Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Psychol., 24 April 2024
Sec. Educational Psychology

From contact to connection: a comprehensive examination of affective touch in educational settings

  • 1University of Valencia, Valencian Community, Valencia, Spain
  • 2Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway

We often talk about the way we talk, and we frequently try to see the way we see, but for some reasons we have rarely touched on the way we touch. The communication we transmit with touch is perceived to be one of the most powerful means of establishing human relationships. In particular, tactile communication with parents, caregivers and teachers is particularly important for infants and students, as it helps make stronger relationships between educators or teachers and schoolers and also between students. Research has demonstrated the numerous benefits that an affective touch has on students, physically, socially and cognitively, or as has observed, touch touches deeper that just one’s skin and it is a recipe for creating meaningful relations. However, in the educational context, touch is perceived to be a complex phenomenon full of tension and emotion. For years, a dilemma has arisen in educational institutions in some countries, whether teachers can touch students or not? Despite the benefits that affective touch brings to students, cases of sexual abuse and inappropriate behavior at school have alerted the education system, to such an extent that many teachers worldwide consider what is and is not appropriate when communicating affectively with their students through touch. In this perspective article, by drawing on previous literature reviews, we shall highlight the benefits that affective touch has on learners.

1 Introduction

Educators’ bodies, embodiment, and touch are not just private, personal phenomena but socially shared and constructed (Merleau-Ponty, 1964). Merleau-Ponty observed that touch is a two-way road: One cannot touch without being touched at the same time (Merleau-Ponty, 1964), or as Jones and Yarborough (1985) have longed observed, one can not touch and, at the same time, be uninvolved with the other person. Touch has been studied in the context of schools (e.g., Heinonen et al., 2020) from the viewpoint of students (Keränen et al., 2020), educators (Johansson et al., 2021) and student teachers (Johansson et al., 2021). Touch, being an extension of proxemics (distance between people), is mediated by the culture of belonging (Hall, 1963; Hall, 1966; Watson, 1968) even in educational settings (Farsani, and Mendes, 2021; Farsani et al., 2022). Thus, the US, the United Kingdom or northern European countries are considered low-contact countries, while Arab, Mediterranean and Latin countries are considered high-contact cultures. This leads us to a reflection: most research on the positive effects of touch is carried out in low-contact countries. And, despite this, the results leave no room for doubt: positive interpersonal touch generates positive reactions and internal states in touch receptors, both in adults and children (Gallace and Spence, 2010; Field, 2019; Suvilehto et al., 2023). The notion of touch in schools is becoming more and more regulated in many countries, to the point we consider the possibility of achieving a scenario of a “zero contact” school, both in low and high contact countries. As Keränen and Uitto (2023, p.181) say, there is a big contradiction in educator’s work: “touch is simultaneously something to value and something to avoid.” At that point, this perspective article aims is to argue about this question: will the benefits of knowing that our students are not at risk of being touched inappropriately by their teachers outweigh the negative effects that deprivation of emotional touch has on them?

2 New perspectives

Ashley Montagu (1971) echoes a 1915 report by Dr. Henry Dwight Chapin, a distinguished pediatrician, in which he stated that in all but one of the orphanages in 10 different cities in the United States, the mortality rate for children under two was 100%. Babies died of a disease called “marasmus,” which was a weakness, an atrophy that had consequences as serious as premature death. And this despite the fact that the children received the “essential care”: hygiene, food, shelter etc. The orphanage in which the babies did survive was distinguished from the others precisely by including affective touch in these “essential cares.” Children are observed to need, for their proper psychological development and well-being, the affective touch on the part of their caregivers (Field, 2001, 2019; Barnett, 2005; Bergnehr and Cekaite, 2018). In fact, a lower emotional tact of parents toward their children correlates with aggressive behaviors of these toward their parents (Prescott, 1990; Field, 1999). According to Carlson and Nelson (2006), children perform aggressive touch because even that type of touch is better than the absence of touch (Owen and Gillentine, 2011).

