- 1Department of Clinical Psychology, Psychobiology and Methodology, Universidad de La Laguna, Tenerife, Spain
- 2Instituto Universitario de Neurociencia (IUNE), Universidad de La Laguna, Tenerife, Spain
Previous research on the motives for lying lacks factorial models that allow grouping of motives into specific categories. The objective of this study is to confirm the factorial structure of the questionnaire of motives for lying (CEMA-A). Participants were 1,722 adults residing in the Canary Islands (Spain) who completed the CEMA-A and the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ-R). The four-dimensional structure of the questionnaire was confirmed (χ2 = 1460.97, df = 325, p = 0.001; CFI = 0.94; TLI = 0.93; NFI = 0.93; NNFI = 0.93; RMSEA = 0.05, CI = 0.051–0.057; SRMR = 0.04). The four factors of the CEMA-A were Intrapersonal Motivation–Emotionality, Interpersonal Motivation–Sociability, Egoism/Hardness Motivation, and Malicious Motivation, with an internal consistency between 0.79 and 0.91. Invariance analyses confirmed the equivalence of the instrument for men and women. The CEMA-A factors positively correlated with Neuroticism and Psychoticism, and negatively with Dissimulation. Extraversion was not related to any of the factors, and only displayed a low negative correlation with Intrapersonal Motivation–Emotionality. Analysis of variance showed that men scored higher in Egoism/Hardness and Malicious Motivation. The CEMA-A has proven capable of apprehending the motives for lying and has adequate psychometric criteria for use in various populations.
1 Introduction
A lie is a multidimensional construct (DePaulo et al., 1996; Phillips et al., 2011; Muzinic et al., 2016), defined as a form of verbal deception, where there is a deliberate attempt to hide, falsify, generate and/or manipulate, in some way, factual, and/or emotional information, to encourage in the other a belief that the communicator themself considers false (Knapp and Comadena, 1979; Ekman, 1985/2001; Miller and Stiff, 1993; Buller et al., 1994; Masip et al., 2004; Vrij, 2008). People evaluate lying from two positions, by assigning a negative image to those who lie and by rationalizing or justifying the lie when it is used by the individual themself (Nyberg, 1993; Kashy and DePaulo, 1996; Bond and DePaulo, 2006). Thus, more intentionality is attributed, and the label of liar is assigned more to others than to oneself when lying (Curtis, 2021). Research suggests that people view their everyday lives as small, and unimportant, rarely plan them, and unconcerned about being discovered (DePaulo et al., 2004; Bond and DePaulo, 2006). Most lies that are considered serious are motivated by the desire to cover up a personal fault, a discredited fact or to hide transgressions that, if discovered, could have serious consequences for the identity and reputation of the liar (McCornack and Levine, 1990; Metts, 1994; DePaulo et al., 2003a, 2004). These types of lies are more carefully planned, and are often unjustifiable, immoral, or illegal (DePaulo et al., 2004). Therefore, unless there is a psychopathological problem (Curtis and Hart, 2022), people often use deception, when telling the truth is a problem (McCornack et al., 2014; Levine et al., 2016; Moshagen et al., 2020). Since lying is intentional, that people lie for a reason or motive is implicit (Bond and DePaulo, 2006; Levine et al., 2010), lying, in itself, is not a goal, but a means to achieve another (Levine et al., 2010). For example, someone tells their partner that they are at home (a lie) when they are in fact with a lover. This lie does not seek to convince the partner of their where about, since they could have excused themselves in another way, to convince them of their fidelity (the goal).
In general, people may tell a lie at some point, despite it being considered a reprehensible act with harmful consequences (Bok, 1978; Teasdale and Kent, 1995; Solomon, 2009; Curtis and Hart, 2015). However, lying every day is not common behavior for most people (Serota et al., 2010, 2022; Serota and Levine, 2015). Studies in the field of everyday lies find that people report an average of one to two lies a day (DePaulo and Kashy, 1998; Serota et al., 2010; Serota and Levine, 2015). However, the average may be distorted by extreme scores from people who often lie. These differences in the frequency of lying may also be related to sociodemographic variables. Some studies suggest that young people and men admit to lying more often (DePaulo et al., 1996; Serota et al., 2010; Armas-Vargas, 2017a), although the difference in frequency of lying between men and women is very small (Gerlach et al., 2019). Other research has found that gender differences vary depending on the subject matter of the lie (DePaulo et al., 1996; Feldman et al., 2002; Haselton et al., 2005; Erat and Gneezy, 2012). Various studies suggest that lying decreases with age (Jensen et al., 2004; Serota et al., 2010). Adolescents tend to lie more than university students, who do so less than the general adult population (DePaulo et al., 1996; Serota et al., 2010; Levine et al., 2013; Armas-Vargas, 2020, 2021b).
Most research has been carried out in the area of lie detection (Vrij and Ganis, 2014). The truth-default theory (Levine, 2014; Levine et al., 2022) is one of the most widely accepted theories about human deception detection. This theory proposes that people tell the truth by default, that is, they are honest most of the time, and are more likely to believe that others tell them the truth rather than lies. Thus, people do not usually lie except when the truth is an obstacle to goal attainment (Levine et al., 2010). However, if a situation becomes problematic, people can then lie. On the other hand, the self-concept maintenance theory (Mazar et al., 2008; Ariely, 2012) proposes that people are more likely to lie when the ego is depleted (Mead et al., 2009). Therefore, the aim of a person who deceives is to satisfy complex intrinsic motivations, such as maintaining a favorable self-concept (Mazar et al., 2008). Similarly, from a self-presentational perspective, DePaulo et al. (2003b) propose that people mainly lie for psychological reasons to protect or to give a better image of themselves, that is, to deliberately try to manage others’ impressions of them. Furthermore, DePaulo et al. (2003b) suggest that deception and truth can be distributed along a continuum rather than considered different dimensions. The reasons for lying or telling the truth are the same: people are interested in giving a good image or describing important aspects of themselves. However, self-presentation is not the only reason why one can lie. In a transcultural study, Levine et al. (2016) found that there were different types of deception motives such as maintaining a positive self-image, protecting others, avoiding others, seeking an advantage, social politeness, hiding a transgression, being malicious, and joking.
Furthermore, some people who lie give socially desirable responses and misrepresent their motivations for lying (DePaulo et al., 2003b). Much research indicates that people offer a positive view of themselves, highlighting positive features such as that they are better, more honest, and more moral than others (Alicke et al., 1995). These beliefs are identified with self-deception. For self-deception to produce positive effects on the person, individuals must, by definition, be unaware of its illusory basis (Baumeister, 1993). According to Trivers (2002), “the hallmark of self-deception in the service of deceit is the denial of deception, the unconscious running of selfish and deceitful ploys, the creation of a public person as an altruist and a person beneffective in the lives of others” (p. 276). Therefore, as they are not fully aware of their motivations for lying, these people can confidently and “honestly” claim that their lies were altruistically motivated. However, according to cognitive dissonance theory (Festinger, 1957), altruistic interpretations of deception may not completely dispel the dissonance of the person lying. In these cases, the person may wield feeling guilty about the lie as a way to reduce cognitive dissonance. When they express guilt for lying to others, they are reinforcing their positive view of themselves. Individuals who feel and express guilt for their misdeeds are often considered better people than those who show no remorse (Baumeister, 1997).
Other authors have attempted to capture and classify the motives for lying. Turner et al. (1975) list five motivations for lying: (a) to save face (to protect identity, self-esteem), (b) to manage or handle relationships (to end a relationship), (c) to exploit others (by manipulating, having control, power, and influence over the other), (d) to avoid tensions or conflicts (controlling a conversation to avoid it being uncomfortable or triggering an argument) and (e) to control situations (to maintain, redirect or end interaction with the other). Buller and Burgoon (1996) point out that lying is employed for three main reasons/motives: (a) “instrumental” (to gain power, influence others, avoid disapproval, or do harm), (b) “identity” (to improve the image we present to others, avoid shame, improve or protect self-esteem, and increase social desirability), and (c) “relational” (to influence our relationships with others).
