Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 11 March 2024
Sec. Environmental Psychology

Personal dispositions explain differences in physical health benefits of nature exposure: the role of restorativeness and affect

  • 1Experimental and Applied Psychology Laboratory, Department of Human Sciences, European University of Rome, Rome, Italy
  • 2Department of Education, Psychology, Communication Sciences, University of Bari Aldo Moro, Bari, Italy
  • 3Department of Education, Roma Tre University, Rome, Italy
  • 4Department of Soil, Plant and Food Sciences, University of Bari Aldo Moro, Bari, Italy

Introduction: Urbanization processes are constantly increasing, and most of the European population currently live in urban areas. Nevertheless, evidence is consistent in highlighting the positive association between nature exposure and human wellbeing, although individual differences might affect this association.

Methods: The present study aimed to investigate the association among nature connectedness, conceptualized as Love and Care for Nature, place identity, and physical wellbeing, via restorativeness and positive and negative affect. A total of 312 visitors of an urban green area (i.e., Milan’s Parco Nord) participated in the study. They completed an anonymous questionnaire.

Results: Findings showed that nature connectedness and place identity positively affect physical wellbeing, via restorativeness and positive affect, but not through the negative ones.

Discussion: Results highlight the importance of the joint role of exposure to nature and individual differences in promoting wellbeing. This study offers implications for interventions aimed at enhancing individuals’ health through exposure to nature. Limitations of the study and future research developments are discussed.

1 Introduction

Urbanization levels are increasing dramatically throughout Europe, with 75% of the population currently living in urban areas (The World Bank, 2020). Considering Italy, the rate of people living in urban areas is around 72%, with a higher rate in Northern Italy, where the present study was carried out.

Urbanization processes significantly shape population health, producing mixed outcomes (Miao and Xiaogang, 2016). On the one hand, urban areas have several functions in our society because they represent the core in which technological and economic growth happens. Thus, urban residents can benefit from several public services and infrastructures (Abdul et al., 2020). Conversely, they might face other issues; for example, people might have difficulties accessing natural areas easily (Miao and Xiaogang, 2016). Within cities, homes and working spaces have become increasingly separated from green areas (D’Alessandro et al., 2015). However, nature has an important role in shaping people’s health (Wood et al., 2017). According to the “biophilia hypothesis,” humans have evolved with nature and, therefore, have an innate affinity with it. The need to establish a connection with nature represents an intrinsic facet of our biological heritage (Kellert and Wilson, 1995). Indeed, exposure to nature leads people to achieve states of restorativeness, i.e., the perception of “being away,” fascinated and totally absorbed by nature (Berto, 2014; Han, 2018; Spano et al., 2023). As a matter of fact, evidence supports the idea that people prefer natural environments (e.g., natural landscapes) over man-made environments (e.g., urban landscapes) (Berg et al., 2003; Capaldi et al., 2014; Dopko et al., 2014). Based on the “biophilia hypothesis,” two theories shed light on the association between contact with nature and people’s wellbeing, namely the Attention Restoration Theory (ART) (Kaplan and Kaplan, 1989) and the Stress Reduction Theory (SRT) (Ulrich et al., 1991). According to the ART, exposure to nature enables people to overcome mental fatigue and restore cognitive resources, such as directed attention. SRT instead states that contact with nature positively influences people’s affect by activating the parasympathetic nervous system, reducing stress levels (Jimenez et al., 2021). Thus, urban green areas, such as gardens and parks, represent an effective resource for both ecological and human health. Indeed, urban green spaces provide biodiversity, mitigate air pollution, and represent a source for people who live in urban areas to alleviate their stress levels and satisfy their inherent need to be connected with nature (Kabisch et al., 2017; Panno et al., 2020). Consistently, several previous studies empirically highlighted the positive impact on people’s health resulting from direct contact with urban natural areas (van den Bosch and Ode Sang, 2017). For instance, Carrus et al. (2017) showed that botanical gardens enhance the perceived restorativeness in their visitors, directly and indirectly via the perceived physical and psychological benefits of the visit. However, the authors noticed that this relationship varies across some respondents’ characteristics (e.g., family status).

Thus, individual differences, such as socio-demographic variables, but also dispositional and personality characteristics, might affect the association between natural areas and the benefits associated with nature’s exposure (Berto et al., 2018; Menardo et al., 2021). Among individual characteristics, a significant role might be played by connectedness to nature. Connectedness to nature can be conceptualized as a “an individual’s affective, experiential connection to nature” (Mayer and Frantz, 2004; p., 504). It has been operationalized in several different ways (Martin and Czellar, 2016); for example, some authors refer to it under the label of Nature Relatedness (Nisbet et al., 2008), or Connectivity with Nature (Dutcher et al., 2007). This difference in construct operationalization is driven by the focus posited on the emotional connectedness or the cognitive processes (Martin et al., 2020). In the present study, we specifically refer to Love and Care for Nature (Perkins, 2010), which pertains to individuals’ personal and explicit emotional connection with the natural world.

