Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 13 March 2024
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Cognitive Mechanisms Underlying Discrimination View all 5 articles

Social media and anti-immigrant prejudice: a multi-method analysis of the role of social media use, threat perceptions, and cognitive ability

Saifuddin Ahmed
Saifuddin Ahmed1*Kokil Jaidka
Kokil Jaidka2*Vivian Hsueh Hua ChenVivian Hsueh Hua Chen3Mengxuan CaiMengxuan Cai1Anfan ChenAnfan Chen4Claire Stravato EmesClaire Stravato Emes1Valerie YuValerie Yu1Arul ChibArul Chib5
  • 1Wee Kim Wee School of Communication and Information, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, Singapore
  • 2Department of Communications and New Media, National University of Singapore, Singapore, Singapore
  • 3Department of Media and Communication, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, Netherlands
  • 4Department of Communication Studies, Hong Kong Baptist University, Kowloon, Hong Kong SAR, China
  • 5International Institute of Social Studies, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, Netherlands

Introduction: The discourse on immigration and immigrants is central to contemporary political and public discussions. Analyzing online conversations about immigrants provides valuable insights into public opinion, complemented by data from questionnaires on how attitudes are formed.

Methods: The research includes two studies examining the expressive and informational use of social media. Study 1 conducted a computational text analysis of comments on Singaporean Facebook pages and forums, focusing on how social media is used to discuss immigrants. Study 2 utilized survey data to examine the use of social media at the individual level, testing the relationships between cognitive ability, perceptions of threat, negative emotions towards immigrants, and social media usage within the Integrated Threat Theory framework.

Results: Study 1 found that discussions about immigrants on social media often involved negative emotions and concerns about economic impact, such as competition for jobs and crime. Complementing these findings about perceived economic threats, Study 2 showed that individuals with higher social media usage and greater perceptions of threat were more likely to have negative emotions towards immigrants. These relationships were mediated by perceptions of threat and were stronger in individuals with lower cognitive abilities.

Discussion: The findings from both studies demonstrate the role of social media in shaping public attitudes towards immigrants, highlighting how perceived threats influence these attitudes. This research suggests the importance of considering how digital platforms contribute to public opinion on immigration, with implications for understanding the dynamics of attitude formation in the digital age.

1 Introduction

In recent years, the scale of international migration has increased due to major displacement events worldwide. Immigration is arduous for immigrants, but it is also regarded as a threat by indigenous societies (Pew Research Centre, 2019; Jedinger and Eisentraut, 2020; Lee et al., 2020) whose fear adverse personal consequences because of incoming immigrants (D’Errico and Paciello, 2018). Immigration policies are often sources of contention in countries with large migrant populations, such as the United States, Canada, Germany, the United Kingdom, Australia, and Singapore (United Nations, 2020, pp. 75). In this context, the Integrated Threat Theory (ITT) (Stephan and Stephan, 1996; Stephan and Stephan, 2000) offers a way to understand how the threat perceptions of the citizens in these countries may predict their prejudice towards immigrants (Stephan et al., 1999b). For instance, locals may perceive “symbolic” threats to their culture because of the different immigrant life habits and ways (see Stephan et al., 1999b). They may also perceive a “realistic” threat to economic stability because of the possible drop in job opportunities (Stephan et al., 1999b). Symbolic and realistic threats can manifest in social media, influencing individuals’ attitudes toward immigrants (Croucher et al., 2020; Mahmood et al., 2021). However, eliciting public opinion on immigrants and immigration is fraught with challenges. For instance, a social desirability bias and interviewer effects could lead to participants sharing mostly positive views toward immigrants. Even if this were not the case, while national surveys can offer more insights into public attitudes, the insights are limited to the items in the survey questionnaire, often without going into the reasons underlying those attitudes or the context in which the answer may have been given.

Mixed-method approaches to studying public opinion combine survey-based approaches with observational studies of social media posts (Maragoudakis et al., 2011; Chen and Tomblin, 2021). In doing so, they can triangulate the findings from analyses of both, how social media users use social media to inform themselves, or how they actively post on social media to express themselves. Such research designs can alleviate some of the concerns about studies reporting only survey methods, such as social desirability biases, and those reporting only social media-based analyses, such as selective participation or non-representativeness. These studies also allow researchers to consider both the expressive and the informational paradigms of social media use as factors associated with public opinion and social attitude formation. The expressive use of social media focuses on users’ self-expression behaviors, such as communicating their identity, opinions, and emotions in online conversations, replies, and reactions to posts (Macafee and De Simone, 2012). On the other hand, the information use of social media involves individuals’ consumption behaviors, such as following news outlets and subscribing to educational channels to seek information. Understanding the informational use of social media is crucial, because on the one hand, in a low-press freedom environment, the chilling effects of governmental surveillance may imply that Singaporeans are reluctant to share their opinion on political hotbed issues, such as the immigrant workforce. On the other hand, the proliferation of antisocial elements online under the guidance of anonymity may imply that the hate speech encountered online is out of proportion with the actual distribution of attitudes amongst the population. Therefore, informational use would be more typical of an average social media user, who consumes ‘filtered’ content, often endorsed by and sourced through their trusted peers.

We situate the current study within Singapore, a society where the expressive use of social media can offer critical insight into the integration of immigrants given the high-censorship, low-press freedom environment. Freedom scores, conducted by the Freedom House, evaluate the status of political rights and civil liberties in countries worldwide. Singapore ranked 47th out of 100 countries in the 2023 freedom scores (Freedom House, 2023). In Singapore, immigrants constitute 37.8% of the population (Statista, 2019). Previous work has reported that foreign workers in Singapore face the brunt of resentment related to their perceived dominance in the job market (McNulty, 2016) and their effect on overcrowded public resources (Dirksmeier, 2020). Some scholars have suggested that Singaporeans’ everyday lives are heightened with anti-immigrant emotions, xenophobia, and racial tension (Gomes, 2014; Velayutham, 2017; Yang, 2018), and they often turn to social media to express their emotional grievances toward immigrant groups (Velayutham, 2017). In 2019, the government acknowledged the danger of inflammatory social media discourse to racial harmony (Ministry of Home Affairs, 2019). Despite the rising concern, there is a lack of quantitative, empirical evidence to understand attitudes toward immigrants and assess the merit of the above arguments. In the following paragraphs, we motivate our key research questions and hypotheses guiding the study of the collected data before discussing the two research designs employed to analyze attitudes on social media.

2 Background

Large-scale social media data collection offers an alternative to understanding public opinion via the expressive use of social media, which provides a platform for individuals’ self-expression. When an individual expresses themself on social media, the act can clarify and reinforce their personal opinion (Cho et al., 2018). This is especially critical when studying interpersonal attitudes such as stereotypes and prejudice, as online anonymity would allow individuals to feel less inhibited and more comfortable sharing their innermost thoughts and desires (Theocharis et al., 2016; Oz et al., 2018). Social media platforms are also unmoderated or loosely moderated, implying that “uninhibited” posts invoking racist stereotypes and hate speech can proliferate on them (Weimann, 2010; D’Errico and Paciello, 2018). Moreover, social media platforms also allow hate speech to be transmitted to a nationwide audience (Hawdon et al., 2017). As a result of this hate speech on social media, the polarization of public opinion would increase, and individuals’ perceived social cohesion would also be negatively affected (Schäfer et al., 2021). In addition, previous research has found that exposure to hate materials online is associated with the occurrence of violence (Kiilakoski and Oksanen, 2011). Although social media discourse constitutes a non-representative sample, they are insightful in identifying majoritarian and emerging views put forth by “the politically active internet users (who) act like opinion-makers” and “who can influence (or to ‘anticipate’) the preference of a wider audience” (Ceron et al., 2014, pp. 345). In contrast, most traditional surveys consider that each individual is equivalent to a single data point, absent their sociocultural influence.

Conceptually, previous research in mining social media data for public opinion has exemplified two primary goals for mining social media users’ emotions and concerns. The first objective is to characterize the affective and cognitive factors underpinning political attitudes. For instance, Grover et al. (2019) found that pro-immigration tweets in the US were closely related to moral foundations of harm, fairness, and loyalty language elements, while anti-immigration tweets were associated with moral foundations of authority and negative emotions. Bartlett and Norrie (2015) surveyed tweets regarding the 2013–2014 Immigration Bill in the United Kingdom and found that half were associated with negative emotions. Furthermore, in a content analysis of Facebook posts by political parties across six European countries, Heidenreich et al. (2020) reported greater negative emotion in discussions about immigrants by political parties on the extreme left and right. In the absence of relevant research on users’ attitudes towards immigrants in Singapore, we therefore pose our first research question:

RQ1: What is the primary emotion in discussions about immigrants on social media?

The second goal of the computational text analysis of social media posts involves identifying the major priorities and concerns in civic discussions. Topic, opinion, and emotion detection from social media content allow scholars and political analysts to identify emerging social trends and public responses to specific events or policies (Sobkowicz et al., 2012). Looking at the emerging discussion topics over time can inspire policies and guide interventions. Previous studies have applied topic modeling to identify individuals’ issues online (Maragoudakis et al., 2011). Similar approaches offer insights about public concerns in a presidential election (Karami et al., 2018) and the Affordable Care Act (Hopkins and Parish, 2019) – but once again, there is a lack of research focus on the discussions around immigrants, thereby motivating our second research question:

RQ2: What are the topical themes in discussing immigrants on social media?

