Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 08 January 2025
Sec. Quantitative Psychology and Measurement

Assessing self-determined motivation for drinking alcohol via the Comprehensive Relative Autonomy Index for Drinking

  • 1Prevention Research Center, Pennsylvania State University, State College, PA, United States
  • 2Health, Exercise, and Lifestyle Lab, Department of Exercise and Sport Science, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC, United States
  • 3Alcohol Habits in Daily Life Lab, Department of Biobehavioral Health, Pennsylvania State University, State College, PA, United States
  • 4Department of Kinesiology, Pennsylvania State University, State College, PA, United States
  • 5School of Kinesiology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, United States

Introduction: Self-Determination Theory (SDT) examines human motivation in multiple domains; however, the only existing measure assessing SDT-informed behavioral regulations for drinking focuses on responsible drinker behaviors, rather than drinking per se, which is important given the alignment between SDT and harm reduction approaches to alcohol use. The aim of this study was to test the structural validity of the SDT-informed Comprehensive Relative Autonomy Index for Drinking (CRAI-Drinking) among college students.

Methods: Participants included two convenience samples with a total of 630 adult drinkers (Mage = 21.5, 55% female, 88% undergraduates). Participants rated drinking behavioral regulations on the 24 original CRAI-Drinking items on a 5-point Likert Scale. Multi-dimensional scaling analyses and factor analyses were used to investigate the underlying autonomy continuum and factor structure of the CRAI-Drinking.

Results: In Sample 1 (n = 274), multi-dimensional scaling analyses confirmed that CRAI-Drinking item and subscale order aligned with SDT's autonomy continuum. Confirmatory factor analyses supported a five factor, 19-item model of the CRAI-Drinking with factors for intrinsic, identified, positive introjected, external, and amotivation regulations (Cronbach's α: 0.68–0.85). In Sample 2 (n = 356), a confirmatory factor analysis confirmed that the 19-item model fit was comparable to Sample 1.

Discussion: This study provides evidence for the structural validity of CRAI-Drinking scores for assessing SDT-based behavioral regulations for drinking in adults.

1 Introduction

Alcohol misuse is a significant public health concern in the U.S. (Hingson et al., 2017; Schulenberg et al., 2020), with over half of U.S. young adults using alcohol in the past month, 31% engaging in heavy episodic drinking (4+/5+ drinks for women/men) and 9% engaging in high intensity drinking (8+/10+ drinks for women/men) (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration Office of Applied Studies, 2021). Investigating individuals' unique reasons for drinking is useful for informing future interventions focused on promoting low-risk, responsible drinking behaviors, and harm reduction strategies. Cognitive, affective, and motivational factors impact drinking behaviors. For example, social cognition processes, explicit alcohol outcome expectancies (i.e., the consequences an individual expects to result from drinking), and implicit cognitions based on past experiences predict current and future drinking behaviors (Jajodia and Earleywine, 2003; Montes et al., 2017; Patrick et al., 2010; Wiers et al., 2002). These outcome expectancies are strongly linked to affect, and a person's affective experiences influence drinking behaviors, such that the effects of alcohol on affect may motivate drinking and drinking may impact affect (Dvorak et al., 2018). Drinking alcohol can enhance positive affect and decrease negative affect (Dvorak et al., 2018), thereby promoting positive outcome expectancies and reinforce coping, enhancing, intrinsic, and other motives for drinking (Cooper et al., 1992, 2008; Dvorak et al., 2018; Sher and Grekin, 2007; Wray et al., 2012). This study specifically focuses on drinking motives.

Drinking motives predict drinking behaviors and consequences and are essential for understanding alcohol use (Cooper, 1994; Cox, 1990; Cox and Klinger, 1988). One existing measure, the Drinking Motives Questionnaire (DMQ) assesses social, coping, conforming, and enhancing motives for drinking (Cooper, 1994; Cox and Klinger, 1988) and has been widely used to examine drinking motives as they relate to drinking contexts, behaviors, and consequences (Cooper, 1994; Gorka et al., 2017; Kuntsche et al., 2005; Kuntsche and Cooper, 2010; Kuntsche and Müller, 2012; Piasecki et al., 2014). DMQ drinking motives also mediate the effects of other psychosocial variables, such as alcohol use expectancies, intentions, social anxiety, and impulsivity, on drinking behaviors (Adams et al., 2012; Ham et al., 2009; Hasking et al., 2011; Kuntsche et al., 2007). However, researchers have recently proposed employing another theory of motivation, Self-Determination Theory (SDT), as an alternative framework for understanding drinking (Richards et al., 2021b; Sharma and Smith, 2011).

Self-Determination Theory is a useful framework for understanding drinking because it accounts for several key psychological factors that predict drinking behaviors and outcomes by assessing behavioral regulations for engaging in a behavior that capture a broad spectrum of motives for consuming alcohol (Bhowmick et al., 2019; Chawla et al., 2009; Koski-Jännes, 1994; Lassi et al., 2019; Levesque et al., 2006; Peele and Brodsky, 2000; Ryan and Deci, 2000, 2017). SDT includes six behavioral regulations that are ordered along the relative autonomy continuum (RAC) from low to high levels of autonomy and external to internal locus of control (Figure 1; Ryan and Connell, 1989; Ryan and Deci, 2000, 2017; Sheldon et al., 2017). Intrinsic regulation is completely autonomous motivation in which people engage in a behavior because it is inherently interesting, stimulating, or enjoyable (Ryan and Connell, 1989; Ryan and Deci, 2000, 2017; Sheldon et al., 2017). Identified regulation is the most autonomous form of extrinsic motivation driven by personally valuing a behavior or its outcomes (Ryan and Connell, 1989; Ryan and Deci, 2000, 2017; Sheldon et al., 2017). Positive introjection describes engaging in a behavior to enhance internal feelings of self-worth (Assor et al., 2009; Sheldon et al., 2017). Negative introjection describes engaging in a behavior to avoid unpleasant internal self-conscious experiences such as loss of self-worth (Assor et al., 2009; Sheldon et al., 2017). External regulation is the least autonomous form of extrinsic motivation driven by the needs to avoid external punishment or to achieve rewards by complying with others' expectations (Ryan and Connell, 1989; Ryan and Deci, 2000, 2017; Sheldon et al., 2017). Amotivation is a completely non-autonomous regulation in which a person experiences no intentional motivation for their behavior (Ryan and Connell, 1989; Ryan and Deci, 2000, 2017; Sheldon et al., 2017).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Self-Determination Theory—relative autonomy continuum, locus of control, and behavioral regulations. This figure shows Self-Determination Theory's conceptualization of human motivation. This shows the six motivational behavioral regulations ranging in their level of autonomy from low (non-autonomous amotivation) to high (completely autonomous intrinsic regulation), their perceived locus of control from impersonal (amotivation) to completely external (external regulation) to completely internal (intrinsic regulation), and the relevant motivational processes for each individual behavioral regulation.

