the questionnaires in the baby lab while lab assistants played with the children and administered tests in the passive presence of the mother. However, the Covid-19 pandemic resulted in some mothers completing the questionnaires from their homes online. There were no significant differences in temperament or language between the questionnaires administered before and during the pandemic.

Ethical approval for the study was granted by ETT-TUKEB (1942-12/2016) and EPKEB (77/2015).

2.2.1 Measurements of child temperament

Infant temperament was assessed using the Very Short Forms of the Infant Behavior Questionnaire (IBQ–R) (Putnam et al., 2014) and the Early Childhood Behavior Questionnaire (ECBQ) (Putnam et al., 2006). Mothers completed the 37-item IBQ–R and 36-item ECBQ (Hungarian versions: Lakatos et al., 2010) either in the baby lab or online at home. The IBQ–R was administered at 6 and 9 months of infant’s age, whereas the ECBQ was at 18, 24 and 30 months. Mothers rated the frequency of their infants’ behaviors over the past two weeks using seven-point Likert scales. Three main factors were computed: Surgency, Effortful Control, and Negative Affectivity. Cronbach alpha coefficients of internal consistency for these factors in this sample were between 0.607 and 0.805 (see Table 3). Missing data were not substituted.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Cronbach’s alpha coefficients of internal consistency of IBQ-R-SF and ECBQ-SF factors.

2.2.2 Measurements of language and communication skills

For the assessment of early language development, the Hungarian adaptation of the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory (CDI) Words & Gestures and Words & Sentences parent report forms (Fenson et al., 2007) has been used (Kas et al., 2010, 2022). This questionnaire relies on maternal (caregiver) reports to explore children’s receptive and expressive vocabulary and assess their level of speech comprehension, gesture use, morpheme acquisition, and syntactic complexity through systematic questions. CDI forms are suitable for assessing language development in typically developing children aged 8–30 months or older with developmental disorders. The present study considers the following CDI variables: (1) receptive vocabulary total score, (2) expressive vocabulary total score, and (3) gestures total score including sub-scores of object manipulation, imitation of adults, symbolic activity, and non-verbal gesture use. The CDI was first administered at 9 months of age, followed by a second administration at 12 months. Thereafter, the course of language development was monitored at two-month intervals until the age of 30 months (Figure 1). For the present report, language data from 18, 24, and 30 months was included in the analyses. Eighteen months of age represents a major turning point in language development, as this is the last age when all 3 dimensions of the CDI (receptivity, expression and gesture) are assessed. Twenty-four-month language data was included because it showed the highest variability. Thirty-month expressive language as measured by CDI was also characterized by good variability. Temperament was also assessed at these ages, thus concurrent associations can be examined.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. CDI expressive vocabulary scores by age.

2.2.3 Analyses

Data were analyzed with IBM SPSS (Version 26). Descriptive measures of linguistic variables obtained from H-CDI (Receptive and Expressive Vocabulary and Communicative Gestures) and of the temperament variables obtained from IBQ-R and ECBQ (Surgency, Effortful Control, Negative Affectivity), perinatal variables, and infant sex were calculated (Table 4). According to the results of Shapiro–Wilk tests, parametric and non-parametric tests were carried out in the analyses. Sex differences were investigated for all variables. To examine the contribution of temperament, perinatal factors to the individual variation in language development, we first analyzed correlations of these variables with language development at various ages (see Table 5). Variables with significant associations were entered as predictors in stepwise linear regression analysis to determine their predictive value on the dependent linguistic variables, such as receptive and expressive vocabulary and communicative gestures at 18 months of age, and expressive vocabulary at 24 and 30 months of age.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Descriptive statistics of and differences by infant sex in perinatal factors, temperament scales, and language skills.

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Correlations between perinatal factors, temperament, and language.

3 Results

3.1 Descriptive statistics

Means and standard deviations for the whole sample, and for boys and girls separately are presented in Table 4. The age-related growth of expressive vocabulary between 9 and 30 months is depicted in Figure 1.

One-way ANOVA or Kruskal-Wallis tests were applied to assess sex differences. No gender differences were observed in gestational age, but boys were significantly heavier at birth [H(1, n = 179] = 20.394, p < 0.001). Likewise, no significant differences between boys and girls appeared on temperament scales at any age, apart from a statistical trend towards boys scoring higher on Surgency at 9 months [F(1,119) = 2.891, p = 0.092]. However, significant sex differences were found in CDI language scores (Table 4). Girls scored higher on all CDI sub-scales at most time points, except for receptive vocabulary at 18 months [H(1, n = 147] = 2.939, p = 0.086), yet with a tendency in favor of girls.

