Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Psychol., 31 January 2024
Sec. Organizational Psychology

Attachment to robots and therapeutic efficiency in mental health

  • 1Department of Personality and Clinical Psychology, Pázmány Péter Catholic University, Budapest, Hungary
  • 2Research Unit for Robophilosophy and Integrative Social Robotics, Jewish Charity Hospital, Budapest, Hungary
  • 3Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark

Previous studies in the mental health context have demonstrated that interactions with social robots can improve the mood and cognitive capacities of their users, and enhance their quality of life. In this Perspective article, our goal is to systematize the possible roles of social robots and to point out that different roles require different levels of attachment. We argue that the attachment between the client and the (robot) therapist is a fundamental ingredient of any helping relationship and that the full potential of using social robots in mental health settings can only be realized if the strength of attachment is appropriately correlated with the type of relationship established.

Introduction: robots in mental health

Social robots are robots that are “designed to interact with people in human-centric terms and to operate in human environments alongside people” (Breazeal et al., 2016). In their interactions with humans, these systems follow the behavioral norms and expectations that are defining features of social interactions, such as emotional expressiveness, verbal communication, user engagement, and an appealing physical appearance (Scassellati et al., 2012).

The rolesocial robots can play in delivering mental health care interventions for children and older adults (especially those with ASD and dementia, respectively) has been widely studied (for recent reviews see Cifuentes et al., 2020, Marchetti et al., 2022). These investigations have shown that social robots can be effective in engaging users, improving their mental health, mood and cognitive capacities, and enhancing their quality of life.

According to a recent review, in mental health settings, social robots are typically used in three major contexts: acting as therapists/coaches, mediators, or assistants (David et al., 2014). In this Perspectives article, our goal is to refine this distinction about the possible roles of social robots and to point out that different roles require different levels of attachment. We will argue that the attachment between the client and the robot is a fundamental ingredient of any helping relationship and that the full potential of using social robots in mental health settings can only be realized if the strength of attachment is appropriately correlated with the type of relationship established.

The role of attachment in human-led therapy

Thinking about attachment in robot therapy should be guided by our understanding of the role attachment plays in traditional (that is, human-led) psychotherapy.

An influential framework from this perspective is Bowlby’s attachment theory (Bowlby, 1982). This theory posits that the psychotherapeutic relationship can offer a significant tool that aids the client in transitioning from insecure to secure attachment. Bowlby suggests that the role of the therapist is to act as an attachment figure by creating a secure base to enable the exploration of attachment-related experiences and to provide corrective emotional experiences to disconfirm insecure working models (Sherman et al., 2015; Degnan et al., 2016; Fraley and Roisman, 2019).

More recent research indicates that clients can develop more secure attachments as a result of therapy (Taylor et al., 2015; Gagliardi, 2022). From this perspective, the central question is whether the therapeutic relationship can truly function as an attachment relationship. Mallinckrodt (2010) identifies five salient characteristics of attachment relationships and analyses evidence to ascertain if the therapeutic relationship meets these criteria. The five key characteristics are: (1) the attachment figure is a target for proximity seeking; (2) the attachment figure supplies a safe haven to offer comfort during periods of distress; (3) the attachment figure lends a sense of security that allows the individual to explore; (4) the individual experiences separation anxiety when the attachment figure is unavailable; and (5) the attachment figure is stronger and wiser than the individual. Although the final aspect is not essential for adult attachment relationships, the rest are observable in therapeutic relationships (Mallinckrodt, 2010). That is, therapeutic relationships do have the potential to serve as attachment relationships, hence they hold the capacity to modify insecure internal working models.

Although some therapeutic relationships manifest all the critical features of attachment, this does not imply that most therapeutic relationships are attachment relationships. The efficacy of many therapeutic approaches does not require attachment, and time constraints on therapeutic relationships often prevent the formation of a fully secure attachment. Still, improving the client’s attachment style via the development of proximity seeking, safe haven, and secure base may be critical goals for the therapist (Mallinckrodt, 2010).

Offering a different perspective, Saunders et al. (2011) propose that the therapist serves as an “alternative support figure” (i.e., adults apart from parents who provide support). If the patient manages to foster a trusting relationship with the therapist, the patient can nurture feelings of worthiness, cultivate a more positive self-perception, and potentially develop a stronger capacity for reflective functioning. Reflective functioning is defined as one’s ability to contemplate their own experiences to draw conclusions about their mental state and that of others (Fonagy et al., 1996).

