- 1Divison of Mental Health & Addiction, Vestold Hospital Trust, Tønsberg, Norway
- 2Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
- 3Faculty of Teacher Education Arts and Sports, Western Norway University of Applied Sciences, Sogndal, Norway
- 4Department of Education and Special Education, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
Introduction: “The moderate brain arousal model” claims that white noise improves attention by optimizing brain arousal. We analyze Conners’ Continuous Performance Test-3 (CCPT-3) performance, expecting to find reduced reaction time variability with noise mediated by decrease under long event-rates and in later parts of the test, indicating that noise reverse fall in phasic and tonic arousal.
Methods: Sixty-five children with high or lower ADHD-symptoms from a child psychiatric unit, succeeded to complete the CCPT-3 with and without white noise.
Results: Noise reduced overall variability, improved performance in later parts of the test, and reduced response variability under the longest event rate particularly in the high symptoms group. No overall change in omissions and commissions, but the high symptoms group made fewer omissions during noise compared the low symptom group.
Discussion: The study indicates an arousal effect of noise but should be replicated with other noise variants and amplitudes to improve effect and compliance.
Introduction
Several studies have shown that auditory “white noise” can extend attention span or improve working memory (Helps et al., 2014; Söderlund and Nilsson Jobs, 2016) and episodic learning (Söderlund et al., 2007, 2010) in children with attention deficits and/or ADHD. The same level of auditory stimulation will, however, lead to decreased performance in children with normal or good attention (Söderlund et al., 2007, 2010; Helps et al., 2014). Departing from the observation that all organisms perform best under optimal stimulation levels, “the moderate brain arousal model” (MBA model; Sikström and Söderlund, 2007) claims that white noise can increase the target stimulation level through the phenomenon of stochastic resonance, where weak signals can be reinforced by the presentation of random white noise together with the target signal. Apart from specifying the phenomenon of stochastic resonance, the predictions about white noise benefit are similar to the claim by arousal models of ADHD that the brain arousal necessary for successful task completion among children with ADHD may lead to over-arousal in typically developing children, resulting in lower performance (Sonuga-Barke et al., 2010; Martella et al., 2020).
Over the past decades, several researchers have suggested that ADHD is associated with a hypo-aroused brain state (Clarke et al., 2020), which causes compensatory motor hyperactivity and slow and variable response patterns observed on sustained attention tasks (Geissler et al., 2014). Hyperactivity is then considered to be an autoregulatory response to a hypo-aroused state (Hegerl and Hensch, 2014). Pribram and McGuinness (1975) were the first to distinguish between phasic and tonic arousal level and point to state regulation impairments as important to understand attention disorders. In ADHD, the tonic and phasic regulation of dopamine in the brain has been especially studied, as tonic levels of dopamine are assumed to be low (Volkow et al., 2009), and that low tonic DA leads to higher phasic release (Seeman and Madras, 2002) in accordance with the tonic-phasic model of dopamine signaling (Grace, 1991). The MBA model thus posits that people with ADHD are more dependent on externally evoked phasic dopamine to raise arousal to levels effective for performance (Ziegler et al., 2016). Central stimulating agents and white noise are both possible ways to increase phasic dopamine and thus increase performance under optimal stimulation and decrease performance when leading to over-arousal (Del Campo et al., 2011).
Behavioral correlates to these concepts were further developed in the cognitive energetic model (CEM) of ADHD (Sergeant, 2005), which, similarly to the MBA model, claims that children with ADHD have an insufficient regulation of brain states. In CEM (Sergeant, 2005), the phasic response to stimulation, influenced by signal intensity and novelty, is called the arousal pool, while the tonic level of arousal is influenced by the activity level of the subject and is called the activation pool (Martella et al., 2020). Varying event rates, i.e., stimulation level in Continuous Performance Tests (CCPTs), is a way to manipulate arousal, while the activity level during the task may exhaust or increase central nervous tonic energy resources (Martella et al., 2020). Thus, Loo et al. (2009) considered the quantitative EEG activation change from eyes closed to eyes open as a change in phasic arousal, while activity change due to responding in the continuous performance test was considered as activation mediated, i.e., measure tonic arousal. Loo et al. (2009) found that adults with ADHD required more cortical arousal in order to sit still in front of the computer and that successful performance on the test demanded more tonic arousal.
Loo et al. (2009), Zhou and Luo (2022), and Lin (2022) are three of the few researchers explicitly linking predictions from the MBA model and the CEM. Lin (2022) found that preschoolers with ADHD normalized their overall performance under white noise conditions on the Conners’ Kiddie Continuous Performance Test. To our knowledge, however, no study has actually tested whether noise benefits could be linked to presumed arousal or activation improvement in sustaining attention. Apart from the new Lin’s (2022) study, the beneficial effect of noise has previously been tested in relatively short intervals, for instance, for the duration of a short memory test with and without noise. More studies are needed to conclude a beneficial effect of working with concomitant noise over a longer period of time (i.e., 14 min), and we need to know whether a beneficial effect can be interpreted in the terms posited by activation models of ADHD, i.e., that improvement is due to normalization of their state regulation deficit during noise.