Motivated by prior multi-method analyses of social media users, the second part of our research project delves into the informational use of social media, different from the expressive use as it would encompass social media users who may not post, yet are avid consumers of social media content. Informational use of social media, different from expressive use, focuses more on seeking factual information, news, and educational content. Compared to expressive use, informational use emphasizes acquiring and sharing knowledge rather than personal feelings (Macafee and De Simone, 2012).

The emotional tone of social media content is pivotal in shaping attitudes. Repeated exposure to negative or dehumanizing content can influence users’ emotions (Utych, 2018) and perceptions (Hsueh et al., 2015; Del Vicario et al., 2016). For instance, a study analyzing Italian Facebook groups found a correlation between user activity and negative emotional states (Del Vicario et al., 2016). The study by Ahmed et al. (2021a) of a student sample in the Singaporean context reported that social media use was negatively related to anti-immigrant attitudes. Emerging evidence suggests that social media use relates to anti-immigrant attitudes. For instance, a recent study suggests a strong time-series correlation between social media consumption and offline hate crimes, as Donald Trump’s tweets on Islam-related topics were associated with a spurt in hate crimes during his presidency (Müller and Schwarz, 2023), while other research has also reported that online hate has offline spillovers in the form of hate crimes (Chan et al., 2016). These studies reinforce the role social media use, especially exposure to online prejudice, may play in subsequent user attitudes and behavior. However, it is essential to note that social media is more than just a platform for negativity. Research has highlighted its potential positive impact, such as fostering intergroup contact in post-conflict societies like Serbia, Croatia, Cyprus (Žeželj et al., 2017), and Israel (Walther et al., 2015). In the Singaporean context, our goal is to provide a comprehensive understanding of whether increased social media activity correlates with heightened anti-immigrant attitudes.

Next, utilizing the integrative threat theory (ITT) (Stephan and Stephan, 1996; Stephan and Stephan, 2000) as a foundation, we aimed to understand social media’s role in shaping anti-immigrant evaluations. ITT is a theoretical framework that explains the elements of individuals’ perceived threat contributing to social group prejudice. Two of the most critical threats in ITT are symbolic and realistic threats. Symbolic threats involve perceived challenges to a group’s cultural, social, or ideological identity, which are symbolic in nature and represent a potential change or shift that is perceived as endangering the values, norms, or worldview of a particular social group. Realistic threats involve more tangible risks that may pose immediate harm to a group or individual. These threats are grounded in observable and empirical phenomena, such as economic competition, territorial disputes, or physical harm (Stephan and Stephan, 2000). Therefore, we collected survey data from an online panel of Singaporeans, focusing on exposure to immigrant-related information and its subsequent relationship with prejudiced emotions toward immigrant groups. In our analysis, we explored the direct relationship between the use of social media and prejudiced emotions toward immigrant groups in Singapore. Building on ITT, we investigated whether symbolic and realistic threat perceptions mediate this relationship. Finally, we examined the role of cognitive ability in explaining the variance in the findings based on individual differences.

We offer an extension to the typical understanding of social media use and attitudes against outgroups (Hameleers and Schmuck, 2017; Davidson and Farquhar, 2020) to consider its association with affective prejudice – the broader set of prejudicial emotions evoked by immigrants (e.g., contempt, disgusted, angry, envious, and jealousy). Although most research discusses the relationship between threat perceptions and prejudice, several studies extend the discussion to focus on negative emotions against the outgroups. For example, some studies indicate that the two threat perceptions predict negative racial attitudes toward immigrant groups (Stephan et al., 2002, 2005). However, when analyzing the relationship between social media use and attitudes against outgroups, most scholars focus on prejudice (Hameleers and Schmuck, 2017; Davidson and Farquhar, 2020), failing to extend the discussion to broader emotions or attitudes such as affective prejudice (e.g., contempt, disgusted, angry, envious, and jealousy).

Recognizing the potential for varied perceptions of foreigners based on ethnicity, we grounded our selection of immigrant groups in both statistical data on the primary immigrant demographics in Singapore (Pan and Theseira, 2023) and prior research findings on outgroup prejudice within the nation (Chen et al., 2021; Ahmed et al., 2021a,b). For instance, Chen et al. (2021) found that Singaporean students exhibited differential warmth towards immigrant groups, with co-ethnic (Chinese) groups perceived differently from other ethnicities, while Americans were often perceived with greater warmth. Accordingly, we examined Singaporeans’ attitudes towards immigrants from both the major minority groups, such as Malays and Indians, as well as the Western or Caucasian immigrants as a broader category. Although the immigrant population in Singapore is diverse, with notable representations from countries like the United Kingdom, France, and Australia, we followed the convention Chen et al. (2021) adopted to term all Caucasian expats as “Americans.” Accordingly, we propose the following hypothesis:

H1: Social media use will be positively associated with negative emotions toward (a) American expatriates, (b) Malays, and (c) Indians.

In exploring the relationship between social media use and prejudice, it is also important to consider how threat perception can explain how social media exposure relates to negative emotions towards outgroups. The role of threat perception as a predictor of prejudice has been widely discussed (Stephan and Stephan, 1996; Stephan and Stephan, 2000; Esses et al., 2001). The Integrated Threat Theory (ITT) (Stephan and Stephan, 1996; Stephan and Stephan, 2000) model proposes four core components associated with outgroup prejudice. These are realistic threats, symbolic threats, negative stereotypes, and intergroup anxiety (Stephan and Stephan, 2000). Based on ITT’s original model, realistic and symbolic threats are most commonly discussed as predictors of prejudice (Stephan et al., 1999b). The realistic threats consist of threats to the ingroup’s existence, political and economic power, and physical well-being. Previous studies show that perceived realistic threat positively relates to prejudice towards outgroups (Quillian, 1995; Stephan et al., 1999b). Similarly, symbolic threat involves perceived group differences in values, norms, morals, or identity of the corresponding ingroup (Stephan et al., 2002). Prior studies also confirm the positive relationship between symbolic threat perception and prejudice (Sears, 1988; Stephan et al., 1999a).

Exposure to anti-immigrant content or discussions on social media can activate realistic and symbolic threat perceptions that are readily accessible in evaluating immigrant outgroups (as found in Study 1) (Mahmood et al., 2021). Previous research on Asian Americans in the United States also found that the stronger the social media users’ believe in the fairness and accuracy of the social media platforms that they use daily, the higher the likelihood that they will perceive Chinese pose a realistic and symbolic threat to the American (Croucher et al., 2020). Therefore, it is reasonable to expect that frequent use of social media will increase the salience of threat perceptions, which could help explain negative emotions toward immigrant outgroups. Furthermore, we expect a greater variance in effect sizes on immigrant attitudes, depending on the minority group under consideration, based on the prior findings from Chen et al. (2021). Hence, we propose the following hypotheses:

H2: Symbolic threat perception positively mediates the relationship between social media use and negative emotions toward (a) American expatriates, (b) Malays, and (c) Indians.

H3: Realistic threat perception positively mediates the relationship between social media use and negative emotions toward (a) American expatriates, (b) Malays, and (c) Indians.

Finally, we are also interested in individual factors such as cognitive ability – the higher-order mental processes that include reasoning, information processing, and problem-solving (Hodson and Dhont, 2015; Onraet et al., 2015) – in how individuals form attitudes. Cognitive ability reflects the mechanisms of how individuals learn, remember, solve problems, and pay attention. It reflects an interplay between different cognitive functions, offering a theoretical basis for anticipating how different cognitive abilities may moderate or influence other psychological processes or outcomes, such as beliefs and attitudes. Cognitive ability is essential to understand why some individuals may be more susceptible than others to online content (Bode, 2012): they may be more prone to believe what they read without critically evaluating it if they have low cognitive abilities (Pennycook and Rand, 2020).

In the context of this study, it is necessary to examine the relationship between cognitive ability and threat perception. Some prior work has accounted for cognitive ability in analyzing the factors predicting prejudice (Dhont and Hodson, 2014; Hodson and Dhont, 2015). Similarly, cognitive abilities are expected to influence individuals’ threat perceptions. For instance, individuals with lower cognitive ability tend to trust others less, are less sensitive to social cues, and are less accurate in perceiving others’ behaviors and intentions (Murphy and Hall, 2011). Higher levels of cognitive ability increase pro-social behavior, such as reducing the anchoring effect, improving altruism and levels of open-mindedness, and fostering more tolerant attitudes toward outgroups (Millet and Dewitte, 2007; Bergman et al., 2010). In contrast, lower levels of cognitive ability increase negative evaluation of outgroups by increasing negative emotional responses (i.e., anxiety, anger, disgust, fear) and negative attitudes (Hodson and Dhont, 2015; Utych, 2018; Croucher et al., 2020).

