Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 02 October 2023
Sec. Movement Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Physical Education, Health and Education Innovation View all 20 articles

Mediating effect of social interaction anxiety between emotional intelligence and life satisfaction in physical education students: post-COVID-19 study

  • 1Faculty of Sports, Autonomous University of Baja California, Tijuana, Mexico
  • 2Department of Education, University of Almeria, Almeria, Spain
  • 3Health Research Centre, University of Almeria, Almeria, Spain
  • 4Department of Musical, Plastic and Corporal Expression, Faculty of Education Sciences, University of Granada, Ceuta, Spain

The aim of this research is to analyze the effect of social interaction anxiety on satisfaction with life mediated by emotional intelligence. The research design was descriptive, cross-sectional, and non-randomized. In total, 1,164 Mexican physical education students participated (Mage = 21.21; SD = 3.26; 30.0% female; 69.6% male; 0.4% other). The scales used were the Social Interaction Anxiety Scale, Trait Meta-Mood Scale and Satisfaction with Life and a structural equation analysis with latent variables was conducted. The results highlight that it can be asserted that emotional clarity and repair had a mediating effect between social interaction anxiety and satisfaction with life, as they did decrease the negative effects of social interaction anxiety on satisfaction with life. In addition, social interaction anxiety had a direct and positive effect on emotional attention and a negative effect on emotional clarity and repair.

1. Introduction

The restrictive measures in response to the COVID-19 pandemic triggered serious consequences on the general health of the world’s population (Faraci et al., 2022). The spread of the pandemic and the ensuing restrictive policies involving confinement, social distancing and mobility had a major impact on the global economy and the daily lives of people around the world (Di Crosta et al., 2020; Xiong et al., 2020; Gollwitzer et al., 2021; Kuo et al., 2021; Racine et al., 2022). These strategies came with severe negative psychological effects (Özdin and Bayrak Özdin, 2020; Pietrabissa and Simpson, 2020; Obschonka et al., 2021), including anxiety, depression, fear of illness, fear of death, fear of social interaction, post-traumatic stress and insomnia (Di Crosta et al., 2020; Rodríguez-Hidalgo et al., 2020; Torales et al., 2020; Sturgill et al., 2021). Said restrictive measures, meant to decrease community transmission (Weeden and Cornwell, 2020), even led to universities shutting down worldwide (Baños et al., 2022).

As a consequence of the COVID-19 pandemic, university education had to undergo a rapid transition from in-person classes to online learning systems (Baños et al., 2021). This increased the daily frequency of technology overuse among university students, especially smartphones (Bhatnagar et al., 2021). This in turn amplified the irrational fear and stress experienced by middle school (Nguyen et al., 2022) and university students (Zwilling, 2022) of being away from a device that facilitates both general communication and attending academic activities, especially in students who struggle to regulate their emotions (Ercengiz et al., 2020; Brown and Medcalf-Bell, 2022). Thus, COVID-19 not only comes with a dreadful pathology, but it is also the source of numerous secondary problems, such as becoming addicted to the use of the Internet, social networks and any form of media associated with recent technologies (Masrek et al., 2022). In fact, the disruptive use of smartphones surged from the onset of the first wave of COVID-19 (Zwilling, 2022), thereby increasing the incidence of pathologies such as stress and anxiety (Nguyen et al., 2022), which even doubled in some countries during the first confinement period (Amerio et al., 2021; Medda et al., 2022). Therefore, as the amount of time spent on screens (e.g., smartphones, computers, tablets, etc.) increases, physical interaction among people diminishes, which has an impact on their social interaction skills (Masrek et al., 2022). For all these reasons, it would be interesting to determine if the levels of social interaction anxiety (SIA) among students can affect their satisfaction with life once the confinement and social distancing measures are lifted and on-site classes return in a post-pandemic context. Despite the significance that the students’ emotional regulation can have in this situation, few studies have analyzed the effect of SIA on satisfaction with life taking into account the emotional intelligence of the students once they have returned to in-person classes at universities following the end of confinement.