Connectedness to nature (broadly conceptualized) is a fairly stable trait over time (Martin et al., 2020). People differ in their levels of connectedness to nature, and this individual difference affects the motivation, emotions, and sense of restorativeness potentially enhanced by contact with nature (Mayer et al., 2009; Tam, 2013; Capaldi et al., 2014). In this regard, evidence is consistent in highlighting that people who reported higher scores in connectedness to nature are more prone to also report benefits from exposure to nature (Nisbet and Zelenski, 2011; Tam, 2013; Berto et al., 2018). For instance, individuals with high levels of connectedness to nature are more likely to experience nature’s restorative effects (Kaplan, 1995; Richardson et al., 2016), positive affect (Mayer et al., 2009), and vitality (Capaldi et al., 2014). Moreover, Howell et al. (2011) showed positive and significant relationships between connectedness to nature and different facets of wellbeing (i.e., emotional, psychological, and social wellbeing). Similarly, Dean et al. (2018) pointed out that people with higher levels of connectedness to nature were less likely to report symptoms of depression or anxiety and to report higher levels of physical health, assessed by the Body Mass Index (BMI).

Another individual factor that can potentially foster the link between exposure to green urban areas and people’s health is the place identity. Indeed, psychological theories of identity stress the idea that the self-concept is not only shaped by the inclusion of other humans in the self but also by the inclusion of other living species or ecosystems (Bragg, 1996). Place identity can be, therefore, conceptualized as a mix of concepts, memories, and ideas related to specific physical settings (Proshansky, 1983), which become an integral part of the self (Hidalgo and Hernandez, 2001). Place identity is built through an iterative process linked to a sense of belonging to a specific place (Twigger-Ross and Uzzell, 1996), reflecting the socio-cultural relationship with this place (Qazimi, 2014). Personal identification with a physical place involves place-related knowledge and feelings allocated in the declarative and autobiographical memory (Conway, 2005). According to Kneiz (2014), place identity is shaped by cognitive and affective components. The first refers to mental temporality, coherence, and reflection processes (Klein et al., 2004). The second one, instead, is related to the processes of attachment, closeness, and belongingness (Knez, 2005). Accordingly, people can think, reason, and remember a specific place that assumes a significant value because they are emotionally attached to that place (Knez and Eliasson, 2017). Thus, the association between nature exposure and benefits from exposure to nature should be influenced by the identity significance that the specific green area assumed for the person (Dean et al., 2018). However, the association between place identity and the positive effects of nature have been sparsely addressed. A few research highlighted a significant and positive relationship between place identity, emotion regulation (Ruiz and Hernández, 2014), and restorative potentials of nature (Knez and Eliasson, 2017; Morton et al., 2017). Finally, Knez et al. (2018) showed that place identity positively and significantly predicted the levels of wellbeing experienced by a group of people who live near an urban green area.

1.1 The present study

Based on the reviewed findings, it is possible to notice that the few studies available in the literature mainly focused on the direct associations between connection to nature, place identity, and benefits from nature exposure. Thus, the main aim of the present study was to contribute to overcoming this research gap, shedding light on the role of love and care for nature and place identity as individual factors in improving physical health. Furthermore, we deepened the underlying mechanisms in the relationship mentioned above, specifically focusing on the potential mediating role of restorativeness and positive and negative affect. We investigated these relationships by involving a sample of visitors to Parco Nord (PNM) a metropolitan peri-urban park located in the northern suburbs of Milan, a metropolitan city in northern Italy.

Specifically, on the one hand, based on previous studies that highlight direct positive associations between connectedness to nature and restorativeness (Richardson et al., 2016), positive affect (Mayer et al., 2009), or various facets of wellbeing (Howell et al., 2011; Dean et al., 2018), and on the other hand, the few studies that showed a positive link between place identity and benefits from exposure to natural areas (Knez and Eliasson, 2017; Morton et al., 2017; Knez et al., 2018), we formulated the following hypotheses:

H1: Love and care for nature would increase restorativeness and positive affect which in turn would lead to greater physical health.

H2: Place identity would increase restorativeness and positive affect, leading to greater physical health.

H3: Love and care for nature would lead to greater physical health through the reduction of negative affect.

H4: Place identity would lead to greater physical health through a decrement in negative affect.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Procedures

The present study involved park visitors recruited during their stay at PNM. We employed a convenience sampling technique to achieve the goal of at least 10 participants for each selected data collection point in the park (for more details see Study area section).

Inclusion criteria for study participation were: (a) age of at least 18 years; (b) absence of cognitive impairment and/or mental or neurodegenerative illnesses. Before starting the interview, the researcher ensured that these inclusion criteria were met. If participants declared that they were under the age of 18 or they received a diagnosis of cognitive impairment and/or mental or neurodegenerative illness, the researcher did not proceed with the interview.

