- 1Experimental and Applied Psychology Laboratory, Department of Human Sciences, European University of Rome, Rome, Italy
- 2Department of Education Science, Roma Tre University, Rome, Lazio, Italy
Introduction: As life expectancy increases, more attention needs to be paid to their mental and physical condition. Many older patients are also bedridden, which makes some treatments, like in vivo exposure to natural environments, more difficult to be applied. This study aimed to systematically review articles that include interventions combining virtual reality and forest environment, targeting a sample of older people.
Methods: Based on PRISMA guidelines, we conducted a literature search in three databases (EBSCO, PubMed, and Scopus), plus gray literature (OpenGrey). We considered only studies that used forest settings via virtual reality and included a sample with age ≥ 65.
Results: After the screening and eligibility stages, 7 articles have been included.
Discussion: The study underlines the need to implement research in this direction to standardize effective procedures that can be used to improve the mental and physical health of the older people and caregivers, while also reducing social costs.
Introduction
The older people represent the most fragile group of the population, toward which, at times, little attention is paid. The World Health Organization (WHO) defines the age of 65 as the transition condition to the “older” (Kowal and Dowd, 2001), and it is confirmed by some studies (e.g., Crews and Zavotka, 2006). Most Western regions show a trend toward an aging population (e.g., Cuijpers et al., 2006; Arean et al., 2008). As a result, more and more people are developing chronic diseases, which in recent years, have seen significant improvements in their better management (Uijen and van de Lisdonk, 2008; Murray et al., 2015). This leads to a significant reduction in mobility, consequently increasing the fragility of physical structures, leading these individuals to live increasingly sedentary lives (El-Khoury et al., 2015). In this regard, the academic literature has shown a strong correlation between chronic diseases and sedentary lives (e.g., Vancampfort et al., 2017). To ward off the adverse effects of old age, various interventions have been tried out to reduce sedentariness and increase psycho-physical well-being (Sollami et al., 2017; Buedo-Guirado et al., 2020). These include forest therapy, which has also been tested in recent years (e.g., Sung et al., 2012). Forest therapy consists of all activities involving the forest environment (e.g., exposure to the forest environment, walking in the forest, etc.) for the promotion of health and well-being (Kotte et al., 2019; Zhang et al., 2020). From a medical point of view, Forest Therapy appears to have a preventive role on chronic diseases and a protective role on health, leading to restorative outcomes (Mattila et al., 2020). Moreover, several studies have revealed the pivotal effects of forest therapy in reducing stress (Jung et al., 2015), decreasing cortisol levels (Sung et al., 2012), as well as in strengthening the immune system (e.g., Li and Kawada, 2011; Chae et al., 2021).
Research has shown how Forest Therapy enables people to control stress, anxiety and depression levels and generates effects primarily on the immune system, respiratory and cardiovascular systems (Song et al., 2016). An additional practice used is Shinrin-yoku, which is used as a synonym of forest therapy or as a part of forest therapy (Rajoo et al., 2020), a technique involving bathing in the forest through total immersion engendering well-being as well as benefits for physical health (Reese et al., 2022). Like Forest Therapy, such an intervention reduces depression, anxiety and heart rate, increasing the perceived positive affect (Hansen et al., 2017). The limitation of this intervention is the exposure to a natural setting in vivo. This feature might be inapplicable to some sub-populations of older people, as they may be bedridden or have walking difficulties. A viable alternative to in vivo stimulation has been, for years now, virtual stimulation via virtual reality (VR) (Spano et al., 2023). VR is defined as a technology that uses special equipment, such as head-mounted displays, to reproduce simulated environments, allowing individuals to undergo an immersive experience (Yu et al., 2018). The use of VR has also been applied in interventions with older people in order to reduce the isolation and monotony of an overly sedentary life due to medical conditions (Baker et al., 2020). This combination allows interaction with virtual environments by creating a realistic sense of presence within the computer-generated environment (e.g., Stanney and Zyda, 2002). In this way, despite the physical hindrances, depressed mood and isolation of the older people can be reduced, and accordingly producing benefits for the medical condition, as well (Woodford and Fisher, 2019). Nonetheless, despite few studies that have combined virtual reality technology with the principles of forest therapy (Annerstedt et al., 2013; Li et al., 2020; Mattila et al., 2020; Yu and Hsieh, 2020), these still appear to be poorly oriented to an older people sample. Therefore, the present review aims to explore the benefits of VR based interventions with forest environments on older people by systematically summarizing the scientific literature on the topic (Table 1).
Methods
For the present systematic review, the guidelines of the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis were followed (PRISMA).1
Search, screening, and selection strategies
The following inclusion criteria were adopted: (a) experimental studies or RCTs (randomized controlled trials); (b) studies with older people samples (age over 64 years, consistent with the age that the World Health Organization defines to indicate the transition to older people status) (Kowal and Dowd, 2001); (c) studies that implemented a VR intervention and used a forest environment. The following exclusion criteria were adopted: (a) nonexperimental studies, (b) studies having a sample age of less than 65 years, (c) studies with interventions other than VR, (d) studies with VR interventions, but without forest environments. The research, therefore, took the PICOS “shown in.
