Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 05 July 2024
Sec. Educational Psychology

The impact of school closures during the COVID-19 pandemic on reading fluency among second grade students: socioeconomic and gender perspectives

  • Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center for the Studies of Learning Disabilities, Department of Learning Disabilities, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel

Introduction: The acquisition of reading skills is a crucial milestone in early education, with formal instruction and practice playing pivotal roles. The outbreak of COVID-19 led to widespread school closures and a shift to remote learning.

Methods: This study aimed to investigate the effects of school closures on reading acquisition and fluency among a large sample of second-grade children, considering socioeconomic status (SES) and gender differences. In 2019, a cohort of 2228 second-grade students from 34 schools was assessed for word reading fluency and comprehension. In 2020, during the pandemic, 765 students from a subsample of 20 original schools were re-evaluated using the same measures. The study also collected school-related data.

Results: The findings from the entire sample indicated no significant differences in fluency and comprehension scores between children in the second grade in 2019 and 2020. However, a significant interaction emerged when analyzing low SES versus high SES children. Children from low SES backgrounds exhibited notably lower reading scores after a year of remote learning due to the COVID-19 outbreak. Moreover, the disparity in reading scores between low SES and high SES children nearly doubled in 2020. Gender differences were also detected.

Discussion: These results underscore the impact of remote learning during the COVID-19 crisis on exacerbating gaps in reading fluency and comprehension between children from high and low SES backgrounds. The implications of these findings highlight the critical role of in-person schooling and targeted support for disadvantaged students, especially during pivotal stages of reading development.

1 Introduction

The global outbreak of COVID-19 in early 2020 prompted widespread school closures across many countries, including Israel, resulting in a significant shift toward remote learning (Kuhfeld et al., 2020; Lake and Dusseault, 2020; United Nations, 2020). This unprecedented situation led to changes in the educational landscape, with students adapting to shortened school days delivered through technological platforms (Hall et al., 2020; Kuhfeld et al., 2020). Moreover, parents took on a more prominent role in delivering the curriculum in many instances (Reimer et al., 2021).

A fundamental milestone in early elementary education is the acquisition of reading skills. The process of learning to read involves substantial formal instruction and practice (Stanovich and West, 1989). However, the adverse impact of COVID-19 on reading acquisition was particularly pronounced among disadvantaged children who faced unequal access to educational resources (UNESCO, 2020). This disparity is a significant concern, particularly with studies highlighting potential “Matthew Effect” dynamics during the pandemic, where existing gaps in reading ability between children from different socioeconomic backgrounds could be further exacerbated (García-Muiña et al., 2021). The “Matthew Effect” concept underscores how initial advantages can magnify disparities over time (Stanovich, 1986), which, in the context of reading, could suggest that children from lower socioeconomic status backgrounds might fall behind even more in their reading development. Furthermore, parents of elementary school children reported a reduction in learning-related activities during COVID-19 closures (Andrew et al., 2020), potentially compounding challenges for struggling readers.

The second grade is a pivotal stage where children transition from decoding-based reading strategies to more fluent and accurate reading (Chall, 1983; Bar-Kochva, 2013). Although studies on the impact of COVID-19 closures on reading have emerged, many have focused on later stages of elementary school (from 3rd grade onwards; Kuhfeld et al., 2020; Engzell et al., 2021; Kaffenberger, 2021; Relyea et al., 2023). Few large-scale studies have addressed the effects of COVID-19 school closures on reading development during earlier foundational stages (Ardington et al., 2021). The Israeli Ministry of Education’s expert panel highlighted the need to investigate and comprehend gaps arising from COVID-19, particularly in early childhood, and emphasized the importance of empirical studies based on validated tools conducted at multiple time points (Kesner Baruch et al., 2021).

This study aims to address a gap in the literature by examining reading acquisition among a substantial sample of Hebrew-speaking second-grade children—an age group that has received less attention during the early elementary years. Specifically, we investigate the trajectory of fluency development among children of diverse socioeconomic backgrounds over a year, encompassing both pre-COVID-19 conditions and the subsequent year, within the same district.

1.1 Reading fluency development

Reading fluency is a critical skill characterized by the ability to read with automaticity, speed, accuracy, proper expression, and appropriate phrasing (National Reading Panel (US), 2000). As reading fluency advances, the cognitive load associated with decoding decreases, allowing more cognitive resources to be allocated to comprehending the text’s meaning (Wolf and Katzir-Cohen, 2001; Perfetti, 2007; Stevens et al., 2017). The progression of oral reading fluency typically takes place between the second and third grades, persistently evolving throughout the elementary years (Chall, 1983). Early elementary oral reading fluency contributes to proficient silent reading, which becomes crucial in later elementary school (Price et al., 2016). Numerous studies across diverse languages underscore the significance of reading fluency, revealing its predictive role in reading comprehension, the ultimate goal of reading (Klauda and Guthrie, 2008; Kim et al., 2010; Stevens et al., 2017; Nevo et al., 2020).

Assessing reading fluency frequently involves measuring the accurate pronunciation of words within a restricted timeframe. For instance, the Test of Word Reading Efficiency (TOWRE) evaluates the ability to pronounce printed words both accurately and fluently, reflecting the comprehension of the read words (Torgeson et al., 1999; Fuchs et al., 2001; Good et al., 2001). Proficient automatic sight-word reading is fundamental for fluid and natural text comprehension (Miller and Schwanenflugel, 2008; Kuhn et al., 2010). Thus, tests gaging the number of correctly read words within a given duration serve as valuable tools for identifying potential reading difficulties (Valencia et al., 2010). Research underscores that during early grades, reading fluency significantly contributes to comprehension, a principle that is particularly pronounced in second-grade readers (Fuchs et al., 2001; Valencia et al., 2010). Reading in context demands the activation of semantics, as readers simultaneously process words while aiming to extract textual meaning (Katzir et al., 2006). Consequently, the amalgamation of syntactic rules and semantic structures is essential for constructing cohesive units of ideas. Insufficient automation at lower processing levels (letters or words) could impede processing at higher levels (sentences or texts; Logan, 1997).

This study’s focus is on Hebrew-speaking children, with Hebrew characterized as an Abjad writing system. An Abjad writing system predominantly consists of consonantal representation with sporadic and incomplete vowel representation (Eviatar and Share, 2013). Hebrew is available in two forms: pointed (shallow orthography) and unpointed (deep orthography). Early reading acquisition in first grade revolves around shallow pointed Hebrew, allowing for rapid association between letters and sounds due to comprehensive phonological cues (Share and Levin, 1999; Shany et al., 2012). As such, most children become skilled decoders by the end of first grade, heightening the importance of speed and fluency (Lipka et al., 2016). The progression to partially pointed texts, particularly in second and third grades, exposes readers to lexico-morpho-orthographic knowledge utilization (Shany et al., 2012).