Another proposed categorization of the motives for lying is based on (a) whether the liar is “centered on themselves” (egotistical, to protect themselves) or on the other person (to protect others), and (b) whether the liar is “altruistically” or “maliciously” motivated (DePaulo et al., 1996, 2003a; DePaulo and Kashy, 1998; Vrij, 2000). Altruistic lies also allow one to protect one’s well-being (Ennis et al., 2008) and have been classified as considerably more acceptable than egotistical lies (for one’s own benefit or for malicious purposes) (Lindskold and Walters, 1983; Seiter et al., 2002). DePaulo et al. (1996) found that people lie far more about themselves than they do about others. The motives behind the lies were mostly selfish, and many more lies were told for emotional reasons (to protect themselves from shame, or their own feelings) than for personal advantage (to obtain benefits or material gain).
From a qualitative perspective, the motivations for lying have been classified from two dimensions: protective versus beneficial lies and self-oriented versus other-oriented lies (Arcimowicz et al., 2015). The combination of both dimensions facilitates the identification of four types of lies: egoistic (self-oriented/beneficial), self-defensive (self-oriented/protective), pleasing (other-oriented/beneficial), and sheltering (other-oriented/protective). Some egoistic lies cited in the interviews were for material gains or admiration from others. Self-defensive lies included avoiding responsibility, discussions, or negative consequences. Pleasing lies were related to making someone happy, and sheltering lies were associated with protecting someone from distress or avoiding hurting someone else. These last two categories were more difficult to distinguish. In general, people lie primarily for protective motivations that allow them to avoid punishment rather than for personal benefits.
The role played by inter-individual differences may affect the probability of lying (McLeod and Genereux, 2008), as well as the different motives for lying and achieving certain goals or desires (Buller and Burgoon, 1996; Olson and Weber, 2004). Some studies point out the importance of personality traits in the probability of and motives for lying (McArthur et al., 2022). Machiavellianism or extraversion are associated with frequency and different types of lying (Kashy and DePaulo, 1996; McLeod and Genereux, 2008; Hart et al., 2019). In the prison population, lying has been found to be associated with both neuroticism and psychoticism (Gudjonsson and Sigurdsson, 2004). Fullam et al. (2009) found that people with a high level of psychoticism showed a low level of conditioning to social norms, a low level of fear and avoidance of harm, and were more likely to lie. The results of the study revealed the importance of analyzing the role of the traits of insensitivity and emotional deficit (typical of psychoticism and neuroticism) in the tasks that evaluate the cognitive elements that may be involved in deceiving and manipulating others. Giammarco et al. (2013) also found an association between greater ability to deceive and the Dark Triad of Personality (Machiavellianism, psychopathy, and narcissism). Furthermore, an important motivator in lying is emotions (Ekman, 1985/2001). Lying is mainly motivated by negative emotions, such as anxiety, fear (Ekman, 1985/2001; Tangney et al., 1996), or guilt, which arises when there is a discrepancy between internalized values and actual behavior (Mosher, 1968; Ekman, 1985/2001; Millar and Tesser, 1988); shame, when a person does not meet their own personal moral standards (Keltner and Buswell, 1996; Tangney et al., 1996; DePaulo et al., 2003a); and insecurity, fear of rejection and criticism (Armas-Vargas, 2021a,b). People are motivated to lie mainly through certain emotional needs, which are satisfied through social interaction, the instrumentalization of relationships, or harming others (Armas-Vargas, 2021a). That is, personal/emotional motives may be based on other more social, instrumental/selfish, or malicious motives (Armas-Vargas, 2021a). Many of these emotional motives may be implicit or escape awareness (McClelland et al., 1989; Bargh, 1990; Bargh and Chartrand, 1999; Bargh et al., 2001; Custers and Aarts, 2005), while interpersonal, instrumental, and malicious motives imply heightened awareness (Schooler and Schreiber, 2005; Touré-Tillery and Fishbach, 2014).
Several studies have tried to classify the motives for deception using different methods, such as researchers’ expert judgment, literature reviews, and analysis of diary records and, interviews, or surveys (Turner et al., 1975; Ekman et al., 1989; DePaulo et al., 1996; Kashy and DePaulo, 1996; McLeod and Genereux, 2008; Phillips et al., 2011; Arcimowicz et al., 2015; Levine et al., 2016). However, few studies have designed self-report instruments to identify and categorize motives using factor analysis. One of the self-report instruments proposed, designed by Hart et al. (2019), identifies two categories that evaluate relational and antisocial motives. In a later study, Hart et al. (2020) found three categories of motives for lying: self-serving lies (such as avoiding the consequences of bad behavior and self-promotion), altruistic or benevolent lies (to benefit another), and vindictive lies to harm another person.
The aim of this study is to analyze the psychometric properties of an instrument that assesses people’s main motives for lying in their daily lives. The instrument was constructed to combine the different theoretical models described, as well as other typologies proposed by various authors on the motives for lying. The CEMA-A questionnaire was based on a review of the literature, to integrate the various motives behind every day lies. The instrument design mainly took into account the role of emotions in lying (Ekman, 1985/2001; Tangney et al., 1996); the five motivations for lying proposed by Turner et al. (1975); the three main reasons/motives of Buller and Burgoon (1996); the 10 pancultural deception motives of Levine et al. (2016); the research on self-presentational motives for lying in everyday life (DePaulo et al., 1996, 2003a; Kashy and DePaulo, 1996; DePaulo and Kashy, 1998), and personality variables related to lying (Olson and Weber, 2004; McLeod and Genereux, 2008; Armas-Vargas, 2017a; Armas-Vargas, 2020; Armas-Vargas, 2021b). In a pilot study (Armas-Vargas, 2021a), a four-factor structure was obtained, after exploratory factor analysis (EFA). The “Intrapersonal Motivation–Emotionality” category evaluates motives related to self-deception and negative emotions (shame, insecurity, fear of rejection and criticism). “Interpersonal Motivation–Sociability” evaluates reasons for the benefit of social relationships (to excuse or justify oneself, avoid conflicts with others, and for reasons of a prosocial nature). “Egoism/Hardness Motivation” measures motives related to using relationships for one’s own benefit (to obtain advantage, manipulate others, present a good image and impress others). And finally, the “Malicious Motivation” category evaluates motives related to covert or direct harm, or false accusations that cause harm (Armas-Vargas, 2021a). Unlike the test proposed by Hart et al. (2019), the CEMA-A posits two new categories: Intrapersonal Motivation and Egoism/Hardness Motivation. The other two factors of relational and antisocial motives proposed by Hart et al. (2019) correspond, to a certain extent, with Interpersonal Motivation–Sociability and Malicious Motivation, respectively.
The objective of this work is to study the psychometric properties of the CEMA-A instrument. Specifically, it will analyze whether the factorial structure found in the previous exploratory analyses (Armas-Vargas, 2021a), remains stable. Next, confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) will be used to check construct validity whether the data conform to the proposed four-factor structure. The internal consistency of the four scales and the total test will be studied, along with the temporal stability provided by the test–retest correlations of the factors. Likewise, factorial invariance will be examined to verify whether the structure is similar between men and women. Convergent and discriminant validity will be checked by analyzing the relationship with other personality variables. Finally, the mean differences of the various factors of the CEMA-A will be analyzed according to gender and level of education.
2 Materials and methods
2.1 Participants
The total sample was 1,722 adults (Sample 3) from the general population of the Canary Islands (Spain), aged 18 to 77 years (Mage = 35.13, SD = 13.74): 55.89% women (N = 962) and 44.11% men (N = 760). The total sample was divided into subsamples for the different phases of the study. Sample 1 consisted of 520 participants aged 18 to 76 years (Mage = 36.80, SD = 14.44) and was used to perform the EFA. Sample 2 consisted of 1,202 participants aged 18 to 77 years (Mage = 34.41, SD = 13.37), and was used for CFA and analysis of invariance, based on gender. Sample 3 was used to perform analysis of variance (MANOVA) based on gender and level of education. Sample 4 consisted of 529 participants from the total sample, aged 18 to 71 years (Mage = 34.90, SD = 13.25), selected to analyze the temporal stability of the factors. Table 1 presents the characteristics of the total sample and the different subsamples.