The review was conducted through three databases: EBSCO, PubMed and Scopus. The following string of keywords, using Boolean operators (AND and OR), was adopted: “elderly” OR “aged” OR “older” OR “geriatric” AND “virtual reality” OR “vr” AND “forest” OR “forest therapy” OR “nature therapy” OR “virtual nature” OR “forest bathing” OR “shinrin-yoku.”
A total of 97 articles were identified, 11 of which were duplicates and were therefore eliminated. Of the initial 86 articles, following an initial selection, 80 were excluded for the exclusion criteria explained above and because off topic. The gray literature was also investigated through the OpenGrey database, but the search yielded no results.
In filter selection, filters were applied by sample age (65 years and older) and VR use with forest environments. The search, conducted through 3 databases, was completed on February 16, 2023. EBSCO produced 12 results, 2 of which were evaluated as eligible (Moyle et al., 2018; Yuan et al., 2022). The PubMed search yielded 53 articles, but none were evaluated as eligible because they were not consistent with the inclusion criteria.
The search through Scopus identified 32 articles, of which 4 articles were considered, but 2 of these were excluded because although it included a sample of older people within it, this was not divided by a sample with other ages considered, and the results were not divided by age. Therefore 2 articles were considered eligible (Graf et al., 2020; Appel et al., 2022). Research was also conducted through other sources such as bibliographic citations (Appel et al., 2020; Brimelow et al., 2020) of study (Appel et al., 2022), and website (this paper is present also in Scopus, but by subsequent and further research, this paper was also selected as the authors noted its eligibility Lundstedt et al., 2021). The selection of articles was carried out independently by two of the authors (Authors 1 and Authors 3); there were no instances of disagreement between the two authors. In case of a non-univocal decision on an article, the opinion of Authors 2 would be sought. All information about the literature search process and study extraction is shown in the flowchart shown in Figure 1. Characteristics of the studies shown in Table 2.
Figure 1. Flowchart of the process of initial literature search and extraction of studies meeting the inclusion criteria.
Characteristics of the included studies
Seven articles were included in this review (Moyle et al., 2018; Appel et al., 2020; Brimelow et al., 2020; Graf et al., 2020; Lundstedt et al., 2021; Appel et al., 2022; Yuan et al., 2022). These articles were published from 2019 to 2022 and were conducted in the following countries: China (Yuan et al., 2022), Sweden (Lundstedt et al., 2021), Australia (Moyle et al., 2018; Brimelow et al., 2020), Canada (Appel et al., 2020), Germany (Graf et al., 2020), and United States (Appel et al., 2022). The number of participants enrolled ranged from a minimum of 7 to a maximum of 66. All studies had a sample population aged over 65 years. The highest mean age of participants was in the study of Appel et al. (2022) (M = 91.6), while the lowest was in Graf et al. (2020) (M = 76.8). Among the 7 selected studies, Yuan et al. (2022) conducted a Randomized Control Trial. Lundstedt et al. (2021) conducted a qualitative study. Furthermore, Appel et al. (2020) and Appel et al. (2022) conducted a prospective, longitudinal, open, non-randomized interventional clinical trial. Besides, Graf et al. (2020), Moyle et al. (2018), and Brimelow et al. (2020) carried out a mixed study. In detail, Graf et al. (2020) combined qualitative questions and quantitative measures; Moyle et al. (2018) alternated video recorders, observations, and interviews; Brimelow et al. (2020) combined validated quantitative observer instruments, a resident feedback survey, and staff interviews.
Visors used
Different viewers and images to create virtual environments were adopted in the selected studies. Yuan et al. (2022) used VR SHINECON AIO5, a head-mounted display with gyroscopes, nine-axis sensors, headphones, and other accessories. Lundstedt et al. (2021), in their study, decided to use three different viewers. (1) An Oculus Go, a self-contained display without cables that can be strapped to the viewer’s head or held in hand and used as if it were binoculars. (2) the Samsung Odyssey, which is a head-mounted display that requires a cable connection to a computer. (3) the HTC Vive; this is an HMD that requires a cable connection to a computer. Instead, Appel et al. (2020) decided to use the Samsung Gear VR HMD (VR hardware system consisting of Samsung S7 smartphones to view VR videos), Samsung VR HMD with a viewing screen and limited real-world vision, Sennheiser HD 221 headphones to play the sound of the movies and minimize the sound of the surrounding environment, and VR technology replaceable health face pads for individual use. Like Graf et al. (2020) and Lundstedt et al. (2021) also used the Oculus Go. The Oculus Go device was also used in the study by Appel et al. (2022), they also used an HMD VR device. Unlike the other authors who used mobile devices, Moyle et al. (2018) used a large screen and provided patients with Microsoft Kinect® motion sensors. In the study by Brimelow et al. (2020), the authors used Samsung Galaxy S7 devices as sensors with Gears VR visors.