Park visitors were approached and invited to participate in the research. Each participant was assured that the questionnaire was anonymous, and that the data would be processed in aggregate form. Furthermore, the possibility to withdraw their participation at any time was explained, providing a contact for questions and/or feedback. Participants were individually interviewed by a well-trained interviewer at the same location where they were initially approached. Data were gathered via an online anonymous questionnaire. The interviewer was tasked with administering the questionnaire and logging participants’ responses using a tablet. At the end of the questionnaire, the interviewer documented the GPS coordinates of the precise location of the administration. The entire procedure typically lasted around 30 min. Data collection occurred between the 23rd of June and the 28th of July 2021. Informed consent was obtained from all participants. The study was approved by the institutional ethics committee of the European University of Rome, Italy (protocol n. 06/2021). All methods were performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments.

2.2 Participants

The final sample involved in the study was composed of 312 visitors to Milan’s Parco Nord. Age ranged from 18 to 91 (M = 52.89; SD = 20.91). As reported in Table 1, of the total sample 176 (56.4%) were males. Concerning marital status, most participants were married/civilly married. Finally, concerning working status, most participants were retirees.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sociodemographic characteristics of the study sample.

2.3 Study area

The study was conducted in a large urban green area situated in the metropolitan area of Milan within the Lombardy region of Northern Italy (coordinates: 45°53′71”N, 9°20′7″E). This area was chosen based on previous studies that highlighted the positive impact of this urban green area on wellbeing (Panno et al., 2017; Spano et al., 2023). The area covers 790 hectares in which there are forest plantations, tree rows, agricultural expanses, open spaces, infrastructure elements, and artificial installations (e.g., out-door gym). To properly represent the multiform characteristics of green spaces throughout the park, a total of 30 sampling points were randomly designated through the “Create Random Points” tool within the ArcMap software, using the study area as a spatial constraint (Figure 1). The park areas involved in the study were: (a) forest and agricultural areas, (b) meadows, (c) park with structures. At least 10 participants were recovered for each point.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Map of data collection points in the study area (Parco Nord Milan, Italy). Reproduced from Spano et al. (2023), licensed under CC BY 4.0.

2.4 Instruments

The questionnaire was composed of a set of sociodemographic questions and the following measures for a total of 44 items (in this final count, sociodemographic variables are included).

2.4.1 Love and care for nature

A selection of five items from the Love and Care for Nature Scale (LCN) (Perkins, 2010) was used to measure love and care for nature. The items were selected among the ones with the highest factor loadings according to the original scale. Specifically, the items extracted from the original scale were: items 1, 2, 3, 4, and 6. The LCN is measured through a 7-points Likert scale (1 = Strongly disagree, 7 = Strongly agree). An item example is “I often feel a sense of awe and wonder when I am in un-spoilt nature.” In the current study, Cronbach’s alpha was 0.88 and McDonald’s omega was 0.89.

2.4.2 Place identity

Two ad hoc items on a 5-points Likert scale (1 = Strongly disagree, 5 = Strongly agree) were created to evaluate place identity. The items are “This park means a lot to me” and “I am very fond of this park.” Cronbach’s alpha was 0.85 and McDonald’s omega was 0.86.

2.4.3 Positive and negative affect

Positive and negative affect were assessed using the Italian validated version of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) (Terraciano et al., 2003). In detail, PANAS encompasses 20 items and two sub-scales, one for positive affect and one for negative ones, both consisting of 10 items each and rated on a 5-points Likert scale (1 = At all, 5 = Very much). Participants were asked to answer a series of adjectives describing their emotions by thinking about how they feel when they are in the park. An example of positive affect is “Interested,” while an example of negative affect is “Anguished.” Cronbach’s alpha was 0.76 for positive affect and 0.81 for negative affect. McDonald’s omega was 0.78 for positive affect and 0.88 for negative affect.

2.4.4 Restorativeness

To assess the restorative quality of the park, the Italian validated version of the Perceived Restorativeness Scale (PRS-11) (Pasini et al., 2014) was used. The scale comprises 11 items on a 5-points Likert scale (1 = At all, 5 = Very much). An item example is “This place is fascinating.” Cronbach’s alpha was 0.81 and McDonald’s omega was 0.83.

2.4.5 Physical health

Two ad hoc items on a 5-points Likert scale (1 = Never, 5 = Always) were created to evaluate perceived physical health related to the park. The items are “When you visit the park, do you feel full of energy?” and “When you visit the park, do you find it easier to engage in physically demanding activities?”. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.71 and McDonald’s omega was 0.73.

2.5 Analysis plan

Preliminary descriptive statistics, such as mean, standard deviation, minimum, maximum, skewness and kurtosis, were performed using SPSS v.21.0 (Statistical Product and Service Solutions, Chicago, IL, USA) to verify the adequate normality of the used variables. As skewness and kurtosis of the PANAS negative affect scale were > 2, a non-normal distribution was assumed and Spearman correlations were performed to test the associations among the study variables.