2.2 Instruments
Questionnaire for the Evaluation of Deceit, Lies and Self-deception (CEMA) (Armas-Vargas, 2021a). The instrument was developed based on Muñiz and Fonseca-Pedrero (2019) recommendations for test construction. This self-report instrument designed to assess variables associated with “deceit, lying, concealment, and self-deception” consists of four sub-questionnaires: The Motives for Lying (CEMA-A); Opinions about Self-Deception Lying (CEMA-B); Content of Lies (CEMA-C); and Receivers of the Lies (CEMA-D). In this study, we validate the CEMA-A subquestionnaire that assesses people’s motives for lying in their daily lives. Questionnaire development drew from a pool of 80 items related to personal–emotional variables (associated with protection of the self, such as fear of rejection, fear of what others will say, insecurity, self-esteem problems, self-deception); items related to instrumental content, manipulation of others, pro-image, and self-presentation (more selfish, intention to benefit oneself); other items concerning lies in social interactions (lies that are altruistic, prosocial, or beneficial to others); and finally, items related to malice or harming others. Two independent experts checked the wording and clarity of the items; when they disagreed, a third expert was consulted. Participants were informed that the aim of the study was to investigate the motives people may have for lying. Specifically, participants received the information that “lying includes both deliberately omitting relevant information and telling someone something that is not true.” Then, to minimize problems of social desirability, participants were also told that it is normal to lie from time to time and the fact of being able to lie is not censored, but research is interested in studying the reasons why one might lie at some point. Finally, participants were asked to indicate the reasons or motives for which they usually deceive, lie, or withhold information from others and to indicate on a Likert-type scale of seven alternatives (1 = rarely, 2 = from time to time, 3 = sometimes, 4 = usually, 5 = very often, 6 = many times, and 7 = always), which of the listed motives they generally use to a greater or lesser extent. They were thanked for their participation and asked to be honest in their answers.
In the previous pilot study (Armas-Vargas, 2021a), an exploratory factor analysis (oblimin rotation) was applied. Items that saturated on two factors and items with factor loadings below 0.40 were eliminated from the factor analysis, reducing the number of items from 80 to 45. The CEMA-A questionnaire was finally composed of 45 items, and a factorial structure of four factors or general categories was obtained: Intrapersonal Motivation–Emotionality, Interpersonal Motivation–Sociability, Egoism/Hardness Motivation, and Malicious Motivation. The Intrapersonal Motivation–Emotionality category evaluates motives related to self-deception and negative emotions; Interpersonal Motivation–Sociability collects motives related to maintaining positive social relationships; Egoism/Hardness Motivation measures motives related to using relationships for one’s own benefit; and the Malicious Motivation category evaluates motives related to covert or direct harm, or false accusations that cause harm (Armas-Vargas, 2021a). Interpersonal Motivation–Sociability and Egoism/Hardness Motivation both refer to the domain of interpersonal relationships. However, in the Egoism/Hardness motives, the intention of the individual who lies is to benefit him/herself with the act of lying, whereas in the Interpersonal Motivation–Sociability, the intention of the individual is more prosocial: he/she intends to benefit others with the act of lying. On the other hand, the Intrapersonal Motivation–Emotionality factor is related to more personal motivations, where the person “avoids or does not want to face the truth and reality,” indirectly obtaining a “self-benefit, without instrumentalizing anyone” by avoiding facing reality. In the Egoism/Hardness Motivation factor, the person intends to gain self-benefit by “manipulating and instrumentalizing others.” In this second case, the person acts and confronts reality in order to achieve a certain goal. The total reliability of Cronbach’s alpha was 0.97 and the omega coefficient ωj = 0.79.
Eysenck Personality Questionnaire − Revised (EPQ-R; Eysenck and Eysenck, 1997). It explores three personality traits: (1) Extraversion (sociable, active, assertive, sensation-seeking); (2) Neuroticism (anxious, depressed, guilt); and (3) Psychoticism (aggressive, cold, egocentric, impulsive, antisocial). It also includes the Lie scale, intended to measure the tendencies of examinees to “fake good” when they complete the questionnaire. It is made up of 83 items with two response alternatives (true or false), referring to the person’s way of acting, feeling and thinking. Because it is a shorter tool, the EPQ-R was used in this study to assess the personality characteristics that have been linked to lying, such as psychoticism, neuroticism, and extraversion. Since no other tests of motives for lying have been validated in Spanish, the EPQ-R was used to assess convergent validity through the lie scale, along with discriminant validity, to distinguish between motives for lying and personality traits that have previously been weakly correlated (Gudjonsson and Sigurdsson, 2004; McLeod and Genereux, 2008; Hart et al., 2019). Internal consistency oscillates between 0.71 and 0.86.
2.3 Procedure
Data collection was done by fourth-year psychology undergraduates and master’s students of general health psychology at the University of La Laguna for three academic years 2020–2023. This study was not preregistered. Samples 1 (N = 520) and 2 (N = 1,202) were obtained in 2020, and 2021 and 2023, respectively. Sample 3 (N = 1722) is the sum of both samples, and Sample 4 (N = 529) was randomly drawn from the whole sample. The students were trained to administer the aforementioned tests, order to play the role of evaluators. Sampling was incidental for convenience (Gil-Escudero and Martínez-Arias, 2001). The students had to select 15 to 20 people from their close environment, homogenized by gender, to whom they would apply the instrument. They were informed about the objective of the study, voluntarily accepted to collaborate, and gave their written informed consent. Participants received an envelope containing an identification code and tests. One week later, the sealed envelope was collected, to guarantee anonymity. Participants were instructed to write a contact telephone number on the envelope, so that they could be contacted for a second retest. After four weeks, half of the sample of 1,200 was randomly selected and, of the 600 participants selected, 529 had returned the envelope with the retest completed. The participants completed the questionnaires independently, at home and on paper in approximately 30 min. No reward was offered for participation. The study was carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Research Ethics and Animal Welfare Committee of the University of La Laguna (Registration Number: CEIBA2023-3299).
2.4 Data analysis
The data were analyzed using R version 4.0.5 (R Core Team, 2017), the Lavaan package (Rosseel, 2012), and the syntax described by ULLRToolbox (Hernández and Betancort, 2018). Initially, an EFA was performed with Sample 1 (N = 520). This sample was used to verify whether the same four-factor structure remained stable with 45 items proposed by Armas-Vargas (2021a). The procedure used to determine the number of factors was the optimal application of Horn’s parallel analysis (Timmerman and Lorenzo-Seva, 2011). An EFA was performed on principal axes and oblique rotation (oblimin) since a correlation between the factors was expected.
Secondly, CFA was performed with 1,202 participants (Sample 2). The objective was to check the factorial structure of the questionnaire using the four-factor model obtained previously. The model fit was estimated using the maximum likelihood estimation method (Brown, 2006) was verified using a comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR), reported in the bibliography as adequate for ordinal data (Abad et al., 2011; Byrne, 2012). The expected values for an acceptable fit were around 0.90 for the CFI, TLI, normed fit (NFI) and non-normed fit (NNFI) indices (Kline, 2011). Values under 0.05 for SRMR and under 0.10 for RMSEA, with a 90% confidence interval, indicate reasonable model fit (Browne and Cudeck, 1989; MacCallum et al., 1996). To statistically compare the four-dimensional model, we used the χ2 difference test. The reliability of the CEMA-A was evaluated using omega coefficient (McDonald, 1999). The omega coefficient (ω) is more precise than Cronbach’s alpha (α) because reliability can be directly calculated using the estimates of the CFA parameters, resulting in much greater stability when dealing with non-continuous data (Gadermann et al., 2012; Dunn et al., 2014).
Thirdly, the factorial invariance of the CEMA-A based on gender was analyzed with the 1,202 participants (Sample 2), using the multigroup CFA. The configural invariance test shows whether the same items are associated with the same construct. After checking the configural invariance we tested the metric invariance by restricting the factorial loadings of similar items, so that they were the same in the different groups. To determine the metric invariance of the groups, we performed a Δχ2 test (Sass, 2011). If the metric model does not differ from the configural model, the metric invariance is inferred.
Fourthly, to analyze convergent and discriminant validity, Sample 1 participants completed the EPQ-R questionnaire. The association between the CEMA-A and the EPQ-R scales was analyzed using Pearson correlation.
Fifthly, with 1,722 participants (Sample 3), we analyzed the mean differences of the different factors of the CEMA-A by MANOVA, according to gender and level of education. The MANOVA effect size was estimated using partial η2, considering 0.01 as small, 0.06 as medium and 0.14 as large.
Finally, we used the test–retest method (Aldridge et al., 2017) to analyze the stability of the CEMA-A (Sample 4), after four weeks. Vuong’s (1989) test was used to compare the predicted probabilities of non-nested models. First, it allows us to check whether two models are distinguishable, and then, to determines whether the second model shows a better fit than the first. Under the premise of the null hypothesis, it is proposed that the two non-nested models fit equally well, that is, the expected value of their log-likelihood coefficient is equal to zero.
