Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 25 July 2024
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Culture and Emotion in Educational Dynamics - Volume II View all 13 articles

Predictors of academic engagement of high school students: academic socialization and motivational beliefs

  • Department of Psychology, Wollo University, Dessie, Ethiopia

The potential of academic engagement to enhance academic outcomes and well-being has been empirically supported, yet studies addressing its predictors are too limited. Hence, the current study collected self-report cross-sectional data from randomly selected 614 (male = 323) high school students in Ethiopia and examined the relations of academic socialization (parental and peer), self-efficacy, and outcome expectations in explaining variance in academic engagement, guided by an integrative model of engagement. Structural equation modeling with the maximum likelihood method indicated that (a) the hypothesized model fit the data well, (b) direct paths from parental and peer academic socialization to self-efficacy, outcome expectations, and academic engagement were positive and significant, (c) the relationships of the constructs in the model explained a significant portion of the variance in academic engagement, and (d) self-efficacy and outcome expectations significantly and positively but partially mediated the pathway from academic socialization to academic engagement. The findings’ implications for boosting student academic engagement were forwarded.

Introduction

Researchers in education and educational psychology have given due attention to student engagement scholarship because it has been recognized mainly for the roles it plays in enhancing students’ learning (Skinner, 2016), persistence in education (Fredricks et al., 2004), academic achievement (Zumbrunn et al., 2014), school completion (Fall and Roberts, 2012), and better well-being (Wang et al., 2015; Moreira et al., 2019). Despite these and other well-noticed and duly acknowledged contributions that student engagement has, little has been done about what factors contribute to its development. This condition actually calls for a study aimed at identifying the potential antecedents of engagement and suggesting possible evidence-based interventions. Furthermore, the construct is highly responsive to changes in the environment (Wang et al., 2016) and malleable. Therefore, the purpose of the present study was to examine some contextual and personal antecedents of academic engagement among Ethiopian high school students. In this pursuit, an academic engagement model, which has been formulated based on a development-in-sociocultural context perspective (Wang et al., 2019, 2020) and prior empirical evidence, was tested with data collected from Ethiopian high school students.

Positive outcomes of academic engagement

Literature on student engagement suggests that a substantial variation exists in how engagement has been defined and conceptualized and this has resulted in (a) the inclusion of multiple variables associated with student success in school, such as students’ school-related conduct (Fredricks et al., 2004; Wang et al., 2019), school belongingness (Finn and Zimmer, 2012), self-regulation (Greene, 2015), and future aspirations and goals (Appleton et al., 2006), (b) measuring student engagement via different tools, (c) inclusion of two, three, or four dimensions (Fredricks et al., 2004; Reeve and Tseng, 2011; Wang et al., 2016), (d) confusion about the facilitators and the indicators of engagement (Lam et al., 2014), and (e) using the same items to measure different aspects of engagement (Eccles and Wang, 2012) which finally caused difficulty in comparing the reported findings.

Nevertheless, despite the discrepancies in conceptualization and measurement of student engagement, studies provided adequate empirical support for the positive effect of engagement on students’ academic performance at all educational levels (Wang and Holcombe, 2010; Dotterer and Lowe, 2011; Li and Lerner, 2011; Finn and Zimmer, 2012; Reeve and Lee, 2014; Dogan, 2017; Fung et al., 2018). The existing empirical works have witnessed the roles of student engagement in enhancing academic success and school completion (e.g., Archambault et al., 2009) and decreasing the risk of school dropout and delinquency (Fall and Roberts, 2012; Henry et al., 2012; Wang and Peck, 2013; Wang and Fredricks, 2014). Student engagement also has long-term effects on emotional well-being and adjustment. The research found that student engagement was positively associated with adjustment (Simons-Morton and Crump, 2003), use of practical coping skills (Reschly et al., 2008), less depressive symptoms (Li and Lerner, 2011), and well-being (Reschly et al., 2008; Salmela-Aro et al., 2009; Cadime et al., 2016; Boulton et al., 2019). Students with positive pathways of engagement are less likely to show problem behaviors (i.e., less likely to be involved in delinquency, serious offenses, and problem substance use) during adolescence and early adulthood (Hirschfield and Gasper, 2011; Li and Lerner, 2011; Henry et al., 2012). Findings from data spanning 40 years of life, taking into account many individual difference variables, also showed that adolescent school engagement had a positive impact on adult educational and employment outcomes (Symonds et al., 2023).

Given that academic engagement plays multifaceted roles in the life of students and it is malleable (Bresó et al., 2011; Wang and Eccles, 2012), more effort has to be invested in identifying what factors positively contribute to its development. In this regard, Alrashidi et al. (2016) noted that “it is important for researchers and educators to consider factors that might help heighten and foster students’ engagement in school and academic-related activities which, eventually, enhance students’ performance outcomes” (p. 47). Although few researchers have recently begun to respond to this call (e.g., Jang et al., 2010; Johnson and Sinatra, 2013; Zhen et al., 2018), more research focusing on what factors shape student engagement in the learning process needs to be conducted to identify appropriate areas of interventions promoting students’ academic engagement. The facilitators of student engagement can be broadly grouped into self-system (personal) and social system (contextual), although they can also be further separated into other classes of variables (Wang and Holcombe, 2010; Bresó et al., 2011; Wang and Eccles, 2012, 2013; Phan and Ngu, 2014; Reeve and Lee, 2014). In this study, academic socialization (parent & peer) represents contextual factors, while self-efficacy and outcome expectations belong to the class of personal factors.

Theoretical framework

The structural relationships among the constructs included in the study were guided by an integrative theoretical model of engagement, also called a development-in-sociocultural context perspective (Wang et al., 2019, 2020). An integrative theoretical model of engagement was developed in an attempt to respond to scholars’ accentuated need for “a synthetic, coherent framework that simultaneously integrates extant literature and clarifies the conceptualization of engagement, identifies its facilitators and consequences, and proffers a theoretical model that elaborates on how engagement functions” (Wang et al., 2019, p. 1087). In response to such need for a theoretically sound, integrative model among the scholarly community, Wang and his colleagues synthesized extant research and integrated relevant concepts from self-system theory (Skinner et al., 2009), expectancy-value theory (Eccles, 2009), and mindset theory (Dweck, 2006) to illuminate the motivational processes underlying engagement. This recent and integrative theoretical perspective accounts for how student academic engagement develops over time and provides a theoretical framework for organizing the predictors and outcomes of student engagement, depicting the structural relations between social context, ‘self,’ engagement, and outcomes (Figure 1 depicts aspects of development-in-sociocultural context model related to the present study). As to the structural relationships between contexts, self, and engagement, the integrative theoretical perspective posits multiple direct and indirect (mediated by self-related variables) causal pathways from social contexts to academic engagement. The integrative theoretical model for children’s engagement in learning is a complex developmental system that incorporates many categories of contextual and personal factors. The present study, however, examined only the structural relations of four constructs (parental academic socialization, peer academic socialization, academic self-efficacy, and educational outcome expectations) with students’ academic engagement. For example, the model posits that motivational beliefs (e.g., self-efficacy and outcome expectancy) partially mediate the causal path from social context to student engagement. According to the integrative model, students’ socialization experiences in family and peer contexts determine their self-efficacy and outcome expectations; in turn, the level of academic self-efficacy and outcome expectations influence the level of students’ academic engagement. That is, the academic socialization that children experience at home and in peer contexts over time accumulates to shape their academic self-efficacy and outcome expectations for learning, which in turn influences their engagement in learning. While the development-in-sociocultural context model suggests causal links from left to right (social context → self → engagement → outcomes), it acknowledges the possibility of intricate feedback cycles or bidirectional processes in certain instances. For example, high-quality academic engagement could reinforce children’s emerging self-appraisals and competencies and determine the level of support from adults and the peer selection process. However, variable specific evidence is needed, instead of broad categories of constructs, to consider specific paths in opposite directions.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Some aspects of development-in-sociocultural context model of engagement (Wang et al., 2019).

Academic socialization and student academic engagement

Parental involvement is vital for their children’s school engagement and success (Melby et al., 2008; Murray, 2009; Fan and Williams, 2010; Fan et al., 2012; Wilder, 2014), although the impact of each form of involvement may vary depending on the developmental stage. Among other states of parental involvement (e.g., attending school events, helping with homework), parental academic socialization (socializing messages) like relating education to future success (Wang et al., 2014), is a vital aspect of the family context for understanding how parents influence their children’s schooling and educational success during the period of adolescence (Hill and Tyson, 2009; Wang et al., 2014; Bæck, 2017).

Nowadays, academic-focused messages that parents convey to their children are considered an important aspect of parental involvement practices in influencing children’s academic outcomes (Hoover-Dempsey and Sandler, 1997; Bempechat et al., 1999; Suizzo and Soon, 2006; Hill and Tyson, 2009) and such form of parental involvement represents parental academic socialization. Academic socialization is a verbal form of parental involvement that denotes academic-related messages exchanged between the parents and adolescent students rather than direct behavioral (e.g., social control & monitoring) or instrumental involvements (e.g., assisting with schoolwork, supplying educational resources). The most common academic messages from parents, representing academic socialization, relate to the value of education, the importance of effort, the pressure to meet parental expectations, and the shame of not meeting academic standards (Bempechat et al., 1999; Suizzo and Soon, 2006). In this study, parental academic socialization included messages aimed at socializing children about the importance of academic effort, the values of education or the value of educational success, and educational expectations. Effort socialization represents those parental messages conveying the value of being effortful and hard-working, as well as associating failure in academic performance and career development with a lack of effort. It denotes inspiring the need to put forth the best academic effort for better academic and career development. Evidence indicates that effort socialization was related to motivational outcomes such as locus of control and classroom engagement (Suizzo et al., 2012, 2016). Parents also socialize their children by conveying their expectations for educational attainment. Educational expectations refer to the anticipation of accomplishment at school or university (Mello, 2008). Parents’ expectations about their children’s future achievement can be expressed in course grades, the highest level of schooling attained, or college/university attendance (Goldenberg et al., 2001; Glick and White, 2004). Research shows that children with high expectations from their parents do better academically, perform better on standardized tests, and stay in school longer than children with low expectations from their parents (Davis-Kean, 2005; Pearce, 2006; Vartanian et al., 2007). Meta-analyses studies also indicated that parental educational expectations are the strongest family-level predictor of student academic outcomes relative to other parental beliefs and behaviors (Jeynes, 2005, 2007). Therefore, educational expectations, values of education, and the importance of effort that parents convey to their children define parental academic socialization and are assumed to influence students’ academic engagement.