Attachment theory, formerly developed by Bowlby (1969) and Ainsworth (1963) correlates warm behaviors from parents to their children such as physical closeness and touch with the perception of children of their parents as “sensitive, reliable, available and supportive” (Beetz et al., 2011, p. 351). Affective touch from caregivers is perceived to be one of the most important warm behavior to develop secure attachment in infants (Anisfeld et al., 1990; Duhn, 2010). Furthermore, it is also perceived to be important for developing attachment security in adults couples (Jakubiak and Feeney, 2016). Interestingly enough, Beetz et al. (2011) concluded that insecurely attached children prefer touching a dog rather touching a friendly person: as they find their parents to be rejecting and unsupportive, they avoid closeness with them. Insecurely attached children were generally unable to use the presence of another unfamiliar person for social support and stress alleviation.

The point for this perspective article is that this attachment is transferred to other close relationships like the one between children and their teachers (Bretherton, 1992). That means the kind of attachment a child has with her/his teachers is congruent with the attachment s/he has with his caregivers (Beetz et al., 2011). So, we can expect the kind of attachment of children with their parents will be determinant in the pleasantness experience of the touch from a teacher: securely attached children will receive touch from their teachers better than insecurely attached children.

Research has also provided evidence for the communication of emotions such as love, gratitude, and sympathy via touch (Hertenstein et al., 2006). Human touch is perceived to be necessary for life (Honig, 2005). Affective touch involves complex neurobiological processes such as release of oxytocin and endorphins, which contribute to generate calm and wellbeing state, the stimulation of special skin receptors (C-LTMRs, that is: C low threshold mechanoreceptors), which information is transmitted through the spinal cord and then reaches the brain through bottom-up pathways (Schirmer and McGlone, 2022). That way, affective touch appears to relieve physical pain, has numerous health benefits, such as improving the immune system, improving asthma, promoting sleep, physical growth (Field, 2001; Owen and Gillentine, 2011). Hugs are yet another form of touch which are perceived to reduce blood pressure and protect against increased heart rate in stressful situations (Grewen et al., 2003) and protect us from the common cold (Cohen et al., 2015). A friendly touch on the back or preschool children by an adult is perceived to improve their self-regulation to postpone gratification affecting not only their agreement to act as requested, but their decision-making and their will (Leonard et al., 2014).

The touching behavior can be understood in different ways depending on different variables (Burgoon et al., 1992; Ellingsen et al., 2016). Recipients’ touch perception depends on different internal elements like attention, internal motivational state, predictions of the meaning of touch, previous experiences (Ellingsen et al., 2016), gender (Stier and Hall, 1984; Hall and Veccia, 1990), age (Hertenstein et al., 2006), personality…, and external to the person touched such as culture (Hall, 1966; Dibiase and Gunnoe, 2004) and context (Macaluso and Driver, 2001), other nonverbal (Patterson et al., 1986; Burgoon, 1991; Burgoon et al., 1992; Soars, 2009; Ellingsen et al., 2014) and verbal (Bohm and Hendricks, 1997) cues or even interpersonal relationship between sender and recipient of touch. It also depends, of course on the physical characteristics of the touch, such as temperature, softness, force and velocity (Ellingsen et al., 2016).

This variety of determinants cause that in some cases, the hedonic experience of touch becomes unpleasant for the recipient (Ellingsen et al., 2016). However, the negative experiences in touch have been studied mainly in adults and in some clinic situations for children like autism (Riquelme et al., 2016).

In educational research, touch is often perceived as a natural and integral part of educators’ work, especially in early childhood education where young children are taken care of and nurtured via touch (Cekaite and Bergnehr, 2018; Svinth, 2018). In early childhood education, it is primarily though touch that educators can, for example, control students by setting rules such as “do not run,” or “sit still” (Lupton, 2013), using touch in its instrumental function (Burgoon et al., 1992; Rosa et al., 2020; Rosa and Farsani, 2021). The diverse functions of touch in care for students have been emphasized, such as controlling, compassionate, comforting, and affectionate touch (Bergnehr and Cekaite, 2018; Cekaite and Bergnehr, 2018). Affective touch appears to bring a physical, psychological and emotional benefit to students. The positive effects of touch in the educational environment have also been studied with university students, improving their conformity with the requests of their teacher (Guéguen, 2004; Leonard et al., 2014), but the positive effects on the attention of students aged five to six years have also been studied, as well as the reduction of disruptive behaviors when receiving positive touch by the teacher (Wheldall et al., 1986). Khatin-Zadeh et al. (2022, 2023) showed that it is through the medium of touch, gestures and embodiment that empower teachers to convey difficult and abstract mathematical concepts into a more tangible and transparent understanding. In another study (Owen and Gillentine, 2011) a survey from 63 teachers in the US was carried out and the results showed that 98% of teachers considered that touching children promotes their emotional development and 92% thought it reduced stress. However, only 30% of those same teachers touched children in situations of emotional discouragement, due to the fear that teachers have developed of being accused of abuse (Owen and Gillentine, 2011).