Images used
Yuan et al. (2022), in their study, used images related to the Black Valley National Forest Park in China, and environmental sounds, such as birdsong and running water, were also reproduced. Lundstedt et al. (2021), in their study, used 3 different conditions about natural environment. A Virtual Island with various environments (e.g., forest environments with trees, a beach, a meadow, etc.), a 360° video of blue space (with Oculus Go), shot at various coastal locations in Cornwall, England, and a virtual nature environment “theBlu” (interactive underwater environments in real time, with Samsung Odyssey). Appel et al. (2020) have chosen five scenes: a dense forest with tall pine trees swaying in the wind; a rocky shore and waves; an open field with colorful foliage blowing in an autumn wind; a black stone beach and waves of icy water surrounded by a tall glacier; a beach with gently flowing waves, a bright blue sky and a family with a child and a dog in the distance. In the study of Graf et al. (2020), the virtual environment used in this experiment, named “VR Forest Walk,” comprises a forest, a mountain landscape, and a virtual ocean. Also, they have used cognitive tasks (Animal Bingo and Memory Parkour). A virtual dog was also played in the virtual environment, whose purpose was to provide companionship to the subject involved, which is not thrilling or exciting. Appel et al. (2022), served themselves 360-degree VR videos that included various nature scenes (e.g., sunny forest, dense forest, shore of a rocky lake, floating icebergs, and sunny beach) while sitting in swivel chairs or lying in bed.
Moyle et al. (2018) showed participants images of a forest environment that include a river, accompanied by a background soundtrack incorporating peaceful, forest noises such as bird calls. Brimelow et al. (2020), used 360-degree videos contained in the virtual library consisting of various relaxing scenes, formed by various natural environments (e.g., forestal, aquatics, scenarios with animals, adventure, and travel).
Quality assessment checklist
To determine the risk of bias for the selected papers, it has opted to follow the Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions (Lundh and Gøtzsche, 2008; McGuinness and Higgins, 2021; Higgins et al., 2022), using the results of this evaluation as presented in Figure 2. Two studies of our systematic review present a low risk of bias overall (Lundstedt et al., 2021; Yuan et al., 2022), three present a high risk of bias (Appel et al., 2020; Brimelow et al., 2020; Appel et al., 2022) and two present an unclear risk of bias (Moyle et al., 2018; Graf et al., 2020). All the papers present no sufficient information for the “allocation concealment” (D2) and low risk of bias for the “other sources of bias” (D6) shown in Figures 2, 3.
Figure 2. Risk of bias graph based on the Cochrane risk of bias tool (McGuinness and Higgins, 2021).
Figure 3. Risk of bias summary based on the Cochrane risk of bias tool (Higgins et al., 2022).
Results
Summary of findings
Yuan et al. (2022) conducted a field experiment to test the restorative effects of VR-simulated forest environment experiences on older people. They were based on the Attention Restoration Theory (ART) (Kaplan and Kaplan, 1989) and Stress Recovery Theory (SRT) (Ulrich et al., 1991). The authors investigated the different effects of a VR forest environment experience on psychological and physiological improvement in the investigated sample, assessing whether these effects were related to the participant’s different personalities. Finally, they sought to assess whether VR forestry experiences can increase the intention to engage in vivo forestry therapy. The study results showed that a brief experience in a VR forest generates immediate psychological restorative effects in older individuals, such as decreasing negative affect, increasing positive affect and enhancing stress recovery. Despite the initial record of significant psychological improvements at post-test compared with baseline, these effects were not maintained over time unless adequately reinforced. This study has shown that experiences in VR forest environments have greater effects on introverted individuals. Moreover, the study showed that participants had a greater intention to engage in real forest therapy.
Lundstedt et al. (2021) carried out a similar investigation in their study. Data were collected through observation of behavior and verbal expressions during the experiment. Participants showed their liking toward the aesthetic qualities of the environments through VR, as well as a fascination with the capabilities of technology. Negative reactions were recorded, such as anger caused by the desire to go outside and breathe fresh air. Participants showed much interest in virtual reality and the latter was able to promote social interactions among participants. The authors have found most expressions of aesthetic pleasure in relation to VR Island and participants express joy, especially in association theBlue. Residents expressed a fascination for the VR technology (especially in theBlue), the experience of the natural environment, its content and/or the strong feeling of presence. Residents experience mild discomfort when accidentally driving through or too close to objects, or when the water in VR Island appears cold and stormy. Most general positive reactions were expressed in relation to theBlue. Unlike the other studios, in this case the forest setting is contingent on the condition of the VR Island, which, however, contains various settings.