In nurturing reading fluency in first and second graders, the recommendation is for students to engage in daily reading aloud and silent practice, utilizing materials tailored to their level of competence (National Reading Panel (US), 2000; The Israeli Ministry of Education, 2014). The shift to remote instruction is believed to have potentially hindered teachers’ ability to facilitate ample reading fluency practice opportunities.

1.2 The challenges of remotely teaching literacy to diverse learners

The abrupt shift to remote learning during the pandemic posed significant challenges for educators, particularly in teaching literacy to young children. These learners, who had not yet become independent readers, faced obstacles in navigating technological tools independently (Sucena et al., 2022). As literacy development heavily relies on face-to-face interaction, the transition to remote learning presented hurdles in providing the necessary constant feedback and personalized attention required for learning to read and write (Relyea et al., 2023).

Teachers were thrust into an unfamiliar landscape, requiring them to adapt and innovate in the realm of online instruction with limited prior experience. This shift was especially arduous for educators in the early elementary grades (Giovannella et al., 2020; Kruszewska et al., 2020; Letzel et al., 2020; Dotan et al., 2021). A study in Israel conducted by Dotan et al. (2021) among first- and second-grade teachers revealed their struggles in remote teaching, including challenges in fostering reading fluency and comprehension, addressing the needs of struggling readers, and assessing literacy skills remotely. Beyond curriculum adaptation, teachers also encountered difficulties in teaching diverse learners. Notably, the digital divide was exacerbated by socioeconomic status (SES) disparities, with 75% of low-SES school teachers reporting unequal access to computers among their students, compared to 46% in middle-high SES schools (Dotan et al., 2021).

Despite the hurdles, some positive outcomes were observed due to school closures. The increased involvement of parents in providing home support during remote learning potentially contributed to emotional and academic advancements (Immerfall, 2020). Nonetheless, the prevailing sentiment from research indicates learning loss resulting from school absences (Kuhfeld et al., 2020; Engzell et al., 2021; OECD, 2023).

In evaluating the pandemic’s impact on learning, the term “unfinished learning” becomes relevant—a concept encompassing missed instruction due to school closures (Lambert and Sassone, 2020; The National Authority for Measurement and Evaluation in Education, 2023). Notably, this term does not imply a permanent deficit; instead, with proper support, students can attain the necessary mastery.

Additionally, the term “vulnerable children” takes on significance in this context, especially concerning children from low SES backgrounds. Their vulnerability extends to economic hardships, limited access to resources, reduced support, and heightened stress at home (Drane et al., 2020; Masters et al., 2020). The literature review reinforces the imperative to attend to these vulnerable learners, particularly those from low SES backgrounds who are at risk of accumulating academic gaps, especially in reading, during the COVID-19 period (Kaffenberger, 2021; Relyea et al., 2023).

In conclusion, the challenges of remotely teaching literacy to diverse learners during the pandemic were multifaceted. Teachers navigated the complexities of adapting to online instruction, while students faced barriers in receiving the personalized attention necessary for literacy development. The unequal access to technology further exacerbated disparities, with vulnerable learners from low SES backgrounds at greater risk of falling behind. Despite the potential benefits of home support, learning loss remained a prevalent concern. The educational community’s focus on addressing these challenges is essential for fostering equitable learning outcomes and supporting vulnerable children’s academic growth.

Several studies have attempted to estimate the extent of learning gaps resulting from school closures, drawing insights from previous instances of learning loss during periods like summer vacations or crises. Bao et al. (2020) predicted that kindergarten children in the United States would experience an average loss of 31% in their reading ability gained in 2020. Kuhfeld et al. (2020) expanded on this by demonstrating that third- to seventh-grade students could lose around 35% of their reading gains during the COVID-19 period compared to a typical school year. Furthermore, the impact was more pronounced among students with low socioeconomic status (SES). In their predictions about school achievement variability during the pandemic, they estimated a reading score decrease of 1.2 times lower than typical year scores (Kuhfeld et al., 2020). Hevia et al. (2022) examined 10-15-year-old readers and indicated that the younger readers, as well as those with low SES, showed the greatest learning loss in reading during the COVID-19 pandemic.

An interesting recent meta-analysis review (Betthäuser et al., 2023) identified 42 studies from 15 countries on learning progress among primary and secondary school children during the COVID-19 pandemic. It was found that students experienced a loss of approximately 35% of a school year’s learning. On average, the learning advancement of school-aged children was significantly reduced during the pandemic. Furthermore, the review implies that the pandemic has intensified educational disparities among children from diverse SES, which have been found before the pandemic.

This trend receives support from research on regular periods, such as the summer vacation, during which the learning loss of children from low socioeconomic backgrounds is significantly more substantial than that of those from moderate to high socioeconomic backgrounds (e.g., Burkam et al., 2004; Downey et al., 2004; Kim and White, 2008; Allington et al., 2010).

A simulation study conducted across seven low- and middle-income countries by Kaffenberger (2021) projected that a school closure lasting one-third of a regular year during third grade could lead to a year-long loss in learning until tenth grade, disproportionately affecting students in lower-income countries.

These trends have been found not only in reading but also in mathematical abilities, Blaskó et al. (2022) sought to assess the potential impact of pandemic-related learning losses in mathematics across 22 European countries, surveying 4,400 4th graders. Their study was based on data from an international achievement survey conducted before the pandemic, namely the Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study 2019. The findings revealed significant disparities among European countries regarding the availability of essential distance-learning resources, parental backgrounds, and school differences. These discrepancies in country standings are likely attributed to both the affluence of and inequalities within the respective countries, which, in turn, can impact the effect of learning loss.

A recent study conducted in the US by Relyea et al. (2023) found that the average reading achievement gain during the 2020–2021 school year was lower compared to the 2018–2019 school year. The observed effect sizes for learning loss were 0.54, 0.27, and 0.28 standard deviations for grades 3, 4, and 5, respectively. Similar gaps in reading skills were detected among second-grade students in South Africa (Ardington et al., 2021). This study compared reading skills of students assessed before (2019) and during the pandemic (2020), revealing a reading gap ranging from 57 to 70% for English-speaking second graders.