During the adolescence period, peer context also becomes the most crucial aspect of the social system in socializing academic behaviors of adolescent students positively (Altermatt and Pomerantz, 2003; Kindermann, 2007; Rodkin and Ryan, 2012). Peer socialization is the process by which individuals learn many things from their peers, particularly from friends with whom they spend much of their time (Ryan, 2000; Kindermann and Gest, 2009). The context of the peer group determines the degree of an individual student’s engagement in academic tasks, either by promoting or discouraging academic attitudes, values, and behaviors (Wang et al., 2018). Hanging out with peers who are academically motivated and committed increases one’s motivation and commitment over time, although the opposite is also true. Evidence from longitudinal data indicated that friends and peers do socialize with each other over time in their academic motivation, engagement, and achievement (Ryan, 2001; Altermatt and Pomerantz, 2003; Kindermann, 2007).

Observation of other peer members changes one’s cognition, behavior, or emotions (Ryan et al., 2019). The behaviors, beliefs, attitudes, values, and other characteristics reflected and demonstrated by the peers can introduce a student to new behaviors, thoughts, perspectives, and feelings either through the process of modeling, the exchange of persuasive messages, or social reinforcement (encouragement or discouragement by peers). Research has indicated that friends are similar in their effortful behavior toward their schoolwork, interest, and enjoyment in their schoolwork (Ryan, 2001; Shin and Ryan, 2014), and preference for challenges (Altermatt and Pomerantz, 2003). These similarities among friends suggest how peers matter in socializing students’ behavioral, affective, and cognitive engagement (Ryan et al., 2019). Adolescent peers interact regularly and spend more time together; hence, they have more opportunities to model and influence each other’s academic behaviors (Wentzel, 2009). However, the attention given to peers matters for students’ academic engagement is not as much as it deserves (Steenberghs et al., 2021). Specifically, peer influence through academic socialization did not receive the attention of the researchers in student engagement research.

Therefore, this study approached peer influence from an academic socialization perspective that includes three components: peers’ educational aspirations, peers’ efforts, and peers’ academic norms. The extent to which students in a peer group exhibit positive academic behaviors (e.g., completing homework, attending class, valuing high grades, etc.) represents the academic norms of peers (McCormick and Cappella, 2014). The academic norms prevailing in the peer group are assumed to be imperative in determining high school student academic engagement. Friends and classmates were found to contribute to the development of individual student’s behavioral and emotional engagement and disengagement (Steenberghs et al., 2021). Wang et al. (2018) also, using the peer nomination approach, found that over time students became more similar to the peers they nominated in their engagement. It is also important to note that in the early years of high school, students begin to think about their future education and careers; hence, they often discuss their views and aspirations with their peers (Eccles et al., 2004; Kiuru et al., 2007). Therefore, the aspirations of other students in the peer groups may be another way in which peers socialize students’ educational aspirations and academic engagement. Adolescents tend to demonstrate similar levels of school-related adjustment to others in their peer group (Ryan, 2001; Chen et al., 2003). In a collectivist society like Ethiopia, the likelihood of being influenced by the perceived views of others in the peer group is believed to be high. Accordingly, the construct of peer academic socialization included peers’ educational aspirations based on findings from other settings and the culture of current participants.

In this study, the peer/friendship group consists of individuals from different schools and classrooms but from the same grade level because a student in a collective society could have friends who are not in the same school but in the same residential area. In the context of Ethiopia, beyond the time spent in the classroom, students walk a long distance in a group from home to school or vis-versa which allows them to exchange more information and influence each other’s behavior. In addition to walking a long distance in a group, students from different schools could meet regularly at home or in areas around their homes to do schoolwork and study for tests together, allowing for more discussions and information exchange about school lessons. As a result, feelings, behaviors, and thoughts regarding learning and school might be shared repeatedly among peer members and influence what students feel, think, and do in school. In Ethiopia, the probability of a student’s academic motivation and behavior being determined by a peer group is higher than the peer context of the Western world, which is dominantly individualistic in life. However, the attention given to the effect of peers on students’ academic matters is not as it deserves, and almost none in the context of Ethiopian high school students.

Motivational beliefs and student academic engagement

In addition to contextual factors, personal or individual-level factors play a vital role in shaping student engagement in academic affairs. Outcome expectations and self-efficacy are the most important cognitive-motivational factors that positively influence learning engagement, according to social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1997). Academic self-efficacy refers to belief in one’s ability to accomplish academic tasks (Bandura, 1997). As self-efficacy beliefs importantly determine one’s effort expenditure, emotional responses, and sense of persistence in carrying out challenging academic tasks, students who have high academic self-efficacy beliefs tend to show greater academic engagement (Martin and Rimm-Kaufman, 2015; Rimm-Kaufman et al., 2015; Liu et al., 2018; Granziera and Perera, 2019; Navarro et al., 2019). It was found that higher self-efficacy beliefs were associated with greater engagement among fifth graders (Rimm-Kaufman et al., 2015) and university students (Wilson et al., 2015). However, self-efficacy alone does not motivate students to engage in academic activities (Wang and Degol, 2014) because outcome expectations for being engaged in the tasks also determine students’ initiation and the decision to engage in the tasks. Outcome expectations uniquely account for motivated actions over and above self-efficacy beliefs (Fouad and Guillen, 2006). Outcome expectations represent the probable outcomes an individual expects for engaging in a particular course of action. They stand for the question, “If I do this, what are the consequences?” (Lent et al., 1994, p. 83). Educational outcome expectations, specifically, denote the probable outcomes a student expects for engaging in academic or educational activities. The role of outcome expectations has been well recognized in career development theories and research, especially in predicting career outcomes, such as interests, goals, persistence, and performance (Lent et al., 1994; Byars-Winston et al., 2010; Lent et al., 2013; Flores et al., 2014; Lent et al., 2016), though less attention is given to be incorporated into student engagement research. If an individual expects more positive outcomes for performing a particular task, the likelihood of being engaged in that task is greater (Bandura, 1997; Fouad and Guillen, 2006). Task values are positively related to engagement (Fan and Williams, 2010; Fan, 2011; Wang and Eccles, 2013) and the intention to continue studying at school or beyond (Fan and Wolters, 2014). Despite a shortage of studies addressing the relationship between outcome expectations and indices of academic engagement, Navarro et al. (2019) reported a positive and unique contribution of engineering outcome expectations to engineering academic engagement based on data collected from undergraduate engineering students. Hence, in the present study, beliefs in one’s academic ability and the expected outcomes are posited to influence the degree of students’ academic engagement. Self-efficacy and outcome expectations are assumed to be the functions of equivalent learning experiences or contexts, so incorporating them simultaneously in a model that incorporates social context as their antecedents and academic engagement as outcomes seems to be sensible.

The current study

Although the role of student engagement in fostering learning outcomes has a substantial amount of empirical support regardless of the level of schooling, studies that examined what factors and how they determine the development of high school students’ academic engagement were too limited. More research is needed to understand the dynamics through which contextual and personal factors shape student engagement and to improve the level and quality of students’ academic engagement and, in turn, to increase their academic success. Therefore, the purpose of the current study was to examine the contextual and personal antecedents that could contribute a substantial amount of variance in the academic engagement of high school students. Based on the integrative theoretical perspective on engagement (Wang et al., 2019, 2020) and prior empirical works, academic socialization experiences from peers and parents, academic self-efficacy, and educational outcome expectations were assumed to explain a substantial amount of variance in high school students’ academic engagement. Hence, this study examined the relations of parental and peer academic socialization with student academic engagement, treating self-efficacy and outcome expectations as mediators to indicate how parents and peers shape students’ academic engagement. The model proposed for the present study (Figure 2) depicts the direct links posited between academic socialization and academic engagement as well as indirect pathways (via academic self-efficacy and outcome expectations) through which the socialization process shapes student engagement in learning. The primary hypothesis is that if students experience positive parental and peer academic socialization, feel efficacious in their academic capability, and expect that their academic pursuit will result in significant outcomes, they will demonstrate greater academic engagement. For instance, parental messages that stress the need to exert academic efforts, convey positive values of education, and communicate high expectations to their children would have both direct and indirect links (via academic self-efficacy and educational outcome expectations) to the academic engagement of high school students.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Proposed structural model of academic socialization, motivational beliefs and academic engagement.

The constructs included in the model and the presumed structural relations with academic engagement were found to be considerable because no studies have investigated these antecedents simultaneously within one conceptual framework using SEM. Specifically, the proposed model was not addressed by prior studies. Moreover, in this study: (1) the conceptualization of academic socialization and associated indices, which carefully considered the existing literature so far, was different from previous studies, and (2) the way academic engagement was defined and measured is unique because it was guided by the most recently proposed perspective on student engagement (Wong and Liem, 2022). The current study defined academic engagement as “students’ psychological state of activity that affords them to feel activated, exert effort, and be absorbed during learning activities” (Wong and Liem, 2022, p. 120), with emotional, behavioral, and cognitive dimensions. Within this conceptualization, emotional engagement represents the level of activation that students experience during learning activities and is indicated by positive feelings, such as vigor, interest, enjoyment, and alertness. Feelings of attachment to the school and its community, unlike most of the prior studies, were not included in emotional engagement because they represent school engagement rather than learning engagement. Feelings of being connected to school (reflected by school attachment, belonging, bonding, and identification) and feelings of connection, closeness, and supportive relationships with teachers and classmates are not indicators of but rather potential antecedents of engagement (Skinner, 2016). Behavioral engagement represents the extent of students’ deliberate effort they exert during learning activities and can be measured by a self-report tool that includes items tapping the students’ effort and persistence during learning activities (Wong and Liem, 2022). Cognitive engagement, the third dimension, denotes the level of student absorption during learning activities, being marked by (a) high-level concentration and task-relevant thoughts and (b) a decline in awareness of irrelevant external events (Wong and Liem, 2022). Scholars (e.g., Ben-Eliyahu et al., 2018; Wong and Liem, 2022) note that cognitive engagement represents the general cognitive processes like absorption instead of the specific learning strategies that students use during learning. Accordingly, Wong and Liem (2022) suggest that researchers can use self-reports of absorption during learning activities (Salmela-Aro and Upadaya, 2012) to assess student cognitive engagement. Unlike most of the prior studies, variables such as attendance, positive conduct, connection with the school and its community, and use of self-regulated learning strategies were not included in the present conceptualization of academic engagement because, most perhaps, they are antecedents or prerequisites of engagement instead of indicators.

The uniqueness of this study is not only in the simultaneous inclusion of the stated constructs in a single model, the order of placement within the model, and the way some of the constructs were conceptualized and measured but also in the context in which the study was conducted. It may be worth assuming that the contributions of social and psychological variables could differ across populations. Some studies that attempted to examine the predictors of student engagement were in the Western culture, and the extent to which the results can be applicable to non-Western contexts, specifically in the context of Ethiopia, is unseen.