1.1. Social interaction anxiety

SIA refers to intense, individual emotional reactions and avoidance behaviors, such as fear, anxiety and distress regarding one or multiple social interactions (Li, 2020). SIA is a widespread condition that can sometimes become chronic, causing severe impact on a person’s academic, occupational and social functioning, as well as their psychological well-being on a general level (Wittchen et al., 2000; Kessler, 2003; O’Toole et al., 2013). The fear of being judged or negatively criticized is the core motive that, together with the fear of contracting COVID-19, prompted people to avoid social interactions, which in turn affected the individuals’ daily functioning (Erliksson et al., 2020). Not addressing this pathology immediately and otherwise allowing it to develop in young people may lead to detrimental effects on their mental health and undermine their academic work and their lives in general (Chartrand et al., 2011). As previously mentioned, social interaction was rare or even non-existent during the pandemic, which could easily trigger SIA and unhealthy emotions, especially in young students who continued their education at home (Hahn, 2020). As age increases, so do the academic load and pressure put on young people, which can lead to an increase in interpersonal communication problems; therefore, the social environment can become overwhelming, leading to psychological conditions common in contemporary youth (Li, 2020). This can be particularly detrimental to students about to graduate, as their interpersonal environment has grown increasingly complex and they may become anxious more easily when facing interpersonal problems (Kwon et al., 2018).

People with high levels of SIA exhibit low self-esteem, depressive symptoms and increased dissatisfaction with life (Makadi and Koszycki, 2020). SIA has also been related to obsessive-compulsive disorders, depression and generalized anxiety disorder (Erliksson et al., 2020). A major characteristic of people with SIA is a lack of emotional regulation (Kashdan and Steger, 2006; Kashdan and Breen, 2008; Werner et al., 2011). Therefore, acknowledging or understanding emotions may play a significant role in the adaptive regulation of emotions during social interactions that cause the person to become anxious (O’Toole et al., 2013).

1.2. Emotional intelligence

In the university context, emotional intelligence has been highlighted as an adequate tool for coping with stressful situations and achieving successful academic performance and emotional well-being (Parhiala et al., 2018; Guil et al., 2021). Emotional intelligence is defined as an individual’s ability to assess and regulate their own emotions and use them to solve problems and accomplish goals (Salovey et al., 1995). Guil et al. (2021) propose that emotional intelligence is composed of three dimensions: emotional attention (i.e., self-perceptions about the degree to which an individual addresses their own emotional experiences), emotional clarity (i.e., self-perceptions regarding how clearly people understand emotional states) and emotional repair (i.e., self-perceptions about the ability to adequately manage emotions). In general, research has found that higher scores on emotional intelligence are associated with better psychological functioning and well-being, whereas low scores are linked to anxiety (Berenbaum et al., 2003; García-Fernández et al., 2015). Likewise, there are individual differences in the degree of the three dimensions (attention, clarity and repair), with each dimension having a different role (García-Fernández et al., 2015). Enhancing our understanding of broad emotional constructs and discrete emotions in SAD can have implications for theoretical models of SAD, for clinical assessment and diagnosis, and for treatment (Rozen and Aderka, 2023). Numerous authors have suggested further research on how these three dimensions interact and their relationship with SIA (Turk et al., 2005; Boden and Berenbaum, 2012; García-Fernández et al., 2015; Guil et al., 2019).

In this vein, the predisposition of SIA increases when one does not pay attention to the information that emotions provide or possesses high emotional attention but poor emotional clarity (Boden and Berenbaum, 2012). The role of emotional attention is less clear than those of emotional repair and clarity (García-Fernández et al., 2015). On the one hand, although individuals must pay at least some attention to their emotions in order to understand them and to remediate negative ones, high levels of attention have been found to be detrimental to emotional well-being (Salovey et al., 1995). On the other hand, emotional attention has been negatively related to SIA (Turk et al., 2005; Guil et al., 2019). As can be seen, there is controversy regarding the relationship between SIA and the dimension of emotional attention.