Prior research also suggests that individuals with lower cognitive ability are more likely to feel lower personal control over their circumstances, and therefore, they may be more likely to attribute agency to external factors (Lusk, 1983; Feather and Volkmer, 1988), such as the perceived threat from outgroups. Corroborating these expectations, experiments among university students have revealed that lower cognitive ability test scores predict higher perceived stereotype threat levels (Sawyer and Hollis-Sawyer, 2005). Other prior research has similarly reported that individuals with high information processing ability are less vulnerable to high-threat, negatively framed information and are better at rationally evaluating an object (Petty and Cacioppo, 1986; Igartua et al., 2012). However, to our knowledge, there is no prior work on how cognitive ability indirectly affects how threat perception mediates the relationship between social media use and anti-immigrant attitudes. Based on prior work, we expect that individuals with lower cognitive abilities who frequently use social media are more likely to perceive immigrants as threats (both symbolic and realistic), leading to stronger negative emotions toward the immigrant groups. On the other hand, the effect of threat perceptions may be weaker or non-existent for those with higher cognitive ability, where even if they use social media frequently, they are less likely to perceive immigrants as threats, resulting in weaker or no significant negative emotions toward the immigrant groups. Accordingly, we can test a moderated mediation relationship, positing that:

Cognitive ability moderates the mediated relationship between social media use and negative emotion toward (a) American expatriates, (b) Malays, and (c) Indians with the symbolic (H4) and realistic threat (H5), such that the mediated relationship is more robust for those with low cognitive ability than those with high cognitive ability.

The conceptual model in Figure 1 illustrates the various hypotheses.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Conceptual framework.

To sum up, this study offers the opportunity to understand social media’s role in public opinion expression in Singapore, a non-liberal context with a complex social and ethnic hierarchy. We also build on prior work by considering and supplementing evidence of social media expression with social media use to better understand these relationships.

3 Study 1: a computational text analysis of Singaporean social media posts

The first aim of Study 1 is to identify the emotional tone with which immigrants are discussed in the public discourse on social media. We aim to do so by examining social media posts in Singaporean communities, where users express themselves and share personal thoughts (Macafee and De Simone, 2012). Analyzing social media posts offers an effective means for us to comprehend emotions and opinions on social media when discussing migrants. Second, we aim to investigate the contexts and connotations of immigrant mentions in popular public discourse in Singapore. Therefore, the data for Study 1 comprised social media discussions posted to internet communities and Facebook pages that are the most frequented by Singaporeans, and the approach involved the emotion analysis and topic modeling of the 86,462 filtered posts and comments about immigrants.

3.1 Data collection and pre-processing

In the first step, all the posts and comments for June 2018 to December 2018 were collected from internet communities that have the highest number of followers, as a representative proxy for the Singaporean online public sphere. Table 1 reports the number of followers for each community included in the data collection and the number of posts yielded from each source. Many Facebook community pages based out of Singapore were chosen, as over 70% of Singaporeans have a Facebook account (Statista, 2023). As seen in Table 1, the selection of Facebook pages comprises widely visited and followed community sites on a broad ideological spectrum (Mainstream Straits Time to the alternative Straits Times Review). The pages included one national newspaper (Straits Times), one broadcaster (Channel News Asia), five community sites (Stomp, Mothership, All Stuff Singapore, SMRT, MustShareNews, Reddit/Singapore), and three alternative news sites (TOC, Independent Sg, STR). Hardwarezone is an internet portal similar to Reddit, popular amongst Singaporeans, with different subcommunities that discuss aspects of Singaporean culture, with 700,000 registered members and 2 million unique visitors every month in 2018.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The Facebook pages and Reddit community used to construct the Singapore Facebook dataset.

Once the communities were chosen, data was collected using researcher APIs and Python code. In the case of Facebook, the Netvizz app (Rieder, 2013) accessed the Facebook Graph API and collected various types of data, including posts and comments. Next, discussion threads from Reddit and other forums were collected using the Python package Praw (Praw, 2020), which extracts data like posts, comments, and metadata from subreddits and user accounts. The data collection yielded 702,607 posts and comments (henceforth, we refer to the joint collection of posts and comments as “posts”).

In the second step, we identified those posts and comments that discussed immigrants. We used the Python natural language toolkit to retain only the posts and comments in English, thereby reducing the dataset to 488,924 posts and comments (henceforth referred to as “posts”). We then separated the dataset into those posts that mentioned immigrants versus those that did not by using a dictionary of 52 immigrant-related words, nouns, colloquial terms, and stereotypes from Singapore from prior work (Ahmed et al., 2021a). The dictionary comprises the lemmatized forms of these words. It includes general terms such as ‘migrant,’ ‘expat,’ (expatriate), certain visa statuses such as ‘CECA,’ ‘MIC,’ and ethnicities such as ‘Chinese,’ ‘PRC,’ ‘Ang Mo,’ Pinoy,’ and ‘FDW.’ The list also includes stereotypes and pejoratives such as “smellies” and “maids,” commonly used in Singapore to refer to working-class immigrants. In this manner, the final dataset mentioning immigrants comprised 105,539 English posts and comments, of which 86,462 posts were at least 10 characters. The final numbers of posts and comments from each source are reported in the last column of Table 1. The average length of a post was 13 words, and the median length was 11 words.

It is possible that some were posted by immigrants relating their personal experiences instead and are, therefore, “false positives” as they are irrelevant to our research objectives of analyzing attitudes towards immigrants. To validate our dataset, we constructed a random sample of 200 posts from the dataset of 86,462 filtered posts. We annotated them according to whether the post indicated either a personal experience of being an immigrant (irrelevant, therefore a false positive) or a comment about immigrants in the third person (relevant, therefore a true positive). A low false-positive rate (less than 1%) belays concerns that the dataset may inadvertently include immigrants’ perspectives. Therefore, we are confident that our dataset is appropriate for studying public discourse about immigrants in Singapore.

The following sections describe how we first conducted emotion analysis to understand public emotions towards immigrants. Emotion analysis involves examining, processing, summarizing, and reasoning about subjective texts infused with emotional tones. The proliferation of social media platforms has generated an extensive pool of emotional data, where emotion analysis techniques can be applied to unlock the meaning behind emotional texts (Chen, 2022). Next, we performed an automatic topic modeling approach to understand the major themes of discussions about immigrants in Singapore. Topic modeling is an approach that can autonomously assign relevant topics to each document based on their closest resemblance. It enables readers to quickly understand the overview of large documents (Sajid et al., 2017).

3.2 Measurements

Negative emotion was measured from the text by conducting an emotion analysis of the text of the discussions. The emotion analysis relied on a Singaporean English (Singlish) Senticnet lexicon (Ho et al., 2018), including emotion weights for Singaporean slang words. The analysis was also replicated with a different emotion measure from the Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC; Pennebaker et al., 2015). LIWC has been frequently used to analyze linguistic properties in social media posts, including Singaporean social media posts (Tov et al., 2013). LIWC generates a continuous measure of the percentage proportion of positive and negative words in text.

Topical themes discussed in the text were identified through an unsupervised probabilistic topic modeling approach. First, stop words (common words which provide no useful information about topics) were removed. Additionally, words specifically referencing Singapore were excluded to ensure the identification of distinct inter-topic differences. However, pronouns, which are often instrumental in ingroup-outgroup analyses (Housley et al., 2010; Robertson et al., 2013), were retained. Subsequently, we adopted topic modeling techniques as suggested by prior research on Facebook and Twitter content (Schwartz and Ungar, 2015; Eichstaedt et al., 2018; Jaidka et al., 2021). Utilizing the Python Mallet package via the Differential Language Analysis Toolkit (DLATK), which is tailored for short social media posts (Schwartz et al., 2017), we conducted a grid search to determine the optimal alpha parameter and the number of topics. This process involved iterative reviews by the authors to ensure minimal word overlap across topics. An alpha value of 5 was selected, allowing the algorithm to detect up to five topics in lengthier posts, aligning with DLATK’s default recommendation for short posts. The final output consisted of 50 distinct topics, each represented by a set of words and their associated conditional probabilities, indicating the likelihood of a text (a social media post) belonging to a specific topic given the presence of a word.

3.3 Results

3.3.1 The emotions expressed in the social media discourse about immigrants in Singapore

Comments with positive emotion, e.g., “Most Malaysians are hoping to see a better future (…)” expressed hope about immigrants, while posts labeled with negative emotions often reflected sarcasm, e.g., “(…) Fake Degree very handy for this FT,” or often compared races, e.g., “The Chinese are taking over smh (…).” We observed that sarcasm often complicated emotion measurement. For instance, the post “SG garmen just want to earn levies from FOREIGN trash from across. Cheaper and convenient…Useful or useless is secondary” reflected both positive and negative emotions towards immigrants.

Therefore, to contextualize measurements to the overall use of emotional words in the post, we designed a comparative analysis that compares the presence of negative and positive words between the immigrant discourse on social media and a referenced baseline of the general discourse on social media. This analytical design is more insightful in answering our research question and less susceptible to labeling errors when negative words are used, but a negative attitude may not be expressed.

We ran Welch Two Sample t-tests to compare the positive and negative emotion levels between the social media discussions mentioning immigrants and those that did not. The findings reported in Table 2 answer RQ1 and suggest that social media discussions involving immigrants are significantly less positive (M = 3.8%, SE = 5.6%) than general social media discussions (M = 5.8%, SE = 10.3%, p < 0.001). Posts mentioning immigrants also indicated significantly higher levels of negative emotion (M = 2.8%, SE = 4.4%) than general discussions (M = 2.6%, SE = 5.2%, p < 0.001). Reanalyzing the data with the LIWC emotion measure did not change the results, as discussions involving immigrants were significantly less positive (M = 3.7%, SE = 8.3%) and more negative (M = 2.2%, SE = 5.5%) than general discussions (M = 4.1%, SE = 11.1% for positive emotion, p < 0.001; M = 2.1%, SE = 6.9% for negative emotion, p < 0.001).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Distribution of positive and negative emotions in posts about immigrants vs. posts about other topics.