A study focused on fifth-grade students in Germany, employing real-time assessments through a reading comprehension task in 2020 after school closures, highlighted a learning loss of 11–17% compared to previous measurements (Schult et al., 2022).

A recent systematic review (Panagouli et al., 2021), synthesizing data from 42 studies primarily conducted in Europe, Asia, and America, investigates the impact of online learning and modified educational methods on school-aged students during the COVID-19 pandemic. The review encompasses students aged 8 to 22 and revealed varied effects: The most prominent trend indicated that students experienced learning loss, especially in math and reading, though some benefited. Younger students and those with neurodevelopmental disorders or special education needs faced greater challenges. Additionally, parents reported similar trends, observing declines in their children’s performance, though some noted benefits from online learning. Teachers mainly reported academic gaps, particularly in mathematics and reading. Despite challenges, younger students showed enthusiasm for interactive learning materials, suggesting their positive effects should be considered.

Furthermore, a meta-analysis of 18 studies (König and Frey, 2022) mainly from the United States and Europe (predominantly Germany and the Netherlands), assessed the impact of COVID-19-related school closures on student achievement. The analysis showed a negative effect, with a weekly learning loss of −0.022. It also tentatively suggested that younger primary school students were more adversely affected compared to older students, possibly due to their lower self-regulated learning capabilities and the vital role of teacher scaffolding in regular instruction. The analysis suggested that remote learning was more effective in later lockdown phases than initially, possibly due to the familiarity gained with established online learning apps.

A study spanning from third to ninth grade in Switzerland investigated the impact of COVID-19-related school closures and the effectiveness of in-person versus distance learning in math and language (Tomasik et al., 2021). It was found that while older students could somewhat offset the effects of school closures, younger students faced significant challenges. Learning progress for younger children not only slowed down, potentially affecting future development, but also became more varied. While a small group of primary school students benefited from closures, others experienced severe declines in performance. These children are at risk of falling behind academically, emphasizing the importance of addressing their needs.

These studies collectively underscore the pervasive impact of COVID-19-induced school closures on students’ reading skills, transcending socioeconomic, cultural, and linguistic boundaries. Overall, these findings emphasize that the pandemic’s repercussions on reading development have been particularly detrimental for children from low-SES backgrounds. Consequently, students returned to school with substantial and divergent learning gaps, necessitating targeted efforts from educators to address and mitigate these disparities. Notably, learning losses were more pronounced among students from less educated and low SES households (Engzell et al., 2021; Kaffenberger, 2021; Betthäuser et al., 2023; Relyea et al., 2023).

1.3 Reading and gender

Gender constitutes another significant contextual factor within the realm of children’s reading development. Despite standardized literacy instruction in classrooms, disparities in reading achievement between boys and girls have been consistently observed. Numerous studies have consistently highlighted noteworthy gender differences in reading achievement across the entire spectrum of reading abilities within educational settings (Chatterji, 2006; Mullis et al., 2007; Logan and Johnston, 2010; Robinson and Lubienski, 2011; Reardon et al., 2019).

Remarkably, girls consistently outperform boys in reading achievement (Chatterji, 2006; Mullis et al., 2007; Logan and Johnston, 2010; Robinson and Lubienski, 2011; Katzir et al., 2018; Reardon et al., 2019), and these gender differences do not display a marked declining trend across elementary or secondary schooling (Reardon et al., 2019; Reilly et al., 2019). Additionally, substantial gender imbalances exist in poor reading, with boys being disproportionately represented (Reilly et al., 2019). Notably, prior empirical evidence (Coles and Hall, 2002; Mullis et al., 2007) consistently indicates that girls report higher reading frequency compared to boys. Gender-linked disparities in reading frequency may indeed influence variations in reading performance.

Support for gender differences can be found in the latest PISA report, in which girls outperformed boys in reading by an average of 24 points across OECD countries, indicating a universal gender gap. Among low performers, boys outnumbered girls, constituting 31% compared to 22% in reading proficiency. Conversely, among top performers, girls slightly outnumbered boys, with 8% versus 6% on average across OECD nations. In Israel, ranked 30th out of 81 countries, girls achieved a mean reading score of 486, surpassing boys by 24 points (462). While girls’ literacy achievements declined compared to previous years, boys showed improvement. Despite this narrowing trend, the gender gap still favors girls in reading proficiency. The gender gap scenario in Israel closely mirrors the OECD average. The Israeli Ministry of Education emphasized, based on the PISA 2022 findings, that the gender gaps in reading proficiency translate to nearly a year of schooling.

While gender effects in remote learning have primarily been explored among older students, limited research has delved into gender-specific effects on young learners during the COVID-19 pandemic. Some studies suggest that females tend to exhibit greater adaptability to collaborative and technology-based instruction, while others find that males often display a higher comfort level with the technical aspects of remote learning platforms (Jones et al., 2021).

It is vital to underscore that most existing studies have focused on older children rather than those in the early stages of elementary school, where reading acquisition begins. As such, this present study emphasizes reading acquisition among second-grade students, aiming to bridge a gap in the literature pertaining to reading development during COVID-19. This research particularly targets children from diverse backgrounds at this pivotal stage. Furthermore, the study’s focus extends to examining whether gender-related differences manifest differently among boys and girls.

Research Questions:

1. What is the effect of COVID-19 on second-grade children’s reading fluency, and is there an interaction between COVID-19, SES, and gender on reading fluency?

2. What is the effect of COVID-19 on second-grade children’s comprehension fluency, and is there an interaction between COVID-19, SES, and gender on comprehension fluency?

2 Methods

2.1 Participants

The study included primary school students from the Israeli public education system, all Hebrew speaking children with typical IQs, encompassing various socioeconomic status (SES) backgrounds in the southern region of Israel. The participants’ age range was between seven and 8 years old, with a relatively equal distribution of boys (49%) and girls (51%). None of the children in the sample exhibited significant neurological difficulties. The division of children into SES groups was based on the Ministry of Education’s scoring system for schools, utilizing neighborhood and parental demographic information including education and income. A total of 20 schools were examined at both time points with 5% of the schools representing high SES, 55% medium SES and 40% of the schools from low SES. A comprehensive overview of sociodemographic characteristics is presented in Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sociodemographic characteristics of the sample.

2.2 Measures

2.2.1 Reading fluency

Word reading fluency was assessed using the TOWRE test (Katzir et al., 2012, based on Torgeson et al., 1999). Administered individually, participants were tasked with orally reading 80 single words as swiftly and accurately as possible within a 45-s timeframe. The words were progressively ordered in terms of complexity. Scores were computed based on the number of correct words read in 45 s and the error percentage. The internal consistency reliability (α) of this assessment was 0.95.