It is important to note that there are many variations between the Western and Ethiopian contexts. The context in Ethiopia is different from the Western context, mainly in terms of culture and socio-economic factors. Western countries endorse individualism and individuals seek to maintain personal autonomy by attending to the self, while Ethiopia advocates collectivism in which the relatedness of individuals to each other and community values are more valued (Markus and Kitayama, 1991). This variation, as noted by Markus and Kitayama, has important effects on cognition, emotion, and motivation. In terms of the level of economic development and technological advancement, Western society is generally higher than that of Ethiopia. Ethiopia is still considered a developing country with a less diversified economy. Such difference also results in variations in the lifestyle, education, and opportunities available to people in these contexts. However, many scientific theories and hypotheses have been developed and tested primarily in Western contexts. Henrich et al. (2010) highlighted the fact that a significant portion of the psychological literature is built on studies conducted within Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic (WEIRD) societies. This raises questions of generalizability to other cultures and societies around the world, highlighting the need for greater diversity and inclusivity in psychological research. Therefore, testing hypotheses generated based on Western contexts in Ethiopia can be plausible because cultural and socioeconomic contexts can greatly affect the applicability of these hypotheses in other parts of the world, particularly in developing countries like Ethiopia. Ethiopia may provide a context that is different enough from Western contexts to test the extent to which Western-based theories and hypotheses can be generalized across cultures and to establish the external validity of findings from Western populations. Evidence indicated that (e.g., Klassen, 2004; Ahn et al., 2016), students from individualistic and collectivistic cultural backgrounds evaluated and interpreted socially conveyed sources of motivational beliefs (e.g., vicarious experience and social persuasion) differently in which for students with collectivistic cultural backgrounds socially conveyed messages were the most important sources for academic self-efficacy formation. Given that Ethiopia’s cultural, social, and economic contexts, as well as its education system significantly differ from those of other countries where theoretical hypotheses are generated and previous studies have been conducted, and considering the substantial influence of cultural contexts, the motive behind the current study seems convincing.

Findings from this study would (a) strengthen the understanding of what factors positively shape student academic engagement across cultures. (b) provide empirical support for the cross-cultural validity of a development-in-sociocultural context theoretical framework, (c) suggest interventions to be initiated and implemented to improve the academic performance of students through augmenting academic engagement, and (d) in general help to expand our understanding of human behavior and psychology beyond the Western contexts. This study has particular importance in the context of Ethiopia, where high school students are becoming lower and lower in their academic motivation, engagement, and achievement to a great extent. For example, among grade 12 students who took Ethiopia’s 2022 national school-leaving examination, only 3.3% of them were able to score 50% and above to qualify for tertiary education.

Method

Participants

The participants were 614 (323 males and 291 females) randomly selected high school students in grades 9 (n = 329) and 10 (n = 285). They were from six randomly chosen public high schools located in six different districts of South Wollo Administrative Zone, Amhara Regional State, Ethiopia. From the six schools, one to three classes of each target grade were selected randomly, and finally, each participant was chosen with a simple random sampling technique. The number of participants across schools ranges from 60 to 145, based on the total number of students in each sample school. The sample had a mean age of 16.1 years (SD = 0.66), ranging from 15 to 17 years of age. All the participants attend regular high school education programs in public schools. Ethnically, participants were Amhara except that 13 students identified as other ethnicities and three students did not indicate.

Procedures

Following receipt of ethical approval from the ethics committee of the Institute of Teachers Education and Behavioural Science, Wollo University, the school principals and teaching staff at each school were approached, and their permission to collect the data was obtained. School teachers administered the questionnaire to participants during the school days, with no time limits and close supervision of the researcher. Students provided their informed consent (dealing with their parents) by singing on the consent forms prepared for such purpose. However, participants were not requested to write a name or other personal identifying variables to ensure the anonymity of data.

Measures

Academic engagement

The academic engagement measure included 16 items. For behavioral and emotional dimensions, the engagement subscales of the Engagement Versus Disaffection with Learning measure (Skinner et al., 2009) were used except for the two items added to the behavioral subscale from Wang et al. (2016). The behavioral engagement scale comprised six items tapping students’ effort and persistence while participating in learning activities. The emotional engagement subscale included five items tapping positive emotions during learning activities. The cognitive engagement was assessed by five items, which included the three-item absorption subscale of the schoolwork engagement inventory (Salmela-Aro and Upadaya, 2012) and two items from the cognitive engagement scale designed by Ben-Eliyahu et al. (2018). This was done to be consistent with the way cognitive engagement has been conceptualized and the recommendation forwarded by Wong and Liem (2022). Moreover, items addressing learning engagement at home were included. Sample items include: “I try hard to do well in school” (behavioral engagement), “Class is fun for me” (emotional engagement), and “Time flies when I am studying” (cognitive engagement). Participants rated the items using a 5-point Likert-type scale, with the options of ‘not at all true for me’ (1) to ‘very true for me’ (5). Previous studies have disclosed adequate reliability estimates for the original items of each dimension (Skinner et al., 2008; Salmela-Aro and Upadaya, 2012; Xiong et al., 2021). The present data disclosed acceptable Cronbach’s alpha values (0.92 for behavioral, 0.89 for emotional, 0.90 for cognitive, and 0.89 for the full engagement scale). Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted to assess each item’s factor loading and factor structure of the constructs included in this study. Within this paper, model fit was evaluated using five indices: the relative chi-square test (ꭓ2/df), the Tucker–Lewis index (TLI), the comparative fit index (CFI), the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the standardized root-mean-square residual (SRMR). Because of much controversy on appropriateness and interpretation of the model-fit criteria, Schumacker and Lomax (2016) recommend reporting more than one model-fit index stating that if “a majority of the fit indices on your list indicate an acceptable model, then your theoretical model is supported by the data” (p. 119). As the chi-square statistic (χ2) is affected by the sample size, it mostly rejects the model when the sample size increases (if N > 200) (Bentler and Bonett, 1980; Schumacker and Lomax, 2016). As a result, a relative chi-square statistic (χ2/df) was used instead of it in this study (Wheaton et al., 1977). If CFI and TLI ≥ 0.95, and RMSEA and SRMR ≤0.06 (Hu and Bentler, 1999), a model adequately fits the data. A relative chi-square test with a value below 3 is considered an acceptable fit (Kline, 2011). For the academic engagement measure, CFA suggested that the three-factor model fit the data well (ꭓ2 /df = 3.2, CFI = 0.96, TLI = 0.95, SRMR = 0.036, RMSEA = 0.06, 90% CI = 0.053, 0.067). Factor loadings reached statistical significance and their standardized estimates ranged from 0.749 to 0.85.

Parental academic socialization

The parental academic socialization (PAS) scale has 17 items and assesses parents’ messages of effort socialization (4 items), educational expectations (5 items), and value of education (8 items). Participants indicated the extent to which they have been experiencing such parental messages from 1 (not at all) to 5 (always). Effort socialization items were from the effort subscale of the Educational Socialization Scale (Bempechat et al., 1999). A sample item from the effort subscale includes, “My parents say you can get smarter and smarter as long as you try hard.” For parental educational expectations, five items were adapted from the perceived parental academic support scale (Chen, 2005; Cross et al., 2019). This scale included items such as “I feel pressured by my parents to do well in school.” The value or importance of education sub-scale consisted of the five-item Benefits of Education subscale of the Economic Benefits and Limitations of Education scale (Murdock et al., 2000) and three items created for the present study to assess the non-economic value of education. A sample item includes: “My parents say if I do well in school, I will get a good job,” Higher scores in each dimension of parental academic socialization reflect a greater frequency of the PAS messages. Previous studies have disclosed adequate reliability estimates for the original items of each dimension (Murdock et al., 2000; Mroczkowski and Sanchez, 2015; Cross et al., 2019). In the present study, the data disclosed Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of 0.86, 0.92, 0.84, and 0.89 for effort, the value of education, educational expectations, and full scale, respectively. CFA also suggested that the three-factor PAS model fit the data well (ꭓ2 /df = 2.85, CFI = 0.96, TLI = 0.95, SRMR = 0.04, RMSEA = 0.055, 90% CI = 0.048, 0.062). Factor loadings reached statistical significance and their standardized estimates ranged from 0.72 to 0.82.

Peer academic socialization

Participants were asked to list a group of friends with whom they spend more time and do many things together, without limit to the number of friends to be listed. They were informed that members of the friendship group they list are limited to their grade level, but they can be from other classrooms and schools. What matters in the selection process is the time spent together and feelings of connectedness to do things together and exchange information. Participants were reminded that the members of this group were referred to as “your friends or friendship group” throughout the items included to assess the context of their peer group. The purpose of the list of friends is to make a focused and pertinent judgment about the context of the peer group.

The measure of peer academic socialization consisted of 16 items and included three subscales. The effort socialization subscale has four items tapping the peer group’s stress on effort and was adapted from the effort subscale of the Educational Socialization Scale (Bempechat et al., 1999). A sample is, “My friends say we could do better in school if we worked harder.” Measures addressing peers’ academic norms and educational aspirations were adapted from the Peers’ Academic Support and Aspirations Scale (Murdock, 1999; Murdock et al., 2000). With regard to assessing peer academic norms, each participant was presented with the seven-item peers’ academic norm scale and asked to rate to what extent most of the members of his/her friendship group had the potential to do a variety of academic tasks and demonstrate positive academic behaviors relevant to high school education. A sample item used to assess the academic norms of the peer group includes: “Most of my friends try to do well in school.” Peers’ educational aspirations had five items to assess students’ perceptions that their friends would complete high school and continue with their education (peers’ aspirations for their academic future). This scale included items such as “Most of my friends plan to go to college/university.” The current study revealed good internal consistency for the peers’ effort subscale (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.77), educational aspirations subscale (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.82), academic norm subscale (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.84), and full academic socialization scale (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.87). CFA also demonstrated an excellent model-data fit for three-factor structure of peer academic socialization scale (ꭓ2 /df = 2.1, CFI = 0.97, TLI = 0.96, SRMR = 0.038, RMSEA = 0.043, 90% CI = 0.035, 0.051). Factor loadings reached statistical significance and their standardized estimates ranged from 0.62 to 0.73.

Academic self-efficacy

Academic self-efficacy was assessed by the academic self-efficacy subscale of the self-efficacy questionnaire for children (SEQ-C; Muris, 2001), which has eight items to tap students’ feelings about their ability to be successful in school and demonstrate appropriate academic behaviors. The participants rated their feeling of efficacy using a four-Likert-type scale (1 = not at all confident to 4 = very confident). The scale included items like “How confident are you that you could study when there are other interesting things to do?” An SEQ-C drew greatly on the concept of self-efficacy, and it has been acknowledged in many of its features, such as it was developed with youth, is simple in terms of format, is domain-specific, and is fairly brief (Minter and Pritzker, 2017). The exploratory factor analysis result indicated that all items of the scale “hung together” to form a one-dimensional scale. CFA also supported the unidimensionalty of the measure (ꭓ2/df = 2.3, CFI = 0.99, TLI = 0.99, SRMR = 0.018, RMSEA = 0.046, 90% CI = 0.028, 0.064). Factor loadings reached statistical significance and their standardized estimates ranged from 0.75 to 0.79. The scale demonstrated a strong internal consistency in previous research (e.g., Landon et al., 2007; Suldo and Shaffer, 2007; Minter and Pritzker, 2017), which also appeared to be good for the current participants (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.92).