3 Results
3.1 Exploratory factor analysis
Sample 1 (N = 529) was used to verify that the properties of the data were adequate to perform EFA. The Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin index (KMO = 0.96) and Bartlett’s test of sphericity were significant (χ2(990) = 16,501; p < 0.001), indicating that the analysis was feasible. The parallel analysis method (Horn, 1965) was used to decide the number of factors to extract. The scree test is a graphical representation of the magnitude of the eigenvalues and helps to identify the optimal number of factors that should be extracted. The scree test yielded only four factors that were included in the final scale (Figure 1). EFA (Sample 1) showed a four-factor structure with 43 items that explained 54.35% of the total variance (Intrapersonal Motivation, 18.37%; Egoism/Hardness Motivation, 15.45%; Interpersonal Motivation, 14.77%; and Malicious Motivation, 5.75%). Table 2 shows the standard deviation, skewness, kurtosis, and factor loading of each item. Of the 45 original CEMA-A items, two were deleted (item 23: “To feign a life I do not have”; item 32: “Out of jealousy”) because their means were too low and produced a floor effect. Table 3 shows the eigenvalue, explained and cumulative variance, as well as the Cronbach’s alpha and Omega hierarchical reliability of the four factors of the CEMA-A with the 43 items.
Table 2. Mean (M), standard deviation (SD), skewness, kurtosis and factor loading for CEMA-A (43 items).
Next, a first-order EFA was applied again. Items whose factor loading was <0.40 and those that saturated in two factors were eliminated (≥ 0.30). Based on these criteria, the following items were eliminated: 7, 9, 13 and 39 of the Intrapersonal Motivation factor; items 23, 28, 32, 37, and 45 of the Egoism/Hardness Motivation factor; items 3, 11, 15, 21, 26, and 41 of the Interpersonal Motivation factor; and items 4 and 25 of the Malicious Motivation factor. With the 28 items, the KMO index was 0.95 and Bartlett’s sphericity test was again significant (χ2(378) = 10,026; p < 0.001). The four-factor structure was maintained with the 28 items. Internal consistency was calculated using Cronbach’s alpha and Hierarchical Omega, which were 0.95 and 0.77, respectively, for the total scale. The Intrapersonal Motivation factor showed α = 0.92 and ωj = 0.72; the Egoism/Hardness Motivation factor, α = 0.93 and ωj = 0.83; the Interpersonal Motivation factor, α = 0.89 and ωj = 0.77; and for the Malicious Motivation factor it was α = 0.77 and ωj = 0.72. Of the final structure of 28 items, the factors for Intrapersonal Motivation, Egoism/Hardness, Interpersonal Motivation, and Malicious Motivation explained 18.74, 16.93, 15.84, and 6.59% of the total variance, respectively. As can be seen, the correlation between the different factors was high, mainly between Intrapersonal Motivation and Egoism/Hardness Motivation (r = 0.70) (Table 4).
3.2 Confirmatory factor analysis
To study the dimensional structure of the scale, we performed CFA with Sample 2, based on the model obtained with Sample 1. To analyze construct validity, we used a four-factor model with the 28 items, using the maximum likelihood estimation method. Figure 1 displays the results of the CFA of the four-factor model. To better evaluate the model parameters, taking into account the recommendations of other authors (Brown, 2015), we considered several indices simultaneously. Figure 1 shows the best fit model and normalized path coefficients for each variable observed. All item loadings were found to be at an acceptable level (≥ 0.47), and all parameter estimates were significantly different from 0. Latent correlation indices between model factors were high, for example, the latent correlation between the Egoism/Hardness and Malicious Motivation factors was r = 0.81.
When the proposed theoretical model was tested (Figure 2), an adequate fit to the data was obtained (Table 5). Applying the good fit statistics in this model resulted in the following: (χ2 = 1,460.97, df = 325, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.94; TLI = 0.93; NFI = 0.93; NNFI = 0.93; RMSEA = 0.05, CI = 0.051–0.057; SRMR = 0.04). It should be noted that all the parameters indicated in Figure 2 (factorial loadings, correlation between factors and measurement errors of the items) were significant for p < 0.001. Internal consistency was calculated using the McDonald omega coefficient for four factors. The Intrapersonal motivation factor presented ω = 0.91, the Egoism/Hardness motivation factor, ω = 0.88, the Interpersonal motivation factor, ω = 0.84, and the Malicious motivation factor, ω = 0.79.
Figure 2. CEMA a four factor models. F1, Intrapersonal Motivation; F2, Egoism/Hardness Motivation; F3, Interpersonal Motivation; F4, Malicious motivation.
3.3 Invariance of the CEMA-A factorial structure
Multigroup Confirmatory Factor Analysis. To check whether the factorial structure was similar according to gender (configural invariance), the parameters were estimated simultaneously for each gender level. The multigroup CFA fit indices were (χ2 = 2082.76, df = 650, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.93; TLI = 0.92; NFI = 0.90; NNFI = 0.92; RMSEA = 0.06, CI = 0.058–0.063; SRMR = 0.048). Therefore, we can conclude that both the number of factors and the factor loading pattern of the items on the CEMA-A scale are similar for men and women.
Regarding metric invariance, the fit indices were acceptable according to gender (χ2 = 2111.75, df = 674, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.93; TLI = 0.92; NFI = 0.90; NNFI = 0.92; RMSEA = 0.06, CI = 0.057–0.062; SRMR = 0.051). The results show that the fit indices between the configural model and the metric model did not differ according to gender (Δχ2 = 28.99, Δdf = 24, p = 0.220) (see Table 5).
3.4 Differences in the sociodemographic data
To explore whether the CEMA-A questionnaire was useful for differentiating the motives for lying of people with different sociodemographic profiles, MANOVA was performed with the total sample (Sample 3). The Intrapersonal, Interpersonal, Egoism/Hardness, and Malicious Motivation scales were taken as dependent variables, and gender and educational level as independent variables. Significant differences were found according to gender [F (1,1718) = 21.04, p < 0.001]. Specifically, men scored higher than women in the Egoism/Hardness and Malicious Motivation scales (Table 6).
Regarding educational level, the MANOVA showed significant differences [F (3,1719) = 1.9, p < 0.05], particularly in the Interpersonal Motivation factor. However, after analyzing the post-hoc contrasts, no significant differences were found between the different levels of education (Table 7).
3.5 Convergent and discriminant validity
Convergent and discriminant validity was analyzed using Pearson’s correlation between the CEMA-A and EPQ-R scales (Sample 1). All the CEMA-A factors correlated positively with Neuroticism and Psychoticism, and negatively with L scale, suggesting convergent validity (Table 8). The highest correlations were between Neuroticism and Intrapersonal Motivation (r = 0.37; p <0.001), as well as between Psychoticism with Egoism/Hardness Motivation (r = 0.29; p < 0.001) and with Malicious Motivation (r = 0.31; p <0. 001). The Extraversion factor demonstrated discriminant validity, since no significant correlations were found with the CEMA-A factors, except for a low negative correlation with Intrapersonal Motivation (r = −0.09; p < 0.05).
3.6 Score stability (test–retest)
Vuong’s (1989) test was applied to assess whether there were differences between the two non-nested models. Both models were verified as indistinguishable (variance test), and the fit of both models was equal for the focal population (non-nested likelihood ratio test) in the four categories of motivations to lie. The test–retest correlation was 0.86 for Intrapersonal Motivation (pretest: z 0.825, p = 0.21; retest: z 0.825, p = 0.80), 0.81 for Intrapersonal Motivation (pretest: z 1.248, p = 0.10; retest: z 1.258, p = 0.90), 0.93 for Egoism/Hardness Motivation (pretest: z 1.225, p = 0.11; retest: z 1.225, p = 0.89), and 0.77, for Malicious Motivation, (pretest: z 0.616, p = 0.27; retest: z 0.616, p = 0.73).
4 Discussion
The aim of this study was to verify the stability of the factorial structure of the CEMA-A questionnaire in the Spanish adult population. The results showed that the CEMA-A has adequate psychometric properties and is valid and reliable instrument to measure different motives behind every day lies. The new structure of the of the 28-item CEMA-A instrument was confirmed, through EFA and CFA, and the four-factor model containing the factors Intrapersonal Motivation, Interpersonal Motivation, Egoism/Hardness Motivation and Malicious Motivation, which concurs with the factorial structure of the preliminary study of 45 items (Armas-Vargas, 2021a). Moreover, the temporal stability of the measurement instrument scores was verified.