Higher levels of more autonomous behavioral regulations (e.g., intrinsic, identified) support overall well-being and human flourishing (Ryan and Connell, 1989; Ryan and Deci, 2000, 2017). Research examining behavioral regulations and health behavior engagement indicates that higher levels of more autonomous regulations are associated with greater engagement in healthy behaviors and vice-versa (Chawla et al., 2009; Knee and Neighbors, 2002; Neighbors et al., 2003, 2004, 2007; Ng et al., 2012; Ryan et al., 2008; Ryan and Deci, 2007). Although few studies have examined self-determined behavioral regulations for drinking, the existing research indicates that lower levels of autonomous behavioral regulations are associated with heavier alcohol use (Knee and Neighbors, 2002; Neighbors et al., 2003, 2004, 2007). Other research examining the constructs underlying SDT's behavioral regulations, such as basic psychological needs (autonomy, competence, relatedness) and locus of control, has mixed findings. For example, several studies found that lower autonomy predicts increased drinking intensity and drinking for social approval (Chawla et al., 2009; Knee and Neighbors, 2002; Neighbors et al., 2003, 2004), whereas one study found that greater autonomy satisfaction corresponded with higher odds of drinking (Enns and Orpana, 2020). Similarly, moderate drinking may be associated with social benefits that support the need for relatedness (Peele and Brodsky, 2000; Sæther et al., 2019), but greater relatedness satisfaction may also correspond with lower odds of drinking (Enns and Orpana, 2020). Regarding locus of control, having a greater external or impersonal locus of control corresponds with greater alcohol use and hazardous drinking (Caudwell and Hagger, 2015; Chawla et al., 2009; Dukes et al., 2022; Koski-Jännes, 1994; Lassi et al., 2019), and people high on alcohol dependence experience a higher external locus of control (Bhowmick et al., 2019). Impersonal locus of control (i.e., amotivation) is associated with impulse control issues and maladaptive outcomes, suggesting it may be particularly relevant for research related to alcohol use disorders (Hofmann et al., 2009). Additional research is needed to understand how behavioral regulations relate to drinking behaviors and outcomes. However, such research is limited by the lack of a validated measure of self-determined behavioral regulations for drinking.

Currently, only one measure related to self-determined drinking behavioral regulations exists, the Treatment Self-Regulation Questionnaire (TSRQ), which assesses self-determined behavioral regulations for responsible drinking, such as being motivated to drink responsibly to take care of one's health or to get approval from others (Richards et al., 2020a,b). Conceptually, behavioral regulations for responsible drinking are not synonymous with behavioral regulations for drinking per se, as shown by research indicating that TSRQ behavioral regulations are not redundant for DMQ-assessed drinking motives and DMQ drinking motives account for more variance in alcohol-related outcomes than responsible drinking (Richards et al., 2021a). This lack of redundancy applies within the context of SDT, such that one person—Kathy—may drink due to enjoying the taste of alcohol (i.e., intrinsic regulation) but may drink responsibly due to pressure from others (i.e., external regulation). Conversely, another person—John—may drink due to external pressure from others but may be intrinsically motivated to drink responsibly. These distinctions are theoretically and practically important. From an intervention perspective, Kathy would benefit from an intervention targeting behavioral regulations for responsible drinking, whereas John would benefit from an intervention targeting behavioral regulations for drinking per se. Unfortunately, no validated measure of SDT-informed behavioral regulations for drinking currently exists, precluding researchers and interventionists from being able to distinguish between people like Kathy and John. Lastly, 37% of college students engage in risky drinking behaviors (Johnston et al., 2009), and behavioral regulations for responsible drinking are likely less relevant for that population than behavioral regulations for drinking in general.

The lack of a validated measure of self-determined behavioral regulations for drinking is problematic given SDT's alignment with psychological factors underlying alcohol use and its potential utility for alcohol use interventions. SDT is widely used in health behavior interventions, including alcohol use interventions (Sheeran et al., 2020), and has been highlighted as a promising theoretical framework for informing the development and refinement of alcohol use interventions (Richards et al., 2021b; Sharma and Smith, 2011; Sheeran et al., 2021). Targeting the psychological factors underlying SDT (e.g., autonomy, relatedness, intrinsic regulation) can effectively promote health behavior change, with interventions that increase more autonomous behavioral regulations resulting in small-to-medium positive changes in health behaviors (Ntoumanis et al., 2020; Ryan et al., 2008; Sheeran et al., 2021). Indeed, a meta-analysis of SDT interventions found that the interventions resulted in significant reductions in alcohol consumption (d = 0.26; Sheeran et al., 2020), and Dukes et al. (2022) cited the value of using SDT to inform substance use prevention and treatment interventions. Unfortunately, SDT is still widely understudied regarding alcohol use, and little is known about which behavioral regulations to target in interventions to reduce alcohol consumption. Developing a validated measure of SDT-informed behavioral regulations for drinking per se may provide researchers with a useful and comprehensive measure for investigating how the psychological factors underlying SDT relate to drinking behaviors and outcomes and has the potential for identifying relevant behavioral regulations that drive alcohol use targets for interventions. In the present research, we use two samples to examine the psychometric properties of a measure for assessing self-determined behavioral regulations for drinking.

Sheldon et al. (2017) developed the 24-item Comprehensive Relative Autonomy Index (CRAI) to create a common core of generic items that would enable researchers to assess the behavioral regulations underlying SDT's RAC across a variety of domains. The purpose of this study was to adapt Sheldon et al.'s (2017) CRAI to assess behavioral regulations for drinking underlying SDT's RAC. This will help provide initial psychometric evaluation of an SDT-based measure of behavioral regulations for drinking in general, rather than behavioral regulations for responsible drinking, among college students. Using two samples, we developed and cross-validated scores from the adapted survey—the Comprehensive Relative Autonomy Index for Drinking (CRAI-Drinking). We hypothesized that the order of CRAI-Drinking items/subscales would align with SDT's RAC and there would be six CRAI-Drinking subscales corresponding with a priori, theoretically-driven assignments for each item (Sheldon et al., 2017). Due to historical gender differences in alcohol use behaviors, we also tested measurement invariance of the CRAI-Drinking by gender (White, 2020; White et al., 2015).