2.2.2 Comprehension fluency

A group-administered task was employed to evaluate semantic comprehension fluency (Yinon and Shaul, 2017, based on Hutzler and Wimmer, 2004). This task consisted of 21 sentences spanning a range of everyday topics. Participants were required to read each sentence and promptly indicate whether it was semantically accurate or erroneous, all within a two-minute timeframe. The scores were calculated based on the number of accurately marked sentences within 2 min and the error percentage. The internal consistency reliability (α) for this task was 0.93.

2.3 Procedure

The necessary approvals were secured from the Ministry of Education and the relevant university’s ethics committee prior to data collection. All assessments were individually administered to participants in a designated quiet room within the school premises. Each assessment session lasted approximately 10 min. During the initial year of the study (October 2019), 1,460 children from 20 schools underwent testing. In the subsequent year (October 2020), 815 children were tested from the same 20 schools. All assessments were conducted individually during school hours in a controlled environment.

3 Results

3.1 First research question: the effect of COVID-19, SES and gender on reading fluency

To answer the first research question regarding the combined effect of COVID-19, SES, and gender on reading fluency, a univariate analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was run with COVID-19, SES, and gender as independent variables, reading fluency as the dependent variable, and school as a covariate variable. The descriptive statistics of the word reading fluency is presented in Table 2. The analysis revealed no main effect of COVID-19 or gender, F’s < 1. The main effect of SES was significant, F(2, 1988) = 39.15, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.04, indicating that participants in the Low SES schools (m = 21.75, SE = 0.45) had lower reading fluency compared to medium SES (m = 25.67, SD = 0.35; p < 0.001) which were lower than the High SES (m = 31.64, SE = 1.32; p < 0.05). There were significant differences between all the different SES in reading fluency (p < 0.001).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mean and (SD) of word reading fluency in the among the different SES groups and gender in both years of the study.

The interaction between COVID-19 and SES was significant, F(2, 1988) = 3.99, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.01. Post-hoc analyses revealed that the negative effect of COVID-19 existed only in low SES schools, F(1, 761) = 6.89, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.01. Low SES Participants in year 2 (post-COVID-19) had lower reading fluency (m = 20.56, SD = 0.64) than year 1 participants (pre-COVID-19; m = 22.66, SD = 0.47). There was no effect of COVID-19 on medium SES, F(1, 1,371) = 2.14, p = 0.14, nor High SES (F < 1). See Figure 1.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Word-reading fluency among the different SES levels in both years.

In addition, the interaction between COVID-19 and gender was significant, F(1, 1988) = 3.82, p = 0.05, η2 = 0.00. Post-hoc analyses revealed a marginally significant effect of gender on reading fluency in year 1, in year 1, F(1, 1,455) = 3.36, p = 0.07, η2 = 0.00, indicating that females’ reading fluency (m = 24.04) was slightly lower than that of males’ (m = 25.08, SD = 10.48). In year 2, there the performance of females was higher than the males.

The interaction between SES and gender, as well as the triple interaction between COVID-19, SES, and gender, were insignificant (F’s < 1).

Following this ANCOVA analysis, another ANCOVA analysis was run without school as a covariate variable. This analysis yielded similar trends: a significant main effect of SES, F(2, 1989) = 24.54, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.02, and interaction of COVID-19 and SES, F(2, 1989) = 3.99, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.01; a marginally significant interaction between COVID-19 and gender, F(1, 1989) = 3.82, p = 0.05, η2 = 0.00; and the insignificant effects were the main effects of gender and COVID-19, and the interactions of SES × COVID-19, and SES × COVID-19 × gender (all F’s < 1).

3.2 Second research question: the effect of COVID-19, SES and gender comprehension fluency

To address the second research question concerning the combined impact of COVID-19, SES, and gender on comprehension fluency, two similar univariate analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) were conducted with COVID-19, SES, and gender as independent variables, comprehension fluency as the dependent variable, and with and without school as a covariate variable. The descriptive statistics of the reading comprehension fluency is presented in Table 3. The analysis that included school as a covariate variable revealed a significant main effect of SES, F(2, 1958) = 14.46, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.02, indicating that participants in low SES schools (m = 5.82, SE = 0.17) had lower reading fluency compared to medium SES (m = 7.00, SD = 0.13; p < 0.001) and high SES (m = 7.21 SD = 0.51). There were no differences in comprehension fluency between high SES and medium (p = 0.66) (Figure 2). This analysis did not indicate main effects of COVID-19, F(1, 1958) = 2.58, p = 0.11, or gender, F < 1. An examination of the interactions indicated that all interactions were insignificant: COVID-19 × gender, F(1, 1958) = 1.87, p = 0.17; and COVID-19 × SES, gender × SES, and COVID-19 × gender × SES, all F’s < 1.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Mean and (SD) of reading comprehension fluency in the among the different SES groups and gender in both years of the study.

The ANCOVA analysis that was run without school as a covariate variable yielded similar trends: a significant main effect of SES, F(2, 1959) = 13.71, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.01. All other effects were insignificant: the main effects of COVID-19 m, F(1,1959) = 2.51, p = 0.11, and gender F < 1, and the interactions of COVID-19 x gender, F(1,1959) = 2.02, p = 0.16, SES × COVID-19, SES X gender, and SES × COVID-19 × gender (all F’s < 1).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Comprehension fluency among the different SES levels in both years.

4 Discussion

The acquisition of reading skills stands as a crucial milestone in early elementary education, a complex process that requires significant hours of formal teaching and practice (Stanovich and West, 1989). Against this backdrop, this study aimed to scrutinize the impact of Coronavirus-related school closures on the development of reading fluency and comprehension among second-grade students. Additionally, it aimed to assess the differential impact of COVID-19 on reading skills among second-grade students with varying socioeconomic backgrounds and to explore potential gender differences. This research was spurred by the dearth of comprehensive large-scale studies employing validated reading assessment tools across distinct time periods among children of the same age (Kesner Baruch et al., 2021). The examination of students from the same schools across both pre-pandemic and face-to-face learning periods allowed for a robust evaluation of the gaps in reading acquisition during the COVID-19 era among second-grade learners.