To test the hypothesized mediation model, a Structural Equation Model (SEM) using Maximum Likelihood approach (ML) and with 5,000 resamples of bootstrapped estimates with a 95% confidence interval (CI) was performed by Mplus v. 8.3 (Muthén and Muthén, Los Angeles, CA, USA). The bootstrapping procedure provides non-symmetrical confidence intervals relevant to parameter estimates with non-normal sampling distributions, such as for variances and indirect effects. Besides, recent findings suggest that, compared to robust standard errors and the robust likelihood-based methods, bootstrap resampling still represents the best approach to use if the normality assumption is violated (Falk, 2018). Concerning the model specification, place identity and love and care for nature were the predictors, PANAS positive and negative affects, restorativeness were the mediators, and perceived physical health was the outcome.

Place identity and physical health were used as latent variables with their items (two for each construct) as observed variables. Furthermore, to avoid model non-identification and for parsimony purposes, the item parceling procedure (item aggregations) was modeled for love and care for nature, PANAS positive and negative affect, and restorativeness. Specifically, two parcels for each construct were used. The item parcels were randomly selected, and parcels had comparable reliabilities (Byrne, 1998). In detail, Cronbach’s alpha of the parcels was: 0.82 (items 6, 4, and 1 of the original scale; Perkins, 2010) and 0.79 (items 3 and 2) for love and care for nature; 0.63 (items 3, 5, 10, 6, and 8 of the original positive affect dimension, see Terraciano et al., 2003) and 0.61 for PANAS positive affect (items 9, 7, 1, 4, and 2 of the original positive affect dimension), 0.76 (items 1, 2, 7, 8, and 10 of the original negative affect dimension) and 0.73 (items 3, 4, 5, 6, and 9 of the original negative affect dimension) for PANAS negative affect; and 0.71 (items 2, 4, 5, 6, and 7 of the original scale; see Pasini et al., 2014) and 0.76 (items 1, 3, 8, 9, 10, and 11) for restorativeness. Respectively, McDonald’s omega of the parcels was: 0.84 and 0.80 for love and care for nature; 0.68 and 0.64 for PANAS positive affect 0.82 and 0.77 for PANAS negative affect; 0.73 and 0.77 for restorativeness.

The following fit indexes were adopted to verify the goodness of fit of the hypothesized model: Chi-square test of exact fit (χ2), comparative fit index (CFI; >0.95 for good; >0.90 for acceptable), Tucker–Lewis index (TLI; >0.95 for good, >0.90 for acceptable), standardized root mean square residual (SRMR; <0.05 for good, <0.10 for acceptable), the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA; <0.06 for good, <0.08 for acceptable) (Bentler and Bonnet, 1980; Hu and Bentler, 1999; McDonald and Ho, 2002). The hypothesized model is shown in Figure 2.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Hypothesized mediation model. Lcn, Love and care for nature; plaid, Place identity; papos, PANAS positive; paneg, PANAS negative; prstot, Restorativeness; phtot, Perceived physical health. Arrow from papos to paneg, arrow from restorativeness to papos, and arrow from restorativeness to paneg were not included in the figure for clarity purpose.

3 Results

3.1 Descriptive statistics and correlations

Table 2 shows means, standard deviations, minimum, maximum, skewness and kurtosis, and the correlation matrix depicting Spearman’s correlations among study variables.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistics and Spearman correlation matrix.

Results highlight that love and care for nature is positively related to PANAS positive affect, place identity, restorativeness, and perceived physical health (p < 0.01). Besides, place identity is positively and significantly associated with restorativeness, PANAS positive affect, and perceived physical health (p < 0.01). Furthermore, restorativeness is positively and significantly associated with PANAS positive affect (p < 0.01), and both restorativeness and PANAS positive affect were positively and significantly related to perceived physical health (p < 0.01). Except for love and care for nature, which is negatively and significantly associated with PANAS negative affect (p < 0.01), no other significant relations emerged between PANAS negative affect and the other variables.

3.2 Mediation analysis results

The structural equation model (SEM) performed to examine the mediation of positive and negative affect and restorativeness in the relation among place identity, love and care for nature (predictors), and perceived physical health (outcome), shows a good fit to the data. In detail, χ2 (40) = 98.422, p < 0.01; root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = 0.068; standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) = 0.033; comparative fit index (CFI) = 0.963; Tucker–Lewis index (TLI) = 0.939. Concerning the mediations among the study variables (see Figure 3), results show that the relation between love and care for nature and perceived physical health (β = 0.015, p > 0.05, 95% CI: [−0.121, 0.146]) goes through PANAS positive affect (β = 0.149, p < 0.05, 95% CI: [0.062, 0.254]) and restorativeness (β = 0.112, p < 0.05, 95% CI: [0.042, 0.209]), but not through PANAS negative affect (β = −0.007, p > 0.05, 95% CI: [−0.028, 0.009]). Similarly, place identity is related to perceived physical health (β = −0.060, p > 0.05, 95% CI: [−0.214, 0.080]) through PANAS positive affect (β = 0.173, p < 0.01, 95% CI: [0.079, 0.286]) and restorativeness (β = 0.153, p < 0.05, 95% CI: [0.058, 0.280]), but not via PANAS negative affect (β = −0.004, p > 0.05, 95% CI: [−0.033, 0.031]).