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants and procedures

Participants included a convenience sample of adults 18 years or older (Sample 1) or 18–25 years (Sample 2) who consumed at least one drink/week. Study recruitment started November 19, 2020 and ended December 31, 2021. Sample 1 data were collected between November 2020 and May 2021. Of the 357 individuals who completed the online screening survey, 46 (12.9%) did not qualify due to insufficient alcohol use (< 1 drink/week), resulting in 306 (85.7%) qualified participants, 274 (89.5%) of whom completed the online study survey and were compensated with extra credit or entered to win one of two $50 gift cards. Sample 2 data were collected between October and December 2021. Of the 669 individuals who completed the online screening survey, 22.6% did not qualify due to insufficient alcohol use (< 1 drink/week), resulting in 515 (77.4%) qualified participants, 356 (69.1%) of whom completed the online study survey and were compensated with extra credit or entry to win 1 of 10 $30 gift cards. This study was approved by the university Institutional Review Board (Protocol #00016554) as an exempt protocol. Participants provided written implied informed consent to participate via the online survey platform (Research Electronic Data Capture).

2.2 Measures

2.2.1 Comprehensive Relative Autonomy Index for Drinking items

The 24 CRAI-Drinking items were adapted from Sheldon et al.'s (2017) domain-agnostic version of the CRAI by modifying item prompts to refer specifically to drinking. Participants were asked: “Thinking of all the times you drink, how often would you say that you drink for each of the following reasons?” and rated each item on a 5-point Likert scale from (0) not true for me to (4) very true for me. Four items each assessed intrinsic, identified, positive introjected, negative introjected, and external regulations, and amotivation.

2.2.2 Demographics

Demographic characteristics were assessed using self-reports of age, sex, ethnicity, race, education, work status, and student status.

2.3 Statistical analyses

Non-metric multidimensional scaling (NMDS) and Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) were used to investigate structural validity. NMDS analyses were used to identify the spatial ordering of the CRAI-Drinking items/subscales to determine whether they followed the order specified by SDT's RAC, which assumes that the subscales follow this order from low to high levels of autonomy: amotivation, external regulation, negative introjected regulation, positive introjected regulation, identified regulation, and intrinsic motivation. NMDS was also employed to replicate Sheldon et al.'s (2017) study in which NMDS analyses were used when developing the domain-agnostic version of the CRAI. NMDS analyses identified whether the ordering of CRAI-Drinking items/subscales followed the assumptions of SDT's RAC based on the location of the items/subscales on the visual map (i.e., their x- and y-coordinates or angles; Hout et al., 2013; Sheldon et al., 2017). NMDS quantifies the similarity between items, and includes a visual map that conveys the spatial relationships among items, whereby similar items are more proximal to one another and dissimilar items are further apart (Hout et al., 2013). NMDS was used to test the simplex structure of the CRAI-Drinking, which would be represented by a semicircle on the map, and to examine the degree of similarity among items and subscales, as well as whether the ordering of items and subscales followed the order of SDT's RAC, both of which are reflected by item or subscale polar coordinates (i.e., angles; Hout et al., 2013; Sheldon et al., 2017). Item- and subscale-level correlation matrices were used to test one- and two-dimensional models. Model fit was assessed based on stress, which measures agreement between the model estimated and raw input data (i.e., correlation matrix), with lower stress values indicating better fit (Hout et al., 2013).

CFA was used to determine the factor structure (Brown, 2015; Kyriazos, 2018; Thompson, 1994, 2013). The NMDS subscale analyses informed the number of factors tested in the CFA, though additional CFA models with 1–6 factors were explored to confirm the best fitting model and to avoid confirmation bias (MacCallum and Austin, 2000; Supplementary Table B). Models were estimated using the weighted least squares mean and variance to account for the ordinal response options (Muthén et al., 1997). Absolute and relative fit indices used to assess model fit included Chi-square (χ2), comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) and its confidence interval (CI), and the standardized root mean square residual (SMSR; Bentler, 1990; Bollen, 1989). Best practices for measurement in psychometrics require cross-validating the factor structure of the measure (Brown, 2015; Kline, 2016; Kyriazos, 2018; Thompson, 1994, 2013). Cross-validation safeguards the validity, reliability, and replicability of measurement in psychometrics by demonstrating the generalizability of factor structure across different samples (Brown, 2015; Kline, 2016; Kyriazos, 2018; Thompson, 1994, 2013). Therefore, Sample 2 was used to cross-validate the factor structure of the CRAI-Drinking. CFAs were conducted using the lavaan package using a full-information maximum likelihood estimator. Based on heuristics in the literature (Mundfrom et al., 2005), a hypothesized structure of six factors with four items per factor (Sheldon et al., 2017) and a moderate level of communality, a sample size of N = 300 was estimated to be sufficient to achieve good agreement (coefficient of congruence: K = 0.982) between sample and population solutions. Measurement invariance (e.g., configural, metric, scalar) was tested between genders. Configural invariance tested whether the overall factor structure fit well in both genders. Metric invariance tested whether the factor loadings were equivalent in both genders, and structural invariance tested whether the item intercepts were equivalent in both genders (Sass and Schmitt, 2013). Configural invariance was tested by fitting the final 5-factor model separately for males and females and comparing fit indices. Metric invariance was tested by constraining factor loadings to be equal and scalar invariance additionally constrained factor intercepts to be equal (Sass and Schmitt, 2013). Due to the potential oversensitivity of chi-square difference tests, models were compared using the criteria of a decrease in CFI ≥ 0.01 (Cheung and Rensvold, 2002) and an increase in RMSEA ≥ 0.015 (Chen, 2007) to be indicative of significantly worse model fit. Data were analyzed in R version 4.0.3 using the isoMDS, factoextra, and lavaan packages in R (Bollen, 1989; R Core Team, 2020; Rosseel, 2012).

3 Results

Sample 1 (N = 274) was 66.8% female, 91.6% non-Hispanic, 81.8% White, and 75.5% undergraduate students with a mean age of 23.0 ± 6.6 years. Sample 2 (N = 356) was 46.1% female, 89.9% non-Hispanic, 84.6% White, and 97.5% undergraduate students with a mean age of 20.4 ± 1.5 years. The combined sample (N = 630) was 55.1% female, 90.6% non-Hispanic, 83.3% White, and 88.1% undergraduate students with a mean age of 21.5 ± 4.7 years. Participants drank alcohol on 3.2 ± 1.5 days per week and consumed 8.5 ± 4.9 drinks per day. Supplementary Table A provides complete descriptive statistics for Sample 1 (N = 274), Sample 2 (N = 356), and the combined sample. Table 1 summarizes CRAI-Drinking item responses for Samples 1 and 2.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive characteristics for the Comprehensive Relative Autonomy Index for Drinking for Sample 1 and Sample 2.