Previous research has illustrated tensions to exist between practices of touch and no-touch in educators’ work: On one hand, educators see touch as a natural touch way to be with students (e.g., Keränen and Uitto, 2023). On the contrary, touch is also perceived to be a risky behavior in education, since educators’ touch can be misinterpreted for example as a physical or sexual assault by students (Piper and Smith, 2003; Andrzejewski and Davis, 2008). Cases of sexual abuse in the classroom are very rare in the United States, but that has not prevented teachers from feeling afraid to touch their students, either a touch to impose discipline or an affective touch, for fear of being denounced (Owen and Gillentine, 2011). Putting the case of Spain, according to a 2021 report by the NGO defending children’s rights Save The Children, in which 394 judicial sentences on child sexual abuse were analyzed, it was found that 6% of these cases are related to educators, compared to 49.5% in which the aggressor is a relative. 9.7% where the aggressor is a partner or friend of the victim or 8.6% where the aggressor is an acquaintance of the family. Therefore, despite the fact that the cases in which the abuse of minors by teachers is the lowest percentage (far, of course, from a desirable 0%), it is only in this area that zero-contact campaigns by teachers toward their pupils have been promoted in some countries. In any case, we must be cautious with the figures of complaints in absolute values, since these may not reflect the reality of the abuses that actually occur in the classroom. And, when we talk about touch between adults, the meaning of tactile interaction with a sexual function is quite clear (Jones and Yarbrough, 1985). However, the function of touch performed by an adult on a child may not be understood by the child. This can lead to situations in which an inappropriate touch is not perceived by the child as such or, conversely, an “innocent” touch is misinterpreted by the child. We strongly believe more studies are needed to explore and better understand this phenomenon, particularly, from the perspective of the children and teacher educators.

3 Conclusion

This short perspective article aimed to raise awareness to the hidden messages of touch and how children consciously and/or unconsciously respond to it. Although this perspective appears to be ‘too one-sided’ on the positive evidence of touch, there could be fundamental reasons as to why it is the case. It could be anything ranging from the feeling of ‘shame’ or/and ‘embarrassment’ talking about (from the perspective of the children) or reporting (from the perspective of adults/researchers) the negative effects of touch. Although from one perspective this speculation is plausible, on the other hand, empirical studies have shown that teachers’ touch is observed to generate positive reactions and reduces disruptive behaviors in classrooms (e.g., Keränen and Uitto, 2023). Touch can be a mediatory tool for boosting students’ engagement in educational activities. Touch, if used properly, can enhance not only social relations but also “learning and teaching connections” (Rosa et al., 2020; Rosa and Farsani, 2021; Farsani and Villa-Ochoa, 2022); that is, it can strengthen students’ involvement in educational activities and create a context in which students are actively engaged in learning activities. Furthermore, we would like to stress and highlight that teachers’ touch appears to be a resource that can empower teachers and educators to better engage with their students. It seems that touch touches deeper than mere skin and touch needs to be studied further from the viewpoint of educators.

Furthermore, in our professional experiences that covers five countries (Iran, England, Chile, Norway and Spain), we have not found any teacher education courses that touches upon this multisided phenomenon and thus many newly graduated teachers may not be aware of the benefits of touch in their professional teaching practice. Touch appears to be a medium to create bonds between humans; but at the same time, it can serve as a tool to strengthen the connection between students’ minds and an educational setting. Again, we would like to stress and highlight that future teacher trainees must be able to understand that touch has/is a multisided phenomenon, and that touch in educators’ work is not just about setting rules of what is and what is not a proper touch, for example, where and when to touch, for how long, the duration and the angle of touch (approaching a student from their left hand side versus their right hand side). Furthermore, as contemporary researchers (Johansson et al., 2021; Keränen and Uitto, 2023) have observed, for touch to be tactful, it must be critically considered, discussed, and evaluated as part of teachers’ work in the teacher training courses. Finally, we strongly believe that such professional debated must have a place and a time where teachers can freely reflect and share their experiences about touch.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

SH and DF both worked on the idea and developed the idea together. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This paper was partially financially supported by the European Union’s Horizon 2020 project Enhancement of research excellence in Mathematics Teacher Knowledge, no. 951822. Furthermore, we thank the Norwegian University of Science and Technology for covering the APC.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank David Matsumoto and Omid Khatin-Zadeh for their comments on our earlier version of our manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Ainsworth, M. D. (1963). “Development of infant-mother interaction among the ganda” in Determinants of Infant Behaviour II. ed. B. M. Foss (New York: Wiley), 67–104.