3.3.2 The topical themes expressed in the social media discourse about immigrants in Singapore

Table 3 addresses RQ2, showcasing the topics discerned through this probabilistic topic modeling of social media discussions about immigrants. Each topic is characterized by its most significant words, ranked by importance. The topic modeling approach we employed is inherently algorithmic and automated. As such, the generation of topics was not subject to human interpretation or bias, eliminating the need for inter-rater reliability assessments. However, to streamline the presentation of results, topics with similar themes, as determined by overlapping frequent words, were grouped for a more coherent illustration.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Some of the main concerns generated through a topic modeling approach on the posts mentioning immigrants.

A notable observation is that over 15% of posts discussing immigrants also reference Singaporeans, implying a comparative stance: “Telling you Singaporeans not working hard enough while their favorite foreign trash (…).” The government emerges as another dominant context in these discussions, accounting for 12.7% of all posts, e.g., “This has nothing at all to do with what party is in control of the government.” The discourse predominantly concerns immigrants’ economic impact, emphasizing jobs (11.6%), e.g., “(Foreign talents) gets the good jobs and pulls down productivity (…)” and the economy (4.5%), e.g., “Our rice bowl is always threatened by others who will work for cheaper and will work longer hours!.” Other discussions highlight concerns related to national security (2.3%) and legal implications (6.9%). Conversely, a smaller proportion of the discourse delves into foreigners’ cultural habits (4.02%).

3.3.3 Discussion

Study 1’s findings yield two pivotal insights that underscore the need for a deeper exploration of inter-group dynamics in Singapore. The thematic exploration revealed a perception of immigrants as potential threats to Singapore’s culture and economic equilibrium. Overall, the pronounced hostility towards immigrants in the online discussions suggests a potential undercurrent of discontent or prejudice within the local populace.

These insights from Study 1 catalyzed the conception of Study 2, aiming to delve deeper into the threat dimension of anti-immigrant emotions in Singapore at a more granular, individual level. Study 2 was designed as a national survey targeting Singaporeans. This survey approach aimed to uncover the root causes of anti-immigrant emotions. By transitioning from a broad observational lens to a more targeted survey methodology, we hoped to enrich our understanding of public discourse. This would allow us to pinpoint both the triggers and outcomes of specific behaviors and their varied impacts across different segments of social media users. For instance, some users are influenced more by their peers (Bode, 2012), or those with lower cognitive abilities might be more susceptible to online misinformation (Pennycook and Rand, 2020), making them potentially more vulnerable to the narratives they encounter online.

4 Study 2: a national survey of Singaporean adults

Study 1 identified a few perceived threats to foreigners in Singapore based on a computational text analysis of the expressive use of social media. To complement this approach, Study 2 focuses on the informational use of social media and its association with audience opinions. It uses the lens of integrated threat theory to empirically examine the psychological mechanisms underlying the association of social media consumption (not expression) with prejudiced emotions against immigrants. We report the findings based on an analysis of data from a national survey, focusing on the relationships between social media use, threat perceptions, cognitive ability, and negative emotions toward immigrant outgroups. Survey responses were collected in February 2020 from an online panel managed by Qualtrics. The sample was closely matched to the population parameters, focusing on age and gender to increase the generalizability.

According to the Department of Statistics Singapore (2016), there were around 4 million Singaporean citizens and permanent residents. Based on prior literature on the mediation of user attitudes through beliefs (Lee et al., 2023), we anticipate small effect sizes (<0.05) for the indirect effects; therefore, to obtain a margin of error of 4% and a confidence interval of 95% for the main study, a power analysis suggested that 857 valid responses will be needed. Therefore, the final sample included responses from 1,036 Singaporean citizens to account for possible issues in data quality.

4.1 Measures

The descriptive details of the main variables of interest are included in Table 4.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Descriptive and reliability of primary variables of interest.

Negative emotions against immigrant groups were measured by asking the respondents, in general, how they feel toward (each immigrant group) in Singapore across five dimensions: (a) contempt, (b) disgust, (c) anger, (d) envious, and (e) jealousy (see Cuddy et al., 2008). The responses measured on a 7-point scale (1 = not at all to 7 = a lot) were averaged to create a scale of negative emotions.

Social media use was measured by asking the respondents how frequently (1 = never to 7 = several times a day) they use the following features of social media for information and news purposes related to political and social issues: (a) post in their timeline (b) comment on posts (c) share posts (d) read their newsfeed (e) read their friends’ timeline (f) search with the search bar on their social media homepage. The responses were averaged to create a scale of social media use.

In the Singaporean context, the symbolic threats measure was adapted from previous work (Tausch et al., 2007; Rupar and Graf, 2019). Respondents were asked to rate their level of agreement with the following statements (a) Singapore’s identity is being threatened because there are too many (immigrant groups) in Singapore; (b) Singaporean norms and values are being threatened because of the presence of (immigrant groups) in Singapore and (c) (Immigrant group) in Singapore are a threat to the Singaporean culture. The items measured on a 5-point scale (1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree) were averaged to create a scale of symbolic threat.

The realistic threats measure was also adapted from previous work (Tausch et al., 2007; Rupar and Graf, 2019) to the Singaporean context. Respondents were asked to rate their level of agreement for four statements: Because of the presence of (immigrant group) (a) Singapore is overcrowded, (b) Singaporeans have difficulties finding a job, (c) Singaporeans have more difficulties finding a house, and (d) Singapore is getting dirty. The items measured on a 5-point scale (1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree) were averaged to create a scale of realistic threat.

Cognitive ability was measured by a frequently used 8-item WordSum test where participants were asked to match a given word to the closest matching word from a target list of five choices. The specific vocabulary test shares high variance with general intelligence and has been consistently used to measure the cognitive ability of respondents (Colom et al., 2005; Brandt and Crawford, 2016; Ganzach et al., 2019) as well as in recent studies related to information consumption and attitude formation (Pennycook and Rand, 2020, 2021). The test is also valid for the Singaporean sample since English is the primary language of the educational system and the most spoken language (Department of Statistics Singapore, 2016).

Demographic variables included age (M = 40.61, SD = 12.91), gender (52.3% males), education (1 = no formal education to 9 = post-graduate degree, Median = college under-graduate degree), income (1 = less than $1,000 to 11 = more than $20,000; Median = $7,000 to $8,999), race (82% Chinese, majority), and religion (33% Christianity, majority).

Media news use variables asked respondents how frequently (1 = never to 7 = several times a day) they use (a) television (M = 4.72, SD = 1.84), (b) radio news (M = 3.97, SD = 2.07) and (c) print newspaper (M = 3.98, SD = 1.96) to get information about political and social issues.

4.2 Results

4.2.1 The informational use of social media and anti-immigrant attitudes

The study employed an ordinary least squares regression to predict negative emotions toward the three immigrant groups (see Table 5). H1 is supported as social media use was positively associated with negative emotions toward all three groups: American expatriates (β = 0.132, p < 0.001), Malays (β = 0.153, p < 0.001), and Indian (β = 0.122, p < 0.001) immigrants. On the other hand, those with higher use of traditional media such as television (American expatriates: β = −0.148, p < 0.001; Malays: β = −0.100, p < 0.001, and Indians: β = −0.151, p < 0.001) are less likely to hold negative emotions.

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. OLS hierarchical regression predicting negative emotions against immigrant groups.

4.2.2 Symbolic and realistic threats mediate the association of social media use and anti-immigrant attitudes

First, considering the direct relationships reported in Table 5, both symbolic (American expatriates: β = 0.220, p < 0.001; Malays: β = 0.279, p < 0.001, and Indians: β = 0.304, p < 0.001) and realistic threat perception (American expatriates: β = 0.295, p < 0.001; Malays: β = 0.366, p < 0.001, and Indians: β = 0.246, p < 0.001) were positively associated with negative emotions toward all three immigrant outgroups.

Hypotheses 2 and 3 predict that symbolic threats (H2a-c) and realistic threats (H3a-c) mediate the relationship between social media use and negative emotions. We employed the SPSS macro PROCESS (Hayes, 2017). We employed a parallel multiple mediation model (Model 4) with bootstrapping to estimate the indirect mediation effects. The relationships between social media use, symbolic threat perception, realistic threat perception, and negative emotions toward immigrant groups are illustrated in Figure 2.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Illustration of the mediation analysis for the three outgroups.

First, the results in Figure 2 suggest that those who frequently use social media are more likely to consider immigrant groups as both symbolic (Figure 2A American expatriates: b = 0.098, se = 0.023, p < 0.001; Figure 2B Malays: b = 0.110, se = 0.023, p < 0.001, and Figure 2C Indians: b = 0.087, se = 0.026, p < 0.001) and realistic threats (Figure 2A American expatriates: b = 0.102, se = 0.021, p < 0.001; Figure 2B Malays: b = 0.120, se = 0.022, p < 0.001, and Figure 2C Indians: b = 0.060, se = 0.025, p < 0.001).

Next, across all models, both symbolic (Figure 2A American expatriates: b = 0.325, se = 0.068, p < 0.001; Figure 2B Malays: b = 0.381, se = 0.068, p < 0.001, and Figure 2C Indians: b = 0.388, se = 0.064, p < 0.001) and realistic threats (Figure 2A American expatriates: b = 0.465, se = 0.072, p < 0.001; Figure 2B Malays: b = 0.520, se = 0.071, p < 0.001, and Figure 2C Indians: b = 0.324, se = 0.067, p < 0.001) were positively associated with negative emotions.