This study explored the influence of COVID-19 on reading and comprehension fluency in second-grade children. The assessment utilized measures of reading fluency for single words (TOWRE; Katzir et al., 2012, based on Torgeson et al., 1999) and comprehension fluency at the sentence level (semantics; Yinon and Shaul, 2017, based on Hutzler and Wimmer, 2004) in two distinct time frames among the second-grade cohort. The measurements occurred both before the onset of Coronavirus-related closures and after their resumption of face-to-face learning. Notably, the two groups of students were drawn from the same schools, exposed to the same educators and curriculum, with the sample adjusted for varying SES levels.

Surprisingly, the results demonstrated no significant disparities in reading fluency between second-grade students assessed before the pandemic in 2019 and those evaluated after the closures in 2020. A plausible explanation for the absence of discrepancies in fluency between these periods pertains to the characteristics of Hebrew orthography. The initial phases of reading acquisition in first grade encompass learning shallow pointed Hebrew, which facilitates the rapid assimilation of the correspondence between letters and sounds due to the provision of comprehensive phonological information (Share and Levin, 1999). As a result, most children become proficient decoders by the end of first grade (Lipka et al., 2016). Crucially, the two cohorts of second-grade students in this study had already acquired these foundational decoding skills during their first-grade year, preceding the pandemic’s advent. This suggests that while remote learning took place during their second-grade year, it did not notably impact the overall fluency and comprehension of these second graders as a whole.

When examining the SES effect, which focused on the differential effects of COVID-19 on reading among second-grade students of varied socioeconomic backgrounds, the study unearthed a significant SES impact on both word-reading fluency and comprehension at the sentence level. The findings highlighted that lower SES corresponded to lower reading and comprehension fluency. Moreover, a noteworthy interaction emerged specifically for reading fluency, rather than comprehension fluency, among students from diverse SES backgrounds. This interaction stemmed from a considerable decline in word-reading fluency and comprehension fluency within children from low SES during the pandemic, in contrast to their higher SES counterparts.

This decline is notable given the widely established SES-based disparities in reading fluency and comprehension (Burkam et al., 2004; Christodoulou et al., 2017). The pandemic exacerbated these gaps, revealing that children from low SES backgrounds faced substantial challenges during remote learning, potentially due to limited access to digital resources, reduced parental support, and heightened familial stress. The substantial decrease in reading fluency and comprehension abilities among low-SES children underscores the urgent need for targeted interventions to mitigate the amplified disparities brought about by the pandemic.

To conclude, the study contributes to our understanding of the ramifications of COVID-19-induced school closures on reading acquisition. The investigation suggests that the impact on reading skills might be mediated by prior decoding proficiency and underlines the significance of mitigating socioeconomic disparities. The findings underscore the urgency of tailored educational support to bridge the gaps that have emerged during the pandemic, particularly among students from low-SES backgrounds.

The observed widening gap in reading fluency and comprehension between children of low SES and those of medium-high SES during 2020 underscores a significant concern within the educational landscape (Burkam et al., 2004; Christodoulou et al., 2017). This finding highlights a pressing need for understanding the factors contributing to this phenomenon in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic. Several plausible explanations for this widening disparity emerge from the current study’s findings.

One conceivable explanation for the increased gap is rooted in the altered learning environment precipitated by school closures due to the pandemic. The significant reduction in the school day’s duration, coupled with the reliance on digital learning platforms for curriculum delivery, has had varying consequences for different student populations (Hall et al., 2020; Kuhfeld et al., 2020). Notably, the majority of second-grade children lack the autonomy required for effective engagement with digital tools, necessitating greater parental involvement. However, parents from low SES backgrounds, who might face financial concerns and time constraints, may have struggled to provide the necessary support for their children’s remote learning (Giovannella et al., 2020; Kruszewska et al., 2020; Letzel et al., 2020). This lack of adequate support could potentially contribute to the observed widening gap.

Furthermore, households with low SES often face challenges related to digital access and availability (UNESCO, 2020). Reports from teachers in low-SES schools corroborate this, revealing that many students lacked access to computers during remote learning (Dotan et al., 2021). This digital divide could have amplified the gap in reading fluency and comprehension skills, as students without access to digital tools were likely further marginalized during remote learning.

The confluence of these factors, coupled with the abrupt transition to remote learning, might have compounded the challenges faced by students from low SES backgrounds. This combined effect likely contributed to the significant decline in reading fluency and comprehension abilities among these students. This explanation finds reinforcement in a study by Domingue et al. (2021) that revealed the impact of SES on oral reading fluency growth during the COVID-19 period, where low SES students experienced a decline compared to the previous year.

Interestingly, during the pandemic, reading comprehension fluency improved among children of medium-high SES. This could be attributed to the comprehensive support these students received at home, allowing them to capitalize on one-on-one learning opportunities with parents or older siblings. This observation emphasizes the advantages of tailored support in affluent households.

In addition, while no significant gender differences were found in general, an unexpected effect of the pandemic was observed on boys. Previous literature has highlighted gendered experiences in education, with girls often encouraged more to read and boys receiving more opportunities for computing (Eccles et al., 1993). The pandemic-induced shift to remote learning could have impacted boys’ confidence and interest in computing-related learning, thereby affecting their academic performance. Conversely, the superior reading proficiency exhibited by girls on average (Logan and Johnston, 2010) and their affinity for reading could have helped them adapt better to self-regulated, computer-based learning.

The findings underscore the significance of addressing the “Matthew effect” (Stanovich, 1986) in the context of the pandemic-induced disparities. The trajectory of reading skill development may exacerbate differences over time, warranting strategic efforts to narrow these gaps. It is crucial to consider the varied impact of remote learning on different student populations and their unique challenges.

This study has several limitations, although there was a large diverse sample from different SES there were no boys in the high SES group and therefore gender differences were examined only in the medium and low SES groups. In addition, all the children were Hebrew speaking children thus the effect of school closure was not examined among bilingual children or children from different minorities, future studies should examine the long-term effect of the COVID and school closure among different types of population, and at various ages to examine the effect at different stages of reading. Furthermore, only one aspect of comprehension was examined which may limit our understanding of the effect of COVID and school closure, this topic should be further examined as well.