Educational outcome expectations

The outcome expectations measure consisted of 14 items taken from the College Outcome Expectations questionnaire (Flores et al., 2008). The items were adapted to the study population by replacing the stem: “A college education will...” with “secondary and post-secondary education will …” The original scale has 19 items, but for the present study, four items related to social affairs (i.e., leaving enough time for family and friends, making several friends, meeting new people, and causing problems in the family) and one item related to college courses, totally five items, were excluded as they are found not applicable for the present participants. Participants rated each item (e.g., “Secondary and post-secondary education will allow me to obtain a well-paying job”) on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). High scores represent a high level of positive academic outcome expectations. The output of exploratory factor analysis suggested that all items of the scale “hung together” to form a one-dimensional scale. CFA also supported the unidimensionalty of the measure (ꭓ2/df = 3.2, CFI = 0.97, TLI = 0.96, SRMR = 0.028, RMSEA = 0.06, 90% CI = 0.051, 0.069). Factor loadings reached statistical significance and their standardized estimates ranged from 0.70 to 0.81. In the present study, Cronbach’s alpha value was 0.94.

Results

Preliminary analyses

During data screening, a closer examination of the data revealed that 21 cases were missing more than 20% of the items and were deleted from the data set (Schlomer et al., 2010; Navarro et al., 2019). The deletion has reduced the data set to 614. In testing the measurement model, three latent variables were represented by their corresponding subscales. Accordingly, parental academic socialization consisted of educational expectations, effort socialization, and value of education; peer academic socialization included peers’ educational aspiration, peers’ effort, and peers’ academic norms; and academic engagement comprised behavioral, emotional, and cognitive dimensions. Self-efficacy and outcome expectations were each indexed by three item parcels formed by random algorithm (Matsunaga, 2008). The number of items in the parcels of academic self-efficacy was from 2 to 3 (ASE1 includes items 1, 2, and 4, ASE2 includes items 3, 6, and 8, and ASE3 includes items 5 and 7), whereas, in outcome expectations, it was from four to five items (OE1 includes items 1, 2, 4, 5, and 7, OE2 includes items 3, 6, 8, 11, and 14, and OE3 includes items 9, 10, 12, and 13). In total, the model included 15 indicator-level variables. The purpose of the present study is to examine the relationships among constructs included in the proposed model rather than focusing on the relationships among individual items, therefore, “parceling is more strongly warranted”(Little et al., 2002, p. 169).

The data was checked for outliers and normality issues at indicator-level variables. The numerical standard deviates method, |z| < 3.0 (Kline, 2023), and the graphic techniques (e.g., box plots) did not display scores further away from the rest of the distribution; hence, there is no issue of univariate outliers in the dataset. Mahalonbis distance, D2, also did not indicate evidence for multivariate outliers (i.e., a small value of D2 and lowest p-value, 0.001). The data was found to be univariate normal as all of the absolute values of the skewness (range: −0.43 to 0.345) and kurtosis (range: −0.783 to −0.218) indices were less than 2 and 7, respectively (Finney and DiStefano, 2013; Kline, 2023). Marida’s normalized estimate of multivariate kurtosis for the model was 4.45 (< 5), indicating no violation of multivariate normality (Byrne, 2016). None of the correlations among indicator variables after parceling (range 0.24 to 0.83) and among predictor latent constructs (range 0.35 to 0.66) surpassed 0.90, suggesting that multicollinearity was not a problem (Abu-Bader, 2010; Kline, 2023). Therefore, the final data set maintained responses from 614 participants.

The descriptive statistics and correlations among the study’s observable variables are presented in Table 1. The relations among the variables were significant. The standardized loadings of indicator variables on respective factors range from 0.76 to 0.92 (Figure 2).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlations among the study’s observable variables.

Primary analysis

Measurement model

Structural equation modeling with a maximum likelihood estimation method was used to estimate the parameters (via AMOS Version 26). As the first step of testing the hypothesized structural model, the fitness of the measurement model was examined to confirm that all latent variables were acceptably represented by their parcels. In doing this, five latent factors were allowed to covary with no specified structural relations among them. All item parcels loaded significantly (p < 0.001) onto their respective factors, with standardized loadings ranging from 0.79 to 0.84 on outcome expectations, from 0.85 to 0.92 on academic engagement, from 0.76 to 0.83 on self-efficacy, from 0.76 to 0.79 on peer academic socialization and from 0.79 to 0.83 on parental academic socialization. Each of the fit indices suggested that the measurement model fits sample data very well (χ2/df = 2, RMSEA = 0.04 [90% CI = 0.031,0.049], CFI =0.97, TLI = 0.98 and SRMR = 0.027). Hence, the results of the measurement model supported that the indicators adequately measured their underlying latent factors.

Structural model

As the measurement model fit the data well, the analysis proceeded to test the hypothesized structural model depicting the relations of the contextual and cognitive-person factors in explaining high school student academic engagement using the maximum likelihood estimation method. The result indicated an excellent model fit to the data (χ2/df = 2.1, CFI = 0.98, TLI = 98, RMSEA = 0.042 [90% CI = 0.033,0.051], and SRMR = 0.032). Also. the standardized residual correlation matrix supports the model as almost all values were within acceptable range. That is, the structural model fits the observed data well. All the hypothesized paths of the model were statistically significant, and they were in the predicted directions. The standardized direct effects are shown in Figure 3. Parental academic socialization was positively associated with students’ academic self-efficacy (β = 0.28, p < 0.001), outcome expectations (β = 0.48, p < 0.001), and academic engagement (β = 0.29, p < 0.001). Peer academic socialization was also positively associated with students’ academic self-efficacy (β = 0.30, p < 0.001), outcome expectations (β = 0.27, p < 0.001), and academic engagement (β = 0.24, p < 0.001). The cognitive-person factors, academic self-efficacy (β = 0.15, p < 0.001), and outcome expectations (β = 0.25, p < 0.001) were positively linked to academic engagement. Together, parental and peer socialization explained a significant amount of variance in academic self-efficacy (27.5%) and educational outcome expectations (46.4%). The four constructs in the model, as a whole, accounted for 58.4% of the variance in academic engagement of high school students.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Tested structural model of academic socialization, motivational beliefs and academic engagement. PEA, Peer educational aspiration; PAN, peer academic norm; PES, peer effort socialization; PAS_EE, parental academic socialization -educational expectations; PAS_EE, parental academic socialization –effort; PAS_VE, parental academic socialization-value of education; CE, cognitive engagement; BE, behavioral engagement; EE, emotional engagement; ASE1, academic self-efficacy parcel 1; ASE2, academic self-efficacy parcel 2; ASE3, academic self-efficacy parcel 3; OE1, outcome expectations parcel 1; OE2, outcome expectations parcel 2; OE3, outcome expectations parcel 3. All p < 0.001.

Mediated effects

Bootstrap analysis, a more preferred method for testing mediation, was utilized to test the significance of indirect effects with the recommended 5,000 bootstrap samples (Fairchild and McQuillin, 2010; Collier, 2020) drawn with replacement. In this analysis, the indirect effects of peer academic socialization and parental academic socialization on student academic engagement through self-efficacy and outcome expectation were tested. The mediating effect is statistically significant if the confidence interval does not cross zero (Shrout and Bolger, 2002). Hence, the specific indirect effects of parental academic socialization on academic engagement of high school students through academic self-efficacy (b = 0.054, BC 95% CI = 0.02, 0.10, p < 0.001) and through outcome expectations (b = 0.156, BC 95% CI [0.090, 0.236], p < 0.001) were significant. Peer academic socialization had a significant indirect relation with academic engagement through self-efficacy (b = 0.059, BC 95% CI [0.026, 0.111], p < 0.001) and outcome expectations (b = 0.088, BC 95% CI [0.04, 0.155], p < 0.001). These findings indicate that cognitive-person variables partially mediated the relations between the social context and academic engagement of high school students.

Discussion

The current study examined the structural relations among contextual (parental & peer academic socialization) and person-cognitive (self-efficacy and outcome expectations) factors in shaping the academic engagement of high school students in Ethiopia. As hypothesized, based on the integrative model of engagement and previous empirical evidence, the results showed that the posited model adequately fit the data, all paths were positive and significant, and the variables within the model accounted for a substantial amount of variance (58.4%) in the academic engagement of high school students. Also, the two exogenous factors of the model explained an adequate amount of variance in self-efficacy and outcome expectations. Parental academic socialization and peer academic socialization, together, accounted for 28% and 46% of the variance in self-efficacy and outcome expectations, respectively. Hence, the findings of the current study, from a sample of Ethiopian high school students, provided empirical support for the hypotheses posited in the development-in-sociocultural context model of engagement in learning (Wang et al., 2019, 2020).

Role of parental academic socialization

As hypothesized, based on the integrative model of engagement and previous findings, high school students who perceived more positive academic socialization from parents endorsed greater self-efficacy to perform academic-related tasks and had stronger convictions that education would result in meaningful outcomes. Students who perceived parent-provided academic socialization messages as positive and encouraging had greater confidence in their abilities to perform academic tasks, associated positive outcomes with engagement in education, and were more likely engaged in academic affairs. Beyond being converged to the integrative model of engagement (Wang et al., 2020), these findings provide support for the perspective that parents are significant sources of self-efficacy and outcome expectations for children through social persuasion mechanisms (Bandura, 1997). When parents frequently convey messages to their children about high academic expectations, the value of education, and the importance of effort, they persuade their children that they are capable and expect positive outcomes in the future. In this way, parents bolster their children’s academic self-efficacy and positive outcome expectations for learning, which are considered the most important motivational beliefs in the academic domain. The finding that parental academic socialization significantly predicted academic self-efficacy corroborates the results of previous studies (e.g., Suizzo et al., 2016) and the conclusion that parents’ messages strongly influence children’s self-efficacy (Usher and Pajares, 2006). There was evidence that parental aspiration for their children’s postsecondary education, which was one aspect of parental academic socialization in this study, positively predicted students’ self-efficacy (Fan and Williams, 2010). Parental academic socialization was also related to high school students’ academic engagement both directly and indirectly through academic self-efficacy and outcome expectations. Hence, findings lead to the conclusion that parental academic socialization represented via three dimensions positively linked to students’ engagement in academic activities. The result was consistent with previous studies (Melby et al., 2008; Murray, 2009; Wilder, 2014) that suggested parental involvement in education matters for children’s engagement. Parents may socialize their children academically through multiple processes, such as the academic values they endorse, the expectations for academic success they set, and the experiences they share. The result of this study provides new evidence that parental involvement in the form of communicating beliefs about and expectations for their children’s success, linking education to future success, and stressing the reimbursement of exerting effort is an essential factor in fostering children’s academic engagement. The present finding suggests that academic socialization appears to be a fundamental substrate for student academic engagement. It has been reported that academic socialization has the strongest positive link with engagement during adolescence (Hill and Tyson, 2009; Wang et al., 2014). Fan and Williams (2010) also found that parental aspiration for their children’s postsecondary education, one aspect of parental academic socialization, positively predicted student engagement. As academic socialization is more likely to correspond with the developmental tasks of adolescents, such as achieving competence and accomplishment, its effect on high school student academic engagement seems to be acceptable.