Finally, Table 6 presents the parallel mediation results comparing the two paths of indirect effects: (i) social media use – symbolic threat – negative emotions, (ii) social media – realistic threat – negative emotions.

Table 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Mediation of social media use through threat perceptions predicting negative emotions against immigrant groups.

Now, consider the results for each immigrant group, reported in Table 6. First, for negative emotions toward American expatriates, the role of both symbolic threat (b = 0.032, SE = 0.011, 95% CI = 0.013 to 0.055) and realistic threat (b = 0.047, SE = 0.013, 95% CI = 0.025 to 0.077) as mediators between social media use and negative emotions was statistically significant. The direct effect was insignificant (b = 0.043, SE = 0.029, 95% CI = −0.015 to 0.101). Thus, H2a and H3a are supported.

Next, for negative emotions toward Malay immigrants, both symbolic (b = 0.042, SE = 0.013, 95% CI = 0.019 to 0.070) and realistic threats (b = 0.063, SE = 0.016, 95% CI = 0.035 to 0.096) were found to be statistically significant mediators. Similar to previous cases, direct effects were found to be statistically insignificant (b = 0.030, SE = 0.026, 95% CI = −0.020 to 0.080). Since indirect effects are statistically significant (b = 0.105, SE = 0.022, 95% CI = 0.063 to 0.147), H2b and H3b are supported.

Finally, for negative emotions toward Indian immigrants, the role of both symbolic (b = 0.034, SE = 0.012, 95% CI = 0.012 to 0.061) and realistic threat (b = 0.019, SE = 0.010, 95% CI = 0.003 to 0.042) as mediators between social media use and negative emotion was found to be statistically significant. Direct effects were also statistically significant (b = 0.060, SE = 0.029, 95% CI = 0.004 to 0.116). Since the indirect effects are significant, H2c and H3c are supported.

To sum up, the parallel multiple mediation analyses suggest that the effects of social media use on negative emotions toward the three immigrant groups are significantly mediated through symbolic and realistic threat perceptions; however, the directionality of the relationship is uncertain, as greater negative emotions may also reinforce more social media use because of higher symbolic and realistic threat perceptions.

4.2.3 Cognitive ability moderates the association of threats with anti-immigrant attitudes

The direct relationships reported in Table 5 indicate that individuals with higher cognitive ability were less likely to feel negatively toward the immigrant groups (American expatriates: β = −0.091, p < 0.001; Malays: β = −0.125, p < 0.001, and Indians: β = −0.159, p < 0.001).

The next step was to test the final set of hypotheses (H4a-c and H5a-c), which predicted that cognitive ability would moderate the mediation of threat perception (symbolic and realistic) on social media. Again, we employed a conditional PROCESS (Model 59). Table 7 presents the indirect effect estimates, standard errors, and the 95% confidence interval significance value of moderated mediations. The indirect effects are estimated at three levels of cognitive ability: one standard deviation below the mean, the mean, and one standard deviation above the mean.

Table 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Moderated mediation results for negative emotions against immigrant groups through threat perceptions as mediators and cognitive ability as a moderator.

Moderated mediation is observed when the strength of an indirect effect depends on the moderator’s levels (Preacher and Hayes, 2004). As observed across all models, the indirect effects of symbolic threats significantly decreased as the level of cognitive ability increased. It was stronger for individuals with lower cognitive ability (American expatriates: b = 0.032, 95% CI = 0.008 to 0.063, Malays: b = 0.030, 95% CI = 0.005 to 0.074; Indians: b = 0.029, 95% CI = 0.005 to 0.068) than average cognitive ability individuals (American expatriates: b = 0.018, 95% CI = 0.003 to 0.037, Malays: b = 0.023, 95% CI = 0.005 to 0.046; Indians: b = 0.027, 95% CI = 0.006 to 0.052). The indirect effects for individuals with high cognitive ability are statistically insignificant.

A similar pattern is observed for the indirect effects of realistic threats, where the effects significantly decrease as the level of cognitive ability increases. The effects were stronger for individuals with lower cognitive ability (American expatriates: b = 0.050, 95% CI = 0.019 to 0.093, Malays: b = 0.072, 95% CI = 0.029 to 0.126; Indians: b = 0.035, 95% CI = 0.004 to 0.076) than average cognitive individuals (American expatriates: b = 0.031, 95% CI = 0.010 to 0.056, Malays: b = 0.039, 95% CI = 0.015 to 0.069; Indians: b = 0.019, 95% CI = 0.011 to 0.043). The indirect effects of high cognitive ability individuals were not statistically significant in any of the models.

These results suggest that cognitive ability moderates the relationship between threats and negative emotions toward immigrants such that the effects are stronger for individuals with low or average cognitive ability; however, as before, the directionality of the relationship cannot be deterministically ascertained. Thus, H4a and H5a (American expatriates), H4b and H5b (Malays), and H4c and H5c (Indians) are supported.

4.3 Discussion

Study 2 drilled down to an individual-level analysis of the drivers of negative emotion towards immigrants, focusing on threat perceptions (realistic and symbolic threats) and cognitive ability. The findings suggest that individuals with higher informational use of social media and higher symbolic and realistic threat perception are more likely to exhibit anti-immigrant emotions. Symbolic and realistic threat perceptions positively mediated the relationship between social media use and anti-immigrant emotions. Furthermore, individuals with low cognitive ability are more likely to have negative emotions toward immigrant groups from gaining threat perceptions while using social media than those with average or high cognitive ability.

The findings are steady across three immigrant groups, highlighting that cognitive ability is a robust predictor of anti-immigrant attitudes. Further, the study confirms and expands existing research, explicating the direct associations. Finally, it shows that cognitive ability can directly impact prejudice and act as a contingency factor in how users evaluate sociopolitical content on social media, leading to prejudiced emotions.

5 General discussion

Social media may become a crucial outlet for citizens to voice concerns and opinions. The findings presented here suggest that social media discussions about immigrants generally adopt a negative tone and touch upon the themes of the economic and cultural threats perceived by the local community. The survey findings highlight that cognitive ability is a robust predictor of anti-immigrant affect, and they are stable across different immigrant groups. Finally, we show that a lack of cognitive ability is directly associated with higher prejudice; it also acts as a contingency factor in how users evaluate sociopolitical content on social media, and is associated with higher negative emotions.

There are three main takeaways from this work. The first takeaway underscores social media’s potential to qualitatively gauge public opinion, even in non-liberal contexts with limited press freedom. By analyzing aggregate emotional and topical signals from thousands of public posts, we can derive theoretically valid insights into the priorities and concerns of a citizenry. This real-time monitoring of public emotion can be instrumental in identifying unexpected surges in discussions, allowing for timely interventions to prevent potential hate crime incidents. For instance, recent acts of hate and extremism in France (Forbes, 2016), New Zealand (Cable News Network (CNN), 2019), and the United States (New York Times, 2021) were premeditated on social media before manifesting as real-life tragedies. The utility of social media as a live emotion monitor lies potentially in the immediacy and widespread reach of social media platforms that might amplify particular emotions, leading to rapid mobilization around specific issues or ideologies (Stephan and Stephan, 2000). By continuously monitoring these platforms, policymakers can proactively address emerging concerns through targeted public and social messaging efforts (Esses et al., 2001).

The second takeaway emphasizes the significance of inter-group differences when exploring social media’s influence on attitude formation. Our findings indicate that informational use of social media correlates positively with negative emotions towards all three outgroups – Indians, Americans, and Malays. Individual differences in perceptions of realistic and symbolic threats were pivotal in mediating this relationship.

The mediation relationship can be explained based on the broader political opinion literature on the role of individual differences. Echo chambers, which can arise due to homophily or contagion, might reinforce existing biases, intensifying threat perceptions against immigrants (O’Hara and Stevens, 2015). However, the causality direction remains uncertain. Alternatively, these findings might stem from individual differences rooted in self-interest, self- and social categorization, and individual social identities (Sears and Funk, 1991; David, 2009; Bachl, 2017). This suggests a complex interplay between social media exposure, individual predispositions, and the formation of prejudiced attitudes. Furthermore, it is indeed possible that more prejudiced media users interpret social media content through a biased lens. Our study does not refute this; instead, it attempts to investigate whether the specific nature of social media engagement can compound or alter these perceptions. In combining our two studies, we aim to provide a more comprehensive understanding of the interplay between media use, individual predispositions, and perceptions toward immigrants. To further refine our understanding, future research could delve deeper into the types of social media content consumed and the context in which social media interactions occur. This would help distinguish the media effects from the individual attribute effects more clearly.

The third takeaway reveals that social media users with low cognitive ability are particularly susceptible to the detrimental effects of social media, exhibiting higher levels of prejudice. Our data suggests a negative association between cognitive ability and anti-immigrant affect across all three immigrant groups. Moreover, cognitive ability was found to moderate the mediation effect via threat perceptions for all these groups. Therefore, the role of realistic and symbolic threats in tuning social media use into prejudiced emotions is more significant in individuals with lower and moderate cognitive ability levels than in individuals with high cognitive ability.