Attachment to robots and therapy

Objects can elicit powerful emotions that extend beyond liking (Norman, 2004). People can become attached to objects, such that they feel a psychological or emotional bond with them (Norberg and Rucker, 2021). Neuroimaging data show that mental processes similar to those involved in perceiving humans are triggered when people anthropomorphise non-human objects (Waytz et al., 2010). Attributing humanlike properties and characteristics to nonhuman agents and objects is at the core of anthropomorphism (Epley et al., 2007). Anthropomorphism can in turn transform human-to-object interactions into human-to-human-like interactions and result in object attachment by fulfilling human needs related to comfort and pleasantness, self-identity, and self-efficacy (Wan and Chen, 2021). Such formation of psychological and emotional bonds (i.e., attachment) can even take the form of companionship (perceived) friendship, or love (e.g., Ki et al., 2020).

Findings reporting empathy with robot ‘pain’ (Suzuki et al., 2015), concerns and pity for robots that are tortured (Rosenthal-von der Pütten et al., 2013), hesitation to strike a robot (Darling et al., 2015) and an overlap in neural activity associated with empathy toward humans and empathy toward robots (Rosenthal-von der Pütten et al., 2013; Chin et al., 2023) indicate that humans do feel empathy for robots. It has also been shown that inducing empathy triggers prosocial human behavior (i.e., increased helpfulness) toward a robot (Kühnlenz et al., 2013), that people seem to be inclined to help a robot find its way (Weiss et al., 2010), and that they empathize with a robot when something bad happens to it (Seo et al., 2015).

All these findings corroborate that the emotional bond and attachment that people feel toward robots is comparable to attachment to humans. Given this, for effective robot-based therapy, the relationship between the client and the robot therapist has to have at least the potential to develop the critical features that define an attachment bond. In such a therapeutic relationship, the client: (1) seeks proximity to the robot therapist in the form of an emotional connection that deepens over the course of regular meetings; (2) is willing to rely upon the robot therapist as a safe haven when feeling threatened or psychologically injured; (3) derives a sense of felt security from the robot therapist, who then functions as a secure base to facilitate healthy growth; (4) experiences separation anxiety when the robot therapist is temporarily unavailable, or as the anticipated end of the relationship approaches; and (5) perceives the robot therapist as stronger and wiser due to its training and experience.

The major roles of social robots in mental health settings

Not all roles that robots can play in mental health settings require the same level of attachment that ideally characterises therapeutic relationships. David et al. (2014) categorize the possible roles as follows. As assistants or tools, social robots are used for assessment/diagnosis and the development and practice of social skills. As mediators, robots enable or facilitate the progress of treatment by acting as an intermediary in interactions between the therapist and the client, and are sources of motivation and encouragement, rendering the treatment engaging. Finally, as therapists or coaches, robots themselves deliver psychotherapy (while their activities are determined and overviewed by practitioners).

This categorization, however, is too coarse-grained and oversimplifies the distinctions between different use cases. An analysis of the literature reveals at least six different categories. Robots can be used as diagnostic tools, interview mediators, promoters of social connections, coaches, social companions and therapists. As we argue below, the strength of attachment evoked by these different roles is distinct in each case, which allows us to systematically position these roles along the dimension of the required level of attachment (see Figure 1).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. An ordering of the roles robots can play in mental health settings in terms of the level of attachment required by the specific roles in question.

Robots as diagnostic tools

Applied together with traditional interviews and self-report inventories, robots can help behavioral assessments and inform a more thorough diagnostic evaluation. For example, in the context of diagnosing autistic spectrum disorder (ASD), robots can elicit social responses, which then have high diagnostic value (Diehl et al., 2012). Similarly, in the case of social anxiety, robot-based behavioral assessment tests have discriminative validity in distinguishing between young people with and without social anxiety disorder (Rasouli et al., 2022).

Given that this type of connection is temporary and does not aim to develop the feeling of safe haven or secure base functions, for this type of usage only a low level of attachment is needed (if any, as attachment usually does not play a role in one’s relation to, e.g., a paper-and-pencil type of personality test).