Hence, the findings of the current study suggest that parental academic socialization matters for students to build higher academic self-efficacy, have positive outcome expectations for education, and be actively engaged in academic tasks. Students from parents who (a) recognized the utility of schooling, (b) had high expectations for their academic success, and (c) stressed the benefits of effort were likely to feel confident about their academic abilities, develop positive outcome expectations, and show a greater degree of academic engagement. It means that parents’ values for education, educational expectations, and beliefs in an effort which are conveyed and communicated through the process of academic socialization importantly determine their children’s motivational factors (i.e., self-efficacy, outcome expectations, and engagement) essential to achieve better academically.

Role of peer academic socialization

In this study, high school students who perceived more positive academic socialization from peers endorsed greater self-efficacy to perform academic-related tasks and had stronger beliefs that education would result in important outcomes. Peer academic socialization was also related to high school students’ academic engagement both directly and indirectly through academic self-efficacy and outcome expectations. This finding supports that peers provide an important developmental context for adolescents (Furrer, 2010), and classmates and friends have a significant effect on students’ academic motivation and engagement (Wentzel, 2009). The effects of peer academic socialization observed in this study support the theories of peer group influence that have postulated peer effects may be conveyed directly through socialization processes that include peer pressure, modeling, reinforcement, and encouragement to follow group norms (Altermatt and Pomerantz, 2003; Lynch et al., 2013) as well as findings that members of adolescent peer groups are similar in many school-related adjustments such as in their learning motivation (Ryan, 2001), effortful behavior toward schoolwork, intrinsic value, interest, and enjoyment in schoolwork (Ryan, 2001; Shin and Ryan, 2014), and preference for challenge (Altermatt and Pomerantz, 2003). Therefore, the findings of this study are consistent with previous empirical evidence and provide support for the theoretical suppositions that peers have a greater potential to socialize the academic engagement of member students and basically, peers matter for individual student involvement in his/her education. Specifically, the finding that peer academic socialization predicts educational outcome expectations strengthens the thought that peers form a natural context for thinking about the future (Nurmi, 2004) and they are an essential source of future-related information for adolescents (Malmberg, 1996).

The findings that peer academic socialization shapes student engagement through multiple pathways suggest that peer groups have potential importance to the development of students’ engagement. The role peer academic socialization played in shaping student academic engagement could have different possible explanations. For example, peer group members: (a) share similar expectations about their future education (Kiuru et al., 2007), (b) are important sources of future-related information (Malmberg, 1996), (c) act as role models for each other in peer groups, (d) have a propensity for adhering to peer group norms and desire to fit in with those peers (Wang and Eccles, 2012), and (e) typically modify their levels of engagement to those of their peer group (Wang et al., 2018); and hence, all these are likely the mechanisms through which peers socialize individual student academic engagement. Together with previous evidence, the present study provides credence that peer context is an important substrate for the development of high school students’ engagement profiles. Students who belong to peer groups in which academic behaviors are normative are more likely to report a high level of engagement than students with less academically inclined peers.

The roles of self-efficacy and outcome expectations as mediators

An integrative model of engagement has posited that self-appraisals or motivational beliefs are pathways through which a variety of external (e.g., academic socialization) assets influence learning engagement, and this was supported in the present study. The result supports the positive roles of self-efficacy and outcome expectations in predicting academic engagement, as students who have greater confidence in their abilities to do academic activities and stronger beliefs that being educated would lead to positive outcomes demonstrated a greater degree of academic engagement. These findings are consistent with the self-system motivational perspective, which posits that more positive self-beliefs are related to a greater level of engagement (Connell and Wellborn, 1991) and social cognitive theories that assert students with higher levels of academic self-efficacy and outcome expectations will engage in their learning more fully (Lent et al., 1994, 2000; Bandura, 1997). The positive relationship between academic self-efficacy and engagement also converges with evidence obtained in previous research (Linnenbrink and Pintrich, 2003; Martin and Rimm-Kaufman, 2015; Rimm-Kaufman et al., 2015; Liu et al., 2018; Navarro et al., 2019; Olivier et al., 2019). The relationship between outcome expectations and measures of academic engagement is consistent with the finding of Navarro et al. (2019) that found engineering outcome expectations positively predicted engineering academic engagement in undergraduate engineering students and Miller et al. (2021) who reported that students with higher math outcome expectations had a much higher likelihood of belonging to the ‘Moderately-to-Highly Engaged’ profile as compared to the ‘Minimally Engaged’ profile. Other studies also reported that task values are positively related to behavioral engagement (Fan, 2011; Wang and Eccles, 2013), intention to continue studying at school or beyond (Fan and Wolters, 2014), and positive classroom affect (Jiang et al., 2018) which support the present finding.

Self-efficacy and outcome expectations partially mediated the relations of academic socialization to student engagement. This result supports the integrative model that posits youth’s self-appraisals mediate the link between engagement and social context (Wang et al., 2020) and the social-cognitive perspective (Lent et al., 1994, 2000; Bandura, 1997) that states social environment operates through motivational self-beliefs to produce the required outcomes like academic engagement. According to the findings of this study, academic socialization facilitates academic engagement by enhancing self-efficacy and outcome expectations. With academic self-efficacy as a mediator of the relation between academic socialization and academic engagement, it was found that students whose parents and peers practice and forward encouraging socialization messages had higher academic self-efficacy, which in turn predicted better academic engagement. With the role of mediating the relation between academic socialization and academic engagement, outcome expectations of high school students whose parents and peers practice and convey positive academic socialization messages were higher, and it, in turn, predicted better academic engagement.

To sum up, the results revealed initial evidence for the utility of the integrated model of student engagement to predict the academic engagement of Ethiopian high school students who grew up and live within the collectivistic culture. Precisely, the findings that parental and peer academic socialization predicted academic engagement, both directly and indirectly through students’ academic self-efficacy and outcome expectations, suggest how family and peer contexts matter to foster student engagement in learning, particularly in the context where collectivism is more valued. These findings extend and support prior research on predicting student engagement from contextual and personal factors and shed light on how contextual and person-cognitive variables might contribute to the academic engagement of high school students. The result further suggests that self-efficacy and outcome expectations develop through comparable learning experiences, and they have comparable motivating effects to outcome variables (Lent et al., 1994; Bandura, 1997).

Limitations and future research

Although this study has notable strengths, such as a large sample size, randomly drawn participants from different districts and schools, and being conducted in a country where almost all previous research on the issue has not been conducted, it has a number of limitations that should be considered while interpreting findings.

The first limitation is associated with the inclusion of variables. This study examined the influence of social systems on academic engagement by incorporating multiple aspects of family and peer contexts; however, there are other social agents (e.g., teachers) that could have been contributing factors to the outcome variable. Previous research has demonstrated that teachers’ messages strongly influence students’ self-efficacy (Usher and Pajares, 2006) and likewise may influence expected outcomes and, eventually, engagement in learning. The included psychological variables are also limited to self-efficacy and outcome expectations. Therefore, future research should consider teacher-related factors (e.g., psychological needs support, academic socialization) and other psychological constructs, especially those included in the integrative engagement model.

Second, the findings were based on cross-sectional design data. Hence, although (1) the proposed model was guided by previously developed theoretical frameworks and reported empirical findings, (2) SEM analysis provides important information about the possible direction of the relationships, and (3) results corroborate previous findings and theoretical predictions, cross-sectional study designs do not lead to firm conclusions regarding the causal ordering among the variables included. Third, the data was based on self-report measures. Self-perceptions are possibly best captured with self-reports, but for some constructs (e.g., academic engagement and parental academic socializations), data only from self-report measures may produce social desirability biases and narrow interpretations. Therefore, future researchers are encouraged to test the model using longitudinal designs and data from multiple sources to address these concerns. Fourth, it’s critical to remember that psychological study does not allow for the precise prediction of what will occur in a new setting based on past experiences. Hence, I encourage researchers to examine the model tested in this study in a way that addresses such concerns and see what the results would be. Finally, the other limitation of the current study may be associated with the influence of confounding variables and/or covariates. While prior studies have not explicitly suggested potential confounders to be considered in the context of the present study, there remains the possibility that unaccounted variables could impact the findings. Therefore, it is essential to interpret the findings while acknowledging these possibilities. Researchers interested in the predictors of student academic engagement should take this concern into account for future investigations. For instance, in the current study, it was assumed that there may not be significant variation in socioeconomic status (SES) within the target population to the extent that it would affect the outcome variable, hence SES was not included as a covariate in the model. However, future studies could benefit from the inclusion of status-based covariates, such as SES as highlighted by Wang et al. (2019), especially in populations where SES differences are more pronounced.

Implications

This study, despite the limitations mentioned above, offers important contributions to theoretical understanding and practice regarding student academic engagement. The study demonstrated evidence for the theoretical model combining variables derived from contextual and personal domains, highlighting the role of person-cognitive factors as mechanisms explaining the relationship between contextual factors and student academic engagement. So, the findings provide substantial theoretical and empirical contributions to broaden knowledge on antecedents and the means of how they contribute to the academic engagement of high school students and suggest researchers in education and educational psychology pay more attention to the contextual and psychological constructs in the scholarship of student engagement. It would also provide support for the applicability of psychological constructs and models derived from theories fundamentally developed in Western cultures to Ethiopian culture. This is related to the issue of whether perspectives commonly supported in one culture (e.g., individualism) could be replicated among participants of other cultures (e.g., collectivism).

The current results, which come from participants with collectivistic cultural values and a different socio-economic background, make a significant contribution to the existing body of knowledge on student engagement which fundamentally represents a limited segment of the globe. They are particularly important in filling gaps in the understanding of student engagement across cultures as most of the previous research has been conducted in the Western contexts. This could be an attempt to address the concern in the field that concepts, practice, and empirical findings of psychology are mostly limited to Western countries but missing from African and other societies (Henrich et al., 2010; Berry, 2013).

Parallel with extending theoretical knowledge, the interrelations among constructs included in the present study suggest student academic engagement be increased and maintained through interventions targeting contextual and individual-level assets. According to the results, interventions targeting social contexts (e.g., academic socialization) may be associated with enrichments of all other variables. That is, promoting a positive academic socialization process appears to be a central component of intervention efforts in bolstering students’ self-efficacy, outcome expectations, and engagement in learning. Self-efficacy beliefs and outcome expectations are malleable enough and can be developed through targeted interventions. Interventions that focus on enhancing self-appraisals (e.g., self-efficacy and outcome expectancy), such as social persuasion and vicarious learning (Bandura, 1986), are helpful to improve student engagement in education, given that academic self-efficacy and outcome expectations are significantly related to student engagement. Furthermore, it would behoove parents, teachers, and school psychologists to focus on strengthening motivational beliefs as a way of enhancing academic engagement. Parents need to communicate to their children the positive outcomes of engaging in education, high academic expectations, and the consequence of lack of effort to promote positive self-efficacy beliefs and outcome expectations in the academic domain, which, in turn, craft motivated engagement in learning. School counselors or psychologists may design psychoeducational interventions that would bolster high school student’s academic efficacy and understanding of what they can gain from pursuing their education.