Appel et al. (2020), investigated the possibility of using virtual reality as a therapeutic tool for the older people with impaired sensory, motor, and/or cognitive ability. The results show that the participants recorded increases in calmness, happiness, energy and sense of relaxation following the VR experience. A significant increase was also recorded in feelings of confidence toward VR. Participants reported less anxiety, worry and stress. Negative emotional states included an increase in feelings of fatigue and loneliness following the implementation of the intervention. No side effects resulting from VR exposure, such as nausea or disorientation were recorded; participants were also able to physically explore virtual environments through head and body movements. This study, therefore, validates the hypothesis that immersive VR exposure is a feasible and safe approach to providing beneficial experiences for the older people with motor, sensory or cognitive disabilities.
Similar results were also achieved by the work of Graf et al. (2020). The authors subjected a sample of older people to an intervention implemented through VR, consisting of a virtual forest walk (VR Forest Walking) during which participants had to solve cognitive tasks. The conclusions show that the intervention with VR produced a change in positive affect and negative affect scores, but was not significant as the authors report [e.g., -positive affect: t(13) = −1.6, p = 0.134, d = −0.55; negative affect t(13) = 1.37, p = 0.195, d = −0.22], the medium-sized effect, according the authors, could indicate that this is due to the small sample size (normality has been ensured using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test). Participants’ interest in technology was assessed; participants used the technology naturally and showed a desire to use it again. The participant’s expressions indicated a strong sense of presence that made them feel positive emotions: the participants interacted with the dog and cared for it. The authors have found high levels of flow from participants, but they were not significant. Virtual reality was found to be effective in stimulating the participants’ memory by evoking in them, through images of the forest, joyful memories of their past. The authors therefore found VR to be effective in treating older participants by showing the possibility of expanding the scenarios used during the intervention.
The study by Moyle et al. (2018) aimed to evaluate the effectiveness of a VR intervention of a forest environment on participants’ engagement, apathy and mood states. The results showed that during immersion in the VR forest, participants experienced higher levels of alertness and a higher degree of pleasure. However, the data also showed, at the group level, a greater increase in levels of fear/anxiety during the forest experience compared to the normative sample in an activity setting. The result is, however, influenced by the sample size, which also according to the authors is too small. The authors recorded reductions in apathy levels during the implementation of the experimental condition. However, apathy levels normalized shortly after the intervention ended. This may have induced a possible confounding effect, failing to define whether the reduced levels of apathy during the experience were related to the experience itself or the interaction with the staff.
The article by Brimelow et al. (2020) represents the first contribution to the literature about the feasibility of a VR intervention on a sample of people with varying degrees of cognitive impairment to reduce behavioral and psychological symptoms. Implementation of VR intervention significantly reduced depression from the pre-test to the post-test condition. The intervention was effective in improving the participants’ mood during the sessions. Positive effects were inconsistent across sessions; this could be caused, first, by the habituation that can result from prolonged exposure to VR. Participants showed a good tolerability of the VR intervention.
In their article, Appel et al. (2022) focused on a group of dementia patients admitted to a long-term care center who exhibit reactive behaviors, including those related to physical and emotional pain experience. The authors evaluated the feasibility of VR therapy (consisting of a collection of short 360° VR videos including various nature scenes). These videos were designed for persons with dementia, and the resulting benefits in order to reduce reactive behaviors, including those related to physical and emotional pain. VR therapy was examined both during specific targeted sessions in conjunction with events known to trigger reactive behaviors and during moments of free time. In the targeted sessions, the therapy was effective in reducing reactive behaviors triggered by the environment. The results identified VR therapy as acceptable and enjoyable for patients, showing potential effectiveness in reducing reactive behaviors, assisting in pain management and supporting enjoyment, relaxation and reminiscence.
With reference to five of the articles analyzed (Appel et al., 2020, 2022; Brimelow et al., 2020; Graf et al., 2020; Lundstedt et al., 2021), a comparison between different natural virtual scenarios was not made, but the virtual scenarios employed were used as a whole. Therefore, the results obtained in these studies do not give us specific indications regarding the effectiveness of forest therapy in VR compared to the other natural conditions tested. Rather, they demonstrate the effectiveness of the VR medium implemented with natural environments, but do not allow us to assume the specific benefits of forest therapy in VR.
Discussion
As evidenced by research, life expectancies continue to increase (Mathers et al., 2015), leading to an increase in the number of older people, whose conditions, however, do not always allow them to be independent, resulting in high health and social costs (Kehusmaa et al., 2012). The living conditions of the older people have been the subject of study and attention (e.g., Hellström and Hallberg, 2001; Doblhammer and Ziegler, 2006; Parker and Thorslund, 2007). Forest therapy is an effective therapy in vivo, but little is known about its effectiveness in VR, although it may be beneficial especially for those who cannot go into nature due to their conditions. Therefore, this study sought to investigate evidence in the scientific literature concerning the beneficial effects of VR forest therapy and the forest environment in older people through a systematic review process. Findings confirm the hypothesis of a beneficial effect of forest therapy and forest environments through VR. Such types of studies, still show shortcomings regarding an older and bedridden sample. Aging naturally causes a reduction in the ability to engage in physical activity, especially when aggravated by the presence of one or more disease conditions. According to the results of the analyzed studies, the use of forest therapy with VR would go a long way in decreasing some psycho-physical effects typical of aging and bedridden conditions (e.g., improved mood and reduced stress). The increased length of time these individuals live with the medical condition results in demand for increased health care. Improving the health condition of the older people through this type of intervention can help reduce social costs. Moreover, given the stress-reducing effects of these interventions, they can also be used on the caregivers themselves. The systematic review reveals the need to compare different environments to see which one is more effective in delivering the effects because in many of the studies identified what emerges is only the efficacy of VR per se but is not clear which scenarios are more effective than others, because in some of the studies considered, environments with forest characteristics were presented together with other natural environments, and thus it is not possible to verify whether the results obtained were attributable to forest environments. In students and adults’ samples, however, differences were found depending on the natural environment used in VR (e.g., Theodorou et al., 2023a, b).