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Mediation model results. Lcn, Love and care for nature; plaid, Place identity; papos, PANAS positive; paneg, PANAS negative; prstot, Restorativeness; phtot, Perceived physical health. For clarity purpose, only significant paths were reported in the figure.

Table 3 summarizes the statistical significance of standardized effects (total, indirect paths, total indirect, and direct) and their relative bootstrap 95% CI.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Results of mediation with standardized effects and bootstrap 95% CI.

4 Discussion

The present study aimed to provide insights into the indirect effects of connectedness to nature, conceptualized as love and care for nature, and place identity on physical wellbeing, through restorativeness and positive and negative affect. In detail, we hypothesized that nature connectedness and place identity enhance restorative states and positive affect, and this in turn would lead to higher levels of physical wellbeing. Moreover, we also hypothesized that nature connectedness and place identity would lead to greater levels of physical wellbeing, via the reduction of negative affect.

Our findings partially confirmed our initial hypotheses. Consistently with the “biophilia hypothesis,” connectedness to nature promotes physical wellbeing, through restorativeness and positive emotions (H1). People who are highly affiliated with nature are more likely to feel an affective, other than cognitive, bond with the surrounding nature (Mayer and Frantz, 2004). Thus, connectedness to nature represents a catalyst in the promotion of positive emotions and states of restorativeness during the interaction with natural environments, enhancing physical wellbeing. Our results are in line with several previous studies that highlighted the role of connectedness to nature in promoting human wellbeing (Mayer and Frantz, 2004; Hinds and Sparks, 2008; Cervinka et al., 2011; Nisbet and Zelenski, 2011; Pensini et al., 2016), and extend them by shedding the light to the mechanisms behind this association. Moreover, adopting the psychodynamic understanding proposed by Cervinka et al. (2011), it is possible to assume that connectedness to nature represents a personal resource that enables individuals to take advantage of the benefits of staying in nature, coping with stressors and experiencing positive emotions, so much so that they can feel “physically alive.”

Additionally, our study highlighted that place identity is positively and significantly associated with wellbeing via the enhancement of positive affect and restorative effects (H2). The identification with a specific place entails place-related knowledge and feelings allocated in the declarative and autobiographical memory (Conway, 2005; Knez et al., 2018). It implies that people who include PNM in their self will again feel all the positive emotions allocated in their memory every time they have the chance to be there (Kihlstrom and Klein, 1994). Moreover, since they are cognitively and emotionally attached to PNM, their experience in the park will be characterized by high levels of the perception of “being away” and fascinated (Korpela and Hartig, 1996). This process will lead, in turn, to a sense of wellness.

Contrary to our expectations, neither love and care for nature nor place identity are related to wellbeing via the reduction of negative affect (H3, H4). In explaining these findings, it is noteworthy to consider that previous findings on the link between nature connectedness and negative emotions are inconsistent. Indeed, on the one hand, previous research highlighted that connectedness with nature is significantly related to negative affect (Dean et al., 2018) or non-adaptive emotion regulation strategies (Richardson et al., 2016). On the other hand, several other studies only reported a significant link between connectedness to nature and positive emotions (Mayer et al., 2009; Nisbet and Zelenski, 2011; Zelenski and Nisbet, 2012) or adaptive emotion regulation strategies (Fido et al., 2020; Bakir-Demir et al., 2021). Furthermore, the literature on place identity is consistent in considering that place attachment, which represents the affective dimension of place identity, develops for those places in which the person feels comfortable and secure (Hidalgo and Hernandez, 2001). Thus, it is plausible to assume that place identity enhances all the (positive) feelings and emotions associated with staying in a perceived safe place (Zheng et al., 2019). Although based on both our and previous findings it might seem that connectedness to nature and place identity might not be enough as a sole filter for mitigating negative outcomes (Bakir-Demir et al., 2021), we might instead adopt a positive framework. It is possible to speculate that both these individual factors bolster human wellbeing by relying on a promotion system strictly linked with personal growth dimensions.

In interpreting these findings, six main limitations must be acknowledged. First, the cross-sectional nature of the present study did not allow us to capture the causality of the relationships among the variables. Second, the relatively small convenience sample prevents the possibility of generalizing the findings. However, in order to partially face this limit, we used the bootstrap method to compute the confidence intervals (Schoemann et al., 2017). Third, we did not obtain information regarding the amount of time people spent in the park. Fourth, for the sake of questionnaire brevity, we used a selection of items to measure some constructs (i.e., love and care for nature, place identity, and physical wellbeing). The use of full-version scales and the adoption of a multimethod approach (e.g., measuring physical wellbeing with body indexes) could give greater solidity to the results obtained. Fifth, the standardized beta values obtained in the mediation model are relatively weak, considering that the closer the values are to one, the stronger the relationship is Pallant (2020). Finally, the internal consistency of PANAS positive affect parcels is low 0.70. However, it is noteworthy to point out that Cronbach’s alpha higher than 0.60 is usually considered acceptable (Taber, 2018).

5 Conclusion

Despite these limits, to the best of our knowledge, this is the first study that addressed the role of nature connectedness and place identity in promoting physical wellbeing, shedding light on the underlying indirect effects through restorativeness and affects.