The NMDS item-level analysis of Sample 1 data indicated that the two-dimensional model fit better (stress = 0.110) than the one-dimensional model (stress = 0.166). The ordering of items predominantly aligned with SDT's RAC (Figure 2A), although identified regulation item 1 (angle = 1.72) had a higher angle (e.g., greater autonomy) than the intrinsic regulation items (angles = 1.31 to 1.60). The subscale-level analysis showed that the ordering of subscales reflected SDT's RAC (Figure 2B). However, external and negative introjected regulation overlapped on the map (external regulation angle = −1.24, negative introjected angle = −1.22), indicating that they were highly similar to one another and essentially represented the same factor. Based on these findings, we dropped the negative introjected items and retained the external regulation items to preserve a parallel structure with the TSRQ (Richards et al., 2020a). The positive introjected items ensured that introjection would still be uniquely represented in the measure. Item 1 for identified regulation did not follow SDT's RAC based on NMDS analysis and was dropped from subsequent models. Of the 24 original CRAI-Drinking items, 19 were retained that represented five factors: intrinsic (4 items), identified (3 items), positive introjected (4 items), and external (4 items) regulations, and amotivation (non-regulation; 4 items).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Results of non-metric multidimensional scaling analysis at the item (A) and scale (B) levels. This figure shows the results of the NMDS analyses of the individual CRAI-Drinking items (1A) and subscales (1B). The analyses revealed a two-dimensional structure for the items and subscales, which are represented by Dimensions 1 and 2 (A, B). Dimension 1 can be inferred as representing the level of autonomy characterized by each item (2A) or subscale (2B). For example, in 2A, item AMOT_3 reflects a low level of autonomy, whereas item INT_2 reflects a high level of autonomy. In 2B, subscale AMOT (representing the four amotivation items) reflects a low level of autonomy, whereas subscale INT (representing the four intrinsic regulation items) reflects a high level of autonomy. As seen in 2B, the external regulation and negative introjected regulation subscales are located in essentially the same place on the map, indicating they are highly similar to one another.

Figure 2 shows the results of the NMDS analyses of the individual CRAI-Drinking items (1A) and subscales (1B). The analyses revealed a two-dimensional structure for the items and subscales, which are represented by Dimensions 1 and 2 in Figures 2A, B. Dimension 1 can be inferred as representing the level of autonomy characterized by each item (2A) or subscale (2B). For example, in 2A, item AMOT_3 reflects a low level of autonomy, whereas item INT_2 reflects a high level of autonomy. In 2B, subscale AMOT (representing the four amotivation items) reflects a low level of autonomy, whereas subscale INT (representing the four intrinsic regulation items) reflects a high level of autonomy. As seen in 2B, the external regulation and negative introjected regulation subscales are located in essentially the same place on the map, indicating they are highly similar to one another.

Results of the NMDS analyses informed the subsequent CFA examining 19 items loading on to five factors. CFA showed that the five-factor model demonstrated good model fit, χ2 (142) = 165.463 (p = 0.087), CFI = 0.991, TLI = 0.990, RMSEA = 0.025 (95% CI [0.000, 0.040]), and SMSR = 0.061. All retained items loaded significantly onto their factors (p < 0.001) with standardized factor loadings ranging from 0.56 to 0.81. Reliability estimates were high for each factor based on Cronbach's alpha values of 0.82 for intrinsic, 0.66 for identified, 0.85 for positive introjected, and 0.80 for external regulations, and 0.76 for amotivation. As expected, correlations were stronger between factors that shared similar levels of autonomy (e.g., positive introjected and external regulations: r = 0.65) and weaker for factors that had less similar levels of autonomy (e.g., intrinsic and external regulations: r = 0.03; intrinsic regulation and amotivation: r = −0.02; see Table 2 for the correlation matrix for the CRAI-Drinking factors). Due to Sheldon et al.'s (2017) original CRAI including six factors, we tested a six-factor model, as well as four additional theoretically-plausible measurement models to avoid confirmation bias (MacCallum and Austin, 2000). Supplementary Table B shows the model fit for these additional models. As expected, the hypothesized six-factor model did not converge due to a perfect correlation between the negative introjected and external regulation factors. Additionally, the five-factor model had better fit indices than alternative models with one to four factors, as demonstrated by a lower χ2 value that was not statistically significant, higher CFI and TLI values, and lower RMSEA and SMSR values than all other models (Bentler, 1990; Bollen, 1989); therefore, the five-factor model was selected as the model of best fit.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Correlation coefficient matrix of mean scores for all factors for the Comprehensive Relative Autonomy Index for Drinkinga.

As shown in Table 3, cross-validation of the five-factor model in Sample 2 revealed similar and good model fit indices to those found in Sample 1. In Sample 2 all items loaded significantly onto their factors (p < 0.001), and the standardized factor loadings and reliability estimates were similar to those in Sample 1 (e.g., factor loadings ranged from 0.41 to 0.83), supporting the five-factor solution. Supplementary Table C includes the variance/covariance matrix for all CRAI-Drinking items for Samples 1 and 2.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Goodness-of-fit indexes for the 5-factor model between Samples 1 and 2 and testing measurement invariance by gender.

Post-hoc analyses testing invariance by gender used the combined sample (males: n = 283, females: n = 347). As shown in Table 3, fit indices for the final five-factor model were slightly better for males, but both genders demonstrated good model fit, and all items loaded significantly onto their factors (p < 0.001), supporting configural invariance, meaning that the five-factor structure fit well in both genders. Neither the metric model nor the scalar model demonstrated significant decrements in fit compared to the configural and metric models, respectively, based on small changes in the CFI and RMSEA that were within the acceptable range to support metric and scalar invariance across genders. Metric invariance indicated that the factor loadings were equivalent in both genders, and scalar invariance indicated that the item intercepts were equivalent in both genders.

4 Discussion

This study provides initial support for the structural validity of CRAI-Drinking scores, a new measure of behavioral regulations for drinking adapted from Sheldon et al.'s (2017) CRAI. The CRAI-Drinking distinguished between five behavioral regulations for drinking, including intrinsic, identified, positive introjected, and external regulations, and amotivation. This initial testing of the CRAI-Drinking fills a gap by providing a novel measure of SDT-based behavioral regulations for general drinking behaviors among young adults. This contributes to the literature, as the only previously existing SDT-informed measure of drinking was the TSRQ, which assesses self-determined behavioral regulations for responsible drinking (Richards et al., 2020a,b). General motives for drinking are conceptually and practically distinct from motives for responsible drinking and account for more variance in drinking outcomes (Richards et al., 2021a). Establishing the CRAI-Drinking as a valid measure of self-determined behavioral regulations for drinking per se contributes to the literature by providing researchers with a comprehensive measure of self-determined behavioral regulations for drinking that will support future research examining how the psychological factors underlying SDT relate to drinking behaviors, outcomes, and alcohol use disorder and may help in identifying relevant psychological targets for developing, mechanistically evaluating, or refining interventions.