It is worth mentioning that VR has also been employed with older people using virtual scenarios other than forest ones, such as beaches, coasts, and underwater, or a distant view of mountains (e.g., Reynolds et al., 2018; Orr et al., 2021). Nonetheless, although VR has been mainly used with older people for therapeutic and rehabilitation purposes (e.g., Rendon et al., 2012; Lee, 2017; Kamińska et al., 2018; Tsao et al., 2019), studies to date that have investigated its use focusing on the beneficial effects of virtual natural environments per se are still scarce. Therefore, due to the different modes in which VR can be employed in older people, it would also be appropriate to understand in future research what the most effective use of this tool on this specific target population could be in order to minimize risks and maximize its beneficial effects.
The present study highlights the need for further research and especially for their standardization. Specifically, the highlighted studies showed heterogeneity in tools used. This heterogeneity can also be found in the type of stimulation (video-derived or software-created forest environment) and instrumentation used to reproduce the stimulation. Standardization would allow us to adopt the best device possible to achieves better results in improving older people’s psycho-physical well-being.
Limitations, strengths, and future perspectives
The study is not without limitations. The first one concerns the low number of studies using forest-related technology with older people samples. In addition, many of these studies have a numerically small sample size. Nonetheless, the difficulty of obtaining high samples for this age group should be considered, due in part to the necessary screening (unimpaired vision and hearing, etc.). These limitations highlight the need for further studies, with larger samples. A general limitation of these studies may also be the heterogeneity of the type of instrumentation used and the type of forest setting and outcomes analyzed. In this regard the use of PSDs (perceived sensory dimensions) could be of help in order to standardize forest images and videos (Grahn and Stigsdotter, 2010). Other limitation pertains to the lack of verification of long-term effects. Future studies to investigate more thoroughly the temporal impact of such interventions are needed. Another limitation consists of publication bias of positive results The strengths of this systematic review are that, at present, this is the first systematic review investigating state of the art concerning the use of forest environments through VR on an older population. A further strength lies in the fact that this systematic review highlights the need to study the differences between different types of natural environments in a sample of older people in greater depth. Our work has shown that for this type of sample, studies using forest therapy through VR are scarce, and some of them do not only use forest therapy, but several natural environments together, making it impossible to understand the real benefits of the specific natural environment. Given the specific population, future studies should also attend to the impact of medications taken by patients, and specific psycho-physical conditions. Regarding bias, only two articles were identified by the authors of this systematic review as having a low risk of bias. This finding highlights the need for more methodologically sound studies.
Finally, the results of this systematic review shed light on the relevance of further investigating the topic by also publishing and disseminating those studies whose results are negative or non-significant. Focusing only on positive and significant results might increase the risk of publication bias (Dickersin and Min, 1993), resulting in the skewing of overall conclusions by overestimating positive effects and, at the same time, underestimating the negative ones (Dwan et al., 2008; Mueller et al., 2013). Although a careful examination of the gray literature was conducted, no further eligible studies emerged through the keywords adopted. Nonetheless, especially with this target population, addressing studies’ negative and/or non-significant results with the VR medium would allow the set of increasingly safe and precise interventions, providing more robust benefits for their health and well-being.
Conclusion
The studies analyzed in this systematic review highlight the usefulness of this type of intervention combining forest setting and VR, where the latter serves as a medium for transmitting the beneficial effects of the former. Although not all studies show significant effects in all outcomes, the effects found would seem sufficiently encouraging. The use of natural environments, and those of forest nature more specifically, contribute to improving psycho-physical well-being. However, due to the in vivo nature of such treatments, this type of intervention is not accessible to all types of patients. The contribution of virtual reality in this regard, given its immersive experience, makes up for this gap and allows exposure to natural environments even for people who are bedridden or have motor difficulties, especially the older people. This systematic review highlighted the need to design further studies having a more structured, rigorous and standardized methodology. More specifically, future studies should recruit well-powered samples including a control group and testing long term effects of exposure to nature through VR. The effects of this type of intervention would have not only benefits on direct recipients but also on indirect recipients (caregivers, nurses, etc.), leading to a reduction in social costs.