3.2 Correlations with language scores

Bivariate relationships between language development and temperament, demographic and perinatal variables were explored by correlation analyses (Table 5) to select variables for regression analyses predicting language outcomes.

Concurrent correlations were investigated at 18, 24 and 30 months. 18-month Surgency showed a consistent relationship with all measures of language development: higher Surgency was related to better language skills. Better Effortful Control was significantly related to more developed use of gestures and there was a tendency toward better receptive vocabulary. At 24 months, higher Surgency was related to better expressive vocabulary. At 30 months, the association between these two measures only showed a trend-level correlation, however, in the same direction as at earlier ages.

Longitudinal correlations were weak and sparse, however, Surgency at various ages tended to be related to measures of language and communicative development at later ages. Higher Surgency at 9 months was related to higher receptive vocabulary and gesture use at 18 months. Similarly, positive associations appeared between 18-month Surgency and expressive vocabulary at 24 months, and 24-month Surgency and expressive vocabulary at 30 months, with higher Surgency being related to a more extensive expressive vocabulary. In addition, 18-month receptive vocabulary and gesture use were associated with 9-month Effortful Control. Higher Negative Affectivity at 6 months was significantly correlated with more developed expressive vocabulary at 24 months. However, later Negative Affectivity (at 18 months) had the opposite relationship with expressive vocabulary at 30 months: more negative affect was associated with lower expressive vocabulary a year later.

Of the temperament factors in infancy and early childhood, Surgency seems to be indicated in language acquisition both concurrently and longitudinally, spanning from receptive to expressive language.

3.3 Longitudinal predictors of language development

To better understand how temperament and perinatal factors affect each language and communication skill measured by the H-CDI, we conducted linear regression analyses with stepwise selection separately for each CDI variable at 18, 24 and 30 months. Temperament variables of preceding ages, perinatal variables, and infant sex showing significant correlations with the predicted variable were included in the regression.

First, we examined receptive vocabulary at 18 months (Table 6). Birthweight, Surgency and Effortful Control at 9 months were entered in the model. In the final model [R2=0.089, F(1,104) = 10.161, p = 0.002], the single significant predictor was Surgency measured at 9 months (β = 0.298, p = 0.002).1

Table 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Receptive vocabulary at 18 months was predicted by 9-month Surgency.

In the model predicting gestures at 18 months, sex, Surgency, and Effortful Control at 9 months were entered (see Table 7). In the final model [R2=0.111, F(2,103) = 7.559, p = 0.001], predictive variables were Surgency measured at 9 months (β = 0.272, p = 0.004) and sex (β = −0.275, p = 0.004).2

Table 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Gestures at 18 months were predicted by infant sex and Surgency (9 months).

To predict expressive vocabulary at 18 months, only sex and gestational age were entered, as no temperament variable showed a significant correlation with this language outcome (see Table 8). Here, only one model was generated [R2=0.076, F(1,143) = 11.778, p < 0.001], with gestational age reaching significance (β = 0.276, p < 0.001).3

Table 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Expressive vocabulary at 18 months was predicted by gestational age.

Table 9 presents the regression model predicting expressive vocabulary at 24 months, in which sex, gestational age, Surgency and Negative Affectivity at 18 months were entered. In the final model [R2=0.112, F(2,129) = 8.163, p < 0.001], gestational age (β = 0.248, p = 0.004) and Negative Affectivity measured at 18 months (β = −0.199, p = 0.019) were the significant predictors.4

Table 9
www.frontiersin.org

Table 9. Expressive vocabulary at 24 months was predicted by gestational age and negative affectivity (18 months).

Infant sex, birthweight, Negative Affectivity at 6 months, and Surgency at 24 months were entered into the regression to predict expressive vocabulary at 30 months (see Table 10). In the final model [R2=0.088, F(1,64) = 6.152, p = 0.016], the only significant contributor was Surgency at 24 months5 (β = 0.296, p = 0.016).

Table 10
www.frontiersin.org

Table 10. Expressive vocabulary at 30 months was predicted by Surgency at 24 month.

4 Discussion

Our study aimed to investigate the longitudinal relationship between infant temperament and early language development, also considering infant sex and gestational age. Several data collection points for temperament (6–30 months) and language development (18–30 months) were included. Our findings support the role of both infant temperament and perinatal factors in early language development. Nine-month Surgency forecasted receptive vocabulary at 18 months and also contributed to gestural communication at 18 months in addition to infant sex. Gestational age predicted expressive vocabulary at 18 and 24 months. In addition, Negative Affectivity at 18 months also contributed to 24-month expressive vocabulary. Thirty-month expressive vocabulary was predicted by Surgency measured at 24 months.