Robot-mediated interview

In case of excessive social anxiety, interview situations and initial treatment sessions may be anxiety-provoking—and this could interfere with the assessment process. One way to decrease these negative effects could be the involvement of a social robot in the interview process. Studies show thatthe responds from children, their engagement and the content provided (i.e., amount and types of information) are similar in the case of robotic interviewers than in the case of a human interviewer (Wood et al., 2013a,b). Some children with special needs are even more interested in and cooperate better with robotic than human interviewers. For instance, when interviewed by a robot, children reported occurrences of bullying significantly more (Bethel et al., 2016).

Since in such cases of robot-mediated interviews, the connection to the robot is established by mutual (verbal and non-verbal) communication, its attachment-evoking effect is theoretically stronger than in the case when a robot plays the role of a purely diagnostic tool.

Robots promoting social connections

One of the main goals of rehabilitation and therapy processes is to increase the frequency of prosocial behaviors. As certain findings suggest, a robot can encourage such desirable prosocial behavior. A robot can elicit and guide joint attention (Dautenhahn, 2003) and can serve as a “catalyst” for social interactions with another individual (Feil-Seifer and Matarić, 2011). Human social mediators can facilitate human interactions in various social settings, such as school classrooms or formal meetings. In a similar vein, it is possible to use a robot as a social mediator (Gillet et al., 2020). Studies show that social robots acting as mediators have been effective in improving human interaction through numerous aspects, such as facilitating conversation, engagement, task collaboration and participation (Adikari et al., 2023). For example, a robot can encourage a child with ASD to interact with an interlocutor who is present in the therapeutic setting (Dautenhahn, 2003). Similarly, it has been observed that two children with ASD continued playing a ball game with each other after learning it from a robot (Costa et al., 2010).

This kind of connection means more involvement than a “simple” interview but requires less attachment than coaching, which is typically a longer-term relationship with the specific target of changing at least certain behavioral patterns.

Robots as coaches

Social robots can act as instructors or a coaches to monitor and engage users in a highly personalized way to improve their social, physical, or cognitive well-being. For instance, the social robot Autom is a behavior change coach facilitating sustained engagement in a diet and exercise program (Kidd and Breazeal, 2008). It can track participants’ weight loss and can provide personalized feedback. According to the findings, this form of coaching is more engaging than computerized or paper-based logs, and the participants felt a form of working alliance and close relationship with the robot. Jeong et al. (2023) used Jibo, a social robot companion as a positive psychology coach to provide positive psychology coaching for university students. After seven sessions of interacting with the robot, participants showed statistically significant improvement in their psychological well-being, mood, and readiness to change health-related behaviors. Students expressed appreciation for the robot’s companionship, desire to talk and communicate with it, and the feeling of attachment to it.

As this type of role in general necessitates a longer relationship than the previous one with the added goal of changing behavioral patterns, it also requires stronger attachment. Compared to social companionship (see below), however, it is less demanding, since whereas in coaching the connection between the robot and the client is extrinsically motivated and focuses on some external reinforcement, in social companionship the motivation of the connection is much more intrinsic.

Robots as social companions

Increased loneliness and social isolation may affect a third of the world’s population, and come with serious health-related consequences, such as increased risk for mental illness, obesity, dementia, and early death (Broadbent et al., 2023). Without a doubt, the most ideal solution for this problem would be a human companion for everyone. But in reality, it is increasingly difficult to make new friends as an adult, so relying on companion robots to support socially isolated adults may prove to be a promising compromise. As investigated in the context of elderly care and children with ASD, the relation to social companion robots seems to be still ambivalent. Konok et al. (2018), for instance, compared people’s reactions to social robots and dogs and found that people’s attitude toward robots is much more negative than toward dogs. The main advantage of dogs over robots seems to be the presence of emotions (e.g., love, faithfulness, kindness) and attachment behavior (e.g., the dog seeks the proximity of the owner and shows stress behavior when separated from the owner).

In the future, newer robots leveraging advanced AI algorithms may foster stronger social connections with humans than earlier generations of robots. Generative AI, like ChatGPT, which is based on large language models, allows robots to engage in more spontaneous conversations, which will support the social companion role.