The CRAI-Drinking model demonstrated good structural validity; however, in contrast to Sheldon et al.'s (2017) original CRAI measure, which was developed as a common core of domain-agnostic items that would enable researchers to assess SDT's individual behavioral regulations across a variety of domains, external and negative introjected regulation were not distinct when considering behavioral regulations for drinking in these samples. After removing five items, a five-factor model with factors for intrinsic, identified, positive introjected, and external regulation and amotivation demonstrated good fit across both samples. The correlations between the five factors reflected SDT's RAC, such that behavioral regulations characterized by more similar levels of autonomy demonstrated stronger correlations with one another than regulations that were less similar to one another (Ryan and Connell, 1989; Ryan and Deci, 2000, 2017; Sheldon et al., 2017).

Contrary to our hypothesis and original CRAI Sheldon et al.'s (2017), external and negative introjected regulation were not distinct from one another. Previous research indicated that external regulations involving social incentives, such as those assessed by the CRAI-Drinking, were closely related to introjection (Gagné et al., 2015; Roth et al., 2006). It is possible that the influence of social norms on drinking behaviors and motives closed the gap between external and negative introjected regulation, such that they were indistinguishable with regard to drinking. Social motives and norms play a particularly important role in drinking among college students (Cooper, 1994; Foster et al., 2015; Sudhinaraset et al., 2016; Patrick et al., 2017), which could have affected our findings given the large proportion of undergraduate students in our sample. Future studies should investigate whether external regulation and negative introjection represent distinct behavioral regulations for drinking among samples with a larger proportion of non-undergraduate student drinkers.

Among all of the behavioral regulations assessed via the CRAI-Drinking, amotivation is distinct because it is a completely non-regulated behavioral regulation characterized by an impersonal locus of control and unknown or unclear reasons for drinking (Ryan and Connell, 1989; Ryan and Deci, 2000, 2017; Sheldon et al., 2017). The CRAI-Drinking is the first measure assessing amotivation for drinking per se and is unique from existing drinking motives measures in accounting for locus of control (Ryan and Connell, 1989; Ryan and Deci, 2000, 2017; Sheldon et al., 2017). This is important given that having an external or impersonal locus of control corresponds with maladaptive behavioral outcomes, including increased alcohol use and temptation to use alcohol (Dukes et al., 2022), worse alcohol use behaviors among people with and without alcohol use disorder (Bhowmick et al., 2019; Caudwell and Hagger, 2015; Chawla et al., 2009; Dukes et al., 2022; Lassi et al., 2019), and worse post-recovery drinking behaviors among recovering alcoholics (Koski-Jännes, 1994). Impersonal locus of control is also related to impulse control issues, suggesting it may be particularly relevant for studying alcohol use disorders (Hofmann et al., 2009). Lastly, Dukes et al. (2022) highlight to importance of targeting amotivation/impersonal locus of control in substance use interventions to empower individuals to feel a greater sense of control and to support better substance use behaviors and outcomes. As such, amotivation is a unique behavioral regulation that likely requires greater attention, and the development of the CRAI-Drinking supports future research and interventions investigating amotivation related to alcohol use.

The five-factor model of CRAI-Drinking scores demonstrated configural, metric, and scalar invariance between genders. This is important given historical differences in alcohol consumption between males and females (White, 2020; White et al., 2015). Configural invariance indicates that the number of factors and the specific pattern of items for each factor are the same for males and females (Brown, 2015), indicating that both genders interpret the items similarly, which is a necessary prerequisite for examining gender differences in drinking behavioral regulations. Achieving metric and scalar invariance supports the ability to compare latent means between groups (Brown, 2015), which is valuable for future research examining gender similarities or differences in self-determined behavioral regulations for drinking.

The study was limited to two convenience samples of young adults who were predominantly White, non-Hispanic undergraduate students. More diverse samples should be tested to examine factor structure and before generalizing inferences about score meaning to other groups, including people with alcohol use disorders or neuropsychiatric disorders. CRAI-Drinking response options were modified from the original survey to align with other SDT-based measures (Markland and Tobin, 2004), which may have affected findings.

Data collection overlapped with the COVID-19 pandemic. Findings from the Monitoring the Future study indicated that more young adults reported drinking to relax/relieve tension or because of boredom and more reported drinking alone or at home from April to November 2020 compared to the 5 years prior to the pandemic (Patrick et al., 2022). Young adults also had significantly lower prevalence rates of past 30-day drinking and binge drinking, though young adults who did drink reported significantly higher frequency of past 30-day drinking and binge drinking from April to November 2020 (Patrick et al., 2022). We found that Sample 1, whose data were collected after lockdowns ceased but prior to vaccines becoming widely available and when many undergraduate universities were holding remote (Fall 2020) and/or a combination of remote, hybrid, and in-person classes (Fall 2021), reported significantly less drinking and lower levels of all SDT-behavioral regulations for drinking compared to Sample 2. Sample 2 data were collected after vaccines became widely available and when most universities were holding in-person classes (Fall 2021). However, these differences between the samples did not affect the structural validity of the CRAI-Drinking, as the 5-factor model showed good fit in both samples. This suggests that, despite potential effects of the COVID-19 pandemic on drinking reasons and/or behaviors, the CRAI-Drinking is a structurally valid measure for assessing SDT's behavioral regulations for drinking.

The negative introjection items assessed avoiding self-conscious experiences (e.g., embarrassment) and loss of status, rather than guilt or shame. This is consistent with other SDT-measures of negative introjection but may have impacted our findings. Studies testing additional negative introjection items directly pertaining to other unpleasant anticipatory emotions (e.g., guilt, shame, anxiety) would be worthwhile. Although we found measurement invariance by gender, additional research examining measurement invariance by other sociodemographic characteristics (e.g., student status, age) and longitudinal invariance is also warranted. Studies employing cognitive interviewing or investigating response processes would be useful for providing additional support for substantive validity and interpretation of CRAI-Drinking items and responses (Boness and Sher, 2020; Hubley and Zumbo, 2017).