This study is also indicative of the need to pay due attention to how peer or friendship groups matter in determining the academic behaviors and success of children. The aspirations of others, academic norms, and beliefs in effort within adolescents’ peer and friendship groups were found to be ways by which peers may influence academic engagement. Adolescents tend to show similar levels of school-related beliefs, attitudes, and behaviors to others in their peer group, including school performance and academic self-perception (Ryan, 2001; Chen et al., 2003). Hence, as conformity to peer groups comes into prominence during adolescence, an intervention targeting peer and friendship groups would have a better effect in boosting students’ motivational beliefs and engagement in learning.

In conclusion, the findings were in line with theoretical predictions and earlier results and underscored the importance of parental and peer academic socialization for fostering students’ academic engagement. The substantial role of academic socialization and motivational beliefs (self-efficacy and outcome expectations) in explaining variance in academic engagement, which, in turn, leads to positive learning outcomes, suggests that educational policies and practices should be designed in a way that offers positive socialization experiences and boost motivational beliefs among students. The primary implication of the current findings is that interventions aimed at improving student self-efficacy, outcome expectations, and engagement, particularly for students who grew up and live within a collectivistic culture like Ethiopia, should take into account and integrate the most significant socializing agents in the academic domain (e.g., parents and peer groups). This study tends to encourage schools to plan and practice peer tutoring sessions, collaborative learning activities, and peer-based study groups to cultivate positive academic socialization among students.

The findings also offer an excellent opportunity for parents and teachers to be optimistic that they can cultivate children’s self-efficacy beliefs and outcome expectations as one means for fostering academic engagement, given that they have a strong desire for more active involvement of students in learning. Supported by the prior evidence that students with collectivistic value orientations benefit more from other or group-related sources such as vicarious experiences, and verbal and social persuasion as compared to students from countries with predominantly individualistic cultures (Klassen, 2004; Ahn et al., 2016), the present study suggests the need to paying due attention to social context in boosting Ethiopian students’ motivational beliefs and academic engagement.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the author, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the Ethics Committee of Institute of Teachers Education and Behavioral Science, Wollo University. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

GW: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1347163/full#supplementary-material

References

Abu-Bader, S. H. (2010). Advanced and multivariate statistical methods for social science research. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Ahn, H. S., Usher, E. L., Butz, A., and Bong, M. (2016). Cultural differences in the understanding of modelling and feedback as sources of self-efficacy information. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 86, 112–136. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12093

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Alrashidi, O., Phan, H. P., and Ngu, B. H. (2016). Academic engagement: an overview of its definitions, dimensions, and major conceptualisations. Int. Educ. Stud. 9:41. doi: 10.5539/ies.v9n12p41

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Altermatt, E. R., and Pomerantz, E. M. (2003). The development of competence-related and motivational beliefs: an investigation of similarity and influence among friends. J. Educ. Psychol. 95, 111–123. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.95.1.111

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., Kim, D., and Reschly, A. L. (2006). Measuring cognitive and psychological engagement: validation of the student engagement instrument. J. Sch. Psychol. 44, 427–445. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2006.04.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Archambault, I., Janosz, M., Morizot, J., and Pagani, L. (2009). Adolescent behavioral, affective, and cognitive engagement in school: relationship to dropout. J. Sch. Health 79, 408–415. doi: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.2009.00428.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bæck, U.-D. K. (2017). It is the air that we breathe. Academic socialization as a key component for understanding how parents influence children’s schooling. Nordic J. Stud. Educ. Policy 3, 123–132. doi: 10.1080/20020317.2017.1372008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: the exercise of control. New York, NY: W H Freeman/Times Books/ Henry Holt & Co.

Google Scholar

Bempechat, J., Graham, S. E., and Jimenez, N. V. (1999). The socialization of achievement in poor and minority students: a comparative study. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 30, 139–158. doi: 10.1177/0022022199030002001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ben-Eliyahu, A., Moore, D., Dorph, R., and Schunn, C. D. (2018). Investigating the multidimensionality of engagement: affective, behavioral, and cognitive engagement across science activities and contexts. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 53, 87–105. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2018.01.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bentler, P. M., and Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychol. Bull. 88, 588–606. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.88.3.588

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Berry, J. W. (2013). Achieving a global psychology. Can. Psychol. 54, 55–61. doi: 10.1037/a0031246

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boulton, C. A., Hughes, E., Kent, C., Smith, J. R., and Williams, H. T. P. (2019). Student engagement and wellbeing over time at a higher education institution. PLoS One 14, e0225770–e0225720. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0225770

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bresó, E., Schaufeli, W. B., and Salanova, M. (2011). Can a self-efficacy-based intervention decrease burnout, increase engagement, and enhance performance? A quasi-experimental study. High. Educ. 61, 339–355. doi: 10.1007/s10734-010-9334-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Byars-Winston, A., Estrada, Y., Howard, C., Davis, D., and Zalapa, J. (2010). Influence of social cognitive and ethnic variables on academic goals of underrepresented students in science and engineering: a multiple-groups analysis. J. Couns. Psychol. 57, 205–218. doi: 10.1037/a0018608

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrne, B. M. (2016). Structural equation modeling with AMOS: basic concepts, applications, and programming. New York, NY: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Cadime, I., Pinto, A. M., Lima, S., Rego, S., Pereira, J., and Ribeiro, I. (2016). Well-being and academic achievement in secondary school pupils: the unique effects of burnout and engagement. J. Adolesc. 53, 169–179. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.10.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, J. J. (2005). Relation of academic support from parents, teachers, and peers to Hong Kong adolescents' academic achievement: the mediating role of academic engagement. Genet. Soc. Gen. Psychol. Monogr. 131, 77–127. doi: 10.3200/MONO.131.2.77-127

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, X., Chang, L., and He, Y. (2003). The peer group as a context: mediating and moderating effects on relations between academic achievement and social functioning in Chinese children. Child Dev. 74, 710–727. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00564

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Collier, J. E. (2020). Applied structural equation modeling using AMOS: Basic to advanced techniques. New York, NY: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Connell, J. P., and Wellborn, J. G. (1991). “Competence, autonomy, and relatedness: a motivational analysis of self-system processes” in Self processes and development. eds. M. R. Gunnar and L. A. Sroufe (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc), 43–77.

Google Scholar

Cross, F. L., Marchand, A. D., Medina, M., Villafuerte, A., and Rivas-Drake, D. (2019). Academic socialization, parental educational expectations, and academic self-efficacy among Latino adolescents. Psychol. Sch. 56, 483–496. doi: 10.1002/pits.22239

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis-Kean, P. E. (2005). The influence of parent education and family income on child achievement: the indirect role of parental expectations and the home environment. J. Fam. Psychol. 19, 294–304. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.19.2.294

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dogan, U. (2017). Student engagement, academic self-efficacy, and academic motivation as predictors of academic performance. Anthropologist 20, 553–561. doi: 10.1080/09720073.2015.11891759

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dotterer, A., and Lowe, K. (2011). Classroom context, school engagement, and academic achievement in early adolescence. J. Youth Adolesc. 40, 1649–1660. doi: 10.1007/s10964-011-9647-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset: the new psychology of success. 1st Edn. New York, NY: Random House.

Google Scholar

Eccles, J. (2009). Who am I and what am I going to do with my life? Personal and collective identities as motivators of action. Educ. Psychol. 44, 78–89. doi: 10.1080/00461520902832368

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eccles, J. S., Vida, M. N., and Barber, B. (2004). The relation of early Adolescents' College plans and both academic ability and task-value beliefs to subsequent college enrollment. J. Early Adolesc. 24, 63–77. doi: 10.1177/0272431603260919

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eccles, J., and Wang, M.-T. (2012). “Part I commentary: so what is student engagement anyway?” in Handbook of research on student engagement. eds. S. L. Christenson, A. L. Reschly, and C. Wylie (New York, NY: Springer), 133–145.

Google Scholar

Fairchild, A. J., and McQuillin, S. D. (2010). Evaluating mediation and moderation effects in school psychology: a presentation of methods and review of current practice. J. Sch. Psychol. 48, 53–84. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2009.09.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fall, A.-M., and Roberts, G. (2012). High school dropouts: interactions between social context, self-perceptions, school engagement, and student dropout. J. Adolesc. 35, 787–798. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2011.11.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, W. (2011). Social influences, school motivation and gender differences: an application of the expectancy-value theory. Educ. Psychol. 31, 157–175. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2010.536525

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, W., and Williams, C. M. (2010). The effects of parental involvement on Students' academic self-efficacy, engagement and intrinsic motivation. Educ. Psychol. 30, 53–74. doi: 10.1080/01443410903353302

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, W., Williams, C. M., and Wolters, C. A. (2012). Parental involvement in predicting school motivation: similar and differential effects across ethnic groups. J. Educ. Res. 105, 21–35. doi: 10.1080/00220671.2010.515625

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, W., and Wolters, C. A. (2014). School motivation and high school dropout: the mediating role of educational expectation. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 84, 22–39. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Finn, J. D., and Zimmer, K. S. (2012). Student engagement: What is it? Why does it matter? in Handbook Res. Student Engag. eds. S. L. Christenson, A. L. Reschly, and C. Wylie (New York: Springer) 97–131.

Google Scholar

Finney, S. J., and DiStefano, C. (2013). “Nonnormal and categorical data in structural equation modeling” in Structural equation modeling: a second course. 2nd ed. eds. G. R. Hancock and R. O. Mueller (Charlotte, NC: IAP Information Age Publishing), 439–492.