In addition to the advancement of AI algorithms, the results from studying human-dog connections could also shape human-robot attachment. Based on the differences between the attachment to dogs and social robots, Konok et al. (2018) suggest the following considerations for planning more attachment-prone social robots. The attitudes toward companion robots might be improved by implementing behaviors that trigger the users to attribute emotions and personality to the robot. Based on the fact that in the case of dogs it is preferred if they are not perfectly obedient, minor disobedience and imperfectness might render robots more “real” or “alive” as if they had their own personality. Similarly to dogs, robots or artificial agents should also be equipped with an ‘attachment system’ (Kovács et al., 2011; Ichikawa et al., 2012) that is able to recognize, prefer and maintain proximity to their users, show signs of stress when separated from and greet them happily when reunited with the users (Konok et al., 2018). Based on pet-attachment results, initiating physical contact might also be useful, if the physical parameters of the robot make it possible, safe (see, e.g., Haddadin et al., 2008), enjoyable and if it fits the personality and preferences of the user (Walters et al., 2005).

Robots as therapists

As already mentioned, one of the main goals of the therapist during any kind of psychotherapy is to act as an attachment figure and create a secure base for the patient to enable exploration of attachment-related experiences and to provide a corrective emotional experience. This corrective emotional experience can reinforce the patient’s capacity for reflective functioning (i.e., to make inferences about the mental states of oneself and others; see Fonagy et al., 1996). Whereas in the case of coaching the focus is on behavioral change, in the case of therapy, the goal often includes improving self-reflection, self-knowledge, and the capacity for mentalization. These goals require a high level of attachment. One could argue that the social companion role requires stronger attachment than the therapist role, as the latter only relies on an “alternative support connection” (Saunders et al., 2011). However, whereas psychotherapy does have indeed an “unrealistic” relationship layer (based on transference and countertransference), it has a real relationship feature as well that is based on attachment and secure rapport (Saeed, 2000).

We can only speak about robot-led psychotherapy if the “robot therapist” can accomplish these requirements for attachment. Various studies show that social robots prompt people to develop emotional bonds (Sharkey and Sharkey, 2021; Law et al., 2022), and it is also known that forming strong emotional bonds often leads to the development of attachment relations (Bowlby, 1982; Cassidy and Shaver, 2016) and that physical availability and emotional connection are the two main communication channels that are relevant in attachment formation (Gagliardi, 2021). However, it is still an open question whether the features of this kind of attachment are suitable for reaching the full potential of psychotherapy.

The future of human-robot attachment in mental health care

Because of the fundamentally social nature of humans, our level of social connectedness is strongly associated with the markers of mental and physical well-being (Kim and Sul, 2023). In the case of treatments in a mental health context, the process of healing (i.e., the speed of recovery) heavily depends on the quality of the attachment between the client and the patient (Mallinckrodt, 2010).

In this paper, we have shown that it is possible to distinguish between different use cases from this perspective. Some roles that robots could play in mental health do not rely on attachment but others can only be effective if the robot can accomplish the safe haven function.

For future developments in the area, it is crucial to focus on the formation of emotional bonds between robots and patients, ideally from an integrative point of view combining social neuroscience, computer science and robotics (see, e.g., Cross et al., 2019). It could also be fruitful to examine the neurological responses of clients to social robots in different types of helping relationships. We hypothesize that at a lower level of attachment (e.g., robots as diagnostic tools) the neurological responses to the robot will be different than at a higher level (e.g., robots as therapists).

As Kaplan (2001) stated, the “Turing test” for social robots is to “pass” the attachment test; that is, to show and trigger attachment behavior to and from the user. Reaching this level of development, the next question is the effect of human-robot attachment. In “traditional” (that is human-led) therapies the secure attachment between the patient and the therapist has the potential to modify the insecure attachment working model of the patient. In successful therapy, the secure base and safe haven functions of the therapist become generalized to other connections. Future studies have to answer whether this generalized positive effect can occur in robot-based psychotherapy as well. Ideally, it should happen because the final and utmost goal of any kind of psychotherapy is to help the patient build deeper, more satisfying and secure connections with other humans.

Conclusion

Social robots could be used in the full spectrum of mental health care, they could decrease the so-called “treatment gap” (the burden implied by the lack of human mental health professionals), and they could also increase the quality of treatments. However, for their effectiveness, their capacity to trigger attachment feelings and behaviors in patients will need to be improved and carefully fine-tuned in line with the requirements of the specific roles the robots would play.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.

