Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol.
Sec. Psychology of Language
Volume 15 - 2024 | doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1352280
This article is part of the Research Topic Experimental Approaches to the Acquisition of Information Structure View all 8 articles

Prosodic focus marking in Seoul Korean-speaking children: The use of prosodic phrasing

Provisionally accepted
  • 1 School of Foreign Languages, Tianjin University, Tianjin, China
  • 2 Hanyang Institute for Phonetics and Cognitive Sciences Of Language (HIPCS), College of Humanities, Hanyang University, Seoul, Republic of Korea
  • 3 Department of English Education, Hongik University, Seoul, Republic of Korea
  • 4 Institute of Linguistics, Utrecht University, Utrecht, Netherlands

The final, formatted version of the article will be published soon.

    Prosodic focus marking in Seoul Korean is known to be achieved primarily through prosodic phrasing, different from the use of prosody for this purpose in many other languages. This study investigates how children use prosodic phrasing for focus-marking purposes in Seoul Korean, compared to adults. Using a picture-matching game, we elicited semi-spontaneous production of SOV sentences in various focus conditions from monolingual Seoul Korean-speaking children aged four to eleven years. We found that the children varied prosodic boundaries to distinguish narrow focus from pre-focus and broad focus in a largely adult-like manner at the age of four to five; at this age, they did not distinguish narrow focus from post-focus or contrastive focus using prosodic boundaries, similar to the adults. Their use of the prosodic boundaries in distinguishing the focus conditions was not fully adult-like in terms of frequency until the age of ten to eleven. In conjunction with the findings of previous studies on the acquisition of focus marking in Germanic languages, performed using a similar experimental method, our findings suggest that Seoul Korean-speaking children acquire the use of prosodic phrasing earlier than Dutch-speaking children acquiring the use of pitch accent but slightly later than Stockholm Swedish-speaking children acquiring the use of a prominence-marking high tone. These findings imply that the rate of focus-marking acquisition depends on the transparency of the form-meaning mapping between the phonological cue and focus.

    Keywords: acquisition, focus, prosodic phrasing, prosodic marking of focus, Seoul Korean

    Received: 07 Dec 2023; Accepted: 30 May 2024.

    Copyright: © 2024 Yang, Cho, Kim and Chen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

    * Correspondence:
    Anqi Yang, School of Foreign Languages, Tianjin University, Tianjin, China
    Aoju Chen, Institute of Linguistics, Utrecht University, Utrecht, Netherlands

    Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.