The most sensitive measure of sustaining attention in tests of sustained attention is reaction time variability or the standard deviation of the reaction time (Kofler et al., 2013; Salum et al., 2019; Pagán et al., 2022). Reaction time variability is particularly sensitive to ADHD by measuring occasional lapses in attention and/or failure to sustain attention that are not severe enough to result in omission errors (Huang-Pollock et al., 2012; Tamm et al., 2012). Reaction time variability is also shown to be most sensitive to central stimulating medication (Levy et al., 2018; Fredriksen et al., 2021). While the standard deviation of the RT is a gross measure of overall variation during the test, the Conners’ Continuous Performance test, version 3 (CCPT-3; Conners, 2014) also offers other measures of variability in performance. The test differentiates between three levels of stimulation intensity, i.e., stimuli might appear with 1, 2, or 4 s of inter-stimulus intervals (ISIs). Performance under varying event rates may serve as an operationalization of phasic arousal effects in the sense that longer ISIs represent a less arousing environment, causing under-arousal in ADHD. A series of studies have found the ISI effect to be the most specific measure of ADHD-related inattention in CCPT (Egeland et al., 2009, 2012; Miranda et al., 2012; Udal et al., 2014; Chiang et al., 2015). Persons with ADHD are found to be performing less impaired in the high-intensive condition with one-second ISI’s, while persons with inattention due to other disorders such as schizophrenia typically perform best under low-intensive stimulation, i.e., 4-s ISI (Egeland, 2012; López-Luengo et al., 2016; Lundervold et al., 2016). The findings by Epstein et al. (2011) sum up the effects described above by finding no medication effect for the mean reaction time nor for omission errors, but that reaction time variability and particularly the coefficient of variation was sensitive to medication and incentives and specifically under low event rates (i.e., long ISI’s).
Another measure of intra-test variability is the block-change measure. It refers to the slope of change in reaction time as a function of time on task, and it serves as an operationalization of tonic arousal changes. Even though many subjects with ADHD have problems attending from the first part of the test, several studies have also found a decay in performance, satisfying the criteria for a specific deficit in sustained attention (Egeland and Kowalik-Gran, 2010; Zane et al., 2016).
As variability scores have emerged as the most sensitive measure of ADHD-related attention deficit, researchers have pointed out that they are not normally distributed. Lapses in attention contribute to long reaction times, but there will be few short reaction times deviating equivalently from the mean. Thus, analyses applying ex-Gaussian methods have become increasingly popular. The ex-Gaussian distribution is an estimated distribution combining a normal distribution and an exponential distribution (Luce, 1986; Heathcote et al., 1991), where mu and sigma represent the mean and the standard deviation of the normal distribution, respectively. Tau describes the mean and the standard deviation of the exponential distribution. This latter measure seems to be a more sensitive measure of RT variability, found to be higher in individuals with ADHD than in healthy controls (Bella-Fernández et al., 2023). Comparing subjects with ADHD and typically developing children, Gu et al. (2013) and Hwang-Gu et al. (2019) found deviating Tau-values both for longer ISIs and later time blocks in ADHD, summing up that their findings reflected an energetic problem in ADHD. If this is so, and noise reduces the energetic impairment, there should be a disproportional improvement by noise on ISI and block change. This is the topic of the present study.
In the present study, we analyze the effect of white noise in CCPT-3 (Conners, 2014). White noise is a random signal with a flat power spectrum having equal intensity over the entire frequency band. According to stochastic resonance, this would mean that white noise, as opposed to other types of noise, increases the neural signal-to-noise ratio equally in the entire spectrum of frequencies that human ears can encode. The white noise signal is considered to interact with the target signal in the narrow time window when neurons are close to the threshold of eliciting an action potential. The noise thus pushes the signal over the threshold, i.e., resonance.
We have applied the same high level of noise usually applied in studies of clinical samples, although most of these studies deliver the noise for shorter periods of time. One study of neurotypical adults suggested that the same level of noise that was optimal for working memory tasks was too high for sustained attention (Awada et al., 2022). This study, however, was performed on neurotypical adults, where we generally do not expect the positive effects of noise.
Summing up, the MBA model of Sikström and Söderlund (2007) resembles the CEM of ADHD (Martella et al., 2020) by emphasizing the value of optimal arousal level. White noise is found to increase performance in many cognitive tasks, and we expect it to also improve sustained attention. A group of drug-naive children, referred for neuropsychological assessment of attention deficits, is tested with CCPT-3 two times: with or without white noise. We expect (1) better performance under white noise conditions evident in a reduction of variability of response time, i.e., smaller standard deviations of reaction times and a smaller between-block variability. (2) We further expect that the improvement is related to arousal, i.e., that the improvement will be specific to the low-intensity stimulation part of the test and the latter part of the test. We divide participants into those having high or low ADHD symptoms. We have no hypothesis of significant group differences as both groups display attention problems, but we expect that the ISI and block effects will be mainly driven by those with higher scores of ADHD symptoms.