Author contributions

MS: Conceptualization, Methodology, Resources, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. PF: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This study was supported by the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme under the Marie Skłodowska Curie grant agreement (no. 754513), and the Aarhus University Research Foundation. We also gratefully acknowledge support by the Nordforsk Foundation for grant nr. 105081 (Facilitation Robotics for Teams - Foundational Research on Group Processes in Human-Robot Ensembles with Social Robots (SFACTOR)).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Adikari, M., Cangelosi, A., and Gomez, R. (2023). Social Robot Mediator for Multiparty Interaction. arXiv:2310.13508. Available at: https://ui.adsabs.harvard.edu/abs/2023arXiv231013508A (Accessed October 1, 2023).

Google Scholar

Bethel, C. L., Henkel, Z., Stives, K., May, D. C., Eakin, D. K., Pilkinton, M., et al. (2016). “Using robots to interview children about bullying: lessons learned from an exploratory study” in 2016 25th IEEE international symposium on robot and human interactive communication (RO-MAN). IEEE, 712–717. doi: 10.1109/ROMAN.2016.7745197

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: retrospect and prospect. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 52, 664–678. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-0025.1982.tb01456.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Breazeal, C., Dautenhahn, K., and Kanda, T. (2016). “Social robotics” in Springer handbook of robotics. eds. B. Siciliano and O. Khatib (Cham: Springer Handbooks), 1935–1972.

Google Scholar

Broadbent, E., Billinghurst, M., Boardman, S. G., and Doraiswamy, P. M. (2023). Enhancing social connectedness with companion robots using AI. Sci. Robot. 8:eadi6347. doi: 10.1126/scirobotics.adi6347

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cassidy, J., and Shaver, P.R. (2016). Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical application. New York: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Chin, J. H., Haring, K. S., and Kim, P. (2023). Understanding the neural mechanisms of empathy toward robots to shape future applications. Front. Neurorobot. 17:https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnbot.2023.1145989. doi: 10.3389/fnbot.2023.1145989

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cifuentes, C. A., Pinto, M. J., Céspedes, N., and Múnera, M. (2020). Social robots in therapy and care. Curr. Robot Rep. 1, 59–74. doi: 10.1007/s43154-020-00009-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, S., Santos, C., Soares, F., Ferreira, M., and Moreira, F. (2010). “Promoting interaction amongst autistic adolescents using robots” in 32nd annual international conference of the IEEE/EMBS; Buenos Aires, Argentina, 3856–3859.

Google Scholar

Cross, E. S., Hortensius, R., and Wykowska, A. (2019). From social brains to social robots: applying neurocognitive insights to human–robot interaction Phil. Trans. R. Soc. 374:20180024. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2018.0024

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Darling, K., Nandy, P., and Breazeal, C. (2015). “Empathic concern and the effect of stories in human-robot interaction,” In 2015 24th IEEE international symposium on robot and human interactive communication (RO-MAN) (Kobe: IEEE), 770–775.

Google Scholar

Dautenhahn, K. (2003). Roles and functions of robots in human society: implications from research in autism therapy. Robotica 21, 443–452. doi: 10.1017/S0263574703004922

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

David, D., Matu, S., and David, O. A. (2014). Robot-based psychotherapy: concepts development, state of the art, and new directions. Int. J. Cogn. Ther. 7, 192–210. doi: 10.1521/ijct.2014.7.2.192

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Degnan, A., Seymour-Hyde, A., Harris, A., and Berry, K. (2016). The role of therapist attachment in Alliance and outcome: a systematic literature review. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 23, 47–65. doi: 10.1002/cpp.1937

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Diehl, J. J., Schmitt, L. M., Villano, M., and Crowell, C. R. (2012). The clinical use of robots for individuals with autism Spectrum disorders: a critical review. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 6, 249–262. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2011.05.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Epley, N., Waytz, A., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2007). On seeing human: a three-factor theory of anthropomorphism. Psychol. Rev. 114, 864–886. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.4.864

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Feil-Seifer, D., and Matarić, M.J. (2011). Automated detection and classification of positive vs. Negative robot interactions with children with autism using distance-based features. Proceedings of the ACM/IEEE international conference on human-robot interaction; New York, NY: ACM press; 323–330.