This study contributes to the literature by providing initial support for the use of the CRAI-Drinking as a measure of self-determined behavioral regulations for general drinking behavior. The 19-item CRAI-Drinking provides scores for five behavioral regulations for alcohol use: intrinsic, identified, positive introjected, and external regulations and amotivation and shows good evidence of internal scale validity and measurement invariance between genders. The CRAI-Drinking has the potential to provide useful information to researchers regarding how self-determined behavioral regulations and key psychological constructs underlying SDT are related to drinking behaviors and outcomes to inform general knowledge and to identify targets for behavioral interventions. Future studies should investigate the validity of the CRAI-Drinking for predicting drinking behaviors and consequences.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Human Research Protection Program/Institutional Review Board, The Pennsylvania State University. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

JC: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Visualization, Writing – original draft. MR: Conceptualization, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. DC: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This project was supported by the Prevention and Methodology Training Program (T32 DA017629; MPIs: J. Maggs & S. Lanza) with funding from the National Institute on Drug Abuse. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute on Drug Abuse or the National Institutes of Health.

Acknowledgments

A preprint of this article is available at https://osf.io/preprints/osf/z3u4b.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1354545/full#supplementary-material

References

Adams, Z. W., Kaiser, A. J., Lynam, D. R., Charnigo, R. J., and Milich, R. (2012). Drinking motives as mediators of the impulsivity-substance use relation: pathways for negative urgency, lack of premeditation, and sensation seeking. Addict. Behav. 37, 848–855. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.03.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Assor, A., Vansteenkiste, M., and Kaplan, A. (2009). Identified versus introjected approach and introjected avoidance motivations in school and in sports: the limited benefits of self-worth strivings. J. Educ. Psychol. 101, 482–497. doi: 10.1037/a0014236

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychol. Bull. 107, 238–246. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhowmick, M., Kulkarni, S., and Gaikwad, S. (2019). Study of locus of control and readiness to change in patients of alcohol dependence syndrome with relation to severity of alcohol dependence. MedPulse Int. J. Psychol. 12, 43–49. doi: 10.26611/1071224

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural Equations with Latent Variables. New York, NY: Wiley.

Google Scholar

Boness, C. L., and Sher, K. J. (2020). The case for cognitive interviewing in survey item validation: a useful approach for improving the measurement and assessment of substance use disorders. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs 81, 401–404. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2020.81.401

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, T. A. (2015). Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: Guildford Publications.

Google Scholar

Caudwell, K. M., and Hagger, M. S. (2015). Predicting alcohol pre-drinking in Australian undergraduate students using an integrated theoretical model. Appl. Psychol. Health Well-Being 7, 188–213. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12044

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chawla, N., Neighbors, C., Logan, D., Lewis, M. A., and Fossos, N. (2009). Perceived approval of friends and parents as mediators of the relationship between self-determination and drinking. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs 70, 92–100. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2009.70.92

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, F. F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Struct. Equat. Model. Multidiscip. J. 14, 464–504. doi: 10.1080/10705510701301834

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheung, G. W., and Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Struct. Equat. Model. Multidiscip. J. 9, 233–255. doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, M. L. (1994). Motivations for alcohol use among adolescents: development and validation of a four-factor model. Psychol. Assess. 6, 117–128. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.6.2.117

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, M. L., Krull, J. L., Agocha, V. B., Flanagan, M. E., Orcutt, H. K., Grabe, S., et al. (2008). Motivational pathways to alcohol use and abuse among Black and White adolescents. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 117, 485–501. doi: 10.1037/a0012592

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, M. L., Russell, M., Skinner, J. B., Frone, M. R., and Mudar, P. (1992). Stress and alcohol use: moderating effects of gender, coping, and alcohol expectancies. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 101, 139–152. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.101.1.139

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, W. M., (ed.). (1990). Why People Drink: Parameters of Alcohol as a Reinforcer. New York, NY: Gardner Press.

Google Scholar

Cox, W. M., and Klinger, E. (1988). A motivational model of alcohol use. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 97, 168–180. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.97.2.168

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dukes, A., Mullen, P. R., Niles, J., Gutierrez, D., and Jensen, S. (2022). Role of causality orientations in predicting alcohol use and abstinence self-efficacy. Subst. Use Misuse 57, 222–229. doi: 10.1080/10826084.2021.2002899

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dvorak, R. D., Stevenson, B. L., Kilwein, T. M., Sargent, E. M., Dunn, M. E., Leary, A. V., et al. (2018). Tension reduction and affect regulation: an examination of mood indices on drinking and non-drinking days among university student drinkers. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 26, 377–390. doi: 10.1037/pha0000210

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Enns, A., and Orpana, H. (2020). Autonomy, competence and relatedness and cannabis and alcohol use among youth in Canada: a cross-sectional analysis. Health Promot. Chron. Dis. Prev. Canada 40, 201–210. doi: 10.24095/hpcdp.40.5/6.09

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Foster, D. W., Neighbors, C., and Krieger, H. (2015). Alcohol evaluations and acceptability: examining descriptive and injunctive norms among heavy drinkers. Addict. Behav. 42, 101–107. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.11.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gagné, M., Forest, J., Vansteenkiste, M., Crevier-Braud, L., van den Broeck, A., Aspeli, A. K., et al. (2015). The multidimensional work motivation scale: validation evidence in seven languages and nine countries. Eur. J. Work Organ. Psychol. 24, 178–196. doi: 10.1080/1359432X.2013.877892

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gorka, S. M., Hedeker, D., Piasecki, T. M., and Mermelstein, R. (2017). Impact of alcohol use motives and internalizing symptoms on mood changes in response to drinking: an ecological momentary assessment investigation. Drug Alcohol Depend. 173, 31–38. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2016.12.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ham, L. S., Zamboanga, B. L., Bacon, A. K., and Garcia, T. A. (2009). Drinking motives as mediators of social anxiety and hazardous drinking among college students. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 38, 133–145. doi: 10.1080/16506070802610889

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasking, P., Lyvers, M., and Carlopio, C. (2011). The relationship between coping strategies, alcohol expectancies, drinking motives and drinking behaviour. Addict. Behav. 36, 479–487. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.01.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hingson, R. W., Zha, W., and White, A. M. (2017). Drinking beyond the binge threshold: predictors, consequences, and changes in the U.S. Am. J. Prev. Med. 52, 717–727. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2017.02.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofmann, W., Friese, M., and Strack, F. (2009). Impulse and self-control from a dual-systems perspective. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 4, 162–176. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01116.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hout, M. C., Papesh, M. H., and Goldinger, S. D. (2013). Multidimensional scaling. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 4, 93–103. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1203

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hubley, A. M., and Zumbo, B. D., (eds.). (2017). Understanding and Investigating Response Processes in Validation Research, 1st Edn. Cham: Springer International Publishing.