Methods
Participants
Children successively referred for neuropsychological assessment in the Child Psychiatric Clinic of Vestfold Hospital Trust, Norway, were invited to take part in the study. The children were referred on the suspicion of problems with attention, executive function, tics, specific learning disorders or dyslexia. A total of 83 children accepted to take part. If the computer scoring program for CCPT-3 did not yield a measure of variability, the test was not considered to be valid. This means that the participants had to have at least three responses to stimuli in each of the 18 sub-parts of the test. In total, four of the subjects did not perform the CCPT validly without noise (three with no noise as the first condition), 10 subjects declined to start the session with noise, and eight subjects starting the session with noise had periods without responding, resulting in no variability measure computed. They were, therefore, considered as drop-outs (all eight had noise as the second condition). In total, 65 participants performed both test sessions validly. Whether starting with noise or without was randomized, but in the final sample of 65 completers, 53% had started without noise and 47% with noise.
The project was approved by the Regional Ethics Committee (2018/446–3) and by the Data Protection Officer of Vestfold Hospital Trust. The data are stored on a secure research server of Vestfold Hospital Trust but can be made available on demand by e-mail to forskning@siv.no. The research was funded by the Norwegian Research Network on ADHD, grant number 18/09147–5.
Of note, 36 boys and 29 girls participated. The mean age was 11.9 years, ranging from 8 to 18 years of age (SD = 2.9).
All data, except for the CCPT-3, were taken from patient file records. Patients were examined with somewhat different scales of attention and psychiatric symptoms. Participants with a T-score above 65 on the Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach, 1991) DSM ADHD scale were considered to have significant ADHD symptoms. A total of 38 participants scored above that level, while 20 scored below, and CBCL were lacking for seven participants due to parents not being able to fill out a Norwegian questionnaire or due to technical errors (failure in scanning the correct pages into patient files). For four of those lacking CBCL, we used the comparable Achenbach’s Teacher Report Form (TRF) filled out by their teachers (Achenbach, 1991). The remaining three were classified into significant ADHD scores or not based on their ADHD Rating Scale, version 4 (ARS-4; DuPaul et al., 1998) scores.
Table 1 shows clinical data differentiating those classified as below or above the cut-off for significant ADHD symptoms. On CBCL, the two groups differed with regard to symptoms of ADHD, ODD, and CD. On TRF, they differed only with regard to the level of ADHD symptoms. On Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF), they differed with regard to inhibition but not with regard to working memory, indicating an impairment in both the ADHD group and the non-ADHD group consisting of clinical cases with other diagnoses expected to also score high on WM. In the same vein, ARS showed no difference in the Inattentive scale but a significant difference regarding the hyperactivity/impulsivity scale. In the forthcoming analyses on CCPT-3 performance, raw scores will be analyzed, but for the information of the reader, the T-scores of the two subgroups are reported here. The two groups differed with regard to the number of omissions, the standard deviations of hit reaction time, and variability. The sample did not differ with regard to ISI change.
Table 1. Clinical data describing those classified below or above the cut-off for significant ADHD symptoms (N = 65).
Instruments
Conners’ continuous performance test, version 3
All participants were tested with CCPT-3 (Conners, 2014) two times, with or without concomitant white noise. In the CCPT-3, the participant is exposed to 360 sequential exposures of letters on a computer screen and is asked to respond as quickly as possible to all letters except X’es, i.e., to 80% of all exposures. After qualifying in a 1-min training session, the entire test lasts for an additional 14 min, with letters being exposed to three different ISIs of 1, 2, or 4 s. For computing changes over time, the test is divided into six time blocks, which then are further subdivided into blocks varying in the three different ISIs, altogether 18 sub-blocks.
A number of omission errors, the standard deviation of the reaction time (hit RT s.d.) and variability are considered most sensitive to overall attention in the test (Egeland and Kowalik-Gran, 2010). While the hit RT s.d. is computed from all correct responses to target items, the variability score is computed as the variation in reaction time between the 18 sub-blocks. Commission errors are the number of false-positive responses to X’es. Block change is a regression factor showing the slope of change in reaction time as time on task. The ISI change is the slope of change in reaction time over the three ISIs. All variability measures are computed without logarithmic transformation.
Since we are particularly interested in the ISI effect, we compare noise-related changes in omission errors to split up into ISI conditions. We report all three ex-Gaussian measures but are expecting only the Tau value to be related to noise effects. This measure is split up into ISI conditions to check for differences in the exponential curve of reaction time by ISI and noise. Due to data loss, the ex-Gaussian measures were computed from 64 rather than 65 persons.