The general category Egoism–Hardness Motivation of the CEMA-A encompasses various subcategories of motives focused on obtaining personal benefits, such as instrumental motives (item 12 “to get what I want”; item 36 “to benefit from something”), motives related to manipulation of others (item 44 “because it is easier to manipulate others”; item 6 “to try to win in an argument with someone”), or motives related to showing a positive self-image (item 5 “to impress others”; item 27 “to give a good image of myself”). Instrumental and manipulative motives are related to those proposed in Levine et al.’s (2016) pancultural model: “non-monetary personal advantage,” while the motives related to showing a positive self-image of the CEMA-A are equivalent, in a way, to Levine et al.’s (2016) “self-image management.” In the case of CEMA, it also includes the search for admiration. The general category Malicious Motivation of the CEMA-A includes content related to harming others (item 1 “to generate doubts about another person”; item 40 “to falsely accuse someone and cause harm”) and has a certain similarity with the “malicious” category of Levine et al.’s (2016) pancultural model. The contents of the Egoism/Hardness and Malicious Motivations find a parallel with the type of serious lies proposed by DePaulo et al. (2004). According to these authors, people who tell serious lies try to profit from dubious deals, and lie instrumentally to get what they want, and to avoid punishment. The truths behind serious lies are often shameful, immoral, or illegal (DePaulo et al., 2004; Palena et al., 2021). Similarly, people high on Machiavellianism tend to engage in “immoral” behaviors to achieve their goals (Monaghan et al., 2020).
The general category Interpersonal Motivation focuses on motives that try to maintain positive social relationships and includes content on prosocial-empathy (item 35 “to make others feel good”; item 43 “to be kind and cordial with others”), sociability and agreeableness (Item 2 “to not offend others”), hide information that could cause harm (item 11 “to hide certain problems or difficulties”), or avoid problems with others (item 18 “to avoid problems with others”; item 34 “to hide something that I know is wrong”). The content of this category is related to the motives proposed in Levine et al.’s (2016) pancultural model, such as altruistic lies, social politeness, personal transgression, and evasion, respectively.
The general category Intrapersonal Motivation includes new content related to self-deception that has not been addressed in the area of motives for lying in the literature (Armas-Vargas, 2021a). Some of the reasons related to self-deception are “so as not to face the truth” (item 17), “for fear of facing reality” (item 19), “because it is difficult for me to accept things as they are” (item 30), where self-deception occurs through denial of a real problem and acting as if it did not exist (Goleman, 1985; Cohen, 2001; Zerubavel, 2006; Friedrichs, 2014). At some point in their lives, people may be exposed to unpleasant or traumatic situations that lead to the need for self-deception in order to survive the negative experience. Self-deception is the result of a functional and adaptive system in the protection of the self and the regulation of goals. It is not pathological in itself, since most people use it at some point in their lives (Sirvent et al., 2019). Some authors consider that self-deception can lead to a gain, such as improving self-image (Starek and Keating, 1991; Bachkirova, 2016). Other authors emphasize its function as an avoidance strategy, such as avoiding distress (Fingarette, 1969; Sackeim, 1983). It has also been proposed that self-deception may arise from selective attention, whereby certain information is ignored or dismissed, despite evidence (Greenwald, 1997; Sharot, 2011), Other research suggests that self-deception is a cognitive process of biasing information to obtain or maintain a false belief that may be beneficial or detrimental to oneself (Mei et al., 2022). A close relationship has been found between self-deception and deception of others (Lu and Chang, 2014). Self-deception functions as an automatic mechanism of protection and adaptation of the “I,” which ultimately seeks to safeguard the psychic order (Armas-Vargas, 2020). These types of reasons fulfil the objective of hiding and/or denying evidence that we do not know or do not want to accept, which, if rejected, would leave us psychologically unprotected (Armas-Vargas, 2020, 2021a). Specifically, there is gain in self-deception: distress is avoided, real damage is minimized, and benefits such as subjective and interpersonal well-being and improving self-image are obtained (Friedrichs, 2014; Bachkirova, 2016). In the “process” of self-deception, many strategies that people use escape their control and awareness. Many implicit and automatic processes may be outside volitional reach (Bargh, 1990; Bargh et al., 2001). The evaluation of self-deception is therefore carried out as an experience already lived and past, whereby the person realizes (either by themselves or with the help of a professional) that they have been self-deceived (Armas-Vargas, 2017a,b, 2020). What is evaluated, therefore, is not the self-deception in the moment, but rather that the person was self-deceived (Martínez-Manrique, 2007).
In addition, intra-personally motivated lying includes personal and emotional reasons that evaluate content related to insecurity, problems of self-esteem, shame, or fear of what others will say (item 10 “because I do not accept myself as I am”; item 16 “so as not to reveal my own meanness”; item 22 “because I feel insecure”). These motives are responsible for adapting reality to our emotional and psychological needs, to protect our identity, self-esteem, and the image others have of us (Turner et al., 1975; Buller and Burgoon, 1996; Armas-Vargas, 2020, 2021a). Many of these emotional motives may be implicit or escape awareness (McClelland et al., 1989; Bargh and Chartrand, 1999; Bargh et al., 2001; Custers and Aarts, 2005).
The relationship between the CEMA-A and EPQ-R factors confirms convergent validity and evidences the role of personality in the motives for lying (Buller and Burgoon, 1996; Olson and Weber, 2004; McLeod and Genereux, 2008; Harhoff et al., 2023). One study found that coldness when lying (e.g., “I do not usually have remorse when I lie”) was positively related to Psychoticism, whereas emotional self-regulation when lying (e.g., “I feel guilty when I’m caught in a lie”) was negatively related. On the other hand, the Neuroticism factor has been found to positively correlate with Self-Deception, Insecurity, or Fear of Rejection and Criticism (Armas-Vargas, 2021b). Neuroticism has been related to the propensity to lie and to different types of lying (Phillips et al., 2011; Hart et al., 2020). Extraversion was not related to any of the CEMA-A factors, only showing a low negative correlation with Intrapersonal Motivation. Extraverted people tend to minimize, hide, and/or deny negative characteristics about themselves, to create a favorable impression to others (DePaulo et al., 1996; Tyler and Feldman, 2004; Armas-Vargas, 2021b).
Likewise, invariance analyses confirmed the equivalence for men and women of the measurements obtained by the instrument. Men scored higher in Egoism/Hardness Motivation and Malicious Motivation, which coincides with the pilot study (Armas-Vargas, 2021a). However, these differences must be taken with caution due to the small effect size found. However, Tyler and Feldman (2004) suggest that men and women may have different reasons for lying depending on circumstance. For women, lies are related to feigning positive feelings others, rather than being selfish (DePaulo et al., 1996; Tyler et al., 2006). A more self-centered lie may attempt to obtain a psychical rather than a monetary reward (DePaulo et al., 1996). These types of results can be explained through emotional variables, since women, tend to feel more distressed and see serious lies as less justifiable (DePaulo et al., 2004). Men tell more lies for their own benefit, despite potential harm to others, and more lies containing false information to manipulate others’ impressions of them (Phillips et al., 2011).
The CEMA-A has shown adequate psychometric properties, although certain limitations should be considered. Firstly, there is no consensus around a single type of motive for lying (Seiter and Bruschke, 2007; Guthrie and Kunkel, 2013). Secondly, the four categories do not include all the reasons for lying, they are not exhaustive or exclusive. Although the CEMA-A was constructed by sampling the different motives for lying that appear in the literature, as well as collecting those such as self-deception that were not assessed through self-report, future research may find other reasons not identified thus far. Thirdly, response biases may occur, both due to the content of the test itself (lies) and because it is a self-reported measure. This type of bias could be minimized by using a social desirability scale.