While Surgency appears to have a significant influence on receptive language and gestures at 18 months, and expressive vocabulary at 30 months, there was a lack of association with expressive vocabulary at 18 and 24 months. Instead, expressive vocabulary at these ages was related to gestational age. Thus, there seems to be a discontinuity in the effect of Surgency, with the emergence of gestational age. Bates et al. (1992) describe increases in vocabulary and grammar along with increases in synaptic density and brain metabolism between the ages of 16–30 months. These brain developmental processes might not be independent of prenatal brain development potentially marked by gestational age. This may be reflected in gestational age predicting 18- and 24-month expressive vocabulary. Surgency, however, may play a role in the expansion of gestural and verbal communication via potentially increased exposure to communicative signals and engagement in social interaction (Laake and Bridgett, 2014). This may be reflected in the association with a more extensive receptive vocabulary and gestures at 18 months, and expressive language at a later age (30 months), when verbal communication is established in most of the children.

4.1 Surgency

Several studies have linked positive affectivity with language development in infancy and early childhood (Laake and Bridgett, 2014; Pérez-Pereira et al., 2016; Davison et al., 2019). Positive affectivity contributes to the Surgency factor in Rothbart’s temperament model (Gartstein and Rothbart, 2003). Laake and Bridgett found that 10-month-old infants with higher Positive Affectivity/Surgency, as measured by IBQ-R, showed improved expressive language at 14 months. Davidson’s study also supported these findings, as infant Positive Affectivity/Surgency measured at 8 and 12 months predicted expressive language skills at 24 months.

Consistent with the literature, we also found Surgency to be related to early language skills. Surgency at 9 months predicted receptive vocabulary and gesture use at 18 months, while Surgency measured at 24 months was a significant contributor to expressive vocabulary at 30 months. Of concurrent associations between Surgency and language measures, only correlations with 18-month receptive vocabulary and gesture use remained significant after Bonferroni correction. However, at least a trend-level association with concurrent Surgency pointing in the same direction can be observed for expressive vocabulary at all ages. Thus, infants with higher Surgency scores demonstrated better language abilities, both in terms of receptive and expressive language. These results suggest that Surgency may be related to language development over an extended period. Since there is some stability in Surgency over time (correlations among Surgency values measured between 9–30 months ranged between 0.358–0.694), temperament can be expected to show a weak longitudinal correlation with expressive communication.

As children with high Positive Affectivity/Surgency are more likely to engage in and elicit social interactions, they have more opportunities to practice and improve their expressive language skills (Laake and Bridgett, 2014). This assumption could also apply to gesture use and receptive language, as both are related to expressive language use. Extensive social interactions provide more opportunities not only for the use of expressive vocabulary but also for gestural communication. More social interactions may result in varied, and increased amounts of language stimuli, fostering the development of language skills.

4.2 Effortful control

In our study, Effortful Control was not a significant predictor of language development in the regression models. Only weak correlations were observed between Effortful Control at 9 months and gesture use and receptive vocabulary at 18 months. Medium concurrent correlation with gesture use was also observed at 18 months.

The link between effortful control and language development remains unclear, despite some studies (Salley and Dixon, 2007; Keller et al., 2016) suggesting a positive relationship that could potentially be attributed to varying attentional capacities, which are thought to support language acquisition (Snijders et al., 2020). Effortful Control, as measured by Rothbart’s temperament questionnaires, is related to the functioning of the executive network (Posner et al., 2016). In turn, a link was demonstrated between the executive network and language development, production, and comprehension (Ye and Zhou, 2009; Shokrkon and Nicoladis, 2022). Furthermore, language development may also contribute to executive function development and self-regulation (Roben et al., 2013; Bruce et al., 2023).

However, Bruce et al. (2022) found that Effortful Control was only related to concurrent language, and 10-month Orienting/Effortful Control did not predict 24-month expressive language. Similarly, Ishikawa-Omori et al. (2022) did not find a predictive link between Effortful Control at 18 months and language development at 40 months. Keller et al. (2016) only demonstrated a significant relationship in the second language competence of dual language learners in childhood. The lack of predictive power of Effortful Control preceding the age of language assessment in the regression models and the separately observed concurrent correlation are in line with these results.