Rating scales
Parents and teachers completed the respective Achenbach scales, namely the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) for parents and the Teacher Report Scale for teachers (Achenbach, 1991). These checklists provide scores for different child psychiatric symptoms as well as DSM-diagnosis-oriented scales for ADHD. The Norwegian version has shown good reliability and validity (Nøvik, 2000). We also report scores for the ARS-4 (DuPaul et al., 1998) and from the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) parent version (Fallmyr and Egeland, 2011). This inventory gives scores for nine scales related to executive function in daily life, where high scores on impulsivity and working memory are typical for ADHD. Four scales measuring aspects of behavior regulation (including impulsivity) are merged into a composite index for behavior regulation, while the remaining five scales give a metacognition index score (among them, working memory). See Table 1, for figures.
Procedure
The noise was delivered binaurally through high-quality headphones (Sennheiser model 201). After testing the noise set to 80 dB, we allowed for lowering the noise to 70 dB if the children preferred so. White noise in the present study was a 15-min recording from an old TV set that was not receiving any signal; this noise was then looped and run on the test computer. Most participants were randomized into either noise or no-noise first, but in some cases, the CCPT-3 had been performed as part of an initial clinical examination prior to joining the study, and a decision was made not to repeat the no-noise condition. The two CPT sessions were part of a more extended assessment, including WISC-V and test screening for language impairments. In all together 21 cases, a decision was made to divide the testing into two sessions on 2 days. For 44 participants, both CCPT test sessions were performed on the same day, but the children were offered a 15-min break before starting session 2. All participants were tested individually in a silent room alone together with qualified staff personnel (coauthors I.K.G and AKAa).
Statistical analyses
Data were analyzed with paired-sample t-tests comparing noise and no-noise conditions. Since we had an expectation of improvement by white noise, a one-tailed t-test with an alpha level of <0.05 was applied. Ex-Gaussian parameters were estimated from the correct responses to target items for each participant, using R Statistical Software (v4.2.1; R Core Team, 2022) and the R Package retimes (Massidda, 2013). Parameters for Mu, Sigma, and Tau are reported for the whole test, and we also report parameters for Tau from each ISI condition. Power analyses showed that a sample of 65 would yield 80% power to detect small effects exceeding Cohen’s d of 0.26. We had no firm hypothesis of significant group differences but expected the group with high ADHD symptoms to improve somewhat more from noise. Thus, the data file was split into low- and high-ADHD symptom scores and the analyses were repeated in each of these subgroups.
Results
Since we analyzed variability measures that were not log-transformed, we checked the kurtosis and skewness of these parameters. Skewness varied from 0.78 for variability in the no-noise condition to 1.49 for the ISI change in the noise condition. Kurtosis varied from −0.30 for variability in the no-noise condition to 3.06 for block change in the no-noise condition. The latter value indicates a small variation.
Table 2 shows the performance of the whole group split up with regard to low or high ADHD symptoms. For the whole sample, there was a positive effect of noise for all variability measures (i.e., the standard deviation of the reaction time, block change and ISI change and variability). There were no differences between the number of omissions and commissions, but splitting up into ISIs, there was a decrease in omissions under noise conditions for the one-second ISI. Analyzing the ex-Gaussian measures showed an overall effect of reduced Tau due to noise but not with regard to Mu and Sigma. Splitting up into ISI showed that the noise effect on Tau was driven by the significant reduction of response time variability in the 4-s ISI condition.
Table 2. CCPT-3 performance without and with white noise for the whole sample and split with regard to low−/high-ADHD symptoms.
Splitting up into low and high ADHD symptoms showed reduced standard deviation of the reaction time, improved block change, and reduced number of omissions in the ISI 1-s condition in the noise condition only in the group with high ADHD symptoms. While there was no overall reduction in omissions due to noise, the splitting into two groups showed that the high-ADHD-symptom group had a significant reduction in omissions, while the low-ADHD-symptom group had a numeric increase in omissions. This group × noise interaction was significant (F = 4.32, p = 0.042, Eta2 = 0.064).
The overall differences in Tau and specifically in the ISI 4-s condition were driven by the high-ADHD-symptom group, showing significantly reduced variance in the noise condition.
Discussion
The study aimed to test whether white noise could have a beneficent arousing effect on performance in a continuous performance test of attention. We had two sets of explicit hypotheses and one additional expectation. First, we hypothesized that overall variability would be reduced by white noise. This was confirmed by the finding that both the standard deviation of the reaction time and time-block variability were improved under noise conditions. This is in line with previous research showing that variability measures are more sensitive to inattention than manifest errors of omissions and commissions (Huang-Pollock et al., 2012). Analyzing the total sample, there were no changes in overall omissions and commission errors, but a significant reduction by noise was found in the subgroup with high ADHD symptoms.