Considering the current global challenges, such as the growing urbanization and the negative effects of climate change, scholars are increasingly interested in deepening the crucial role of urban green areas in mitigating the harmful effects of these threats on human health and wellbeing. Evidence in this direction is even more needed if focusing on the impact of urban green spaces in large metropolitan cities like Milan. Moreover, our study paves the way for the importance of pointing out the role of individual differences in mitigating or enhancing the effects of exposure to nature. Indeed, the joint investigation of both the role of exposure to nature and individual differences in promoting wellbeing makes it possible to define interventions that make people more sensitive to the beneficial effects that nature can offer them.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The study was approved by the institutional ethics committee of the European University of Rome, Italy (protocol n. 06/2021). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

CR: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. LR: Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. GSp: Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. AT: Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. GC: Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review & editing. SM: Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review & editing. CA: Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review & editing. GSa: Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. AP: Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was carried out under the project “Establishing Urban FORest based solutions In Changing Cities” (EUFORICC) and financially supported by the Ministry of Education, University and Research (MIUR) of Italy (PRIN 20173RRN2S).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Abdul, K. M., Ynan, E., and McBryde, E. (2020). Urbanization: a problem for the rich and the poor? Public Health Rev. 41, 1–4. doi: 10.1186/s40985-019-0116-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakir-Demir, T., Berument, S. K., and Akkaya, S. (2021). Nature connectedness boosts the bright side of emotion regulation, which in turn reduces stress. J. Environ. Psychol. 76:101642. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2021.101642

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bentler, P. M., and Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychol. Bull. 88, 588–606. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.88.3.588

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Berg, A. E., Koole, S. L., and van der Wulp, N. Y. (2003). Environmental preference and restoration:(how) are they related? J. Environ. Psychol. 23, 135–146. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(02)00111-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Berto, R. (2014). The role of nature in coping with psycho-physiological stress: a literature review on restorativeness. Behav. Sci. 4, 394–409. doi: 10.3390/bs4040394

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Berto, R., Barbiero, G., Barbiero, P., and Senes, G. (2018). An individual’s connection to nature can affect perceived restorativeness of natural environments. Some observations about biophilia. Behav. Sci. 8:34. doi: 10.3390/bs8030034

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bragg, E. A. (1996). Towards ecological self: deep ecology meets constructionist self-theory. J. Environ. Psychol. 16, 93–108. doi: 10.1006/jevp.1996.0008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrne, B. M. (1998). Structural equation modeling with LISREL, PRELIS, and SIMPLIS: Basic concepts, applications, and programming. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Google Scholar

Capaldi, C. A., Dopko, R. L., and Zelenski, J. M. (2014). The relationship between nature connectedness and happiness: a meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 5:976. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00976

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrus, G., Scopelliti, M., Panno, A., Lafortezza, R., Colangelo, G., Pirchio, S., et al. (2017). A different way to stay in touch with ‘urban nature’: the perceived restorative qualities of botanical gardens. Front. Psychol. 8:914. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00914

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cervinka, R., Röderer, K., and Hefler, E. (2011). Are nature lovers happy? On various indicators of well-being and connectedness with nature. J. Health Psychol. 17, 379–388. doi: 10.1177/1359105311416873

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Conway, M. A. (2005). Memory and the self. J. Mem. Lang. 53, 594–628. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2005.08.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Alessandro, D., Buffoli, M., Lorenzo Capasso, G. M., Fara, A. R., and Capolongo, S. (2015). Green areas and public health: improving wellbeing and physical activity in the urban context. Epidemiol. Prev. 39, 8–13.

Google Scholar

Dean, J. H., Shanahan, D. F., Bush, R., Gaston, K. J., Lin, B. B., Barber, E., et al. (2018). Is nature relatedness associated with better mental and physical health? Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 15:1371. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15071371

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dopko, R. L., Zelenski, J. M., and Nisbet, E. K. (2014). Nature salience increases judgments of environmental satisfaction. Ecopsychology 6, 207–217. doi: 10.1089/eco.2014.0042

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dutcher, D. D., Finley, J. C., Luloff, A. E., and Johnson, J. B. (2007). Connectivity with nature as a measure of environmental values. Environ. Behav. 39, 474–493. doi: 10.1177/0013916506298794

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Falk, C. F. (2018). Are Robust Standard Errors the Best Approach for Interval Estimation With Nonnormal Data in Structural Equation Modeling? Struct. Equ. Model. A Multidiscip. J. 25, 244–266. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2017.1367254

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fido, D., Rees, A., Wallace, L., and Mantzorou, L. (2020). Psychopathy moderates the relationship between nature connectedness and cognitive reappraisal. Ecopsychology 12, 301–308. doi: 10.1089/eco.2019.0075

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, K.-T. (2018). A review of self-report scales on restoration and/or Restorativeness in the natural environment. J. Leis. Res. 49, 151–176. doi: 10.1080/00222216.2018.1505159