4.3 Negative affectivity

Negative Affectivity was entered in regressions at 24 and 30 months, however, only 18-month Negative Affectivity proved to be a significant predictor for lower expressive vocabulary at 24 months. This result supports earlier findings that Negative Affectivity may be associated with worse language skills (Dixon and Smith, 2000; Garello et al., 2012; Ishikawa-Omori et al., 2022). For instance, Garello et al. found that at the ages of 24–30 months, increased Negative Emotionality and Motor Activity correlated with poorer language production and comprehension. Similarly, Ishikawa-Omori et al. (2022) reported that Motor Activity, a scale of the Negative Affectivity factor, measured at 18 months, predicted lower expressive and receptive language skills at 40 months. They suggested that fidgeting behavior may reduce the availability of attentional resources, and as a result, it could hinder language learning. Excessive negative emotions could limit the resources children can allocate for information processing and language learning. They may also influence the way the children and their social partners interact. Children displaying more negative affect indeed performed worse on a joint attention task at 21 months (Salley and Dixon, 2007).

4.4 Gestational age

Gestational age proved to be a significant predictor for expressive vocabulary at 18 and 24 months. It’s been well-documented that both preterm birth and low birthweight can negatively impact language development into school age and beyond (Husby et al., 2023). Emerging findings, however, suggest variation in the development of term-born children, indicating differing developmental trajectories for early-term, full-term, late-term and post-term children (MacKay et al., 2010; Espel et al., 2014; Bentley et al., 2016; Snijders et al., 2020; Dhamrait et al., 2021). Our results suggest that longer in-utero development may support the development of expressive language. The final weeks of intrauterine development are characterized by rapid brain development. Children born early-term will not benefit from the effect of uterine neurosteroids (Hüppi et al., 1998; Limperopoulos et al., 2005; Shaw et al., 2019) as long as children born at later gestational ages. Increasing evidence shows long-lasting brain structure differences in preterm infants (Inder et al., 2005; Rogers et al., 2018). For instance, variations in functional connectivity were present even in adolescence after preterm birth, suggesting distinctive neurodevelopment potentially underlying behavioral differences (Lubsen et al., 2011). Term-born infants’ brain development also seems to benefit from longer gestation, within the time window of 37–41st weeks. Gestational age was related to differences in brain development in school-age children (Davis et al., 2011; Nivins et al., 2023). Such a variation may contribute to the observed differences in cognitive functioning and language skills (Ma et al., 2022).

4.5 Infant sex

Other than temperament and gestational age, infant sex also seems to contribute to variations in language skills. Previous studies have shown sex differences in language acquisition (Eriksson et al., 2012; Law et al., 2019), which aligns with our findings. Except for receptive vocabulary at 18 months, girls performed significantly better on all language measures and infant sex predicted the use of gestures at 18 months. Although we have only assessed gestures at 18 months, previous studies found girls using more gestures and starting earlier than boys (Özçalişkan and Goldin-Meadow, 2010; Germain et al., 2022).

4.6 Conclusion

Our aim was to investigate the role of temperament and some perinatal and maternal characteristics on early language development in a sample of low-social-risk, first-born term infants. Our sample was rather homogeneous as all participants were Caucasian of Hungarian origin, and the maternal education level was generally high across the sample. Results indicate the contribution of Surgency both concurrently and longitudinally on various measures of language development and the influence of gestational age on expressive vocabulary at 18 and 24 months. Negative Affectivity only predicted expressive vocabulary at 24 months. Despite Effortful Control being correlated with 18-month language, it was not a significant predictor in the regression models.

However, a major limitation of the study was the sample size and the missing data due to the pandemic. The COVID-19 pandemic hit during data collection and caused unexpected loss of data (data collection could not be conducted due to closures) and thus a higher dropout rate. Although the pandemic had no direct influence on the data presented (no differences were observed on any measures between data collected pre-pandemic and pandemic, post-pandemic periods), there might be hidden underlying effects of the quarantine period. Only 3 families reported contracting COVID-19 during the data collection. Thus, we may assume that results were not influenced by the neurological effects of the viral infection. Another limitation was the relatively low reliability of some temperament factors (Surgency at 9 months: 0.61, Negative Affectivity at 24 months: 0.63). Since correlations of the Surgency factor were consistent with those of other ages (albeit weak across the board), we have decided to include it in the regression analyses.

Our results extend previous findings as we have demonstrated associations with Surgency at the early stages of language acquisition for both receptive and expressive vocabulary, and showed the additional significant contribution of gestational age and Negative Affectivity. Gestational age was identified as a predictor for language in preterm infants previously. Our results extend this association to the narrower time window of gestational age of full-term infants. The latter finding may have relevance for medical practice and child educational support agencies. In line with other studies highlighting difficulties in later academic performance, this calls for increased attention to the early development of early-term and term infants.