Google Scholar

Fonagy, P., Leigh, T., Steele, M., Steele, H., Kennedy, R., Mattoon, G., et al. (1996). The relation of attachment status, psychiatric classification, and response to psychotherapy. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 64, 22–31. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.64.1.22

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fraley, R. C., and Roisman, G. I. (2019). The development of adult attachment styles: four lessons. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 25, 26–30. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.02.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gagliardi, M. (2021). How our caregivers shape who we are: the seven dimensions of attachment at the core of personality. Front. Psychol. 12:657628. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.657628

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gagliardi, M. (2022). Human attachment as a multi-dimensional control system: a computational implementation. Front. Psych. 13:844012. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.844012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillet, S., van den Bos, W., and Leite, I. (2020). “A social robot mediator to foster collaboration and inclusion among children” in Robotics: Science and systems XVI (robotics: science and systems). eds. M. Toussaint, A. Bicchi, and T. Hermans, MIT Press Journals.

Google Scholar

Haddadin, S., Albu-Schaffer, A., De Luca, A., and Hirzinger, G. (2008). Collision Detection and Reaction: A Contribution to Safe Physical Human-Robot Interaction, 2008 IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems, Nice, France), 3356–3363.

Google Scholar

Ichikawa, T., Yuki, M., Korondi, P., Hashimoto, H., Gácsi, M., and Niitsuma, M. (2012). Impression evaluation for different behavioral characteristics in ethologically inspired human-robot communication. Proceedings - IEEE international workshop on robot and human interactive communication, 55–60. doi: 10.1109/ROMAN.2012.6343731

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeong, S., Aymerich-Franch, L., and Arias, K. (2023). Deploying a robotic positive psychology coach to improve college students’ psychological well-being. User Model User-Adap Inter 33, 571–615. doi: 10.1007/s11257-022-09337-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, F. (2001). Artificial attachment: Will a robot ever pass Ainsworth’s Strange Situation Test? in Proceedinas of Humanoids. (Ed.) S. Hashimoto, (Tokyo, Japan: IEEE-RAS International Conference of Humanoid Robots), 99–106.

Google Scholar

Ki, C.-W., Cho, E., and Lee, J.-E. (2020). Can an intelligent personal assistant (IPA) be your friend? Para-friendship development mechanism between IPAs and their users. Comput. Hum. Behav. 111:106412. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2020.106412

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kidd, C.D., and Breazeal, C. (2008). Robots at home: understanding long-term human-robot interaction, 2008 IEEE/RSJ international conference on intelligent robots and systems, Nice, France, 3230–3235.

Google Scholar

Kim, M. J., and Sul, S. (2023). On the relationship between the social brain, social connectedness, and wellbeing. Front. Psych. 14:2438. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1112438

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Konok, V., Korcsok, B., Miklósi, Á., and Gácsi, M. (2018). Should we love robots? – the most liked qualities of companion dogs and how they can be implemented in social robots. Comput. Hum. Behav. 80, 132–142. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.11.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kovács, Sz., Gácsi, M., Vincze, D., Korondi, P., and Miklósi, A. (2011). A novel, ethologically inspired HRI model implementation: Simulating dog-human attachment. In 2nd International Conference on Cognitive Infocommunications, Budapest, Hungary.

Google Scholar

Kühnlenz, B., Sosnowski, S., Buß, M., Wollherr, D., Kühnlenz, K., and Buss, M. (2013). Increasing helpfulness towards a robot by emotional adaption to the user. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 5, 457–476. doi: 10.1007/s12369-013-0182-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Law, T., Chita-Tegmark, M., Rabb, N., and Scheutz, M. (2022). Examining attachment to robots: benefits, challenges, and alternatives. J. Hum.-Robot Interact. 11, 1–18. doi: 10.1145/3526105

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mallinckrodt, B. (2010). The psychotherapy relationship as attachment: evidence and implications. J. Soc. Pers. Relatsh. 27, 262–270. doi: 10.1177/0265407509360905

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marchetti, A., Di Dio, C., Manzi, F., and Massaro, D. (2022). Robotics in clinical and Developmental Psychology. Comp. Clin. Psych. 121–140. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-818697-8.00005-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Norberg, M., and Rucker, D. (2021). The psychology of object attachment: our bond with teddy bears, coffee mugs, and wedding rings. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 39, v–x. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.03.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Norman, D.A. (2004). Emotional design: Why we love (or hate) everyday things. New York: Basic Civitas Books.

Google Scholar

Rasouli, S., Gupta, G., Nilsen, E., and Dautenhahn, K. (2022). Potential applications of social robots in robot-assisted interventions for social anxiety. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 14, 1–32. doi: 10.1007/s12369-021-00851-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenthal-von der Pütten, A. M., Krämer, N. C., Hoffmann, L., Sobieraj, S., and Eimler, S. C. (2013). An experimental study on emotional reactions towards a robot. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 5, 17–34. doi: 10.1007/s12369-012-0173-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Saeed, S. A. (2000). The psychotherapy relationship: theory, research, and practice. J. Psychother. Pract. Res. 9, 106–107.