The study confirms existing research explicating the direct associations and expands on it, showing that cognitive ability might influence how individuals process and evaluate information, especially sociopolitical content, on social media platforms (Pennycook and Rand, 2020). Those with lower cognitive abilities might be more prone to accepting misinformation or being influenced by biased narratives, leading to heightened prejudiced emotions (Ahmed et al., 2021a,b). This aligns with existing research that has explored direct associations between cognitive ability and prejudice, suggesting that cognitive ability can influence both direct prejudice and how users interpret sociopolitical content, culminating in prejudiced emotions.

Our studies have a few limitations, which we attempted to offset through a multi-method analysis. For Study 1, first, despite meticulous efforts to curate data from local Singaporean platforms and validate its authenticity, there remains an inherent uncertainty about the nationality of all contributors. This potential mix could influence the overall tenor of the findings derived solely from social media data. Second, by its nature, social media might amplify the voices of a vocal minority, potentially overshadowing the more nuanced emotions of the silent majority. Third, while the observational nature of social media data can spotlight the prevailing negative rhetoric about immigrants, it offers limited insights into the impact of such rhetoric on its audience. Finally, aggregate trends from social media analysis can not delve into individual variances in immigrant perceptions or elucidate the personal traits and mechanisms driving these attitudes.

Although a multi-methodological framework combining social media and survey data increases the reliability of the findings, the study has several limitations that need to be discussed. First, while we anticipate that social media collected from popular internet communities would offer an unfiltered and unsolicited insight into attitudes towards immigrants, it is also true that relying on these posts alone is prone to reflect the most extreme views rather than those of the general public (Bail, 2021). Therefore, it is crucial to vet social media-based findings against survey-based results. Second, analyzing a multilingual sample also has its limitations. We restricted our analysis to include only English posts because of the lack of resources to translate other posts. While most Singaporeans express themselves in English on social media, we still lose out on discourse in other local languages and, therefore, by minority populations (e.g., Tamils and Malays). Finally, regarding the survey component of the analysis, while high threat perceptions and low cognitive ability are associated with heightened anti-immigrant feelings in response to social media use, a causal claim cannot be tested without a longitudinal research design.

6 Conclusion

The principal goal of this study was to compare the signals available through the expressive and informational use of social media in terms of their ability to explain local attitudes towards immigrants in Singapore. Therefore, this study conducted a quantitative cross-platform topic modeling of discussions about immigrants in online Singaporean communities (Study 1) to understand the expressive use of social media to discuss immigrants in Singapore. We supplemented the analyses on the expressive use of social media with a survey-based examination (Study 2) of the threats associated with the informational use of social media for anti-immigrant affect and the moderating variables that affect these relationships.

Our findings offer two main theoretical contributions. First, they address a preeminent research gap suggesting the theoretical link between cognitive ability and prejudice (Brandt and Crawford, 2016). Furthermore, while ITT traditionally focuses on cognitive prejudice, our findings emphasize the importance of affective prejudice, such as feelings of contempt, disgust, anger, envy, and jealousy. This broadens the understanding of prejudice from mere cognitive evaluations to more visceral emotional reactions, suggesting that social media shapes people’s thoughts about outgroups and how they emotionally respond to them.

A second contribution is the empirical evidence for the relationship between social media use and negative emotions toward outgroups. By their algorithms and user behaviors, social media platforms often present a skewed or biased view of outgroups, leading to heightened threat perceptions (Guess et al., 2023). Our findings corroborate the implications from Guess et al. (2023) to suggest that it is not just the algorithms encountered nor the sheer volume of social media use that matters, but who uses them and how they perceive the threats in the content and discussion they encounter.

Our research recommends using multi-method approaches to offer a comprehensive understanding of different media, such as social media, in terms of their role as a space for building and influencing public opinion. Findings related to heightened affective prejudice in Singapore emphasize the need to foster cross-group harmony through interactions and communities on social media and possibly other public spaces that eschew the social and economic constraints evident in offline societies. We recommend that policy efforts focus on literacy as a supplementary way to improve ingroup-outgroup relationships. Dispelling prevailing stereotypes through fact checks and educational efforts on social media may offer a more viable alternative than efforts to curtail social media content altogether.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the Institutional Review Board, Nanyang Technological University. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants. Social media analyses were conducted on anonymized posts, aggregated from all the sources, to minimize the risks of accidental deidentification.

Author contributions

SA: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Project administration. KJ: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Investigation. VC: Data curation, Project administration, Writing – review & editing. MC: Writing – original draft. AChe: Writing – original draft. CE: Data curation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. VY: Writing – original draft. AChi: Funding acquisition, Project administration, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by the Ministry of Education in Singapore (grant number MOE2017-T2-2-145). The publication of the article was supported by the NUS Centre for Trusted Internet and Community (grant number A-0003503-14-00).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted without any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Ahmed, S., Chen, V. H. H., and Chib, A. I. (2021a). Xenophobia in the time of a pandemic: social media use, stereotypes, and prejudice against immigrants during the COVID-19 crisis. Int. J. Pub. Opin. Res. 33, 637–653. doi: 10.1093/ijpor/edab014

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahmed, S., Chen, V. H. H., Jaidka, K., Hooi, R., and Chib, A. (2021b). Social media use and anti-immigrant attitudes: evidence from a survey and automated linguistic analysis of Facebook posts. Asian J. Commun. 31, 276–298. doi: 10.1080/01292986.2021.1929358

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bachl, M. (2017). Selective exposure and hostile media perceptions during election campaigns. Int. J. Pub. Opin. Res. 29, 352–362. doi: 10.1093/ijpor/edw014

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bail, C. (2021). Breaking the social media prism. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

Google Scholar

Bartlett, J., and Norrie, R. (2015). Immigration on twitter: Understanding public attitudes online. Demos. Available at: https://apo.org.au/node/54788 (Accessed March 13, 2022).

Google Scholar

Bergman, O., Ellingsen, T., Johannesson, M., and Svensson, C. (2010). Anchoring and cognitive ability. Econ. Lett. 107, 66–68. doi: 10.1016/j.econlet.2009.12.028

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bode, L. (2012). Facebooking it to the polls: a study in online social networking and political behavior. J. Inform. Technol. Politics 9, 352–369. doi: 10.1080/19331681.2012.709045

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brandt, M. J., and Crawford, J. T. (2016). Answering unresolved questions about the relationship between cognitive ability and prejudice. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 7, 884–892. doi: 10.1177/1948550616660592

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cable News Network (CNN) (2019). How the Christchurch terrorist attack was made for social media. Available at: https://edition.cnn.com/2019/03/15/tech/christchurch-internet-radicalization-intl/index.html (Accessed March 13, 2022).

Google Scholar

Ceron, A., Curini, L., Iacus, S. M., and Porro, G. (2014). Every tweet counts? How sentiment analysis of social media can improve our knowledge of citizens’ political preferences with an application to Italy and France. New media and society 16, 340–358. doi: 10.1177/1461444813480466

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, J., Ghose, A., and Seamans, R. (2016). The internet and racial hate crime: offline spillovers from online access. MIS Q. 40, 381–403. doi: 10.25300/MISQ/2016/40.2.05

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, K., and Tomblin, D. (2021). Using data from Reddit, public deliberation, and surveys to measure public opinion about autonomous vehicles. Public Opin. Q. 85, 289–322. doi: 10.1093/poq/nfab021

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, M. (2022). Emotion analysis based on deep learning with application to research on development of Western culture. Front. Psychol. 13:911686. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.911686

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, V. H. H., Ahmed, S., and Chib, A. (2021). The role of social media behaviors and structural intergroup relations on immigrant stereotypes. Int. J. Commun. 15:24.

Google Scholar

Cho, J., Ahmed, S., Keum, H., Choi, Y. J., and Lee, J. H. (2018). Influencing myself: self-reinforcement through online political expression. Commun. Res. 45, 83–111. doi: 10.1177/0093650216644020

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Colom, R., Abad, F. J., Rebollo, I., and Shih, P. C. (2005). Memory span and general intelligence: a latent-variable approach. Intelligence 33, 623–642. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2005.05.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Croucher, S. M., Nguyen, T., and Rahmani, D. (2020). Prejudice toward Asian Americans in the Covid-19 pandemic: the effects of social media use in the United States. Front. Commun. 5:39. doi: 10.3389/fcomm.2020.00039

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cuddy, A. J., Fiske, S. T., and Glick, P. (2008). Warmth and competence as universal dimensions of social perception: the stereotype content model and the BIAS map. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 40, 61–149. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(07)00002-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Errico, F., and Paciello, M. (2018). Online moral disengagement and hostile emotions in discussions on hosting immigrants. Internet Res. 28, 1313–1335. doi: 10.1108/IntR-03-2017-0119

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

David, C. C. (2009). Intergroup attitudes and policy support: how prejudice against minority groups affects support for public policies. Int. J. Public Opin. Res. 21, 85–97. doi: 10.1093/ijpor/edp005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Davidson, T., and Farquhar, L. (2020). “Prejudice and social media: attitudes toward illegal immigrants, refugees, and transgender people” in Gender, sexuality and race in the digital age. eds. D. Farris, D. Compton, and A. Herrera (Cham, CH: Springer), 151–167.