Our second hypothesis was that the overall improvement by noise would be mediated by changes, especially under long event rates and later time blocks. Event rates represent different short-term changes in the intensity of external stimulation, considered to be a proxy of changes in phasic dopaminergic neuromodulation, previously found to be improved by white noise (Rausch et al., 2014). The fall under the no-noise condition in sustaining performance over time is related to a fall in tonic arousal level, as shown by Shalev and Nobre (2022), taking long-scale pupil dynamics to measure tonic arousal. The hypothesis was confirmed in the sense that white noise improved performance under the longest event rate but not under the shortest and the intermediate ISI and that the block-change measure showed an increase in performance due to changes in the later parts of the test. Thus, the improvement reduced the low arousal deficit characteristic of ADHD according to the CEM (Hwang-Gu et al., 2019) and the MBA model (Sikström and Söderlund, 2007; Söderlund et al., 2016).
We had no hypothesis regarding significant differences between the high- and low-ADHD-symptom groups, as both groups included clinical subjects referred for assessment of potential attention deficits. However, we expected the arousal-related performance effect to be mostly driven by those with high ADHD symptoms, and this was confirmed in that the effect sizes were numerically higher in this group. In fact, we also found a double dissociation with regard to omission errors, as they improved in the high-ADHD symptom group but decayed under noise conditions in the low-ADHD symptom group.
One finding that did not fit the expectations is the finding that the subjects with high-ADHD symptoms had fewer omissions in the 1-s ISI and that Tau showed less variability of reaction time also under the 1-s ISI. The effect size of the latter finding was, however, clearly lower than the effect under 4-s ISI. The potential for a reduction in omissions was larger in the 1-s ISI, but we acknowledge that it is not as expected.
The effect sizes of noise were mostly small to medium. It is also important to stress that the noise condition was unpleasant for several children, in the sense that some children refused to take the test or quit during the noise condition. Thus, we are hesitant to conclude that the noise effects shown in this study could be regarded as an effective tool for improving the attention of those with high ADHD symptoms.
On the other hand, this is the first study to find a significant association between noise benefit and a model for measuring arousal in attention. Several previous studies have shown a beneficial effect of noise for a short period but have not explicitly tested the brain arousal theory lying behind the noise benefit model of Sikström and Söderlund (2007). Loewen and Suedfeld (1992) suggested that auditory masking of naturalistic background noise could be one benign effect of noise, but the present study was performed in a silent room with no such possible distracting noise that could represent this alternative explanation of the noise effect.
Finding a small general regulating effect (the reduced reaction time variability) and the theoretically interesting ISI and block effects are encouraging and should lead to further research trying to improve the noise effect so that it could serve as a possible treatment. Since this study was started, both “pink” and “brown” noises have been suggested to have effects on cognitive functioning (Guo et al., 2022). White noise is a random signal with a flat power spectrum having equal intensity over the entire frequency band. To the participant, it sounds like a radio tuned to an unused frequency. Brown noise has a larger amount of lower frequencies than white noise, which does not have a flat power spectrum. It is a duller sound that resembles the sound of a river current or strong wind. Pink noise sounds like a waterfall. Considering the lack of studies, especially with subjects with ADHD, one nevertheless could speculate that the tolerability of the intervention would have been better if we had used pink noise. However, the effectiveness of the intervention might have been somewhat smaller for those who completed the trial with noise. We had, however, no possibility to test out different noise types and could neither test out different noise amplitudes. In the present study, it was 70–80 decibels, which could be too high for some individuals or too long period for some. A study by Awada et al. (2022) indicated that lower amplitude is best for sustained attention, while higher amplitude may be better for shorter working memory tasks in neurotypical young adults. An interesting idea is to let participants choose their own noise levels, thus using noise level as a dependent variable and thereby be able to explore if the preferred noise levels correlate with symptom severity in ADHD. Altogether, these results highlight the need for more research on noise levels, noise characters, and how to adapt these both to individuals and to different tasks. There is still a big gap in the literature before we can establish patient-specific guidelines for the use of noise stimulation or a best practice protocol for the administration of noise treatment (Pickens et al., 2019).
There are several limitations to the study. As it was integrated into clinical practice, we had to make some choices balancing between clinical demands and the rigor necessary for good research. While the majority were tested two times on the same day, some subjects were tested on different days. We have run all analyses limited to those tested on the same day, and it did not change any of the significant findings. We apologize that there are missing data points in the descriptive data, but for the most part, this is due to questionnaires only available in Norwegian, prohibiting many immigrant parents from filling them out. There were also examples of questionnaires belonging to the research protocol that were not applied due to the judgment of the treating clinician as not necessary. There is, however, no reason to believe that these inaccuracies will change anything regarding data.
However, a possible more important limitation is that the researchers administering the tests were not blinded as to the noise/no-noise conditions. Could it be that they unconsciously signaled an anticipation of better performance with noise? In fact, we consider it the other way around; the clinicians were faced with many negative comments from the participating children toward the high noise, and mid-way in the project, the administrators clearly signaled to the research group an expectation that noise would show up to be detrimental. In hindsight, it might be that these expectations lowered the threshold for letting participants abandon the repeated testing, particularly with noise.