4.7 Future directions

Our results highlight the importance of longitudinal studies using tools to measure temperament based on the same theoretical concept over time. Also, investigating the small differences in gestational age in term infants in a larger sample may reveal important effects on language acquisition. With more evidence on how early-term status may influence later cognitive and language development, research on how certain environmental factors, such as socioeconomic status, maternal education, and quality of mother–child interaction might interact with gestational age can yield important results that can be translated into practices supporting early childhood development.

Extending the study beyond 30 months is crucial to identify early characteristics of developmental pathways leading to language impairment. The role of Surgency and the relative lack of power for Effortful Control in this sample calls for experimental investigation of the development of very early executive functions and attentional functioning.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Medical Research Council, Scientific and Research Ethics Committee (ETT-TUKEB) United Ethical Review Committee for Research in Psychology (EPKEB). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

AB: Formal analysis, Investigation, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Data curation. KL: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. VH-P: Investigation, Writing – review & editing. IT: Data curation, Investigation, Project administration, Writing – review & editing. BK: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was funded by the Hungarian National Research Development and Innovation Office (Grant: K124477 to BK).

Acknowledgments

The first phase of the study providing infant temperament data was funded by the grant K115385 awarded to I. Winkler. We are grateful for making these data available. The authors thank Judit Rochéné Farkas, Lívia Őzéné Kende, Zsófia Haraszti, Tamara Lakatos, Ágnes Kata Szerafin, Kinga Kerner, Anna Radányi, Blanka Horváth, Beatrix Uhrin, Rebeka Szekeres, and Eszter Rozgonyiné Lányi for their contribution in data collection.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

1. ^Multicollinearity was not detected (birthweight, Tolerance = 0.99, VIF = 1.00; Surgency at 9 months, Tolerance = 1.00 VIF = 1.00; Effortful Control at 9 months, Tolerance = 0.84, VIF = 1.19).

2. ^Multicollinearity was not detected (sex, Tolerance = 0.98, VIF = 1.02; Surgency at 9 months, Tolerance = 0.98, VIF = 1.02; Effortful Control at 9 months, Tolerance = 0.83, VIF = 1.21).

3. ^Multicollinearity was not detected (gestational age, Tolerance = 1.00, VIF = 1.00; sex, Tolerance = 1.00, VIF = 1.00).

4. ^Multicollinearity was not detected (gestational age, Tolerance = 0.98, VIF = 1.01; Negative Affectivity at 18 months, Tolerance = 0.98, VIF = 1.01; sex, Tolerance = 0.98, VIF = 1.01; Surgency at 18 months, Tolerance = 0.97, VIF = 1.03).

5. ^Multicollinearity was not detected (Surgency at 24 months, Tolerance = 1.00, VIF = 1.00; sex, Tolerance = 0.98, VIF = 1.02; birthweight, Tolerance = 0.99, VIF = 1.00; Negative Affectivity at 6 months, Tolerance = 0.98 VIF = 1.02).

References

AlHammadi, F. S. (2017). Prediction of child language development: A review of literature in early childhood communication disorders. Lingua 199, 27–35. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2017.07.007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barre, N., Morgan, A., Doyle, L. W., and Anderson, P. J. (2011). Language abilities in children who were very preterm and/or very low birthweight: a meta-analysis. J. Pediatr. 158, 766–774.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2010.10.032

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, E., Thal, D., and Janowsky, J. S. (1992). Early language development and its neural correlates. In Handbook of neuropsychology. (eds.) S. J. Segalowitz and I. Rapin Vol. 7. Child neuropsychology. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.

Google Scholar

Bentley, J. P., Roberts, C. L., Bowen, J. R., Martin, A. J., Morris, J. M., and Nassar, N. (2016). Planned birth before 39 weeks and child development: a population-based study. Pediatrics 138:e20162002. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-2002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruce, M., Ermanni, B., and Bell, M. A. (2023). The longitudinal contributions of child language, negative emotionality, and maternal positive affect on toddler executive functioning development. Infant Behav. Dev. 72:101847. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101847

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruce, M., McFayden, T. C., Ollendick, T. H., and Bell, M. A. (2022). Expressive language in infancyand toddlerhood: the roles of child temperament and maternal parenting behaviors. Dev. Psychobiol. 64:e22287. doi: 10.1002/dev.22287

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Buss, A. H., and Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: Early developing personality traits. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Can, D. D., Richards, T., and Kuhl, P. K. (2013). Early gray-matter and white-matter concentration in infancy predict later language skills: a whole brain voxel-based morphometry study. Brain Lang. 124, 34–44. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.10.007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D. J., and Marchman, V. A. (2001). A parent report measure of language development for three-year-olds. Columbia, MO: University of Missouri-Columbia.