Google Scholar

Del Vicario, M., Vivaldo, G., Bessi, A., Zollo, F., Scala, A., Caldarelli, G., et al. (2016). Echo chambers: emotional contagion and group polarization on facebook. Sci. Rep. 6:37825. doi: 10.1038/srep37825

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Department of Statistics Singapore (2016). General household survey. Department of Statistics Singapore. Available at: https://www.singstat.gov.sg/publications/ghs/ghs2015 (Accessed March 13, 2022).

Google Scholar

Dhont, K., and Hodson, G. (2014). Does lower cognitive ability predict greater prejudice? Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 23, 454–459. doi: 10.1177/0963721414549750

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dirksmeier, P. (2020). Resentments in the Cosmopolis: anti-immigrant attitudes in postcolonial Singapore. Cities 98:102584. doi: 10.1016/j.cities.2019.102584

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eichstaedt, J. C., Smith, R. J., Merchant, R. M., Ungar, L. H., Crutchley, P., Preoţiuc-Pietro, D., et al. (2018). Facebook language predicts depression in medical records. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 115, 11203–11208. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1802331115

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Esses, V. M., Dovidio, J. F., Jackson, L. M., and Armstrong, T. L. (2001). The immigration dilemma: The role of perceived group competition, ethnic prejudice, and national identity. Journal of Social issues 57, 389–412. doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.00220

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Feather, N. T., and Volkmer, R. E. (1988). Preference for situations involving effort, time pressure, and feedback in relation to type a behavior, locus of control, and test anxiety. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 55, 266–271. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.55.2.266

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Forbes (2016). Terrorism Suspect’s use of ‘Facebook live’ stream highlights challenges of live video. Available at: https://www.forbes.com/sites/kathleenchaykowski/2016/06/15/terrorist-suspects-use-of-facebook-live-stream-highlights-challenges-of-live-video/?sh=502d3d922408 (Accessed March 13, 2022).

Google Scholar

Ganzach, Y., Hanoch, Y., and Choma, B. L. (2019). Attitudes toward presidential candidates in the 2012 and 2016 American elections: cognitive ability and support for trump. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 10, 924–934. doi: 10.1177/1948550618800494

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gomes, C. (2014). Xenophobia online: unmasking Singaporean attitudes towards ‘foreign talent’ migrants. Asian Ethnicity 15, 21–40. doi: 10.1080/14631369.2013.784511

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grover, T., Bayraktaroglu, E., Mark, G., and Rho, E. H. R. (2019). Moral and affective differences in us immigration policy debate on twitter. Proceed. Conf. Comp. Supp. Coop. Work 28, 317–355. doi: 10.1007/s10606-019-09357-w

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Guess, A. M., Malhotra, N., Pan, J., Barberá, P., Allcott, H., Brown, T., et al. (2023). How do social media feed algorithms affect attitudes and behavior in an election campaign? Science 381, 398–404. doi: 10.1126/science.abp9364

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hameleers, M., and Schmuck, D. (2017). It’s us against them: a comparative experiment on the effects of populist messages communicated via social media. Inf. Commun. Soc. 20, 1425–1444. doi: 10.1080/1369118X.2017.1328523

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawdon, J., Oksanen, A., and Räsänen, P. (2017). Exposure to online hate in four nations: a cross-national consideration. Deviant Behav. 38, 254–266. doi: 10.1080/01639625.2016.1196985

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, A.F., (2017). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: a regression-based approach. New York: Guilford publications.

Google Scholar

Heidenreich, T., Eberl, J. M., Lind, F., and Boomgaarden, H. (2020). Political migration discourses on social media: a comparative perspective on visibility and sentiment across political Facebook accounts in Europe. J. Ethn. Migr. Stud. 46, 1261–1280. doi: 10.1080/1369183X.2019.1665990

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, D., Hamzah, D., Poria, S., and Cambria, E. (2018). Singlish Sentic net: a concept-based sentiment resource for Singapore English. In Proceedings of the 2018 IEEE symposium series on computational intelligence (SSCI) (IEEE), 1285–1291. doi: 10.1109/SSCI.2018.862879

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodson, G., and Dhont, K. (2015). The person-based nature of prejudice: individual difference predictors of intergroup negativity. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 26, 1–42. doi: 10.1080/10463283.2015.1070018

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hopkins, D. J., and Parish, K. (2019). Medicaid expansion and attitudes toward the affordable care act: testing for a policy feedback on mass opinion. Public Opin. Q. 83, 123–134. doi: 10.1093/poq/nfz004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

House, Freedom. (2023). Freedom in the World 2023. Available at: https://freedomhouse.org/country/singapore/freedom-world/2023 (Accessed January 21, 2024).

Google Scholar

Housley, M. K., Claypool, H. M., Garcia-Marques, T., and Mackie, D. M. (2010). “We” are familiar but “it” is not: Ingroup pronouns trigger feelings of familiarity. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 46, 114–119. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.08.011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsueh, M., Yogeeswaran, K., and Malinen, S. (2015). “Leave your comment below”: can biased online comments influence our own prejudicial attitudes and behaviors? Hum. Commun. Res. 41, 557–576. doi: 10.1111/hcre.12059

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Igartua, J. J., Moral-Toranzo, F., and Fernández, I. (2012). Cognitive, attitudinal, and emotional effects of news frame and group cues, on processing news about immigration. J. Med. Psychol. 23, 174–185. doi: 10.1027/1864-1105/a000050

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaidka, K., Guntuku, S. C., Lee, J. H., Luo, Z., Buffone, A., and Ungar, L. H. (2021). The rural–urban stress divide: obtaining geographical insights through twitter. Comput. Hum. Behav. 114:106544. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2020.106544

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jedinger, A., and Eisentraut, M. (2020). Exploring the differential effects of perceived threat on attitudes toward ethnic minority groups in Germany. Front. Psychol. 10:2895. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02895

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Karami, A., Bennett, L. S., and He, X. (2018). Mining public opinion about economic issues: twitter and the us presidential election. Int. J. Strat. Decis. Sci. 9, 18–28. doi: 10.4018/IJSDS.2018010102

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiilakoski, T., and Oksanen, A. (2011). Soundtrack of the school shootings: cultural script, music and male rage. Young 19, 247–269. doi: 10.1177/110330881101900301

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, I., Permyakova, T., and Sheveleva, M. (2020). Reducing negative attitudes toward immigrants in Russia and Taiwan: possible beneficial effects of naïve dialecticism and an incremental worldview. Front. Psychol. 11:572386. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.572386

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, S., Tandoc, E. C. Jr., and Diehl, T. (2023). Uninformed and misinformed: advancing a theoretical model for social media news use and political knowledge. Digit. Journal., 1–20. doi: 10.1080/21670811.2023.2180404

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lusk, S. L. (1983). Interaction of test anxiety and locus of control on academic performance. Psychol. Rep. 53, 639–644. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1983.53.2.639

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Macafee, T., and De Simone, J. J. (2012). Killing the bill online? Pathways to young people's protest engagement via social media. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 15, 579–584. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2012.0153

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahmood, Q. K., Jafree, S. R., Mukhtar, S., and Fischer, F. (2021). Social media use, self-efficacy, perceived threat, and preventive behavior in times of COVID-19: results of a cross-sectional study in Pakistan. Front. Psychol. 12:562042. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.562042

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Maragoudakis, M., Loukis, E., and Charalabidis, Y. (2011). “A review of opinion mining methods for analyzing citizens’ contributions in public policy debate” in International conference on electronic participation (Heidelberg, DE: Springer), 298–313.

Google Scholar

McNulty, Y. (2016). “Do”t hate me because “m a foreigner”: historical and narrative analyses of Western expatriates’ experiences in Singapore. Int. J.Multinat. Corp. Strat. 1, 217–246. doi: 10.1504/IJMCS.2016.081124

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Millet, K., and Dewitte, S. (2007). Altruistic behavior as a costly signal of general intelligence. J. Res. Pers. 41, 316–326. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2006.04.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ministry of Home Affairs. (2019). Ministerial statement on restricting hate speech to maintain racial and religious harmony in Singapore, speech by Mr K Shanmugam, minister for home affairs and minister for law. Available at: https://www.mha.gov.sg/mediaroom/parliamentary/ministerial-statement-on-restricting-hate-speech-to-maintain-racial-and-religious-harmony-in-singapore-speech-by-mr-k-shanmugam-minister-for-home-affairs-and-minister-for-law/ (Accessed March 13, 2022).

Google Scholar

Müller, K., and Schwarz, C. (2023). From hashtag to hate crime: twitter and antiminority sentiment. Am. Econ. J. Appl. Econ. 15, 270–312. doi: 10.1257/app.20210211

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, N. A., and Hall, J. A. (2011). Intelligence and interpersonal sensitivity: a meta-analysis. Intelligence 39, 54–63. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2010.10.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

New York Times (2021). The storming of Capitol Hill was organized on social media. Available at: https://www.nytimes.com/2021/01/06/us/politics/protesters-storm-capitol-hill-building.html (Accessed March 13, 2022).