Data availability statement
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.
Author contributions
DC: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. LR: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. EZ: Investigation, Writing – original draft. GC: Writing – review & editing. AP: Supervision, Writing - review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was carried out under the project “Establishing Urban FORest based solution in Changing Cities” (EUFORICC) and financially supported by the Ministry of Education, University and Research (MIUR) of Italy (PRIN 20173RRN2S).
Conflict of interest
The reviewer GS declared a past co-authorship with the author AP to the handling editor.
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Footnotes
References
Annerstedt, M., Jönsson, P., Wallergård, M., Johansson, G., Karlson, B., Grahn, P., et al. (2013). Inducing physiological stress recovery with sounds of nature in a virtual reality forest—results from a pilot study. Physiol. Behav. 118, 240–250. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.05.023
*Appel, L., Appel, E., Bogler, O., Wiseman, M., Cohen, L., Ein, N., et al. (2020). Older adults with cognitive and/or physical impairments can benefit from immersive virtual reality experiences: a feasibility study. Front. Med. 6:329. doi: 10.3389/fmed.2019.00329
*Appel, L., Appel, E., Kisonas, E., Lewis, S., and Sheng, L. Q. (2022). Virtual reality for veteran relaxation: can VR therapy help veterans living with dementia who exhibit responsive behaviors?. Front. Virt. Real. 2:724020. doi: 10.3389/frvir.2021.724020
Arean, P., Hegel, M., Vannoy, S., Fan, M. Y., and Unuzter, J. (2008). Effectiveness of problem-solving therapy for older, primary care patients with depression: results from the IMPACT project. Gerontologist 48, 311–323. doi: 10.1093/geront/48.3.311
Baker, S., Waycott, J., Robertson, E., Carrasco, R., Neves, B. B., Hampson, R., et al. (2020). Evaluating the use of interactive virtual reality technology with older adults living in residential aged care. Inf. Process. Manag. 57:102105. doi: 10.1016/j.ipm.2019.102105
*Brimelow, R. E., Dawe, B., and Dissanayaka, N. (2020). Preliminary research: virtual reality in residential aged care to reduce apathy and improve mood. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 23, 165–170. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2019.0286
Buedo-Guirado, C., Rubio, L., Dumitrache, C. G., and Romero-Coronado, J. (2020). Active aging program in nursing homes: effects on psychological well-being and life satisfaction. Psychosoc. Interv. 29, 49–57. doi: 10.5093/pi2019a18
Chae, Y., Lee, S., Jo, Y., Kang, S., Park, S., and Kang, H. (2021). The effects of forest therapy on immune function. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:8440. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18168440
Crews, D. E., and Zavotka, S. (2006). Aging, disability, and frailty: implications for universal design. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 25, 113–118. doi: 10.2114/jpa2.25.113
Cuijpers, P., van Straten, A., and Smit, F. (2006). Psychological treatment of late-life depression: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 21, 1139–1149. doi: 10.1002/gps.1620
Dickersin, K., and Min, Y. I. (1993). NIH clinical trials and publication bias. Online J. Curr. Clin. Trials 4967 words; 53 paragraphs
Doblhammer, G., and Ziegler, U. (2006). “Future elderly living conditions in Europe: demographic insights” in Gender, health and ageing: European perspectives on life course, health issues and social challenges, eds. G. M. Backes, V. Lasch, and K. Reimann (Wiesbaden: VS Verlag für Sozialwissenschaften), 267–292. doi: 10.1007/978-3-531-90355-2_13
Dwan, K., Altman, D. G., Arnaiz, J. A., Bloom, J., Chan, A. W., Cronin, E., et al. (2008). Systematic review of the empirical evidence of study publication bias and outcome reporting bias. PLoS One 3:e3081. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003081
El-Khoury, F., Cassou, B., Charles, M. A., and Dargent-Molina, P. (2015). The effect of fall prevention exercise programmes on fall induced injuries in community dwelling older adults. Br. J. Sports Med. 49:1348. doi: 10.1136/bmj.f6234
Graf, L., Liszio, S., and Masuch, M. (2020). “Playing in virtual nature,” in Proceedings of the Conference on Mensch Und Computer (MuC ’20), (New York, NY, USA: Association for Computing Machinery), 155–164. doi: 10.1145/3404983.3405507
Grahn, P., and Stigsdotter, U. K. (2010). The relation between perceived sensory dimensions of urban green space and stress restoration. Landsc. Urban Plan. 94, 264–275. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2009.10.012
Hansen, M. M., Jones, R., and Tocchini, K. (2017). Shinrin-Yoku (Forest bathing) and nature therapy: a state-of-the-art review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 14:851. doi: 10.3390/ijerph14080851
Hellström, Y., and Hallberg, I. R. (2001). Perspectives of elderly people receiving home help on health, care and quality of life. Health Soc. Care Community 9, 61–71. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2524.2001.00282.x
Higgins, J. P. T., Thomas, J., Chandler, J., Cumpston, M., Li, T., Page, M. J., et al. (Eds.) Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions version 6.3 (updated February 2022). Cochrane (2022). Available at: www.training.cochrane.org/handbook
Jung, W. H., Woo, J. M., and Ryu, J. S. (2015). Effect of a forest therapy program and the forest environment on female workers’ stress. Urban For. Urban Green. 14, 274–281. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2015.02.004
Kamińska, M. S., Miller, A., Rotter, I., Szylińska, A., and Grochans, E. (2018). The effectiveness of virtual reality training in reducing the risk of falls among elderly people. Clin. Interv. Aging 13, 2329–2338. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S183502
Kaplan, R., and Kaplan, S. (1989). The experience of nature: a psychological perspective. New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press.