Google Scholar

Davis, E. P., Buss, C., Muftuler, L. T., Head, K., Hasso, A., Wing, D. A., et al. (2011). Children’s brain development benefits from longer gestation. Front. Psychol. 2:1. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Davison, L., Warwick, H., Campbell, K., and Gartstein, M. A. (2019). Infant temperament affects toddler language development. J. Educ. e-Learn. Res. 6, 122–128. doi: 10.20448/journal.509.2019.63.122.128

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dhamrait, G. K., Christian, H., O’Donnell, M., and Pereira, G. (2021). Gestational age and child development at school entry. Sci. Rep. 11:14522. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-93701-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixon, W. E. Jr., and Shore, C. (1997). Temperamental predictors of linguistic style during multiword acquisition. Infant Behav. Dev. 20, 99–103. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(97)90065-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixon, W. E., and Smith, P. H. (2000). Links between early temperament and language acquisition. Merrill-Palmer Q. 46, 417–440.

Google Scholar

Eriksson, M., Marschik, P. B., Tulviste, T., Almgren, M., Pérez Pereira, M., Wehberg, S., et al. (2012). Differences between girls and boys in emerging language skills: evidence from 10 language communities. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 30, 326–343. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02042.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Espel, E. V., Glynn, L. M., Sandman, C. A., and Davis, E. P. (2014). Longer gestation among children born full term influences cognitive and motor development. PLoS One 9:e113758. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0113758

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D., Dale, P., Reznick, J. S., and Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur-Bates communicative development inventories: User's guide and technical manual. 2nd Edn. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing Co.

Google Scholar

Foster-Cohen, S., Edgin, J., Champion, P., and Woodward, L. (2007). Early delayed language development in very preterm infants: evidence from the MacArthur-Bates CDI. J. Child Lang. 34, 655–675. doi: 10.1017/S0305000907008070

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Foster-Cohen, S. H., Friesen, M. D., Champion, P. R., and Woodward, L. J. (2010). High prevalence/low severity language delay in preschool children born very preterm. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 31, 658–667. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181e5ab7e

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Frank, M. C., Braginsky, M., Yurovsky, D., and Marchman, V. A. (2021). Variability and Consistency in Early Language Learning: The Wordbank Project. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Garello, V., Viterbori, P., and Usai, M. C. (2012). Temperamental profiles and language development: A replication and an extension. Infant Behav. Dev. 35, 71–82. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.09.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gartstein, M. A., and Rothbart, M. K. (2003). Studying infant temperament via the revised infant behavior questionnaire. Infant Behav. Dev. 26, 64–86. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00169-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Germain, N., Gonzalez-Barrero, A. M., and Byers-Heinlein, K. (2022). Gesture development in infancy: effects of gender but not bilingualism. Infancy 27, 663–681. doi: 10.1111/infa.12469

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldsmith, H. H. (1996). Studying temperament via construction of the toddler behavior assessment questionnaire. Child Dev. 67, 218–235. doi: 10.2307/1131697

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldsmith, H. H., Buss, A. H., Plomin, R., Rothbart, M. K., Thomas, A., Chess, S., et al. (1987). Roundtable: what is temperament? Four approaches. Child Dev. 58, 505–529. doi: 10.2307/1130527

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hampson, S. E., and Goldberg, L. R. (2006). A first large cohort study of personality trait stability over the 40 years between elementary school and midlife. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 91, 763–779. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.763

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hüppi, P. S., Warfield, S., Kikinis, R., Barnes, P. D., Zientara, G. P., Jolesz, F. A., et al. (1998). Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging of brain development in premature and mature newborns. Ann. Neurol. 43, 224–235. doi: 10.1002/ana.410430213

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Husby, A., Wohlfahrt, J., and Melbye, M. (2023). Gestational age at birth and cognitive outcomes in adolescence: population based full sibling cohort study. BMJ 380:e072779. doi: 10.1136/bmj-2022-072779

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Inder, T. E., Warfield, S. K., Wang, H., Hüppi, P. S., and Volpe, J. J. (2005). Abnormal cerebral structure is present at term in premature infants. Pediatrics 115, 286–294. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-0326

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishikawa-Omori, Y., Nishimura, T., Nakagawa, A., Okumura, A., Harada, T., Nakayasu, C., et al. (2022). Early temperament as a predictor of language skills at 40 months. BMC Pediatr. 22, 1–10. doi: 10.1186/s12887-022-03116-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kas, B., Jakab, Z., and Lőrik, J. (2022). Development and norming of the Hungarian CDI-III: A screening tool for language delay. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 57, 252–273. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12686

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kas, B., Lőrik, J., Andrea, S. V., and Henrietta, K. K. (2010). A korai nyelvi fejlődés új vizsgálóeszköze, a MacArthur-Bates Kommunikatív Fejlődési Adattár (KOFA) bemutatása és validitási vizsgálata. Gyógypedagógiai Szemle 38, 114–125.