In future, analysis of the invariance in clinical and forensic samples, and in other cultures, could be interesting. Lying depends largely on the ethical and moral values of individuals and cultural conventions. Behaviors that are immoral in one culture may not be immoral in another (Kwiatkowska, 2015). Thus, it is important to identify whether the reasons for lying are similar, regardless of cross-cultural differences. Conversely, the reasons may vary, depending on whether the culture is individualistic or collectivist (Giles et al., 2019). In this sense, it could be of interest to adapt the CEMA-A to other cultures and verify its factorial invariance in different cultures. In addition, the CEMA-A questionnaire on motives for lying can be used to identify profiles of individuals according to their personality characteristics (e.g., the characteristics that define the person whose main motivation for lying is personal–emotional (fears, insecurity), as opposed to another whose motives are more focused on manipulating or instrumentalizing others). Previous research has shown that people with high anxiety, low self-esteem, and high Machiavellianism have motivations that will benefit them or others, whereas lies with protective motivation are associated with high empathy and low Machiavellianism (Cantarero et al., 2018). Furthermore, the CEMA-A could capture the motives for lying of different pathological populations, such as in the dark triad (psychopathy, Machiavellianism, and narcissism), where more malicious motives could appear. Michels et al. (2020) found a relationship between the dark triad and lying ability to achieve one’s objectives, though this relationship was moderated by intelligence. In the same line, it could be of interest to use an instrument on lies in the forensic population, such as gender violence, or in contentious procedures for the custody of children. Intrapersonal motives may appear in victims of gender violence, while in aggressors the motivation would be more instrumental or malicious. In men convicted of gender violence, self-deception and an absolutist morality have been found to explain in some way the violent behavior against their partners (Vecina, 2018). Future studies could examine whether the CEMA-A questionnaire is useful for identifying populations that have a greater propensity to lie, depending on type of motive.
In summary, the CEMA-A questionnaire is based on an exhaustive review of the literature on motives for lying, including from social psychology models and personality psychology. The instrument therefore provides an empirical framework to identify the various motives for lying. They are grouped into four broad categories in which intrapersonal motivation related to self-deception and individual differences, previously little studied as motives for lying in the literature, play a major role. The CEMA-A has proven to be an adequate instrument for identifying categories, motives, situations, and moments that lead to lying; it is the first instrument in Spanish to assess motives for lying. These findings have important practical implications and could be a useful tool for analyzing the motives for lying in different clinical, forensic, and/or employment contexts. These types of lies may be interesting for future research on lying and understanding liars.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
The studies involving humans were approved by the Research Ethics and Animal Welfare Committee of the University of La Laguna (Registration Number: CEIBA2023-3299). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author contributions
EA-V: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. RM: Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. JH-C: Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Project administration, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank the participants for their participation and involvement in this study.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1289209/full#supplementary-material
References
Abad, F. J., Olea, J., Ponsoda, V., and y García, C. (2011). Medición en ciencias sociales y de la salud [Measurement in Social and Educational Sciences]. Madrid, España: Síntesis.
Aldridge, V. K., Dovey, T. M., and Wade, A. (2017). Assessing test-retest reliability of psychological measures: persistent methodological problems. Eur. Psychol. 22, 207–218. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000298
Alicke, M. D., Klotz, M. L., Breitenbecher, D. L., Yurak, T. J., and Vredenburg, D. S. (1995). Personal contact, individuation, and the better-than-average effect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 68, 804–825. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.68.5.804
Arcimowicz, B., Cantarero, K., and y Soroko, E. (2015). Motivation and consequences of lying. A qualitative analysis of everyday lying. Forum qualitative. Soc. Res. 16:21. doi: 10.17169/fqs-16.3.2311
Armas-Vargas, E. (2017a). “Adaptación del cuestionario ATRAMIC: Personalidad y predisposición a mentir en adolescentes” in Psicología Jurídica: Conocimiento y Práctica. eds. C. Bringas and M. Novo (Sevilla: España)
Armas-Vargas, E. (2017b). “Autoengaño: Autoconocimiento y autoestima” in En Actas del III Congreso Nacional de Psicología (Oviedo, España: Consejo General de Psicología), 212–218.
Armas-Vargas, E. (2020). Autoengaño y mentira en adolescentes: Personalidad y autoestima. En A. M. Martín, F. Fariña, and R. Arce (Eds.), Psicología Jurídica y Forense: Investigación para la Práctica Profesional. Madrid: España
Armas-Vargas, E. (2021a). Motivos para mentir (CEMA-A): predisposición a mentir y sesgo de respuesta. En L. Rodríguez Franco, D. Seijo, and F. Fariña (Eds.), Ciencia psicológica al servicio de la justicia y la ley. Vigo: España.
Armas-Vargas, E. (2021b). La mentira como rasgo disposicional (Test ATRAMIC): personalidad y tendencia a mentir. En L. Rodríguez Franco, D. Seijo, and F. Fariña (Eds.), Ciencia psicológica al servicio de la justicia y la ley. Vigo: España.
Bachkirova, T. (2016). A new perspective on self-deception for applied purposes. New Ideas Psychol. 43, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2016.02.004
Bargh, J. A. (1990). “Auto-motives: preconscious determinants of social interaction” in Handbook of motivation and cognition. eds. E. T. Higgins and R. M. Sorrentino (New York: Guilford), 93–130.
Bargh, J. A., and Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being. Am. Psychol. 54, 462–479. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.54.7.462
Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Lee-Chai, A., Barndollar, K., and Trötschel, R. (2001). The automated will: nonconscious activation and pursuit of behavioral goals. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 81, 1014–1027. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.81.6.1014
Baumeister, R. F. (1993). “Lying to yourself: the enigma of self-deception” in Lying and deception in everyday life. eds. M. Lewis and C. Saarni (The Guilford Press)
Bond, C. F., and DePaulo, B. M. (2006). Accuracy of deception judgments. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 10, 214–234. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr1003_2
Brown, T. A. (2015). Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Publications.
Browne, M. W., and Cudeck, R. (1989). Single sample cross-validation indices for covariance structures. Multivar. Behav. Res. 24, 445–455. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr2404_4
Buller, D. B., and Burgoon, J. K. (1996). Interpersonal deception theory. Commun. Theory 6, 203–242. doi: 10.1002/9781118540190.wbeic170
Buller, D. B., Burgoon, J. K., White, C. H., and Ebesu, A. S. (1994). Interpersonal deception VII: behavioral profiles of falsification, equivocation, and concealment. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 13, 366–395. doi: 10.1177/0261927X94134002
Byrne, B. M. (2012). Structural equation modeling with Mplus: Basic concepts, applications, and programming. New York, NY: Routledge.
Cantarero, K., Van Tilburg, W. A. P., and Szarota, P. (2018). Differentiating everyday lies: a typology of lies based on beneficiary and motivation. Personal. Individ. Differ. 134, 252–260. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.05.013
Cohen, S. (2001). States of denial: Knowing about atrocities and suffering. Cambridge, UK: Malden, Ma., Polity.
Curtis, D. A. (2021). You liar! Attributions of lying. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 40, 504–523. doi: 10.1177/0261927X21999692
Curtis, D. A., and Hart, C. L. (2015). Pinocchio’s nose in therapy?: therapists’ attitudes and beliefs toward client deception. Int. J. Adv. Couns. 37, 279–292. doi: 10.1007/s10447-015-9243-6
Curtis, D. A., and Hart, C. L. (2022). Pathological lying: Theory, research, and practice. Washington: American Psychological Association.
Custers, R., and Aarts, H. (2005). Positive affect as implicit motivator: on the nonconscious operation of behavioral goals. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 89, 129–142. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.2.129
DePaulo, B. M., Ansfield, M. E., Kirkendol, S. E., and Boden, J. M. (2004). Serious lies. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 26, 147–167. doi: 10.1207/s15324834basp2602&3_4
DePaulo, B. M., and Kashy, D. A. (1998). Everyday lies in close and casual relationships. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 63–79. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.63
DePaulo, B. M., Kashy, D. A., Kirkendol, S. E., Wyer, M. M., and Epstein, J. A. (1996). Lying in everyday life. J Pers. Soc. Psychol. 70, 979–995. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.5.979
DePaulo, B. M., Lindsay, J. J., Malone, B. E., Muhlenbruck, L., Charlton, K., and Cooper, H. (2003b). Cues to deception. Psychol. Bull. 129, 74–118. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.1.74
DePaulo, B. M., Wetzel, C., Sternglanz, R. W., and Walker Wilson, M. J. (2003a). Verbal and nonverbal dynamics of privacy, secrecy, and deceit. J. Soc. Issues 59, 391–410. doi: 10.1111/1540-4560.00070
Dunn, T. J., Baguley, T., and Brunsden, V. (2014). From alpha to omega: a practical solution to the pervasive problem of internal consistency estimation. Br. J. Psychol. 105, 399–412. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12046
Ekman, P. (1989). “Why lies fail and what behaviors betray a lie” in Credibility assessment. ed. J. C. Yuille (Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer), 71–82.