In conclusion, the study highlights the importance of targeted interventions to address the widening gaps exacerbated by the pandemic, particularly among students from low SES backgrounds, as well as gender differences. The repercussions of learning loss and increased stress and anxiety during the pandemic cannot be ignored. Educators and policymakers must channel resources and efforts toward supporting these vulnerable populations to ensure equitable academic outcomes. An exploration of the pandemic’s impact on diverse populations will be integral to comprehending its full educational implications.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Ethics Committee University of Haifa Faculty of Education Chief scientist ministry of Education Israel. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

SS: Writing – original draft, Methodology, Investigation, Formal analysis, Conceptualization. OL: Writing – review & editing, Methodology, Conceptualization. DT-C: Writing – original draft, Methodology. AB: Writing – review & editing, Data curation. SD: Writing – review & editing, Data curation.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This study was founded by the chief scientist of the ministry of education, Israel.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the Edmond J. Safra Foundation for their generous support and Tami Katzir for her helpful insights. In addition, great appreciation is conveyed to the students and teachers who participated in the present study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Allington, R., McGill-Franzen, A., Camilli, G., Williams, L., Graff, J., Zeig, J., et al. (2010). Addressing summer reading setback among economically disadvantaged elementary students. Read. Psychol. 31, 411–427. doi: 10.1080/02702711.2010.505165

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Andrew, A., Cattan, S., Costa-Dias, M., Farquharson, C., Kraftman, L., Krutikova, S., et al. (2020). Learning during the lockdown: real-time data on children’s experiences during home learning. Institute for. Fisc. Stud. 41, 653–683. doi: 10.1111/1475-5890.12240

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ardington, C., Wills, G., and Kotze, J. (2021). COVID-19 learning losses: early grade reading in South Africa. Int. J. Educ. Dev. 86:102480. doi: 10.1016/j.ijedudev.2021.102480

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bao, X., Qu, H., Zhang, R., and Hogan, T. P. (2020). Modeling reading ability gain in kindergarten children during COVID-19 school closures. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17, 63–71. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17176371

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar-Kochva, I. (2013). What are the underlying skills of silent reading acquisition? A developmental study from kindergarten to the 2nd grade. Read. Writ. 26, 1417–1436. doi: 10.1007/s11145-012-9414-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Betthäuser, B. A., Bach-Mortensen, A. M., and Engzell, P. (2023). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the evidence on learning during the COVID-19 pandemic. Nat. Hum. Behav. 7, 375–385. doi: 10.1038/s41562-022-01506-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blaskó, Z., Costa, P.Da, and Schnepf, S. V. (2022). Learning losses and educational inequalities in Europe: mapping the potential consequences of the COVID-19 crisis. J. European Soc. 34, 361–375. doi: 10.2139/ssrn.3833230

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Burkam, D. T., Ready, D. D., Lee, V. E., and LoGerfo, L. F. (2004). Social-class differences in summer learning between kindergarten and first grade: model specification and estimation. Sociol. Educ. 77, 1–31. doi: 10.1177/003804070407700101

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chall, J. S. (1983). Stages of reading development. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Google Scholar

Chatterji, M. (2006). Reading achievement gaps, correlates, and moderators of early reading achievement: evidence from the early childhood longitudinal study (ECLS) kindergarten to first grade sample. J. Educ. Psychol. 98, 489–507. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.98.3.489

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Christodoulou, J. A., Cyr, A., Murtagh, J., Chang, P., Lin, J., Guarino, A. J., et al. (2017). Impact of intensive summer reading intervention for children with reading disabilities and difficulties in early elementary school. J. Learn. Disabil. 50, 115–127. doi: 10.1177/0022219415617163

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Coles, M., and Hall, C. (2002). Gendered readings: Learning from children’s reading choices. J. Res. Read. 25, 96–108.

Google Scholar

Domingue, B., Dell, M., Lang, D. N., Silverman, R. D., Yeatman, J., and Hough, H. (2021). The effect of COVID on oral reading fluency during the 2020–2021 academic year. AERA Open. 8, 1–12. doi: 10.35542/osf.io/6zqjr

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dotan, S., Katzir, T., Lipka, O., and Shaul, S. (2021). Elementary school literacy teachers’ perceptions and challenges during COVID-19. Academia Letters. 1–7. doi: 10.20935/al898

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Downey, D. B., von Hippel, P. T., and Broh, B. A. (2004). Are schools the great equalizer? Cognitive inequality during the summer months and the school year. Am. Sociol. Rev. 69, 613–635. doi: 10.1177/000312240406900501

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Drane, C. F., Vernon, L., and O’Shea, S. (2020). Vulnerable learners in the age of COVID-19: a scoping review. Aust. Educ. Res. 48, 585–604. doi: 10.1007/s13384-020-00409-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eccles, J., Wigfield, A., Harold, R. D., and Blumenfeld, P. (1993). Age and gender differences in children's self-and task perceptions during elementary school. Child Dev. 64, 830–847. doi: 10.2307/1131221

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Engzell, P., Frey, A., and Verhagen, M. D. (2021). Learning loss due to school closures during the COVID-19 pandemic. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 118. doi: 10.1080/17586801.2013.863086

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eviatar, Z., and Share, D. (2013). Processing Semitic writing systems: introduction to a special issue of writing systems research. Writing Syst. Res. 5, 131–133. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2022376118

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Hosp, M. K., and Jenkins, J. R. (2001). Oral reading fluency as an indicator of reading competence: a theoretical, empirical, and historical analysis. Sci. Stud. Read. 5, 239–256. doi: 10.1207/S1532799XSSR0503_3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

García-Muiña, F., Medina-Salgado, M. S., González-Sánchez, R., Huertas-Valdivia, I., Ferrari, A. M., Settembre-Blundo, D., et al. (2021). Industry 4.0-based dynamic Social Organizational Life Cycle Assessment to target the social circular economy in manufacturing. J. Clean. Product. 327:129439. doi: 10.1016/j.jclepro.2021.129439

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Giovannella, C., Marcello, P., and Donatella, P. (2020). The effects of the Covid-19 pandemic on Italian learning ecosystems: the school teachers’ perspective at the steady state. ID&A Interaction Design & Architecture(s) 45, 264–286. doi: 10.55612/s-5002-045-012

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Good, R. H., Simmons, D. C., and Kame'enui, E. J. (2001). The importance and decision-making utility of a continuum of fluency-based indicators of foundational reading skills for third-grade high stakes outcomes. Sci. Stud. Read. 5, 257–288. doi: 10.1207/S1532799XSSR0503_4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, T., Connolly, C., Grádaigh, S. Ó., Burden, K., Kearney, M., Schuck, S., et al. (2020). Education in precarious times: a comparative study across six countries to identify design priorities for mobile learning in a pandemic. Inf. Learn. Sci. 121, 433–442. doi: 10.1108/ILS-04-2020-0089

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hevia, F. J., Vergara-Lope, S., Velásquez-Durán, A., and Calderón, D. (2022). Estimation of the fundamental learning loss and learning poverty related to COVID-19 pandemic in Mexico. Int. J. Educ. Dev. 88:102515. doi: 10.1016/j.ijedudev.2021.102515

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutzler, F., and Wimmer, H. (2004). Eye movements of dyslexic children when reading in a regular orthography. Brain and language. 89, 235–242.