Google Scholar

Andrzejewski, C. E., and Davis, H. A. (2008). Human contact in the classroom: exploring how teachers talk about and negotiate touching students. Teach. Teach. Educ. 24, 779–794. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2007.02.013

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Anisfeld, E., Casper, V., Nozyce, M., and Cunningham, N. (1990). Does infant carrying promote attachment? An experimental study of the effects of increased physical contact on the development of attachment. Child Dev. 61, 1617–1627. doi: 10.2307/1130769

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barnett, L. (2005). Keep in touch: the importance of touch in infant development. Infant Obs. 8, 115–123. doi: 10.1080/13698030500171530

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Beetz, A., Kotrschal, K., Turner, D. C., Hediger, K., Uvnäs-Moberg, K., and Julius, H. (2011). The effect of a real dog, toy dog and friendly person on insecurely attached children during a stressful task: an exploratory study. Anthrozoös 24, 349–368. doi: 10.2752/175303711X13159027359746

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergnehr, D., and Cekaite, A. (2018). Adult-initiated touch and its functions at a Swedish preschool: controlling, affectionate, assisting and educative haptic conduct. Int. J. Early Years Educ. 26, 312–331. doi: 10.1080/09669760.2017.1414690

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blackwell, P. L. (2000). The influence of touch on child development: implications for intervention. Infants Young Child. 13, 25–39. doi: 10.1097/00001163-200013010-00006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bohm, J. K., and Hendricks, B. (1997). Effects of interpersonal touch, degree of justification, and sex of participant on compliance with a request. J. Soc. Psychol. 137, 460–469. doi: 10.1080/00224549709595462

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment 1. New York: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Bretherton, I. (1992). The origins of attachment theory: John Bowlby and Mary Ainsworth. Dev. Psychol. 28, 759–775. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.28.5.759

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgoon, J. K. (1991). Relational message interpretations of touch, conversational distance, and posture. J. Nonverbal Behav. 15, 233–259. doi: 10.1007/BF00986924

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgoon, J. K., Walther, J. B., and Baesler, E. J. (1992). Interpretations, evaluations, and consequences of interpersonal touch. Hum. Commun. Res. 19, 237–263. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2958.1992.tb00301.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlson, F. M., and Nelson, B. G. (2006). Reducing aggression with touch. Dimensions Early Childhood 34:9.

Google Scholar

Cekaite, A., and Bergnehr, D. (2018). Affectionate touch and care: embodied intimacy, compassion and control in early childhood education. Eur. Early Child. Educ. Res. J. 26, 940–955. doi: 10.1080/1350293X.2018.1533710

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, S., Janicki-Deverts, D., Turner, R. B., and Doyle, W. J. (2015). Does hugging provide stress-buffering social support? A study of susceptibility to upper respiratory infection and illness. Psychol. Sci. 26, 135–147. doi: 10.1177/0956797614559284

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dibiase, R., and Gunnoe, J. (2004). Gender and culture differences in touching behavior. J. Soc. Psychol. 144, 49–62. doi: 10.3200/SOCP.144.1.49-62

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Duhn, L. (2010). The importance of touch in the development of attachment. Adv. Neonatal Care 10, 294–300. doi: 10.1097/ANC.0b013e3181fd2263

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellingsen, D. M., Leknes, S., Løseth, G., Wessberg, J., and Olausson, H. (2016). The neurobiology shaping affective touch: expectation, motivation, and meaning in the multisensory context. Front. Psychol. 6:1986. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01986

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellingsen, D. M., Wessberg, J., Chelnokova, O., Olausson, H., Laeng, B., and Leknes, S. (2014). In touch with your emotions: oxytocin and touch change social impressions while others’ facial expressions can alter touch. Psychoneuroendocrinology 39, 11–20. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.09.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Farsani, D., and Mendes, J. (2021). Proxêmica e comunicação não verbal na interação em sala de aula. Psicologia Escolar e Educacional, v 25. doi: 10.1590/2175-35392021229866