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hidalgo, M. C., and Hernandez, B. (2001). Place attachment: conceptual and empirical questions. J. Environ. Psychol. 21, 273–281. doi: 10.1006/jevp.2001.0221

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hinds, J., and Sparks, P. (2008). Engaging with the natural environment: the role of affective connection and identity. J. Environ. Psychol. 28, 109–120. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2007.11.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Howell, A. J., Dopko, R. L., Passmore, H.-A., and Buro, K. (2011). Nature connectedness: associations with well-being and mindfulness. Personal. Individ. Differ. 51, 166–171. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.03.037

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jimenez, M. P., DeVille, N. V., Elliott, E. G., Schiff, J. E., Wilt, G. E., Hart, J. E., et al. (2021). Associations between nature exposure and health: a review of the evidence. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:4790. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18094790

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kabisch, N., van den Bosch, M., and Lafortezza, R. (2017). The health benefits of nature-based solutions to urbanization challenges for children and the elderly–a systematic review. Environ. Res. 159, 362–373. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2017.08.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, S. (1995). The restorative benefits of nature: toward an integrative framework. J. Environ. Psychol. 15, 169–182. doi: 10.1016/0272-4944(95)90001-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, R., and Kaplan, S.. (1989). “The experience of nature: a psychological perspective ”. Cambridge University Press. Cambridge

Google Scholar

Kellert, S. R., and Wilson, E. O. (1995). “The biophilia hypothesis.” Island Press. Washington, D.C.

Google Scholar

Kihlstrom, J. F., and Klein, S. B.. (1994), Handbook of social cognition. 153–208. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah.

Google Scholar

Klein, S. B., German, T. P., Cosmides, L., and Gabriel, R. (2004). A theory of autobiographical memory: necessary components and disorders resulting from their loss. Soc. Cogn. 22, 460–490. doi: 10.1521/soco.22.5.460.50765

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kneiz, I. (2014). Place and the self: an autobiographical memory synthesis. Philos. Psychol. 27, 164–192. doi: 10.1080/09515089.2012.728124

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Knez, I. (2005). Attachment and identity as related to a place and its perceived climate. J. Environ. Psychol. 25, 207–218. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2005.03.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Knez, I., and Eliasson, I. (2017). Relationships between personal and collective place identity and well-being in mountain communities. Front. Psychol. 8:79. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00079

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Knez, I., Sang, Å. O., Gunnarsson, B., and Hedblom, M. (2018). Wellbeing in urban greenery: the role of naturalness and place identity. Front. Psychol. 9:491. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00491

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Korpela, K., and Hartig, T. (1996). Restorative qualities of favorite places. J. Environ. Psychol. 16, 221–233. doi: 10.1006/jevp.1996.0018

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, C., and Czellar, S. (2016). The extended inclusion of nature in self scale. J. Environ. Psychol. 47, 181–194. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.05.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, L., White, M. P., Hunt, A., Richardson, M., Pahl, S., and Burt, J. (2020). Nature contact, nature connectedness and associations with health, wellbeing and pro-environmental Behaviours. J. Environ. Psychol. 68:101389. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101389

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, F. S., and Frantz, C. M. P. (2004). The connectedness to nature scale: a measure of individuals’ feeling in community with nature. J. Environ. Psychol. 24, 503–515. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2004.10.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, F. S., Frantz, C. M. P., Bruehlman-Senecal, E., and Dolliver, K. (2009). Why is nature beneficial? The role of connectedness to nature. Environ. Behav. 41, 607–643. doi: 10.1177/0013916508319745

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McDonald, R. P., and Ho, M.-H. R. (2002). Principles and practice in reporting structural equation analyses. Psychol. Methods 7, 64–82. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.7.1.64

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Menardo, E., Brondino, M., Hall, R., and Pasini, M. (2021). Restorativeness in natural and urban environments: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Rep. 124, 417–437. doi: 10.1177/0033294119884063

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Miao, J., and Xiaogang, W. (2016). Urbanization, socioeconomic status and health disparity in China. Health Place 42, 87–95. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2016.09.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morton, T. A., van der Bles, A. M., and Haslam, S. A. (2017). Seeing our self reflected in the world around us: the role of identity in making (natural) environments restorative. J. Environ. Psychol. 49, 65–77. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.11.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nisbet, E. K., and Zelenski, J. M. (2011). Underestimating nearby nature: affective forecasting errors obscure the happy path to sustainability. Psychol. Sci. 22, 1101–1106. doi: 10.1177/0956797611418527

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nisbet, E. K., Zelenski, J. M., and Murphy, S. A. (2008). The nature relatedness scale: linking individuals’ connection with nature to environmental concern and behavior. Environ. Behav. 41, 715–740. doi: 10.1177/0013916508318748

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pallant, J.. (2020). “SPSS survival manual: a step by step guide to data analysis using IBM SPSS ”. McGraw-Hhill education, New York.