Although we classify the participants into high or low levels of ADHD symptoms, they constitute a clinical sample; thus, we did not compare the two subgroups. If we had compared the high symptoms group to a neurotypical control sample, we would have expected significant differences. It follows that the benign effect of white noise in this study cannot be generalized to children in general. Since we not only find a benign effect of noise but also that its effect is driven by improved attention under long event rates and the last part of the test, the study should be replicated with a healthy control group to test the specificity of these mechanisms of change. We expect that the inverted U-curve of optimal stimulation is generally moved rightward among subjects with ADHD. However, in this study, we merely study half of the inverted U. We study contingencies varying from intense stimulation not in need of additional extrinsic stimulation (i.e., white noise) to low-intensity stimulation needing noise to raise attention, but we do not continue into an expected decay even when noise is added.
The participants are compared on 15 measures from the CCPT-3. A possible critique would be that we should have applied a Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. However, the analyses are theory driven rather than an exploratory search for significant results in one direction or the other. In fact, some of the noise/no-noise comparisons are performed just to be comprehensive with an explicit expectation of a null finding, such as the Mu and Sigma analyses and the Tau analyses of 1- and 2-s ISI. One could have analyzed only Tau 4-s ISI, but then the reader could question whether the finding was specific to the longest inter-stimulus interval or a general effect. Thus, many comparisons are necessary to give a detailed and nuanced picture of the relationship between noise and arousal.
In conclusion, trying to integrate the cognitive energetics model and the MBA by their joint focus on state regulation deficits in ADHD, we found indications that noise reduces what is considered indicative of arousal impairment, both by reducing reaction time variability and specifically by reducing the fall in attention under low-intensity-stimulus conditions and in later time blocks. The effect is small but encouraging and should lead to further research on varying noise conditions, in particular, while there is a demand for non-pharmacological treatment alternatives in ADHD.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
The studies involving humans were approved by Regional Ethics Committee Sør-Øst. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.
Author contributions
JE: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Software, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. OL: Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. IK-G: Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. AA: Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. GS: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. The research was funded by the Norwegian Research Network on ADHD, grant number 18/09147-5.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Integrative guide for the CBCL/4–18, YSR and TRF profiles. Burlington: Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont.
Awada, M., Becerik-Gerber, B., Lucas, G., and Roll, S. (2022). Cognitive performance, creativity and stress levels of neurotypical young adults under different white noise levels. Sci Rep 12, 1–9. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-18862-w
Bella-Fernández, M., Martin-Moratinos, M., Li, C., Wang, P., and Blasco-Fontecilla, H. (2023). Differences in ex-Gaussian parameters from response time distributions between individuals with and without attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a Meta-analysis. Neuropsychol Rev 1–18. doi: 10.1007/s11065-023-09587-2
Chiang, H. L., Chen, Y. J., Lo, Y. C., Tseng, W. Y. I., and Gau, S. S. F. (2015). Altered white matter tract property related to impaired focused attention, sustained attention, cognitive impulsivity and vigilance in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of psychiatry and neuroscience, 40, 325–335. doi: 10.1503/jpn.140106
Clarke, A. R., Barry, R. J., and Johnstone, S. (2020). Resting state EEG power research in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a review update. Clin Neurophysiol 131, 1463–1479. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2020.03.029
Conners, C. K. , (2014). Manual for the Conners’ continuous performance test 3rd Edition, Toronto, Canada: Multi-Health Systems Inc.
Del Campo, N., Chamberlain, S. R., Sahakian, B. J., and Robbins, T. W. (2011). The roles of dopamine and noradrenaline in the pathophysiology and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 69, e145–e157. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.036
DuPaul, G. J., Power, T. J., Anastoupolous, A. D., and Reid, R. (1998). “ADHD rating scale-IV” in Checklists, norms & clinical interpretation (New York: Guildford)
Egeland, J., Johansen, S. N., and Ueland, T. (2009). Differentiating between HD sub-types on CCPT measures of sustained attention and vigilance. Scand J Psychol 50, 347–354. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00717.x
Egeland, J. (2012). Cutting corners: Neuropsychological Research into the Energetics of ADHD. In: Contemporary Trends in ADHD research. pp. 69–84.
Egeland, J., Johansen, S. N., and Ueland, T. (2012). Central processing energetic factors mediate impaired motor control in ADHD combined subtype but not in ADHD inattentive subtype. J Learn Disabil 45, 361–370. doi: 10.1177/0022219411407922
Egeland, J., and Kowalik-Gran, I. (2010). Validity of the factor structure of Conners’ CPT. J Atten Disord 13, 347–357. doi: 10.1177/1087054709332477
Epstein, J., Brinkman, W., Froehlich, T., Langberg, J. M., Narad, M. E., Antonini, T. N., et al. (2011). Effects of stimulant medication, incentives, and event rate on reaction time variability in children with ADHD. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 1060–1072. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.243
Fallmyr, Ø., and Egeland, E. (2011). Psychometric properties of the Norwegian version of BRIEF – for children from 5 to 18 years old. J Norwegian Psychological Association 48, 339–343.