Google Scholar

Jajodia, A., and Earleywine, M. (2003). Measuring alcohol expectancies with the implicit association test. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 17, 126–133. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.17.2.126

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnston, L. D., O'Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., and Schulenberg, J. E. (2009). Monitoring the Future National Survey Results on Drug Use, 1975-2008. Volume I: Secondary School Students (NIH Publication 09–7402). Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse.

Google Scholar

Kline, R. B. (2016). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling, 4th Edn. New York, NY: Guildford Press.

Google Scholar

Knee, C. R., and Neighbors, C. (2002). Self-determination, perception of peer pressure, and drinking among college students. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 32, 522–543. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2002.tb00228.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koski-Jännes, A. (1994). Drinking-related locus of control as a predictor of drinking after treatment. Addict. Behav. 19, 491–495. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(94)90004-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuntsche, E., and Cooper, M. L. (2010). Drinking to have fun and to get drunk: motives as predictors of weekend drinking over and above usual drinking habits. Drug Alcohol Depend. 110, 259–262. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.02.021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuntsche, E., Knibbe, R., Engels, R., and Gmel, G. (2007). Drinking motives as mediators of the link between alcohol expectancies and alcohol use among adolescents. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs 68, 76–85. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2007.68.76

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuntsche, E., Knibbe, R., Gmel, G., and Engels, R. (2005). Why do young people drink? A review of drinking motives. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 25, 841–861. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.06.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuntsche, E., and Müller, S. (2012). Why do young people start drinking: motives for first-time alcohol consumption and links to risky drinking in early adolescence. Eur. Addict. Res. 18, 34–39. doi: 10.1159/000333036

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kyriazos, T. A. (2018). Applied psychometrics: writing-up a factor analysis construct validation study with examples. Psychology 9, 2503–2530. doi: 10.4236/psych.2018.911144

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lassi, G., Taylor, A. E., Mahedy, L., Heron, J., Eisen, T., and Munafò, M. R. (2019). Locus of control is associated with tobacco and alcohol consumption in young adults of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. R. Soc. Open Sci. 6:181133. doi: 10.1098/rsos.181133

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Levesque, C. S., Williams, G. C., Elliot, D., Pickering, M. A., Bodenhamer, B., and Finley, P. J. (2006). Validating the theoretical structure of the Treatment Self-Regulation Questionnaire (TSRQ) across three different health behaviors. Health Educ. Res. 22, 691–702. doi: 10.1093/her/cyl148

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

MacCallum, R. C., and Austin, J. T. (2000). Applications of structural equation modeling in psychological research. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 51, 201–226. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.201

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Markland, D., and Tobin, V. (2004). A modification to the Behavioural Regulation in Exercise Questionnaire to include an assessment of amotivation. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 26, 191–196. doi: 10.1123/jsep.26.2.191

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Montes, K. S., Witkiewitz, K., Andersson, C., Fossos-Wong, N., Pace, T., Berglund, M., et al. (2017). Trajectories of positive alcohol expectancies and drinking: an examination of young adults in the US and Sweden. Addict. Behav. 73, 74–80. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.04.021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mundfrom, D. J., Shaw, D. G., and Ke, T. L. (2005). Minimum sample size recommendations for conducting factor analyses. Int. J. Test. 5, 159–168. doi: 10.1207/s15327574ijt0502_4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Muthén, B. O., du Toit, S. H. C., and Spisic, D. (1997). Robust Inference Using Weighted Least Squares and Quadratic Estimating Equations in Latent Variable Modeling with Categorical and Continuous Outcomes. Available at: https:/www.statmodel.com/download/Article_075.pdf (accessed January 28, 2024).

Google Scholar

Neighbors, C., Larimer, M. E., Markman Geisner, I., and Knee, C. R. (2004). Feeling controlled and drinking motives among college students: contingent self-esteem as a mediator. Self Identity 3, 207–224. doi: 10.1080/13576500444000029

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Neighbors, C., Lewis, M. A., Fossos, N., and Grossbard, J. R. (2007). “Motivation and risk behaviors: a self-determination perspective,” in Psychology of Motivation, ed. L. V. Brown (Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers), 99–113.

Google Scholar

Neighbors, C., Walker, D. D., and Larimer, M. E. (2003). Expectancies and evaluations of alcohol effects among college students: self-determination as a moderator. J. Stud. Alcohol. 64, 292–300. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2003.64.292

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ng, J. Y. Y., Ntoumanis, N., Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C., Deci, E. L., Ryan, R. M., Duda, J. L., et al. (2012). Self-Determination Theory applied to health contexts: a meta-analysis. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 7, 325–340. doi: 10.1177/1745691612447309

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ntoumanis, N., Ng, J. Y. Y., Prestwich, A., Quested, E., Hancox, J. E., Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C., et al. (2020). A meta-analysis of Self-Determination Theory-informed intervention studies in the health domain: effects on motivation, health behavior, physical, and psychological health. Health Psychol. Rev. 15, 214–244. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2020.1718529

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Patrick, M. E., Evans-Polce, R., Kloska, D. D., Maggs, J. L., and Lanza, S. T. (2017). Age-related changes in associations between reasons for alcohol use and high-intensity drinking across young adulthood. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs 78, 558–570. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2017.78.558

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Patrick, M. E., Terry-McElrath, Y. M., Miech, R. A., Keyes, K. M., Jager, J., and Schulenberg, J. E. (2022). Alcohol use and the COVID-19 pandemic: historical trends in drinking, contexts, and reasons for use among U.S. adults. Soc. Sci. Med. 301:114887. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2022.114887

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Patrick, M. E., Wray-Lake, L., Finlay, A. K., and Maggs, J. L. (2010). The long arm of expectancies: adolescent alcohol expectancies predict adult alcohol use. Alcohol Alcohol. 45, 17–24. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agp066

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Peele, S., and Brodsky, A. (2000). Exploring psychological benefits associated with moderate alcohol use: a necessary corrective to assessments of drinking outcomes? Drug Alcohol Depend. 60, 221–247. doi: 10.1016/S0376-8716(00)00112-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Piasecki, T. M., Cooper, M. L., Wood, P. K., Sher, K. J., Shiffman, S., and Heath, A. C. (2014). Dispositional drinking motives: associations with appraised alcohol effects and alcohol consumption in an ecological momentary assessment investigation. Psychol. Assess. 26, 363–369. doi: 10.1037/a0035153

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2020). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available at: https://www.r-project.org (accessed July 30, 2020).