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Farsani, D., and Villa-Ochoa, J. A. (2022). Analyzing students’ visual attention through spy glasses. Uniciencia. 36, 1–17. doi: 10.15359/ru.36-1.34

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Farsani, D., Breda, A., and Sala, V. (2022). Non-verbal interaction and students’ visual engagement in mathematics and English classes. Acta Scientiae 24, 1–26. doi: 10.17648/acta.scientiae.6721

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jakubiak, B. K., and Feeney, B. C. (2016). A sense of security: touch promotes state attachment security. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 7, 745–753. doi: 10.1177/1948550616646427

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, T. (1999). Preschoolers in America are touched less and are more aggressive than preschoolers in France. Early Child Dev. Care 151, 11–17. doi: 10.1080/0300443991510102

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, T. (2001). Touch. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Field, T. (2019). Social touch, CT touch and massage therapy: a narrative review. Dev. Rev. 51, 123–145. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2019.01.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallace, A., and Spence, C. (2010). The science of interpersonal touch: an overview. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 246–259. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.10.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grewen, K. M., Anderson, B. J., Girdler, S. S., and Light, K. C. (2003). Warm partner contact is related to lower cardiovascular reactivity. Behav. Med. 29, 123–130. doi: 10.1080/08964280309596065

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Guéguen, N. (2004). Nonverbal encouragement of participation in a course: the effect of touching. Soc. Psychol. Educ. 7, 89–98. doi: 10.1023/B:SPOE.0000010691.30834.14

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, E. T. (1963). A system for the notation of proxemic behavior. Am. Anthropol. 65, 1003–1026. doi: 10.1525/aa.1963.65.5.02a00020

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, E. T. (1966). The hidden dimension. Garden City, NY: Doubleday

Google Scholar

Hall, J. A., and Veccia, E. M. (1990). More" touching" observations: New insights on men, women, and interpersonal. J. Personal. Soci. Psychol. 59, 1155–1162. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.59.6.1155

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Heinonen, P., Karvonen, U., and Tainio, L. (2020). Hand-on-shoulder touch as a resource for constructing a pedagogically relevant participation framework. Linguis. Edu. 56, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.linged.2020.100795

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hertenstein, M., and Weiss, S. (2011). The handbook of touch. New York. Springer

Google Scholar

Hertenstein, M. J., Verkamp, J. M., Kerestes, A. M., and Holmes, R. M. (2006). The communicative functions of touch in humans, nonhuman primates, and rats: a review and synthesis of the empirical research. Genet. Soc. Gen. Psychol. Monogr. 132, 5–94. doi: 10.3200/MONO.132.1.5-94

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Honig, A. S. (2005). Infants & toddlers: development--the power of touch. Early Childhood Today 19, 25–26.

Google Scholar

Johansson, C., Åberg, M., and Hedlin, M. (2021). Touch the children, or please don’t–preschool teachers’ approach to touch. Scand. J. Educ. Res. 65, 288–301. doi: 10.1080/00313831.2019.1705893

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, S. E., and Yarbrough, A. E. (1985). A naturalistic study of the meanings of touch. Communications Monographs 52, 19–56. doi: 10.1080/03637758509376094

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keränen, V., and Uitto, M. (2023). Can I kiss a child? Balancing between different views of touch in early childhood educators’ work. Scand. J. Educ. Res. 67, 181–193. doi: 10.1080/00313831.2021.1990124

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keränen, V., Viljamaa, E., and Uitto, M. (2020). Touch in children’s everyday peer relations in preschools. Childhood. 28, 86–102. doi: 10.1177/0907568220953514

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Khatin-Zadeh, O., Farsani, D., Eskandari, Z., and Marmolejo-Ramos, F. (2022). The roles of motion, gesture, and embodied action in the processing of mathematical concepts. Front. Psychol. 13. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.969341

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Khatin-Zadeh, O., Farsani, D., and Breda, A. (2023). How can transforming representation of mathematical entities help us employ more cognitive resources? Front. Psychol. 14. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1091678

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leonard, J. A., Berkowitz, T., and Shusterman, A. (2014). The effect of friendly touch on delay-of-gratification in preschool children. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 67, 2123–2133. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2014.907325

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lupton, D. (2013). Infant embodiment and interembodiment: a review of sociocultural perspectives. Childhood 20, 37–50. doi: 10.1177/0907568212447244

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Macaluso, E., and Driver, J. (2001). Spatial attention and crossmodal interactions between vision and touch. Neuropsychologia 39, 1304–1316. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00119-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Merleau-Ponty, M. (1964). Signs. Northwestern University Press.