Google Scholar

Keller, K., Troesch, L. M., Loher, S., and Grob, A. (2016). The relation between effortful control and language competence—A small but mighty difference between first and second language learners. Front. Psychol. 7:1015. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01015

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kopala-Sibley, D. C., Olino, T., Durbin, E., Dyson, M. W., and Klein, D. N. (2018). The stability of temperament from early childhood to early adolescence: A multi-method, multi-informant examination. Eur. J. Personal. 32, 128–145. doi: 10.1002/per.2151

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kort, B., Reilly, R., and Picard, R. W. (2001). “An affective model of interplay between emotions and learning: reengineering educational pedagogy-building a learning companion” in Proceedings IEEE international conference on advanced learning technologies, 43–46.

Google Scholar

Laake, L. M., and Bridgett, D. J. (2014). Happy babies, chatty toddlers: infant positive affect facilitates early expressive, but not receptive language. Infant Behav. Dev. 37, 29–32. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.12.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lakatos, K., Tóth, I., and Gervai, J. (2010). Csecsemő Viselkedési Kérdőív és Kora Gyermekkori Viselkedési Kérdőív Available at: https://research.bowdoin.edu/rothbart-temperament-questionnaires/instrument-descriptions/the-early-childhood-behavior-questionnaire/

Google Scholar

Law, J., Clegg, J., Rush, R., Roulstone, S., and Peters, T. J. (2019). Association of proximal elements of social disadvantage with children's language development at 2 years: an analysis of data from the children in focus (CiF) sample from the ALSPAC birth cohort. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 54, 362–376. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12442

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Limperopoulos, C., Soul, J. S., Gauvreau, K., Huppi, P. S., Warfield, S. K., Bassan, H., et al. (2005). Late gestation cerebellar growth is rapid and impeded by premature birth. Pediatrics 115, 688–695. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-1169

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lubsen, J., Vohr, B., Myers, E., Hampson, M., Lacadie, C., Schneider, K. C., et al. (2011). Microstructural and functional connectivity in the developing preterm brain. Semin. Perinatol. 35, 34–43. doi: 10.1053/j.semperi.2010.10.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, Q., Wang, H., Rolls, E. T., Xiang, S., Li, J., Li, Y., et al. (2022). Lower gestational age is associated with lower cortical volume and cognitive and educational performance in adolescence. BMC Med. 20:424. doi: 10.1186/s12916-022-02627-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

MacKay, D. F., Smith, G. C. S., Dobbie, R., and Pell, J. P. (2010). Gestational age at delivery and special educational need: retrospective cohort study of 407,503 schoolchildren. PLoS Med. 7:e1000289. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000289

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marinopoulou, M., Billstedt, E., Lin, P. I., Hallerbäck, M., and Bornehag, C. G. (2021). Number of words at age 2.5 years is associated with intellectual functioning at age 7 years in the SELMA study. Acta Paediatr. 110, 2134–2141. doi: 10.1111/apa.15835

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Matte, T. D., Bresnahan, M., Begg, M. D., and Susser, E. (2001). Influence of variation in birth weight within normal range and within sibships on IQ at age 7 years: cohort study. BMJ 323, 310–314. doi: 10.1136/bmj.323.7308.310

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nivins, S., Kennedy, E., McKinlay, C., Thompson, B., and Harding, J. E.Children with Hypoglycemia and Their Later Development (CHYLD) Study Team (2023). Size at birth predicts later brain volumes. Sci. Rep. 13:12446. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-39663-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Özçalişkan, Ş., and Goldin-Meadow, S. (2010). Sex differences in language first appear in gesture. Dev. Sci. 13, 752–760. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00933.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pérez-Pereira, M., Fernández, P., Resches, M., and Gómez-Taibo, M. L. (2016). Does temperament influence language development? Evidence from preterm and full-term children. Infant Behav. Dev. 42, 11–21. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.10.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., and Voelker, P. (2016). Developing brain networks of attention. Curr. Opin. Pediatr. 28, 720–724. doi: 10.1097/MOP.0000000000000413