Kehusmaa, S., Autti-Rämö, I., Helenius, H., Hinkka, K., Valaste, M., and Rissanen, P. (2012). Factors associated with the utilization and costs of health and social services in frail elderly patients. BMC Health Serv. Res. 12:204. doi: 10.1186/1472-6963-12-204
Kotte, D., Li, Q., Shin, W. S., and Michalsen, A. (2019). International handbook of forest therapy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.
Kowal, P., and Dowd, J. E. (2001). Definition of an older person. Proposed working definition of an older person in Africa for the MDS Project. Geneva, CH: World Health Organization.
Lee, G. H. (2017). Effects of a virtual reality exercise program (Wii) on cognitive function of elderly people with Alzheimer dementia. Offic. J. Korean Acad. Kinesiol. 19, 35–44. doi: 10.15758/jkak.2017.19.1.35
Li, Q., and Kawada, T. (2011). Effect of forest therapy on the human psycho-neuro-endocrino-immune network. Nihon eiseigaku zasshi. Japanese. J. Hyg. 66, 645–650. doi: 10.1265/jjh.66.645
Li, C., Sun, C., Sun, M., Yuan, Y., and Li, P. (2020). Effects of brightness levels on stress recovery when viewing a virtual reality forest with simulated natural light. Urban For. Urban Green. 56:126865. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2020.126865
Lundh, A., and Gøtzsche, P. C. (2008). Recommendations by Cochrane review groups for assessment of the risk of bias in studies. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 8:22. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-8-22
*Lundstedt, R., Håkansson, C., Lõhmus, M., and Wallergård, M. (2021). Designing virtual natural environments for older adults in residential care facilities. Technol. Disabil. 33, 305–318. doi: 10.3233/TAD-210344
Mathers, C. D., Stevens, G. A., Boerma, T., White, R. A., and Tobias, M. I. (2015). Causes of international increases in older age life expectancy. Lancet 385, 540–548. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(14)60569-9
Mattila, O., Korhonen, A., Pöyry, E., Hauru, K., Holopainen, J., and Parvinen, P. (2020). Restoration in a virtual reality forest environment. Comput. Hum. Behav. 107:106295. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2020.106295
McGuinness, L. A., and Higgins, J. P. (2021). Risk-of-bias VISualization (robvis): an R package and shiny web app for visualizing risk-of-bias assessments. Res. Synth. Methods 12, 55–61. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.1411
*Moyle, W., Jones, C., Dwan, T., and Petrovich, T. (2018). Effectiveness of a virtual reality forest on people with dementia: a mixed methods pilot study. Gerontologist 58, 478–487. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnw270
Mueller, K. F., Meerpohl, J. J., Briel, M., Antes, G., Elm, E. V., Lang, B., et al. (2013). Detecting, quantifying and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analyses: protocol of a systematic review on methods. Syst. Rev. 2, 1–4. doi: 10.1186/2046-4053-2-60
Murray, C. J., Barber, R. M., Foreman, K. J., Ozgoren, A. A., Abd-Allah, F., Abera, S. F., et al. (2015). Global, regional, and national disability-adjusted life years (DALYs) for 306 diseases and injuries and healthy life expectancy (HALE) for 188 countries, 1990–2013: quantifying the epidemiological transition. Lancet 386, 2145–2191. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)61340-X
Orr, N., Yeo, N. L., Dean, S. G., White, M. P., and Garside, R. (2021). “It makes you feel that you are there”: exploring the acceptability of virtual reality nature environments for people with memory loss. Geriatrics 6:27. doi: 10.3390/geriatrics6010027
Parker, M. G., and Thorslund, M. (2007). Health trends in the elderly population: getting better and getting worse. The Gerontologist 47, 150–158. doi: 10.1093/geront/47.2.150
Rajoo, K. S., Karam, D. S., and Abdullah, M. Z. (2020). The physiological and psychosocial effects of forest therapy: a systematic review. Urban For. Urban Green. 54:126744. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2020.126744
Reese, G., Stahlberg, J., and Menzel, C. (2022). Digital shinrin-yoku: do nature experiences in virtual reality reduce stress and increase well-being as strongly as similar experiences in a physical forest? Virt. Real. 26, 1245–1255. doi: 10.1007/s10055-022-00631-9
Rendon, A. A., Lohman, E. B., Thorpe, D., Johnson, E. G., Medina, E., and Bradley, B. (2012). The effect of virtual reality gaming on dynamic balance in older adults. Age Ageing 41, 549–552. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afs053