Fredriksen, M., Egeland, J., Haavik, J., and Fasmer, O. B. (2021). Individual variability in reaction time and prediction of clinical response to methylphenidate in adult ADHD: a prospective open label study using conners’ continuous performance test II. J Atten Disord 25, 657–671. doi: 10.1177/1087054719829822
Geissler, J., Romanos, M., Hegerl, U., and Hensch, T. (2014). Hyperactivity and sensation seeking as autoregulatory attempts to stabilize brain arousal in ADHD and mania? Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder 6, 159–173. doi: 10.1007/s12402-014-0144-z
Grace, A. A. (1991). Phasic versus tonic dopamine release and the modulation of dopamine system responsivity: a hypothesis for the etiology of schizophrenia. Neuroscience 41, 1–24. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(91)90196-U
Gu, S. L. H., Gau, S. S. F., Tzang, S. W., and Hsu, W. Y. (2013). The ex-Gaussian distribution of reaction times in adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Res Dev Disabil 34, 3709–3719. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.07.025
Guo, K., Wu, Y., and Zhang, H. (2022). The effects of color noises on attention. In 2022 International Conference on Science Education and Art Appreciation (SEAA 2022) (pp. 576–583). Atlantis Press.
Heathcote, A., Popiel, S. J., and Mewhort, D. J. (1991). Analysis of response time distributions: An example using the Stroop task. Psychological bulletin, 109:340.
Hegerl, U., and Hensch, T. (2014). The vigilance regulation model of affective disorders and ADHD. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 44, 45–57. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.10.008
Helps, S. K., Bamford, S., Sonuga-Barke, E. J., and Söderlund, G. B. W. (2014). Different effects of adding white noise on cognitive performance of sub-, normal and super-attentive school children. PloS One 9:e112768. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0112768
Huang-Pollock, C. L., Karalunas, S. L., Tam, H., and Moore, A. N. (2012). Evaluating vigilance deficits in ADHD: a meta-analysis of CPT performance. J Abnorm Psychol 121, 360–371. doi: 10.1037/a0027205
Hwang-Gu, S. L., Chen, Y. C., Liang, S. H. Y., Ni, H. C., Lin, H. Y., Lin, C. F., et al. (2019). Exploring the variability in reaction times of preschoolers at risk of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: an ex-Gaussian analysis. J Abnorm Child Psychol 47, 1315–1326. doi: 10.1007/s10802-018-00508-z
Kofler, M. J., Rapport, M. D., Sarver, D. E., Raiker, J. S., Orban, S. A., Friedman, L. M., et al. (2013). Reaction time variability in ADHD: a meta-analytic review of 319 studies. Clin Psychol Rev 33, 795–811. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.06.001
Levy, F., Pipingas, A., Harris, E. V., Farrow, M., and Silberstein, R. B. (2018). Continuous performance task in ADHD: Is reaction time variability a key measure? Neuropsychiatric disease and treatment, 781–786.
Lin, H. Y. (2022). The Effects of White Noise on Attentional Performance and On-Task Behaviors in Preschoolers with ADHD. International journal of environmental research and public health, 19:15391. doi: 10.3390/ijerph192215391
Loewen, L. J., and Suedfeld, P. (1992). Cognitive and arousal effects of masking office noise. Environ Behav 24, 381–395. doi: 10.1177/0013916592243006
Loo, S. K., Hale, T. S., Macion, J., Hanada, G., McGough, J. J., McCracken, J. T., et al. (2009). Cortical activity patterns in ADHD during arousal, activation and sustained attention. Neuropsychologia 47, 2114–2119. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.04.013
López-Luengo, B., González-Andrade, A., and García-Cobo, M. (2016). Not all differences between patients with schizophrenia and healthy subjects are pathological: performance on the conners’ continuous performance test. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 31, 983–995. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acw075
Lundervold, A. J., Stickert, M., Hysing, M., Sørensen, L., Gillberg, C., and Posserud, M. B. (2016). Attention deficits in children with combined autism and ADHD: a CPT study. J Atten Disord 20, 599–609. doi: 10.1177/1087054712453168
Martella, D., Aldunate, N., Fuentes, L. J., and Sánchez-Pérez, N. (2020). Arousal and executive alterations in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Front Psychol 11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01991
Massidda, D. (2013). Retimes: reaction time analysis. R Package Version 0.1–2. Available at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=retimes
Miranda, M. C., Barbosa, T., Muszkat, M., Rodrigues, C. C., Sinnes, E. G., Coelho, L. F. S., et al. (2012). Performance patterns in Conners' CPT among children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and dyslexia. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 70, 91–96. doi: 10.1590/S0004-282X2012000200004
Nøvik, T. S. (2000). Child behavior checklist item scores in Norwegian children. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 9, 54–60. doi: 10.1007/s007870050116
Pagán, A. F., Huizar, Y. P., and Schmidt, A. T. (2022). Conner’s continuous performance test and adult ADHD: a systematic literature review. J Atten Disord. 231–249. 10870547221142455
Pickens, T. A., Khan, S. P., and Berlau, D. J. (2019). White noise as a possible therapeutic option for children with ADHD. Complement Ther Med 42, 151–155. doi: 10.1016/j.ctim.2018.11.012
Pribram, K. H., and McGuinness, D. (1975). Arousal, activation, and effort in the control of attention. Psychol Rev 82, 116–149. doi: 10.1037/h0076780
R Core Team . (2022). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria.