Google Scholar

Richards, D. K., Morera, O. F., and Field, C. A. (2020a). The psychometric properties of a version of the Treatment Self-Regulation Questionnaire for assessing motivations for responsible drinking. J. Am. Coll. Health 69, 742–749. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2019.1706536

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Richards, D. K., Pearson, M. R., and Field, C. A. (2020b). Profiles of motivations for responsible drinking among college students:a Self-Determination Theory perspective. Addict. Behav. 111:106550. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106550

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Richards, D. K., Pearson, M. R., and Field, C. A. (2021a). Further validation of the Treatment Self-Regulation Questionnaire for assessing motivations for responsible drinking: a test of Self-Determination Theory. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 29, 679–688. doi: 10.1037/pha0000405

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Richards, D. K., Pearson, M. R., and Witkiewitz, K. (2021b). Understanding alcohol harm reduction behaviors from the perspective of Self-Determination Theory: a research agenda. Addict. Res. Theory 29, 392–397. doi: 10.1080/16066359.2020.1863378

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosseel, Y. (2012). lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. J. Stat. Softw. 48(2), 1–36. doi: 10.18637/jss.v048.i02

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roth, G., Assor, A., Kanat-Maymon, Y., and Kaplan, H. (2006). Assessing the experience of autonomy in new cultures and contexts. Motiv. Emot. 30, 361–372. doi: 10.1007/s11031-006-9052-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of causality and internalization: examining reasons for acting in two domains. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 57, 749–761. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.5.749

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-Determination Theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am. Psychol. 55, 68–78. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2007). “Active human nature: Self-Determination Theory and the promotion and maintenance of sport, exercise, and health,” in Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Exercise and Sport, eds. M. S. Hagger and N. Chatzisarantis (Leeds: Human Kinetics Europe Ltd), 1–19.

Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L., (eds.). (2017). Self-Determination Theory: Basic Psychological Needs in Motivation, Development, and Wellness. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., Patrick, H., Deci, E. L., and Williams, G. C. (2008). Facilitating health behaviour change and its maintenance: interventions based on Self-Determination Theory. Eur. Health Psychol. 10, 2–5.

Google Scholar

Sæther, S. M. M., Knapstad, M., Askeland, K. G., and Skogen, J. C. (2019). Alcohol consumption, life satisfaction and mental health among Norwegian college and university students. Addict. Behav. Rep. 10:100216. doi: 10.1016/j.abrep.2019.100216

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sass, D. A., and Schmitt, T. A. (2013). “Testing measurement and structural invariance,” in Handbook of Quantitative Methods for Educational Research, ed. T. Teo (Rotterdam: SensePublishers), 315–345.

Google Scholar

Schulenberg, J. E., Johnston, L. D., O'Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., Miech, R., and Patrick, M. E. (2020). “Monitoring the future national survey results on drug use, 1975-2019, Vol 2,” in College Students and Adults Ages 19-60. Available at: https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED608266.pdf (accessed January 21, 2021).

Google Scholar

Sharma, M., and Smith, L. (2011). Self determination theory and potential applications to alcohol and drug abuse behaviors. J. Alcohol Drug Educ. 55, 3–7. Available at: http://www.jstor.org/stable/45128443

Google Scholar

Sheeran, P., Wright, C. E., Avishai, A., Villegas, M. E., Lindemans, J. W., Klein, W. M. P., et al. (2020). Self-Determination Theory interventions for health behavior change: meta-analysis and meta-analytic structural equation modeling of randomized controlled trials. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 88, 726–737. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000501

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheeran, P., Wright, C. E., Avishai, A., Villegas, M. E., Rothman, A. J., and Klein, W. M. P. (2021). Does increasing autonomous motivation or perceived competence lead to health behavior change? A meta-analysis. Health Psychol. 40, 706–716. doi: 10.1037/hea0001111

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheldon, K. M., Osin, E. N., Gordeeva, T. O., Suchkov, D. D., and Sychev, O. A. (2017). Evaluating the dimensionality of Self-Determination Theory's Relative Autonomy Continuum. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 43, 1215–1238. doi: 10.1177/0146167217711915

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sher, K. J., and Grekin, E. R. (2007). “Alcohol and affect regulation,” in Handbook of Emotion Regulation (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 560–580.

Google Scholar

Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration Office of Applied Studies (2021). Key Substance Use and Mental Health Indicators in the United States: Results from the 2020 National Survey on Drug Use and Health. Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. Available at: https://www.samhsa.gov/data (accessed April 4, 2022).

Google Scholar

Sudhinaraset, M., Wigglesworth, C., and Takeuchi, D. T. (2016). Social and cultural contexts of alcohol use: influences in a social-ecological framework. Alcohol Res. Curr. Rev. 38, 35–45.

Google Scholar

Thompson, B. (1994). The pivotal role of replication in psychological research: empirically evaluating the replicability of sample results. J. Pers. 62, 157–176. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1994.tb00289.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, B. (2013). Overview of Traditional/Classical Statistical Approaches. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

White, A. (2020). Gender differences in the epidemiology of alcohol use and related harms in the United States. Alcohol Res. Curr. Rev. 40:01. doi: 10.35946/arcr.v40.2.01

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

White, A., Castle, I. P., Chen, C. M., Shirley, M., Roach, D., and Hingson, R. (2015). Converging patterns of alcohol use and related outcomes among females and males in the United States, 2002 to 2012. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 39, 1712–1726. doi: 10.1111/acer.12815

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiers, R. W., Van Woerden, N., Smulders, F. T. Y., and De Jong, P. J. (2002). Implicit and explicit alcohol-related cognitions in heavy and light drinkers. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 111, 648–658. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.111.4.648

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wray, T. B., Simons, J. S., Dvorak, R. D., and Gaher, R. M. (2012). Trait-based affective processes in alcohol-involved “risk behaviors.” Addict. Behav. 37, 1230–1239. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.06.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: alcohol drinking, motivation, Self-Determination Theory, confirmatory factor analysis, validation

Citation: Courtney JB, Russell MA and Conroy DE (2025) Assessing self-determined motivation for drinking alcohol via the Comprehensive Relative Autonomy Index for Drinking. Front. Psychol. 15:1354545. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1354545

Received: 13 December 2023; Accepted: 16 December 2024;
Published: 08 January 2025.

Edited by:

Pietro Cipresso, University of Turin, Italy

Reviewed by:

Maria Rita Sergi, University of G.'d'Annunzio, Italy
Nicoletta Cera, University of Porto, Portugal
Francesca Borghesi, University of Turin, Italy

Copyright © 2025 Courtney, Russell and Conroy. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jimikaye Beck Courtney, amltaWtheWVAdW5jLmVkdQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.