Google Scholar

Saunders, R., Jacobvitz, D., Zaccagnino, M., Beverung, L. M., and Hazen, N. (2011). Pathways to earned-security: the role of alternative support figures. Attach Hum. Dev. 13, 403–420. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2011.584405

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Scassellati, B., Admoni, H., and Matarić, M. (2012). Robots for use in autism research. Annu. Rev. Biomed. Eng. 14, 275–294. doi: 10.1146/annurev-bioeng-071811-150036

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Seo, S. H., Geiskkovitch, D., Nakane, M., King, C., and Young, J. E. (2015). “Poor thing! Would you feel sorr for a simulated robot? A comparison of empathy toward a physical and a simulated robot” in 2015 10th ACM/IEEE international conference on human-robot interaction (HRI) (Portland, OR: IEEE), 125–132.

Google Scholar

Sharkey, A., and Sharkey, N. (2021). We need to talk about deception in social robotics! Ethics Inf. Technol. 23, 309–316. doi: 10.1007/s10676-020-09573-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherman, L. J., Rice, K., and Cassidy, J. (2015). Infant capacities related to building internal working models of attachment figures: a theoretical and empirical review. Dev. Rev. 37, 109–141. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2015.06.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Suzuki, Y., Galli, L., Ikeda, A., Itakura, S., and Kitazaki, M. (2015). Measuring empathy for human and robot hand pain using electroencephalography. Sci. Rep. 5, 1–9. doi: 10.1038/srep15924

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, P., Rietzschel, J., Danquah, A., and Berry, K. (2015). Changes in attachment representations during psychological therapy. Psychother. Res. 25, 222–238. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2014.886791

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Walters, M.L., Dautenhahn, K., Boekhorst, R., Koay, K.L., Kaouri, C., and Woods, S. (2005) The influence of subjects' personality traits on personal spatial zones in a human-robot interaction experiment. In ROMAN 2005. IEEE International Workshop on Robot and Human Interactive Communication (IEEE Robotics & Automation Society), 347–352.

Google Scholar

Wan, E. W., and Chen, R. P. (2021). Anthropomorphism and object attachment. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 39, 88–93. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.08.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Waytz, A., Morewedge, C. K., Epley, N., Monteleone, G., Gao, J.-H., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2010). Making sense by making sentient: effectance motivation increases anthropomorphism. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 99, 410–435. doi: 10.1037/a0020240

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, A., Igelsböck, J., Tscheligi, M., Bauer, A., Kühnlenz, K., Wollherr, D., et al. (2010). “Robots asking for directions–the willingness of passers-by to support robots” in In: 2010 5th ACM/IEEE international conference on human-robot interaction (HRI) (Osaka: IEEE), 23–30.

Google Scholar

Wood, L. J., Dautenhahn, K., Lehmann, H., Robins, B., Rainer, A., and Syrdal, D. S. (2013b). Robot-mediated interviews: does a robotic interviewer impact question difficulty and information recovery. Assist. Technol. 33:131. doi: 10.3233/978-1-61499-304-9-131

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, L. J., Dautenhahn, K., Rainer, A., Robins, B., Lehmann, H., and Syrdal, D. S. (2013a). Robot-mediated interviews—how effective is a humanoid robot as a tool for interviewing young children? PLoS One 8:e59448. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0059448

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: mental health, social robots, treatment gap, attachment formation, levels of attachment

Citation: Szondy M and Fazekas P (2024) Attachment to robots and therapeutic efficiency in mental health. Front. Psychol. 15:1347177. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1347177

Received: 30 November 2023; Accepted: 12 January 2024;
Published: 31 January 2024.

Edited by:

Ryuji Yamazaki-Skov (Yamazaki), Osaka University, Japan

Reviewed by:

Isabella Poggi, Roma Tre University, Italy

Copyright © 2024 Szondy and Fazekas. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mate Szondy, c3pvbmR5bWF0ZUBnbWFpbC5jb20=; Peter Fazekas, ZmF6ZWthcy5wZXRlckBnbWFpbC5jb20=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.