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Putnam, S. P., Gartstein, M. A., and Rothbart, M. K. (2006). Measurement of fine-grained aspects of toddler temperament: the early childhood behavior questionnaire. Infant Behav. Dev. 29, 386–401. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.01.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Putnam, S. P., Helbig, A. L., Gartstein, M. A., Rothbart, M. K., and Leerkes, E. (2014). Development and assessment of short and very short forms of the infant behavior questionnaire–revised. J. Pers. Assess. 96, 445–458. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2013.841171

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Putnam, S. P., Rothbart, M. K., and Gartstein, M. A. (2008). Homotypic and heterotypic continuity of fine-grained temperament during infancy, toddlerhood, and early childhood. Infant Child Dev. 17, 387–405. doi: 10.1002/icd.582

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roben, C. K. P., Cole, P. M., and Armstrong, L. M. (2013). Longitudinal relations among language skills, anger expression, and regulatory strategies in early childhood. Child Dev. 84, 891–905. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12027

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, C. E., Lean, R. E., Wheelock, M. D., and Smyser, C. D. (2018). Aberrant structural and functional connectivity and neurodevelopmental impairment in preterm children. J. Neurodev. Disord. 10, 1–13.

Google Scholar

Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Temperament, development, and personality. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 16, 207–212. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00505.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothbart, M. K., and Bates, J. E. (2007). Temperament. In Handbook of child psychology. (eds.) W. Damon and N. Eisenberg Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (New York: Wiley), pp. 105–176.

Google Scholar

Rothbart, M. K., and Derryberry, D. (1981). “Development of individual differences in temperament” in Advances in Developmental Psychology. eds. M. E. Lamb and A. L. Brown, vol. 1 (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum), 37–86.

Google Scholar

Salley, B. J., and Dixon, W. E. Jr. (2007). Temperamental and joint attentional predictors of language development. Merrill-Palmer Q. 53:7. doi: 10.1353/mpq.2007.0004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaw, J. C., Berry, M. J., Dyson, R. M., Crombie, G. K., Hirst, J. J., and Palliser, H. K. (2019). Reduced Neurosteroid exposure following preterm birth and its’ contribution to neurological impairment: A novel avenue for preventative therapies. Front. Physiol. 10:599. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2019.00599

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shiner, R., Buss, K., McClowry, S., Putnam, S., and Saudino, K. (2012). What is temperament now? Assessing Progress temperament research on the twenty-fifth anniversary of Goldsmith et al. Child Dev. Perspect. 6, 436–444. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00254.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shokrkon, A., and Nicoladis, E. (2022). The directionality of the relationship between executive functions and language skills: a literature review. Front. Psychol. 13:848696. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.848696

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Snijders, V. E., Bogicevic, L., Verhoeven, M., and van Baar, A. L. (2020). Toddlers’ language development: the gradual effect of gestational age, attention capacities, and maternal sensitivity. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:7926. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17217926

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stene-Larsen, K., Brandlistuen, R. E., Lang, A. M., Landolt, M. A., Latal, B., and Vollrath, M. E. (2014). Communication impairments in early term and late preterm children: A prospective cohort study following children to age 36 months. J. Pediatr. 165, 1123–1128. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2014.08.027

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stolt, S., Haataja, L., Lapinleimu, H., and Lehtonen, L. (2009). The early lexical development and its predictive value to language skills at 2 years in very-low-birth-weight children. J. Commun. Disord. 42, 107–123. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2008.10.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, A., Crawford, H., Morales, S., Degnan, K. A., Pine, D. S., and Fox, N. A. (2020). Infant behavioral inhibition predicts personality and social outcomes three decades later. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 117, 9800–9807. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1917376117

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, A., and Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. New York: New York University Press.

Google Scholar

Yang, H., Yang, S., and Isen, A. M. (2013). Positive affect improves working memory: implications for controlled cognitive processing. Cognit. Emot. 27, 474–482. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2012.713325

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ye, Z., and Zhou, X. (2009). Executive control in language processing. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33, 1168–1177. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.03.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: language development, temperament, gestational age, longitudinal study, sex differences

Citation: Balázs A, Lakatos K, Harmati-Pap V, Tóth I and Kas B (2024) The influence of temperament and perinatal factors on language development: a longitudinal study. Front. Psychol. 15:1375353. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1375353

Received: 23 January 2024; Accepted: 17 June 2024;
Published: 04 July 2024.

Edited by:

Marcela Pena, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Chile

Reviewed by:

Caroline Junge, Utrecht University, Netherlands
Joana L. Gonçalves, Lusíada University, Portugal

Copyright © 2024 Balázs, Lakatos, Harmati-Pap, Tóth and Kas. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Krisztina Lakatos, lakatos.krisztina@ttk.hu

These authors share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.