Google Scholar

O’Hara, K., and Stevens, D. (2015). Echo chambers and online radicalism: assessing the Internet’s complicity in violent extremism. Policy Internet 7, 401–422. doi: 10.1002/poi3.88

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Onraet, E., Van Hiel, A., Dhont, K., Hodson, G., Schittekatte, M., and De Pauw, S. (2015). The association of cognitive ability with right-wing ideological attitudes and prejudice: a meta-analytic review. Eur. J. Personal. 29, 599–621. doi: 10.1002/per.2027

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Oz, M., Zheng, P., and Chen, G. M. (2018). Twitter versus Facebook: comparing incivility, impoliteness, and deliberative attributes. New Media Soc. 20, 3400–3419. doi: 10.1177/1461444817749516

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pan, J., and Theseira, W. (2023). Immigration in Singapore [Working paper]. The World Bank. Available at: https://thedocs.worldbank.org/en/doc/080a4bc64cc8a9eb8a2a0e98d97a260a-0050062023/immigration-in-singapore

Google Scholar

Pennebaker, J. W., Boyd, R. L., Jordan, K., and Blackburn, K. (2015). The development and psychometric properties of LIWC2015. Austin, TX: University of Texas at Austin.

Google Scholar

Pennycook, G., and Rand, D. G. (2020). Who falls for fake news? The roles of bullshit receptivity, overclaiming, familiarity, and analytic thinking. J. Pers. 88, 185–200. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12476

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pennycook, G., and Rand, D. G. (2021). The psychology of fake news. Trends Cogn. Sci. 25, 388–402. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2021.02.007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Petty, R. E., and Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Communication and persuasion. Central and peripheral routes to attitude change. New York: Springer.

Google Scholar

Pew Research Centre (2019). Around the world, more say immigrants are a strength than a burden. Available at: https://www.pewresearch.org/global/2019/03/14/around-the-world-more-say-immigrants-are-a-strength-than-a-burden/ (Accessed June 9, 2021).

Google Scholar

Praw (2020). PRAW: The Python Reddit API wrapper. https://praw.readthedocs.io/

Google Scholar

Preacher, K. J., and Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 36, 717–731. doi: 10.3758/BF03206553

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Quillian, L. (1995). Prejudice as a response to perceived group threat: Population composition and anti-immigrant and racial prejudice in Europe. American sociological review 586–611. doi: 10.2307/2096296

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rieder, B. (2013). “Studying Facebook via data extraction: the Netvizz application” in Proceedings of the 5th annual ACM web science conference, vol. 28, (New York, NY, USA: Association for Computing Machinery), 346–355.

Google Scholar

Robertson, S. P., Douglas, S., Maruyama, M., and Semaan, B. (2013). Political discourse on social networking sites: sentiment, ingroup/outgroup orientation and rationality. Inform. Polity 18, 107–126. doi: 10.3233/IP-130303

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rupar, M., and Graf, S. (2019). Different forms of intergroup contact with former adversary are linked to distinct reconciliatory acts through symbolic and realistic threat. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 49, 63–74. doi: 10.1111/jasp.12565

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sajid, A., Jan, S., and Shah, I. A. (2017). “Automatic topic modeling for single document short texts” in Proceedings of the 2017 international conference on frontiers of information technology. (Islamabad, Pakistan), 70–75.

Google Scholar

Sawyer, T. P. Jr., and Hollis-Sawyer, L. A. (2005). Predicting stereotype threat, test anxiety, and cognitive ability test performance: an examination of three models. Int. J. Test. 5, 225–246. doi: 10.1207/s15327574ijt0503_3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schäfer, S., Sülflow, M., and Reiners, L. (2021). Hate speech as an Indicator for the state of the society. J. Med. Psychol. 34, 3–15. doi: 10.1027/1864-1105/a000294

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartz, H. A., Giorgi, S., Sap, M., Crutchley, P., Ungar, L., and Eichstaedt, J. (2017). “Dlatk: differential language analysis toolkit” in Proceedings of the 2017 conference on empirical methods in natural language processing: System demonstrations. (Copenhagen, Denmark: Association for Computational Linguistics), 55–60.

Google Scholar

Schwartz, H. A., and Ungar, L. H. (2015). Data-driven content analysis of social media: a systematic overview of automated methods. Ann. Am. Acad. Pol. Soc. Sci. 659, 78–94. doi: 10.1177/0002716215569197

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sears, D. O., and Funk, C. L. (1991). The role of self-interest in social and political attitudes. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 24, 1–91. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60327-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sears, D. O.. (1988). Symbolic racism, in Eliminating racism: Profiles in controversy. (Eds.) P. A. Katz and D. A. Taylor (Springer), 53–84.

Google Scholar

Sobkowicz, P., Kaschesky, M., and Bouchard, G. (2012). Opinion mining in social media: modeling, simulating, and forecasting political opinions in the web. Gov. Inf. Q. 29, 470–479. doi: 10.1016/j.giq.2012.06.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Statista (2019). Number of immigrants in Singapore from 2005 to 2019 (in 1,000s) [graph]. In Statista. Available at: https://www-statista-com/statistics/698035/singapore-number-of-immigrants/ (Accessed March 12, 2022).

Google Scholar

Statista (2023). Number of Facebook users in Singapore 2018-2017 (in millions) [graph]. In Statista. Available at: https://www.statista.com/statistics/490492/number-of-singapore-facebook-users/ (Accessed Jan 13, 2024).

Google Scholar

Stephan, W. G., Boniecki, K. A., Ybarra, O., Bettencourt, A., Ervin, K. S., Jackson, L. A., et al. (2002). The role of threats in the racial attitudes of blacks and whites. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 28, 1242–1254. doi: 10.1177/01461672022812009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephan, W. G., Renfro, C. L., Esses, V. M., Stephan, C. W., and Martin, T. (2005). The effects of feeling threatened on attitudes toward immigrants. International Journal of Intercultural Relations 29, 1–19. doi: 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2005.04.011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephan, W. G., and Stephan, C. W. (1996). Predicting prejudice. International Journal of Intercultural Relations 20, 409–426. doi: 10.1016/0147-1767(96)00026-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephan, W. G., Stephan, C. W., and Gudykunst, W. B. (1999a). Anxiety in intergroup relations: a comparison of anxiety/uncertainty management theory and integrated threat theory. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 23, 613–628. doi: 10.1016/S0147-1767(99)00012-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephan, W. G., and Stephan, C. W. (2000). “An integrated threat theory of prejudice” in Reducing prejudice and discrimination. ed. S. Oskamp (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 23–46.

Google Scholar

Stephan, W. G., Ybarra, O., and Bachman, G. (1999b). Prejudice toward immigrants: an integrated threat theory. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 29, 2221–2237. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1999.tb00107.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tausch, N., Tam, T., Hewstone, M., Kenworthy, J. B., and Cairns, E. (2007). Individual-level and group-level mediators of contact effects in Northern Ireland: the moderating role of social identification. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 46, 541–556. doi: 10.1348/014466606X155150

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Theocharis, Y., Barberá, P., Fazekas, Z., Popa, S. A., and Parnet, O. (2016). A bad workman blames his tweets: the consequences of citizens' uncivil twitter use when interacting with party candidates. J. Commun. 66, 1007–1031. doi: 10.1111/jcom.12259

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tov, W., Ng, K. L., Lin, H., and Qiu, L. (2013). Detecting well-being via computerized content analysis of brief diary entries. Psychol. Assess. 25, 1069–1078. doi: 10.1037/a0033007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

United Nations (2020). World migration report. Available at: https://www.un.org/sites/un2.un.org/files/wmr_2020.pdf (Accessed June 9, 2021).

Google Scholar

Utych, S. M. (2018). How dehumanization influences attitudes toward immigrants. Polit. Res. Q. 71, 440–452. doi: 10.1177/1065912917744897

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Velayutham, S. (2017). Races without racism?: everyday race relations in Singapore. Identities 24, 455–473. doi: 10.1080/1070289X.2016.1200050

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Walther, J. B., Hoter, E., Ganayem, A., and Shonfeld, M. (2015). Computer-mediated communication and the reduction of prejudice: a controlled longitudinal field experiment among Jews and Arabs in Israel. Comput. Hum. Behav. 52, 550–558. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.08.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Weimann, G. (2010). Terror on facebook, twitter, and Youtube. Brown J. World Affairs 16, 45–54.

Google Scholar

Yang, P. (2018). Desiring ‘foreign talent’: lack and Lacan in anti-immigrant sentiments in Singapore. J. Ethn. Migr. Stud. 44, 1015–1031. doi: 10.1080/1369183X.2017.1384157

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Žeželj, I. L., Ioannou, M., Franc, R., Psaltis, C., and Martinovic, B. (2017). The role of inter-ethnic online friendships in prejudice reduction in post-conflict societies: evidence from Serbia, Croatia and Cyprus. Comput. Hum. Behav. 76, 386–395. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.07.041

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: social media, realistic threat, symbolic threat, cognitive ability, emotion, prejudice, immigrant

Citation: Ahmed S, Jaidka K, Chen VHH, Cai M, Chen A, Emes CS, Yu V and Chib A (2024) Social media and anti-immigrant prejudice: a multi-method analysis of the role of social media use, threat perceptions, and cognitive ability. Front. Psychol. 15:1280366. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1280366

Received: 12 December 2023; Accepted: 21 February 2024;
Published: 13 March 2024.

Edited by:

Eddy J. Davelaar, Birkbeck, University of London, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Franca Crippa, University of Milano-Bicocca, Italy
Özen Odag, Touro College Berlin, Germany

Copyright © 2024 Ahmed, Jaidka, Chen, Cai, Chen, Emes, Yu and Chib. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Saifuddin Ahmed, sahmed@ntu.edu.sg; Kokil Jaidka, jaidka@nus.edu.sg

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.