Ennis, E., Vrij, A., and Chance, C. (2008). Individual differences and lying in everyday life. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 25, 105–118. doi: 10.1177/0265407507086808
Eysenck, H. J., and Eysenck, S. B. G. (1997). EPQ-R. Cuestionario revisado de personalidad de Eysenck. Madrid: TEA
Feldman, R. S., Forrest, J. A., and Happ, B. R. (2002). Self-presentation and verbal deception: do self-presenters lie more? Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 24, 163–170. doi: 10.1207/S15324834BASP2402_8
Friedrichs, J. (2014). Useful lies: the twisted rationality of denial. Philos. Psychol. 27, 212–234. doi: 10.1080/09515089.2012.724354
Fullam, R. S., McKie, S., and Dolan, M. C. (2009). Psychopathic traits and deception: Functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Br J Psychiatry. 194, 229–235. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.053199
Gadermann, A. M., Guhn, M., and Zumbo, B. D. (2012). Estimating ordinal reliability for Likert-type and ordinal item response data: a conceptual, empirical, and practical guide. Pract. Assess. Res. Eval. 17, 1–13. doi: 10.7275/n560-j767
Gerlach, P., Teodorescu, K., and Hertwig, R. (2019). The truth about lies: a meta-analysis on dishonest behavior. Psychol. Bull. 145, 1–44. doi: 10.1037/bul0000174
Giammarco, E. A., Atkinson, B., Baughman, H. M., Veselka, L., and Vernon, P. A. (2013). The relation between antisocial personality and the perceived ability to deceive. Personality and Individual Differences 54, 246–250. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.09.004
Giles, R. M., Rothermich, K., and Pell, M. D. (2019). Differences in the evaluation of prosocial lies: a cross-cultural study of Canadian, Chinese, and German adults. Front. Commun. 4:38. doi: 10.3389/fcomm.2019.00038
Gil-Escudero, G., and Martínez-Arias, M. R. (2001). “Metodología de encuestas [Survey methodology]” in Métodos, Diseños y Técnicas de Investigación Psicológica. ed. M. J. Navas (Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia)
Goleman, D. (1985). Vital lies, simple truths: The psychology of self-deception. New York, NY: Simon and Schuster.
Greenwald, A. G. (1997). Validity concerns and usefulness of student ratings of instruction. Am Psychol. 52, 1182–1186. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.52.11.1182
Gudjonsson, G. H., and Y Sigurdsson, J. F. (2004). The relationship of suggestibility and compliance with self-deception and other-deception. Psychol Crime Law. 10, 447–453. doi: 10.1080/10683160310001634278
Guthrie, J., and Kunkel, A. (2013). Tell me sweet (and not-so-sweet) little lies: deception in romantic relationships. Commun. Stud. 64, 141–157. doi: 10.1080/10510974.2012.755637
Harhoff, N., Reinhardt, N., Reinhard, M. A., and Mayer, M. (2023). Agentic and communal narcissism in predicting different types of lies in romantic relationships. Front. Psychol. 14:1146732. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1146732
Hart, C. L., Jones, J. M., Terrizzi, J. A., and Curtis, D. A. (2019). Development of the lying in everyday situations scale. Am. J. Psychol. 132, 343–352. doi: 10.5406/amerjpsyc.132.3.0343
Hart, C., Lemon, R., Curtis, D., and Griffith, J. (2020). Personality traits associated with various forms of lying. Psychol. Stud. 65, 239–246. doi: 10.1007/s12646-020-00563-x
Haselton, M. G., Buss, D. M., Oubaid, V., and Angleitner, A. (2005). Sex, lies, and strategic interference: the psychology of deception between the sexes. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 31, 3–23. doi: 10.1177/0146167204271303
Hernández, J. A., and Betancort, M. (2018). ULLRtoolbox. Available at: https://sites.google.com/site/ullrtoolbox/
Horn, J. L. (1965). A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometrika 30, 179–185. doi: 10.1007/BF02289447
Jensen, L., Arnett, J., Feldman, S., and Cauffman, E. (2004). The right to do wrong: lying to parents among adolescents and emerging adults. J. Youth Adolesc. 33, 101–112. doi: 10.1023/B:JOYO.0000013422.48100.5a
Kashy, D. A., and DePaulo, B. M. (1996). Who lies? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 70, 1037–1051. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.5.1037
Keltner, D., and Buswell, B. N. (1996). Evidence for the distinctness of embarrassment, shame, and guilt: a study of recalled antecedents and facial expressions of emotion. Cognit. Emot. 10, 155–172. doi: 10.1080/026999396380312
Kline, R. B. (2011). “Convergence of structural equation modeling and multilevel modeling” in The SAGE handbook of innovation in social research methods. eds. M. Williams and W. P. Vogt (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage), 562–589.
Knapp, M. L., and Comadena, M. A. (1979). Telling it like it isn’t: a review of theory and research on deceptive communications. Hum. Commun. Res. 5, 270–285. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2958.1979.tb00640.x
Kwiatkowska, A. (2015). “How do others deceive? Cultural aspects of lying and cheating” in The small and big deceptions: In psychology and evolutionary sciences perspective. eds. A. Kwiatkowska and A. Łukasik (Rzeszów, Poland: Wydawnictwo), 46–72.
Levine, T. R. (2014). Truth-default theory (TDT): a theory of human deception and deception detection. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 33, 378–392. doi: 10.1177/0261927X14535916
Levine, T. R., Ali, M. V., Dean, M., Abdulla, R. A., and Garcia-Ruano, K. (2016). Toward a pan-cultural typology of deception motives. J. Intercult. Commun. Res. 45, 1–12. doi: 10.1080/17475759.2015.1137079
Levine, T. R., Kim, R. K., and Hamel, L. M. (2010). People lie for a reason: three experiments documenting the principle of veracity. Commun. Res. Rep. 27, 271–285. doi: 10.1080/08824096.2010.496334
Levine, T. R., Serota, K. B., Carey, F., and Messer, D. (2013). Teenagers lie a lot: a further investigation into the prevalence of lying. Commun. Res. Rep. 30, 211–220. doi: 10.1080/08824096.2013.806254
Levine, T. R., Serota, K. B., and Punyanunt-Carter, N. M. (2022). Sender and receiver lie frequencies and motives: testing predictions from truth-default theory. South Commun. J. 87, 220–234. doi: 10.1080/1041794X.2022.2052745
Lindskold, S., and Walters, P. S. (1983). Categories for acceptability of lies. J. Soc. Psychol. 120, 129–136. doi: 10.1080/00224545.1983.9712018
Lu, H. J., and Chang, L. (2014). Deceiving yourself to better deceive high-status compared to equal-status others. Evol. Psychol. 12, 635–654. doi: 10.1177/147470491401200310
MacCallum, R. C., Browne, M. W., and Sugawara, H. M. (1996). Power analysis and determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psychol. Methods 1, 130–149. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.1.2.130
Masip, J., Garrido, E., and y Herrero, C. (2004). Defining deception. Anales de Psicología 20, 147–171.