Google Scholar

Montagu, A. (1971). Touching, the human significance of the skin. Perennial Library, 98–99.

Google Scholar

Owen, P. M., and Gillentine, J. (2011). Please touch the children: appropriate touch in the primary classroom. Early Child Dev. Care 181, 857–868. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2010.497207

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Piper, H., and Smith, H. (2003). ‘Touch’ in educational and child care settings: dilemmas and responses. Br. Educ. Res. J. 29, 879–894. doi: 10.1080/0141192032000137358

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Patterson, M. L., Powell, J. L., and Lenihan, M. G. (1986). Touch, compliance, and interpersonal affect. J. Nonverbal Behav. 10, 41–50. doi: 10.1007/BF00987204

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Prescott, J. W. (1990). “Affectional bonding for the prevention of violent behaviors; neurobiological, psychological and religious/spiritual determinants” in Violent behavior. eds. L. J. Hertzberg, G. F. Ostrum, and J. R. Field (Great Neck, N.Y: PMA Publishing), 95–124.

Google Scholar

Riquelme, I., Hatem, S. M., and Montoya, P. (2016). Abnormal pressure pain, touch sensitivity, proprioception, and manual dexterity in children with autism spectrum disorders. Neural Plast. 2016, 1–9. doi: 10.1155/2016/1723401

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosa, M., and Farsani, D. (2021). Two fish moving in their seas: how does the body language of teachers show itself who teach mathematical equations? Acta Scientiae 23, 141–168. doi: 10.17648/acta.scientiae.6391

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosa, M., Farsani, D., and Silva, C. (2020). Mathematics education, body and digital games: the perception of body-proper opening up horizons of mathematical knowledge constitution. Math. Teach. Res. J. 12, 310–324.

Google Scholar

Save the Children. (2021). Analysis: sexual abuse of children in Spain. Available at: https://www.savethechildren.es/actualidad/analisis-abusos-sexuales-infancia-espana

Google Scholar

Schirmer, A., and McGlone, F. (2022). Editorial overview: affective touch: neurobiology and function. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 45:101129. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2022.101129

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Soars, B. (2009). Driving sales through shoppers' sense of sound, sight, smell and touch. Int. J. Retail Distrib. Manag. 37, 286–298. doi: 10.1108/09590550910941535

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stier, D. S., and Hall, J. A. (1984). Gender differences in touch: an empirical and theoretical review. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 47, 440–459. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.47.2.440

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Suvilehto, J. T., Cekaite, A., and Morrison, I. (2023). The why, who and how of social touch. Nat Rev Psychol 2, 606–621. doi: 10.1038/s44159-023-00217-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Svinth, L. (2018). Being touched – the transformative potential of nurturing touch practices in relation to toddlers’ learning and emotional well-being. Early Child Dev. Care 188, 924–936. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2018.1446428

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vincent, J., Schirrer, M., and Andrieu, B. (2023). Le toucher comme facteur de bien-être des élèves en cours d’EPS. Droit, santé et société 1, 47–54.

Google Scholar

Watson, O. M. (1968). Proxemic behavior: A cross-cultural study University of Colorado at Boulder.

Google Scholar

Wheldall, K., Bevan, K., and Shortall, K. (1986). A touch of reinforcement: the effects of contingent teacher touch on the classroom behaviour of young children. Educ. Rev. 38, 207–216. doi: 10.1080/0013191860380301

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: touch, teacher education, embodied cognition, engagement, embodiment

Citation: El Hakim S and Farsani D (2024) From contact to connection: a comprehensive examination of affective touch in educational settings. Front. Psychol. 15:1230796. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1230796

Received: 29 May 2023; Accepted: 26 March 2024;
Published: 24 April 2024.

Edited by:

Douglas F. Kauffman, Medical University of the Americas – Nevis, United States

Reviewed by:

Langer Janet, University of Rostock, Germany

Copyright © 2024 El Hakim and Farsani. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Danyal Farsani, ZGFueWFsLmZhcnNhbmlAbnRudS5ubw==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.