Google Scholar

Flores, L. Y., Navarro, R. L., and DeWitz, S. J. (2008). Mexican American high school Students' postsecondary educational goals applying social cognitive career theory. J. Career Assess. 16, 489–501. doi: 10.1177/1069072708318905

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Flores, L. Y., Navarro, R. L., Hang Shim, L., Addae, D. A., Gonzalez, R., Luna, L. L., et al. (2014). Academic satisfaction among Latino/a and White men and women engineering students. J. Couns. Psychol. 61, 81–92. doi: 10.1037/a0034577

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fouad, N. A., and Guillen, A. (2006). Outcome expectations: looking to the past and potential future. J. Career Assess. 14, 130–142. doi: 10.1177/1069072705281370

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., and Paris, A. H. (2004). School engagement: potential of the concept, state of the evidence. Rev. Educ. Res. 74, 59–109. doi: 10.3102/00346543074001059

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fung, F., Tan, C. Y., and Chen, G. (2018). Student engagement and mathematics achievement: unraveling main and interactive effects. Psychol. Sch. 55, 815–831. doi: 10.1002/pits.22139

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Furrer, C. J. (2010). Capturing the friendship context with a collective property: friendship group engagement vs. disaffection. J. Adolesc. 33, 853–867. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.07.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Glick, J. E., and White, M. J. (2004). Post-secondary school participation of immigrant and native youth: the role of familial resources and educational expectations. Soc. Sci. Res. 33, 272–299. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2003.06.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldenberg, C., Gallimore, R., Reese, L., and Garnier, H. (2001). Cause or effect? A longitudinal study of immigrant Latino Parents' aspirations and expectations, and their Children's school performance. Am. Educ. Res. J. 38, 547–582. doi: 10.3102/00028312038003547

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Granziera, H., and Perera, H. N. (2019). Relations among teachers' self-efficacy beliefs, engagement, and work satisfaction: a social cognitive view. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 58, 75–84. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2019.02.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Greene, B. A. (2015). Measuring cognitive engagement with self-report scales: reflections from over 20 years of research. Educ. Psychol. 50, 14–30. doi: 10.1080/00461520.2014.989230

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., and Norenzayan, A. (2010). Most people are not WEIRD. Nature 466:29. doi: 10.1038/466029a

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Henry, K. L., Knight, K. E., and Thornberry, T. P. (2012). School disengagement as a predictor of dropout, delinquency, and problem substance use during adolescence and early adulthood. J. Youth Adolesc. 41, 156–166. doi: 10.1007/s10964-011-9665-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, N. E., and Tyson, D. F. (2009). Parental involvement in middle school: a meta-analytic assessment of the strategies that promote achievement. Dev. Psychol. 45, 740–763. doi: 10.1037/a0015362

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirschfield, P. J., and Gasper, J. (2011). The relationship between school engagement and delinquency in late childhood and early adolescence. J. Youth Adolesc. 40, 3–22. doi: 10.1007/s10964-010-9579-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoover-Dempsey, K. V., and Sandler, H. M. (1997). Why do parents become involved in their children's education? Rev. Educ. Res. 67, 3–42. doi: 10.3102/00346543067001003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L. T., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model: Multidiscip. J. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jang, H., Reeve, J., and Deci, E. L. (2010). Engaging students in learning activities: it is not autonomy support or structure but autonomy support and structure. J. Educ. Psychol. 102, 588–600. doi: 10.1037/a0019682

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeynes, W. H. (2005). A Meta-analysis of the relation of parental involvement to urban elementary school student academic achievement. Urban Educ. 40, 237–269. doi: 10.1177/0042085905274540

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeynes, W. H. (2007). The relationship between parental involvement and urban secondary school student academic achievement: a meta-analysis. Urban Educ. 42, 82–110. doi: 10.1177/0042085906293818

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, Y., Rosenzweig, E. Q., and Gaspard, H. (2018). An expectancy-value-cost approach in predicting adolescent students’ academic motivation and achievement. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2018.06.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, M. L., and Sinatra, G. M. (2013). Use of task-value instructional inductions for facilitating engagement and conceptual change. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 38, 51–63. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2012.09.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kindermann, T. A. (2007). Effects of naturally existing peer groups on changes in academic engagement in a cohort of sixth graders. Child Dev. 78, 1186–1203. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01060.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kindermann, T. A., and Gest, S. D. (2009).“Assessment of the peer group: Identifying naturally occurring social networks and capturing their effects” in Handbook of peer interactions, relationships, and groups. eds. J. F. Dovidio, P. Glick, and L. A. Rudman (Malden, MA: Blackwell) 100–117.

Google Scholar

Kiuru, N., Aunola, K., Vuori, J., and Nurmi, J.-E. (2007). The role of peer groups in adolescents’ educational expectations and adjustment. J. Youth Adolesc. 36, 995–1009. doi: 10.1007/s10964-006-9118-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klassen, R. M. (2004). A Cross-cultural investigation of the efficacy beliefs of south Asian immigrant and Anglo Canadian nonimmigrant early adolescents. J. Educ. Psychol. 96, 731–742. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.96.4.731

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. 3rd ed. New York, NY: Guilford Publications.

Google Scholar

Kline, R. B. (2023). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York, NY: Guilford Publications.

Google Scholar

Lam, S. F., Jimerson, S., Wong, B. P. H., Kikas, E., Shin, H., Veiga, F. H., et al. (2014). Understanding and measuring student engagement in school: the results of an international study from 12 countries. Sch. Psychol. Q. 29, 213–232. doi: 10.1037/spq0000057

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Landon, T. M., Ehrenreich, J. T., and Pincus, D. B. (2007). Self-efficacy: a comparison between clinically anxious and non-referred youth. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 38, 31–45. doi: 10.1007/s10578-006-0038-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lent, R. W., Brown, S. D., and Hackett, G. (1994). Toward a unifying social cognitive theory of career and academic interest, choice, and performance. J. Vocat. Behav. 45, 79–122. doi: 10.1006/jvbe.1994.1027

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lent, R. W., Brown, S. D., and Hackett, G. (2000). Contextual supports and barriers to career choice: a social cognitive analysis. J. Couns. Psychol. 47, 36–49. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.47.1.36

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lent, R. W., Miller, M. J., Smith, P. E., Watford, B. A., Lim, R. H., and Hui, K. (2016). Social cognitive predictors of academic persistence and performance in engineering: applicability across gender and race/ethnicity. J. Vocat. Behav. 94, 79–88. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2016.02.012

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lent, R. W., Miller, M. J., Smith, P. E., Watford, B. A., Lim, R. H., Hui, K., et al. (2013). Social cognitive predictors of adjustment to engineering majors across gender and race/ethnicity. J. Vocat. Behav. 83, 22–30. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2013.02.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Y., and Lerner, R. M. (2011). Trajectories of school engagement during adolescence: implications for grades, depression, delinquency, and substance use. Dev. Psychol. 47, 233–247. doi: 10.1037/a0021307

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Linnenbrink, E. A., and Pintrich, P. R. (2003). The role of self-efficacy beliefs in student engagement and learning in the classroom. Read. Writ. Q. 19, 119–137. doi: 10.1080/10573560308223

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Little, T. D., Cunningham, W. A., Shahar, G., and Widaman, K. F. (2002). To parcel or not to parcel: exploring the question, Weighing the Merits. Struct. Eq. Model. 9, 151–173. doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, R. D., Zhen, R., Liu, Y., Wang, J., Jiang, R., Xu, L., et al. (2018). Teacher support and math engagement: roles of academic self-efficacy and positive emotions. Educ. Psychol. 38, 3–16. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2017.1359238

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lynch, A. D., Lerner, R. M., and Leventhal, T. (2013). Adolescent academic achievement and school engagement: an examination of the role of school-wide peer culture. J. Youth Adolesc. 42, 6–19. doi: 10.1007/s10964-012-9833-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Malmberg, L.-E. (1996). How do Finnish students prepare for their future in three school types? The relation between content of plans, information gathering and self-evaluations. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 66, 457–469. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.1996.tb01212.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Markus, H. R., and Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychol. Rev. 98, 224–253. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, D. P., and Rimm-Kaufman, S. E. (2015). Do student self-efficacy and teacher-student interaction quality contribute to emotional and social engagement in fifth grade math? J. Sch. Psychol. 53, 359–373. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2015.07.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsunaga, M. (2008). Item parceling in structural equation modeling: a primer. Commun. Methods Meas. 2, 260–293. doi: 10.1080/19312450802458935

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McCormick, M. P., and Cappella, E. (2014). Conceptualizing academic norms in middle school. J. Early Adolesc. 35, 441–466. doi: 10.1177/0272431614535093

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Melby, J. N., Shu-Ann, F., Wickrama, K. A. S., Conger, R. D., and Conger, K. J. (2008). Adolescent family experiences and educational attainment during early adulthood. Dev. Psychol. 44, 1519–1536. doi: 10.1037/a0013352

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mello, Z. R. (2008). Gender variation in developmental trajectories of educational and occupational expectations and attainment from adolescence to adulthood. Dev. Psychol. 44, 1069–1080. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.4.1069

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, C. J., Perera, H. N., and Maghsoudlou, A. (2021). Students' multidimensional profiles of math engagement: predictors and outcomes from a self-system motivational perspective. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 91, 261–285. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12358

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Minter, A., and Pritzker, S. (2017). Measuring adolescent social and academic self-efficacy:Cross-ethnic validity of the SEQ-C. Res. Soc. Work. Pract. 27, 818–826. doi: 10.1177/1049731515615677

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreira, P., Cunha, D., and Inman, R. A. (2019). An integration of multiple student engagement dimensions into a single measure and validity-based studies. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 38, 564–580. doi: 10.1177/0734282919870973

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mroczkowski, A. L., and Sanchez, B. (2015). The role of racial discrimination in the economic value of education among urban, low-income Latina/o youth: ethnic identity and gender as moderators. Am. J. Community Psychol. 56, 1–11. doi: 10.1007/s10464-015-9728-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Murdock, T. B. (1999). The social context of risk: status and motivational predictors of alienation in middle school. J. Educ. Psychol. 91, 62–75. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.91.1.62

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Murdock, T. B., Anderman, L. H., and Hodge, S. A. (2000). Middle-grade predictors of students’ motivation and behavior in high school. J. Adolesc. Res. 15, 327–351. doi: 10.1177/0743558400153002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Muris, P. (2001). A brief questionnaire for measuring self-efficacy in youths. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 23, 145–149. doi: 10.1023/A:1010961119608

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, C. (2009). Parent and teacher relationships as predictors of school engagement and functioning among low-income urban youth. J. Early Adolesc. 29, 376–404. doi: 10.1177/0272431608322940

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Navarro, R. L., Flores, L. Y., Legerski, J. P., Brionez, J., May, S. F., Suh, H. N., et al. (2019). Social cognitive predictors of engineering students' academic persistence intentions, satisfaction, and engagement. J. Couns. Psychol. 66, 170–183. doi: 10.1037/cou0000319

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nurmi, J. E. (2004). “Socialization and self-development: channeling, selection, adjustment, and reflection”, in Handbook Adolescent Psychol. eds. R. M. Lerner and L. D. Steinberg (NJ: John Wiley & Sons). 85–124. doi: 10.1002/9780471726746.ch4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Olivier, E., Archambault, C., de Clercq, M., and Galand, B. (2019). Student self-efficacy, classroom engagement, and academic achievement: comparing three theoretical frameworks. J. Youth Adolesc. 48, 326–340. doi: 10.1007/s10964-018-0952-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearce, R. R. (2006). Effects of cultural and social structural factors on the achievement of White and Chinese American students at school transition points. Am. Educ. Res. J. 43, 75–101. doi: 10.3102/00028312043001075

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Phan, H. P., and Ngu, B. H. (2014). Longitudinal examination of personal self-efficacy and engagement-related attributes: how do they relate. Am. J. Appl. Psychol. 3:80. doi: 10.11648/j.ajap.20140304.11

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reeve, J., and Lee, W. (2014). Students' classroom engagement produces longitudinal changes in classroom motivation. J. Educ. Psychol. 106, 527–540. doi: 10.1037/a0034934

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reeve, J., and Tseng, C.-M. (2011). Agency as a fourth aspect of students’ engagement during learning activities. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 36, 257–267. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2011.05.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reschly, A. L., Huebner, E. S., Appleton, J. J., and Antaramian, S. (2008). Engagement as flourishing: the contribution of positive emotions and coping to adolescents' engagement at school and with learning. Psychol. Sch. 45, 419–431. doi: 10.1002/pits.20306

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., Baroody, A. E., Larsen, R. A. A., Curby, T. W., and Abry, T. (2015). To what extent do teacher-student interaction quality and student gender contribute to fifth graders' engagement in mathematics learning? J. Educ. Psychol. 107, 170–185. doi: 10.1037/a0037252

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodkin, P. C., and Ryan, A. M. (2012). “Child and adolescent peer relations in educational context” in APA educational psychology handbook, Vol 2: Individual differences and cultural and contextual factors. eds. K. R. Harris, S. Graham, T. Urdan, S. Graham, J. M. Royer, and M. Zeidner (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 363–389.