Mazar, N., Amir, O., and Ariely, D. (2008). The dishonesty of honest people: a theory of self-concept maintenance. J. Mark. Res. 45, 633–644. doi: 10.1509/jmkr.45.6.633
McArthur, J., Jarvis, R., Bourgeois, C., and Ternes, M. (2022). Lying motivations: exploring personality correlates of lying and motivations to lie. Can. J. Behav. Sci. 54, 335–340. doi: 10.1037/cbs0000328
McClelland, D. C., Koestner, R., and Weinberger, J. (1989). How do self-attributed and implicit motives differ? Psychol. Rev. 96, 690–702. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.96.4.690
McCornack, S. A., and Levine, T. R. (1990). When lies are uncovered: emotional and relational outcomes of discovered deception. Commun. Monogr. 57, 119–138. doi: 10.1080/03637759009376190
McCornack, S. A., Morrison, K., Paik, J. E., Wisner, A. M., and Zhu, X. (2014). Information manipulation theory 2: a propositional theory of deceptive discourse production. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 33, 348–377. doi: 10.1177/0261927X14534656
McLeod, B. A., and Genereux, R. L. (2008). Predicting the acceptability and likelihood of lying: the interaction of personality with type of lie. Personal. Individ. Differ. 45, 591–596. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.06.015
Mead, N. L., Baumeister, R. F., Gino, F., Schweitzer, M. E., and Ariely, D. (2009). Too tired to tell the truth: self-control resource depletion and dishonesty. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 45, 594–597. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.02.004
Mei, D., Ke, Z., Li, Z., Zhang, W., Gao, D., and Yin, L. (2022). Self-deception: distorted metacognitive process in ambiguous contexts. Hum. Brain Mapp. 44, 948–969. doi: 10.1002/hbm.26116
Metts, S. (1994). “Relational transgressions” in The dark side of interpersonal communication. eds. W. R. Cupach and B. H. Spitzberg (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum)
Michels, M., Molz, G., and Bermpohl, F. M. (2020). The ability to lie and ist relations tot he dark triad and general intelligence. Personal. Individ. Differ. 166:110195. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110195
Millar, K. U., and Tesser, A. (1988). Deceptive behavior in social relationships: a consequence of violated expectations. J. Psychol. Interdisc. Appl. 122, 263–273. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1988.9915514
Monaghan, C., Bizumic, B., Williams, T., and Sellbom, M. (2020). Two-dimensional Machiavellianism: conceptualization, theory, and measurement of the views and tactics dimensions. Psychol. Assess. 32, 277–293. doi: 10.1037/pas0000784
Moshagen, M., Zettler, I., and Hilbig, B. E. (2020). Measuring the dark core of personality. Psychol. Assess. 32, 182–196. doi: 10.1037/pas0000778
Mosher, D. L. (1968). Measurement of guilt in females by self-report inventories. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 32, 690–695. doi: 10.1037/h0026589
Muñiz, J., and Fonseca-Pedrero, E. (2019). Diez pasos para la construcción de un test. Psicothema 31, 7–16. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2018.291
Muzinic, L., Kozaric-Kovacic, D., and Marinic, I. (2016). Psyquiatric aspectos of normal and pathological lying. Int. J. Law Psychiatry 46, 88–93.
Olson, K. R., and Weber, D. A. (2004). Relations between big five traits and fundamental motives. Psychol. Rep. 95, 795–802. doi: 10.2466/PR0.95.7.795-802
Palena, N., Caso, L., Cavagnis, L., and Greco, A. (2021). Profiling the interrogee: applying the person-centered approach in investigative interviewing research. Front. Psychol. 12:722893. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.722893
Phillips, M. C., Meek, S. W., and Vendemia, J. (2011). Understanding the underlying structure of deceptive behaviors. Personal. Individ. Differ. 50, 783–789. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.12.031
R Core Team. (2017). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
Rosseel, Y. (2012). Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. J. Stat. Softw. 48, 1–36. doi: 10.18637/jss.v048.i02
Sass, D. A. (2011). Testing measurement invariance and comparing latent factor means within a confirmatory factor analysis framework. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 29, 347–363. doi: 10.1177/0734282911406661
Sackeim, H. A. (1983). “Self-deception, self-esteem, and depression: The adaptive value of lying to oneself” in Empirical studies of psychoanalytic theories. ed. J. Masling (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), 101–157.
Schooler, J. W., and Schreiber, C. (2005). To know or not to know: Consciousness, metaconsciousness, and motivation. in Social motivation: Conscious and non-conscious processes. J. P. Forgas, K. R. Forgas, and W. von Hippel (Eds.) (New York: Cambridge University Press), 351–372.
Seiter, J. S., and Bruschke, J. (2007). Deception and emotion: the effects of motivation, relationship type, and sex on expected feelings of guilt and shame following acts of deception in United States and Chinese samples. Commun. Stud. 58, 1–16. doi: 10.1080/10510970601168624
Seiter, J. S., Bruschke, J., and Bai, C. (2002). The acceptability of deception as a function of perceivers’ culture, deceiver’s intention, and deceiver-deceived relationship. West. J. Commun. 66, 158–180. doi: 10.1080/10570310209374731
Serota, K. B., and Levine, T. R. (2015). A few prolific liars: variation in the prevalence of lying. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 34, 138–157. doi: 10.1177/0261927X14528804
Serota, K. B., Levine, T. R., and Boster, F. J. (2010). The prevalence of lying in America: three studies of self-reported lies. Hum. Commun. Res. 36, 2–25. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2958.2009.01366.x
Serota, K. B., Levine, T. R., and Docan-Morgan, T. (2022). Unpacking variation in lie prevalence: prolific liars, bad lie days, or both? Commun. Monogr. 89, 307–331. doi: 10.1080/03637751.2021.1985153
Sirvent, C., Herrero, J., de la Villa Moral, M., and Rodríguez, F. J. (2019). Evaluation of self-deception: Factorial structure, reliability and validity of the SDQ-12 (Self-Deception Questionnaire). PLoS ONE 14:e0210815. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0210815
Solomon, R. C. (2009). “Self, deception, and self-deception in philosophy” in The philosophy of deception. ed. C. Martin (New York: Oxford University Press), 15–36.
Starek, J. E., and Keating, C. F. (1991). Self-deception and its relationship to success in competition. Basic Appl. Soc. Psych. 12, 145–155. doi: 10.1207/s15324834basp1202_2
Tangney, J. P., Miller, R. S., Flicker, L., and Barlow, D. H. (1996). Are shame, guilt, and embarrassment distinct emotions? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 70, 1256–1269. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.6.1256
Teasdale, K., and Kent, G. (1995). The use of deception in nursing. J. Med. Ethics 21, 77–81. doi: 10.1136/jme.21.2.77
Timmerman, M. E., and Lorenzo-Seva, U. (2011). Dimensionality assessment of ordered polytomous items with parallel analysis. Psychol. Methods 16, 209–220. doi: 10.1037/a0023353
Touré-Tillery, M., and Fishbach, A. (2014). How to measure motivation: a guide for the experimental social psychologist. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 8, 328–341. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12110
Trivers, R. (2002). “Self-deception in the service of deceit” in Natural selection and social theory: Selected papers of Robert Trivers. ed. R. Trivers (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 255–293.
Turner, R. E., Edgley, C., and Olmstead, G. (1975). Information control in conversations: honesty is not always the best policy. Kansas J. Sociol. 11, 69–89. doi: 10.17161/STR.1808.6098
Tyler, J. M., and Feldman, R. S. (2004). Truth, lies, and self-presentation: how gender and anticipated future interaction relate to deceptive behavior. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 34, 2602–2615. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2004.tb01994.x
Tyler, J. M., Feldman, R. S., and Reichert, A. (2006). The price of deceptive behavior: disliking and lying to people who lie to us. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 42, 69–77. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2005.02.003
Vecina, M. L. (2018). How can men convicted of violence against women feel moral while holding sexist and violent attitudes? A homeostatic moral model based on self-deception. Am. J. Mens Health 12, 1554–1562. doi: 10.1177/1557988318774218
Vrij, A. (2000). Detecting lies and deceit: The psychology of lying and its implications for professional practice. Chichester: John Wiley and Sons.
Vrij, A. (2008). Detecting lies and deceit: Pitfalls and opportunities. West Sussex, UK: John Wiley.
Vrij, A., and Ganis, G. (2014). “Theories in deception and lie detection” in Credibility assessment: Scientific research and applications. eds. D. C. Raskin, C. R. Honts, and J. C. Kircher (Elsevier Academic Press)
Vuong, H. (1989). Likelihood ratio tests for model selection and non-nested hypotheses. Econometrica 57, 307–333. doi: 10.2307/1912557
Keywords: motives for lying, intrapersonal motivation-emotionality, interpersonal motivation-sociability, egoism/hardness motivation, malicious motivation
Citation: Armas-Vargas E, Marrero RJ and Hernández-Cabrera JA (2023) Psychometric properties of the CEMA-A questionnaire: motives for lying. Front. Psychol. 14:1289209. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1289209
Edited by:
Carmelo Mario Vicario, University of Messina, ItalyReviewed by:
Drew A. Curtis, Angelo State University, United StatesChristian L. Hart, Texas Woman's University, United States
Copyright © 2023 Armas-Vargas, Marrero and Hernández-Cabrera. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Enrique Armas-Vargas, ZXh0ZWFybWFzdmFAdWxsLmVkdS5lcw==