Reynolds, L., Rodiek, S., Lininger, M., and McCulley, M. A. (2018). Can a virtual nature experience reduce anxiety and agitation in people with dementia? J. Hous. Elder. 32, 176–193. doi: 10.1080/02763893.2018.1431583
Sollami, A., Gianferrari, E., Alfieri, M., Artioli, G., and Taffurelli, C. (2017). Pet therapy: an effective strategy to care for the elderly? An experimental study in a nursing home. Acta Bio Medica: Atenei Parmensis 88, 25–31. doi: 10.23750/abm.v88i1-S.6281
Song, C., Ikei, H., and Miyazaki, Y. (2016). Physiological effects of nature therapy: a review of the research in Japan. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 13:781. doi: 10.3390/ijerph13080781
Spano, G., Theodorou, A., Reese, G., Carrus, G., Sanesi, G., and Panno, A. (2023). Virtual nature, psychological and psychophysiological outcomes: A systematic review. J Environ Psychol 89, 102–144. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2023.102044
Stanney, K. M., and Zyda, M. (2002). Virtual environments in the 21st century., in Handbook of virtual environments: Design, implementation, and applications, eds. K. S. Hale and K. M. Stanney (New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.), 1–14.
Sung, J., Woo, J. M., Kim, W., Lim, S. K., and Chung, E. J. (2012). The effect of cognitive behavior therapy-based “forest therapy” program on blood pressure, salivary cortisol level, and quality of life in elderly hypertensive patients. Clin. Exp. Hypertens. 34, 1–7. doi: 10.3109/10641963.2011.618195
Theodorou, A., Romano, L., Bratman, G. N., Carbone, G. A., Rodelli, R., Casagrande, G., et al. (2023a). Different types of virtual natural environments enhance subjective vitality through restorativeness. J Environ Psychol. 87, 101–981. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2023.101981
Theodorou, A., Spano, G., Bratman, G. N., Monneron, K., Sanesi, G., Carrus, G., et al. (2023b). Emotion regulation and virtual nature: cognitive reappraisal as an individual-level moderator for impacts on subjective vitality. Sci Rep 13, 5028. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-30287-7
Tsao, Y. C., Shu, C. C., and Lan, T. S. (2019). Development of a reminiscence therapy system for the elderly using the integration of virtual reality and augmented reality. Sustainability 11:4792. doi: 10.3390/su11174792
Uijen, A. A., and van de Lisdonk, E. H. (2008). Multimorbidity in primary care: prevalence and trend over the last 20 years. Eur. J. Gen. Pract. 14, 28–32. doi: 10.1080/13814780802436093
Ulrich, R. S., Simons, R. F., Losito, B. D., Fiorito, E., Miles, M. A., and Zelson, M. (1991). Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 11, 201–230. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(05)80184-7
Vancampfort, D., Stubbs, B., and Koyanagi, A. (2017). Physical chronic conditions, multimorbidity and sedentary behavior amongst middle-aged and older adults in six low-and middle-income countries. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 14, 1–13. doi: 10.1186/s12966-017-0602-z
Woodford, H. J., and Fisher, J. (2019). New horizons in deprescribing for older people. Age Ageing 48, 768–775. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afz109
Yu, C. P. S., and Hsieh, H. (2020). Beyond restorative benefits: evaluating the effect of forest therapy on creativity. Urban For. Urban Green. 51:126670. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2020.126670
Yu, C. P., Lee, H. Y., and Luo, X. Y. (2018). The effect of virtual reality forest and urban environments on physiological and psychological responses. Urban For. Urban Green. 35, 106–114. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2018.08.013
*Yuan, S., Tao, F., and Li, Y. (2022). The restorative effects of virtual reality forests on elderly individuals during the COVID-19 lockdown. J. Organ. End User Comput. 34, 1–22. doi: 10.4018/JOEUC.297626
Keywords: virtual reality, forest therapy, virtual natural environment, virtual forest environment, exposure to nature, older people, natural environment, virtual environment
Citation: Clemente D, Romano L, Zamboni E, Carrus G and Panno A (2024) Forest therapy using virtual reality in the older population: a systematic review. Front. Psychol. 14:1323758. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1323758
Edited by:
Francesco Aletta, University College London, United KingdomReviewed by:
Giuseppina Spano, University of Bari Aldo Moro, ItalyAndrea Bosco, University of Bari Aldo Moro, Italy
Copyright © 2024 Clemente, Romano, Zamboni, Carrus and Panno. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Davide Clemente, dott.davideclemente@gmail.com;davide.clemente@unier.it; Angelo Panno, angelo.panno@unier.it