Google Scholar

Immerfall, S. (2020). Schule in der Pandemie: Erfahrungen aus Ostwürttemberg [Schools during the pandemic: Experiences from Eastern Württemberg]. Aus Politik und Zeitgeschichte 70, 22–27.

Google Scholar

Jones, N., Sanchez Tapia, I., Baird, S., Guglielmi, S., Oakley, E., Yadete, W., et al. (2021). Intersecting barriers to adolescents’ educational access during COVID-19: exploring the role of gender, disability and poverty. Int. J. Educ. Dev. 85:102428. doi: 10.1016/j.ijedudev.2021.102428

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaffenberger, M. (2021). Modelling the long-run learning impact of the Covid-19 learning shock: actions to (more than) mitigate loss. Int. J. Educ. Dev. 81:102326. doi: 10.1016/j.ijedudev.2020.102326

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Katzir, T., Kim, Y. S. G., and Dotan, S. (2018). Reading self-concept and reading anxiety in second grade children: the roles of word reading, emergent literacy skills, working memory and gender. Front. Psychol. 9:1180. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01180

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Katzir, T., Kim, Y., Wolf, M., O’Brien, B., Kennedy, B., Lovett, M., et al. (2006). Reading fluency: the whole is more than the parts. Ann. Dyslexia 56, 51–82. doi: 10.1007/s11881-006-0003-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Katzir, T., Schiff, R., and Kim, Y. S. (2012). The effects of orthographic consistency on reading development: A within and between cross-linguistic study of fluency and accuracy among fourth grade English-and Hebrew-speaking children. Learning and Individual Differences. 22, 673–679.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kesner Baruch, Y., Ziv, Y., and Sela, O. (2021). Gaps and inequality processes in early childhood following COVID 19 period. The Israeli Ministry of Education. Available at: https://meyda.education.gov.il/files/LishcatMadaan/ChildhoodGap.pdf

Google Scholar

Kim, Y. S., Petscher, Y., Schatschneider, C., and Foorman, B. (2010). Does growth rate in oral reading fluency matter in predicting reading comprehension achievement? J. Educ. Psychol. 102, 652–667. doi: 10.1037/a0019643

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, J. S., and White, T. G. (2008). Scaffolding voluntary summer reading for children in grades 3–5: an experimental study. Sci. Stud. Read. 12, 1–23. doi: 10.1080/10888430701746849

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klauda, S. L., and Guthrie, J. T. (2008). Relationships of three components of reading fluency to reading comprehension. J. Educ. Psychol. 100, 310–321. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.100.2.310

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

König, C., and Frey, A. (2022). The impact of COVID-19-related school closures on student achievement—a Meta-analysis. Educ. Meas. Issues Pract. 41, 16–22. doi: 10.1111/emip.12495

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kruszewska, A., Nazaruk, S., and Szewczyk, K. (2020). Polish teachers of early education in the face of distance learning during the COVID-19 pandemic–the difficulties experienced and suggestions for the future. Education. 50, 304–315. doi: 10.1080/03004279.2020.1849346

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhfeld, M., Soland, J., Tarasawa, B., Johnson, A., Ruzek, E., and Liu, J. (2020). Projecting the potential impact of COVID-19 school closures on academic achievement. Educ. Res. 49, 549–565. doi: 10.3102/0013189X20965918

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhn, M. R., Schwanenflugel, P. J., and Meisinger, E. B. (2010). Aligning theory and assessment of reading fluency: automaticity, prosody, and definitions of fluency. Read. Res. Q. 45, 230–251. doi: 10.1598/RRQ.45.2.4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lake, R., and Dusseault, B. (2020). Remote classes are in session for more school districts, but attendance plans are still absent. Center for Reinventing Public Education. Available at: https://www.crpe.org/thelens/remote-classes-aresession-more-school-districts-attendance-plansare-still-absent

Google Scholar

Lambert, M., and Sassone, J. (2020). Accelerate, don't remediate: an instructional framework for meeting the needs of the most vulnerable students after COVID school closures. J. Leadership and Instruction 19, 8–13.

Google Scholar

Letzel, V., Pozas, M., and Schneider, C. (2020). Energetic students, stressed parents, and nervous teachers: a comprehensive exploration of inclusive homeschooling during the COVID-19 crisis. Open Educ. Stud. 2, 159–170. doi: 10.1515/edu-2020-0122

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lipka, O., Katzir, T., and Shaul, S. (2016). “The basis of Reading fluency in first grade of Hebrew speaking children” in Reading fluency: Current insights from neurocognitive research and intervention studies. eds. A. Khateb and I. Bar-Kochva (Springer, Cham), 12, 91–104. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-30478-6_6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Logan, G. D. (1997). Automaticity and reading: perspectives from the instance theory of automatization. Read. Writing Q. Overcoming Learn. Difficulties 13, 123–146. doi: 10.1080/1057356970130203

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Logan, S., and Johnston, R. (2010). Investigating gender differences in reading. Educ. Rev. 62, 175–187. doi: 10.1080/00131911003637006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Masters, G.N., Taylor-Guy, P., Fraillon, J., and Chase, A.M. (2020). Ministerial briefing paper on evidence of the likely impact on educational outcomes of vulnerable children learning at home during COVID-19. Australian Council for Educational Research. Available at: https://research.acer.edu.au/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1025&context=learning_processs

Google Scholar

Miller, J., and Schwanenflugel, P. J. (2008). A longitudinal study of the development of reading prosody as a dimension of oral reading fluency in early elementary school children. Read. Res. Q. 43, 336–354. doi: 10.1598/RRQ.43.4.2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O., Kennedy, A. M., and Foy, P. (2007). PIRLS 2006 international report: IEA’s progress in international reading literacy study in primary schools in 40 countries. Chestnut Hill, MA: Boston College.

Google Scholar

National Reading Panel (US) (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel: Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction: Reports of the subgroups. US: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, National Institutes of Health.

Google Scholar

Nevo, E., Vaknin-Nusbaum, V., Brande, S., and Gambrell, L. (2020). Oral reading fluency, reading motivation and reading comprehension among second graders. Read. Writ. 33, 1945–1970. doi: 10.1007/s11145-020-10025-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

OECD (2023). Equity in education in PISA 2022, in PISA 2022 results (volume I): The state of learning and equity in education. Paris: OECD Publishing.