Google Scholar

Panno, A., Carrus, G., Lafortezza, R., Mariani, L., and Sanesi, G. (2017). Nature-based solutions to promote human resilience and wellbeing in cities during increasingly hot summers. Environ. Res. 159, 249–256. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2017.08.016

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Panno, A., Theodorou, A., Carrus, G., Imperatori, C., Spano, G., and Sanesi, G. (2020). Nature reappraisers, benefits for the environment: a model linking cognitive reappraisal, the ‘being away’ dimension of Restorativeness and eco-friendly behavior. Front. Psychol. 11:1986. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01986

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pasini, M., Berto, R., Brondino, M., Hall, R., and Ortner, C. (2014). How to measure the restorative quality of environments: the PRS-11. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 159, 293–297. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.12.375

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pensini, P., Horn, E., and Caltabiano, N. J. (2016). An exploration of the relationships between adults’ childhood and current nature exposure and their mental well-being. Child. Youth Environ. 26, 125–147. doi: 10.7721/chilyoutenvi.26.1.0125

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Perkins, H. E. (2010). Measuring love and Care for Nature. J. Environ. Psychol. 30, 455–463. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2010.05.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Proshansky, H. M. (1983). Place identity: physical world socialisation of the self. J. Environ. Psychol. 3, 57–83. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(83)80021-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Qazimi, S. (2014). Sense of place and place identity. Eur. J. Soc. Sci. Educ. Res. 1, 306–310. doi: 10.26417/ejser.v1i1.p306-310

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, M., McEwan, K., Maratos, F., and Sheffield, D. (2016). Joy and calm: how an evolutionary functional model of affect regulation informs positive emotions in nature. Evol. Psychol. Sci. 2, 308–320. doi: 10.1007/s40806-016-0065-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruiz, C., and Hernández, B. (2014). Emotions and coping strategies during an episode of volcanic activity and their relations to place attachment. J. Environ. Psychol. 38, 279–287. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.03.008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schoemann, A. M., Boulton, A. J., and Short, S. D. (2017). Determining power and sample size for simple and complex mediation models. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 8, 379–386. doi: 10.1177/1948550617715068

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spano, G., Ricciardi, E., Theodorou, A., Giannico, V., Caffò, A. O., Bosco, A., et al. (2023). Objective greenness, connectedness to nature and sunlight levels towards perceived Restorativeness in urban nature. Sci. Rep. 13:18192. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-45604-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Taber, K. S. (2018). The use of Cronbach’s alpha when developing and reporting research instruments in science education. Res. Sci. Educ. 48, 1273–1296. doi: 10.1007/s11165-016-9602-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tam, K.-P. (2013). Concepts and measures related to connection to nature: similarities and differences. J. Environ. Psychol. 34, 64–78. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.01.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Terraciano, A., McCrae, R. R., and Costa Jr, P. T. (2003). Factorial and construct validity of the Italian positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS). Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 19, 131–141. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759.19.2.131

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

The World Bank. (2020). “Urban Population -European Union.” Available at: https://data.worldbank.org/indicator/SP.URB.TOTL.IN.ZS?locations=EU.

Google Scholar

Twigger-Ross, C., and Uzzell, D. L. (1996). Place and identity processes. J. Environ. Psychol. 16, 205–220. doi: 10.1006/jevp.1996.0017

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulrich, R. S., Simons, R. F., Losito, B. D., Fiorito, E., Miles, M. A., and Zelson, M. (1991). Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 11, 201–230. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(05)80184-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Bosch, M., and Ode Sang, Å. (2017). Urban natural environments as nature-based solutions for improved public health –a systematic review of reviews. Environ. Res. 158, 373–384. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2017.05.040

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, L., Hooper, P., Foster, S., and Bull, F. (2017). Public green spaces and positive mental health – investigating the relationship between access, quantity and types of parks and mental wellbeing. Health Place 48, 63–71. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2017.09.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zelenski, J. M., and Nisbet, E. K. (2012). Happiness and feeling connected: the distinct role of nature relatedness. Environ. Behav. 46, 3–23. doi: 10.1177/0013916512451901

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zheng, C., Zhang, J., Guo, Y., Zhang, Y., and Qian, L. (2019). Disruption and reestablishment of place attachment after large-scale disasters: the role of perceived risk, negative emotions, and coping. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 40:101273. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdrr.2019.101273

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: nature connectedness, love and care for nature, place identity, positive and negative affect, physical wellbeing

Citation: Russo C, Romano L, Spano G, Theodorou A, Carrus G, Mastandrea S, Angelini C, Sanesi G and Panno A (2024) Personal dispositions explain differences in physical health benefits of nature exposure: the role of restorativeness and affect. Front. Psychol. 15:1365512. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1365512

Received: 04 January 2024; Accepted: 21 February 2024;
Published: 11 March 2024.

Edited by:

Bernardo Hernández, University of La Laguna, Spain

Reviewed by:

Christian Rosales, University of La Laguna, Spain
Maryurena Lorenzo, University of La Laguna, Spain

Copyright © 2024 Russo, Romano, Spano, Theodorou, Carrus, Mastandrea, Angelini, Sanesi and Panno. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Claudia Russo, claudia.russo@unier.it; Angelo Panno, angelo.panno@unier.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.