Google Scholar

Ryan, A. M. (2000). Peer groups as a context for the socialization of Adolescents' motivation, engagement, and achievement in school. Educ. Psychol. 35, 101–111. doi: 10.1207/S15326985EP3502_4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, A. M. (2001). The peer group as a context for the development of young adolescent motivation and achievement. Child Dev. 72, 1135–1150. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00338

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, A. M., North, E. A., and Ferguson, S. (2019). “Peers and Engagement”, in Handbook of student engagement interventions. eds. J. A. Fredricks, A. L. Reschly, and S. L. Christenson (London, United Kingdom: Academic Press). 73–85.

Google Scholar

Salmela-Aro, K., Savolainen, H., and Holopainen, L. (2009). Depressive symptoms and school burnout during adolescence: evidence from two Cross-lagged longitudinal studies. J. Youth Adolesc. 38, 1316–1327. doi: 10.1007/s10964-008-9334-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Salmela-Aro, K., and Upadaya, K. (2012). The schoolwork engagement inventory: energy, dedication, and absorption (EDA). Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 28, 60–67. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000091

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlomer, G. L., Bauman, S., and Card, N. A. (2010). Best practices for missing data Management in Counseling Psychology. J. Couns. Psychol. 57, 1–10. doi: 10.1037/a0018082

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schumacker, R. E., and Lomax, R. G. (2016). A Beginner’s guide to structural equation modeling. New York, NY: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Shin, H., and Ryan, A. M. (2014). Early adolescent friendships and academic adjustment: examining selection and influence processes with longitudinal social network analysis. Dev. Psychol. 50, 2462–2472. doi: 10.1037/a0037922

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shrout, P. E., and Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: new procedures and recommendations. Psychol. Methods 7, 422–445. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.7.4.422

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Simons-Morton, B. G., and Crump, A. D. (2003). Association of Parental Involvement and Social Competence with school adjustment and engagement among sixth graders. J. Sch. Health 73, 121–126. doi: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.2003.tb03586.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Skinner, E. A. (2016). “Engagement and disaffection as central to processes of motivational resilience and development” in Handbook of motivation at school. eds. K. R. Wentzel and A. Wigfield (New York, NY: Routledge), 145–168.

Google Scholar

Skinner, E., Furrer, C., Marchand, G., and Kindermann, T. (2008). Engagement and disaffection in the classroom: part of a larger motivational dynamic? J. Educ. Psychol. 100, 765–781. doi: 10.1037/a0012840

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Skinner, E. A., Kindermann, T. A., and Furrer, C. J. (2009). A motivational perspective on engagement and disaffection: conceptualization and assessment of Children's behavioral and emotional participation in academic activities in the classroom. Educ. Psychol. Measurement 69, 493–525. doi: 10.1177/0013164408323233

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Steenberghs, N., Lavrijsen, J., Soenens, B., and Verschueren, K. (2021). Peer effects on engagement and disengagement: differential contributions from friends, popular peers, and the entire class. Front. Psychol. 12:726815. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.726815

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Suizzo, M.-A., Pahlke, E., Chapman-Hilliard, C., and Harvey, K. E. (2016). African American and Mexican American youths’ college adjustment and perceptions of parental academic socialization: interactions between ethnicity and parental education. Res. Hum. Dev. 13, 241–257. doi: 10.1080/15427609.2016.1194709

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Suizzo, M.-A., Pahlke, E., Yarnell, L., Chen, K.-Y., and Romero, S. (2012). Home-based parental involvement in young Children’s learning across U.S. ethnic groups. J. Fam. Issues 35, 254–287. doi: 10.1177/0192513x12465730

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Suizzo, M. A., and Soon, K. (2006). Parental academic socialization: effects of home-based parental involvement on locus of control across U.S. ethnic groups. Educ. Psychol. 26, 827–846. doi: 10.1080/01443410600941961

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Suldo, S. M., and Shaffer, E. J. (2007). Evaluation of the self-efficacy questionnaire for children in two samples of American adolescents. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 25, 341–355. doi: 10.1177/0734282907300636

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Symonds, J. E., D'Urso, G., and Schoon, I. (2023). The long-term benefits of adolescent school engagement for adult educational and employment outcomes. Dev. Psychol. 59, 503–514. doi: 10.1037/dev0001458

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Usher, E. L., and Pajares, F. (2006). Sources of academic and self-regulatory efficacy beliefs of entering middle school students. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 31, 125–141. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2005.03.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vartanian, T. P., Karen, D., Buck, P. W., and Cadge, W. (2007). Early factors leading to college graduation for ASIANS and non-ASIANS in the UNITED STATES. Sociol. Q. 48, 165–197. doi: 10.1111/j.1533-8525.2007.00075.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, M.-T., Chow, A., Hofkens, T., and Salmela-Aro, K. (2015). The trajectories of student emotional engagement and school burnout with academic and psychological development: findings from Finnish adolescents. Learn. Instruct. 36, 57–65. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2014.11.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, M. T., and Degol, J. (2014). Staying engaged: knowledge and research needs in student engagement. Child Dev. Perspect. 8, 137–143. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12073

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, M. T., Degol, J. L., and Henry, D. A. (2019). An integrative development-in-sociocultural-context model for children's engagement in learning. Am. Psychol. 74, 1086–1102. doi: 10.1037/amp0000522

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, M. T., and Eccles, J. S. (2012). Social support matters: longitudinal effects of social support on three dimensions of school engagement from middle to high school. Child Dev. 83, 877–895. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01745.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, M.-T., and Eccles, J. S. (2013). School context, achievement motivation, and academic engagement: a longitudinal study of school engagement using a multidimensional perspective. Learn. Instr. 28, 12–23. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.04.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, M. T., and Fredricks, J. A. (2014). The reciprocal links between school engagement, youth problem behaviors, and school dropout during adolescence. Child Dev. 85, 722–737. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12138

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, M.-T., Fredricks, J. A., Ye, F., Hofkens, T. L., and Linn, J. S. (2016). The math and science engagement scales: scale development, validation, and psychometric properties. Learn. Instr. 43, 16–26. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2016.01.008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, M.-T., Henry, D. A., and Degol, J. L. (2020). “A development-in-sociocultural-context perspective on the multiple pathways to youth's engagement in learning” in Advances in motivation science. (San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press), 113–160.

Google Scholar

Wang, M. T., Hill, N. E., and Hofkens, T. (2014). Parental involvement and African American and European American Adolescents' academic, behavioral, and emotional development in secondary school. Child Dev. 85, 2151–2168. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12284

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, M.-T., and Holcombe, R. (2010). Adolescents’ perceptions of school environment, engagement, and academic achievement in middle school. Am. Educ. Res. J. 47, 633–662. doi: 10.3102/0002831209361209

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, M.-T., Kiuru, N., Degol, J. L., and Salmela-Aro, K. (2018). Friends, academic achievement, and school engagement during adolescence: a social network approach to peer influence and selection effects. Learn. Instruct. 58, 148–160. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2018.06.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, M. T., and Peck, S. C. (2013). Adolescent educational success and mental health vary across school engagement profiles. Dev. Psychol. 49, 1266–1276. doi: 10.1037/a0030028

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wentzel, K. R. (2009). “Peers and academic functioning at school” in Handbook of peer interactions, relationships, and groups. (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 531–547.

Google Scholar

Wheaton, B., Muthén, B., Alwin, D. F., and Summers, G. F. (1977). Assessing reliability and stability in panel models. Sociol. Methodol. 8, 84–136. doi: 10.2307/270754

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilder, S. (2014). Effects of parental involvement on academic achievement: a meta-synthesis. Educ. Rev. 66, 377–397. doi: 10.1080/00131911.2013.780009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, D., Jones, D., Bocell, F., Crawford, J., Kim, M. J., Veilleux, N., et al. (2015). Belonging and academic engagement among undergraduate STEM students: a multi-institutional study. Res. High. Educ. 56, 750–776. doi: 10.1007/s11162-015-9367-x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, Z. Y., and Liem, G. A. D. (2022). Student engagement: current state of the construct, conceptual refinement, and future research directions. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 34, 107–138. doi: 10.1007/s10648-021-09628-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiong, Y., Qin, X., Wang, Q., and Ren, P. (2021). Parental involvement in Adolescents' learning and academic achievement: Cross-lagged effect and mediation of academic engagement. J. Youth Adolesc. 50, 1811–1823. doi: 10.1007/s10964-021-01460-w

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhen, R., Liu, R. D., Ding, Y., Liu, Y., Wang, J., and Xu, L. (2018). The moderating role of intrinsic value in the relation between psychological needs support and academic engagement in mathematics among Chinese adolescent students. Int. J. Psychol. 53, 313–320. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12374

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zumbrunn, S., McKim, C., Buhs, E., and Hawley, L. R. (2014). Support, belonging, motivation, and engagement in the college classroom: a mixed method study. Instr. Sci. 42, 661–684. doi: 10.1007/s11251-014-9310-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: academic engagement, academic socialization, motivational beliefs, self-efficacy, outcome expectations, high school, peer academic socialization, parental academic socialization

Citation: Woreta GT (2024) Predictors of academic engagement of high school students: academic socialization and motivational beliefs. Front. Psychol. 15:1347163. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1347163

Received: 30 November 2023; Accepted: 25 June 2024;
Published: 25 July 2024.

Edited by:

Weihua Fan, University of Houston, United States

Reviewed by:

Nan Li, The University of Texas Rio Grande Valley, United States
Daijiazi Tang, University of Michigan, United States

Copyright © 2024 Woreta. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Getachew Tassew Woreta, aGFpbHdvcmV0YTE2QGdtYWlsLmNvbQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.