Google Scholar

Panagouli, E., Stavridou, A., Savvidi, C., Kourti, A., Psaltopoulou, T., Sergentanis, T. N., et al. (2021). School performance among children and adolescents during COVID-19 pandemic: a systematic review. Child. Aust. 8:1134. doi: 10.3390/children8121134

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Perfetti, C. (2007). Reading ability: lexical quality to comprehension. Sci. Stud. Read. 11, 357–383. doi: 10.1080/10888430701530730

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Price, K. W., Meisinger, E. B., Louwerse, M. M., and D’Mello, S. (2016). The contributions of oral and silent reading fluency to reading comprehension. Read. Psychol. 37, 167–201. doi: 10.1080/02702711.2015.1025118

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reardon, S. F., Fahle, E. M., Kalogrides, D., Podolsky, A., and Zárate, R. C. (2019). Gender achievement gaps in US school districts. Am. Educ. Res. J. 56, 2474–2508. doi: 10.3102/0002831219843824

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., and Andrews, G. (2019). Gender differences in reading and writing achievement: evidence from the National Assessment of educational Progress (NAEP). Am. Psychol. 74, 445–458. doi: 10.1037/amp0000356

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reimer, D., Smith, E., Andersen, I. G., and Sortkaer, B. (2021). What happens when schools shut down? Investigating inequality in students’ reading behavior during Covid-19 in Denmark. Res. Social Stratification and Mobility 71, 100568–100565. doi: 10.1016/j.rssm.2020.100568

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Relyea, J. E., Rich, P., Kim, J. S., and Gilbert, J. B. (2023). The COVID-19 impact on reading achievement growth of grade 3–5 students in a US urban school district: variation across student characteristics and instructional modalities. Read. Writ. 36, 317–346. doi: 10.1007/s11145-022-10387-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, J. P., and Lubienski, S. T. (2011). The development of gender achievement gaps in mathematics and reading during elementary and middle school: examining direct cognitive assessments and teacher ratings. Am. Educ. Res. J. 48, 268–302. doi: 10.3102/0002831210372249

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schult, J., Mahler, N., Fauth, B., and Lindner, M. A. (2022). Did students learn less during the COVID-19 pandemic? Reading and mathematics competencies before and after the first pandemic wave. Sch. Eff. Sch. Improv. 33, 544–563. doi: 10.1080/09243453.2022.2061014

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shany, M., Bar-On, A., and Katzir, T. (2012). Reading different orthographic structures in the shallow-pointed Hebrew script: a cross-grade study in elementary school. Read. Writ. 25, 1217–1238. doi: 10.1007/s11145-011-9314-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Share, D. L., and Levin, I. (1999). “Learning to read and write in Hebrew” in Learning to read and write. eds. M. Harris and G. Hatano (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 89–111.

Google Scholar

Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Cognitive processes and the reading problems of learning disabled children: Evaluating the assumption of specificity. Psychological and educational perspectives on learning disabilities. eds. J. Torgesen and B. Wong (New York: Academic Press), 87–131.

Google Scholar

Stanovich, K. E., and West, R. F. (1989). Exposure to print and orthographic processing. Read. Res. Q. 24, 402–433. doi: 10.2307/747605

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevens, E. A., Walker, M. A., and Vaughn, S. (2017). The effects of reading fluency interventions on the reading fluency and reading comprehension performance of elementary students with learning disabilities: a synthesis of the research from 2001 to 2014. J. Learn. Disabil. 50, 576–590. doi: 10.1177/0022219416638028

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sucena, A., Silva, A. F., and Marques, C. (2022). Reading skills intervention during the Covid-19 pandemic. Human. Soc. Sci. Commun. 9, 1–6. doi: 10.1057/s41599-022-01059-x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

The Israeli Ministry of Education (2014). Teaching program for the assimilation of Hebrew goals (language education) in grades, Jerusalem, Israel: The Israeli Ministry of Education. 1–2.

Google Scholar

The National Authority for Measurement and Evaluation in Education (2023). PISA 2022: Literacy in math, science, and reading among 15-year-old students in Israel. The Israeli Ministry of Education. Available at: https://meyda.education.gov.il/files/Rama/PISA-2022-report.pdf

Google Scholar

Tomasik, M. J., Helbling, L. A., and Moser, U. (2021). Educational gains of in-person vs. distance learning in primary and secondary schools: a natural experiment during the COVID-19 pandemic school closures in Switzerland. Int. J. Psychol. 56, 566–576. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12728

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Torgeson, J. K., Wagner, R. K., and Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Test of word reading efficiency. Austin, TX: Pro-ed.

Google Scholar

UNESCO (2020). COVID-19 educational disruption and response. Available at: https://en.unesco.org/covid19/educationresponse

Google Scholar

United Nations (2020). Policy brief: the impact of COVID-19 on children. Available at: https://unsdg.un.org/sites/default/files/2020- 04/160420_Covid_Children_Policy_Brief.pdf

Google Scholar

Valencia, S. W., Smith, A. T., Reece, A. M., Li, M., Wixson, K. K., and Newman, H. (2010). Oral reading fluency assessment: issues of construct, criterion, and consequential validity. Read. Res. Q. 45, 270–291. doi: 10.1598/rrq.45.3.1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolf, M., and Katzir-Cohen, T. (2001). Reading fluency and its intervention. Sci. Stud. Read. 5, 211–239. doi: 10.1207/S1532799XSSR0503_2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yinon, R., and Shaul, S. (2017). Sentence comprehension fluency test. Unpublished test. Israel: University of Haifa.

Google Scholar

Keywords: COVID-19 pandemic, reading acquisition, reading fluency, comprehension, socioeconomic status, gender differences

Citation: Shaul S, Lipka O, Tal-Cohen D, Bufman A and Dotan S (2024) The impact of school closures during the COVID-19 pandemic on reading fluency among second grade students: socioeconomic and gender perspectives. Front. Psychol. 15:1289145. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1289145

Received: 05 September 2023; Accepted: 20 June 2024;
Published: 05 July 2024.

Edited by:

Douglas F. Kauffman, Consultant, Greater Boston area, United States

Reviewed by:

Ann Dowker, University of Oxford, United Kingdom
Kai S. Cortina, University of Michigan, United States

Copyright © 2024 Shaul, Lipka, Tal-Cohen, Bufman and Dotan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Shelley Shaul, U2hlbGxleXNAZWR1LmhhaWZhLmFjLmls

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.