Rausch, V. H., Bauch, E. M., and Bunzeck, N. (2014). White noise improves learning by modulating activity in dopaminergic midbrain regions and right superior temporal sulcus. J Cogn Neurosci 26, 1469–1480. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00537
Salum, G. A., Sato, J. R., Manfro, A. G., Pan, P. M., Gadelha, A., do Rosário, M. C., et al. (2019). Reaction time variability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: is increased reaction time variability specific to attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Testing predictions from the default-mode interference hypothesis. ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders 11, 47–58. doi: 10.1007/s12402-018-0257-x
Seeman, P., and Madras, B. (2002). Methylphenidate elevates resting dopamine which lowers the impulse-triggered release of dopamine: a hypothesis. Behav Brain Res 130, 79–83. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00435-1
Sergeant, J. A. (2005). Modeling attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a critical appraisal of the cognitive-energetic model. Biological psychiatry, 57, 1248–1255.
Shalev, N., and Nobre, A. C. (2022). Eyes wide open: regulation of arousal by temporal expectations. Cognition 224:105062. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105062
Sikström, S., and Söderlund, G. B. W. (2007). Stimulus-dependent dopamine release in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychol Rev 114, 1047–1075. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.4.1047
Söderlund, G. B. W., Björk, C., and Gustafsson, P. (2016). Comparing auditory noise treatment with stimulant medication on cognitive task performance in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: results from a pilot study. Front Psychol 7. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01331
Söderlund, G. B. W., and Nilsson Jobs, E. (2016). Differences in speech recognition between children with attention deficits and typically developed children disappear when exposed to 65 dB of auditory noise. Front Psychol 7, 1–11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00034
Söderlund, G. B. W., Sikström, S., Loftesnes, J. M., and Sonuga-Barke, E. (2010). The effects of background white noise on memory performance in inattentive school children. Behav Brain Funct 6:55. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-6-55
Söderlund, G. B. W., Sikstrom, S., and Smart, A. (2007). Listen to the noise: noise is beneficial for cognitive performance in ADHD. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 48, 840–847. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01749.x
Sonuga-Barke, E., Wiersema, J. R., van der Meere, J. J., and Roeyers, H. (2010). Context-dependent dynamic processes in attention deficit/hyperactivity Diosrder: differentiating common and unique effects of state regulation deficits and delay aversion. Neuropsychol Rev 20, 86–102. doi: 10.1007/s11065-009-9115-0
Tamm, L., Narad, M. E., Antonini, T. N., O’Brien, K. M., Hawk, L. W., and Epstein, J. N. (2012). Reaction time variability in ADHD: a review. Neurotherapeutics 9, 500–508. doi: 10.1007/s13311-012-0138-5
Udal, A. H., Egeland, J., Øygarden, B., Malt, U. F., Løvdal, H., Pripp, A. H., et al. (2014). Differentiating between comorbidity and symptom overlap in ADHD and early onset bipolar disorder. Dev Neuropsychol 39, 249–261. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2014.886692
Volkow, N. D., Wang, G.-J., Kollins, S. H., Wigal, T. L., Newcorn, J. H., Telang, F., et al. (2009). Evaluating dopamine reward pathway in ADHD: clinical implications. JAMA 302, 1084–1091. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.1308
Zane, K. L., Gfeller, J. D., Roskos, P. T., and Bucholz, R. D. (2016). The clinical utility of the Conners’ continuous performance test-II in traumatic brain injury. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 31, 996–1005. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acw078
Zhou, Q., and Luo, Y. (2022). Event rate effects on children with attention-deficit/hyperactive disorder: test predictions from the moderate brain arousal model and the neuro-energetics theory using the diffusion decision model. Res Dev Disabil 127:104262. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2022.104262
Keywords: noise, sustained attention, Conners’ CPT-3, ADHD, arousal
Citation: Egeland J, Lund O, Kowalik-Gran I, Aarlien AK and Söderlund GBW (2023) Effects of auditory white noise stimulation on sustained attention and response time variability. Front. Psychol. 14:1301771. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1301771
Edited by:
Fabio Giovannelli, University of Florence, ItalyReviewed by:
Susana Silva, University of Porto, PortugalNicolas Zink, University of California, United States
Yanlong Zhang, Xi'an International Studies University, China
Copyright © 2023 Egeland, Lund, Kowalik-Gran, Aarlien and Söderlund. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Jens Egeland, jens.egeland@siv.no