Because the ability to clearly identify one’s emotions is the first step to successful emotional regulation and coping (Butler et al., 2006), the importance of emotional clarity in regulating emotions has been particularly underscored. In this case, as opposed to the dimension of emotional attention, most studies agree that a lack of emotional clarity greatly increases SIA (Dixon-Gordon et al., 2014; Thompson et al., 2017; Butler et al., 2018; Guil et al., 2019). Likewise, individuals with lower levels of emotional clarity tend to describe more paranoid beliefs (Boden and Berenbaum, 2012). O’Toole et al. (2013) suggest that deficits in emotional clarity and difficulties in remediating negative emotions are key factors to consider when addressing SIA. In fact, several studies have negatively related emotional repair to SIA (Bigman et al., 2015; Klemanski et al., 2017; Guil et al., 2019; Masters et al., 2019). Specifically in the university context, it has been found that students who hoped to be more successful in regulating their negative emotions showed fewer signs of anxiety (Catanzaro and Mearns, 1999). Furthermore, middle school students who exhibited higher levels of emotional intelligence during the pandemic scored higher on satisfaction with life (Correa-Barwick et al., 2022; Torres-Gázquez et al., 2023). Along these lines, Sturgill et al. (2021) found that a Mindfulness program with university students increased their emotional intelligence and satisfaction with life, however, studies on this population conducted during the pandemic are scarce.

1.3. Psychological well-being

The concept of psychological well-being is closely linked to the subjective well-being and the quality of life or satisfaction with life (SWL) concepts (Baños et al., 2019). Diener and Emmons (1985) postulated the Subjective Well-Being Theory to analyze people’s SWL, defining “subjective well-being” as the subjective assessment of one’s own life quality, that is, the range of elements from transitory stages to relatively abstract assessments or evaluations of the meaning of one’s life. These authors stated that people can express being satisfied with their lives either from a global evaluation or after making different assessments in specific areas of their lives (e.g., family, work, social relationships, etc.; Diener and Emmons, 1985). Several studies have associated SWL with low levels of SIA (Wittchen et al., 2000; Kessler, 2003; O’Toole et al., 2013) and with high levels of emotional intelligence (Hodzic et al., 2016; Sánchez-Álvarez et al., 2016; Blasco-Belled et al., 2020).

In terms of the dimensions of emotional intelligence, a study conducted in Spain, Portugal and Brazil found that SWL was predicted by emotional clarity and emotional repair, but not by emotional attention (Hodzic et al., 2016). Blasco-Belled et al. (2020) also found that emotional attention was negatively related to subjective well-being, albeit this relationship was not significant in the study conducted by Ramos-Díaz et al. (2019). In this line, it is emphasized that a decrease in emotional attention and an increase in emotional clarity and repair should be the target of interventions in adolescents to improve their SWL (Guerra-Bustamante et al., 2019; Martínez-Marín and Martínez, 2019; Azpiazu et al., 2022; De la Barrera et al., 2023), since the intelligent management of emotions helps to prevent negative feelings and fosters positive ones, thus promoting greater SWL (Sánchez-Álvarez et al., 2016). Several studies have highlighted the importance of emotional clarity and repair for improved psychological functioning (Petrides et al., 2018; Masters et al., 2019), psychological adjustment (Salguero et al., 2012; García-Fernández et al., 2015; Butler et al., 2018) and psychological well-being (Gohm and Clore, 2002; Extremera and Fernández, 2005; Salguero et al., 2012).

1.4. The present study

After analyzing the scientific literature and observing the importance of emotional clarity and repair in preventing SIA and improving people’s SWL, both before and during the COVID-19 pandemic, the predictive analysis of SIA and emotional intelligence on SWL can be deemed relevant, understood that the sanitary restrictions have been lifted. In summary, on the one hand, studies have related SIA to emotional intelligence (Parhiala et al., 2018; Guil et al., 2021) and SIA to satisfaction with life (Wittchen et al., 2000; Kessler, 2003; O’Toole et al., 2013), and, on the other hand, emotional intelligence to satisfaction with life (Hodzic et al., 2016; Sánchez-Álvarez et al., 2016; Blasco-Belled et al., 2020). All these studies were conducted before or during the pandemic, however, we are unaware of the existence of studies that have analyzed emotional intelligence as a mediating variable between SIA and SWL, and if the relationships between these variables have been analyzed following the end of mobility restrictions and the return to in-person university classes. Moreover, research conducted with Mexican students was scarce even before the pandemic. Therefore, this study represents a contribution to the understanding of the relationships among SIA, the dimensions of emotional intelligence and SWL in the Mexican university context. Thus, the objective of this research is to analyze the effect of SIA on SWL mediated by emotional intelligence. Figure 1 shows the hypothesized model for examining the relationships described above. The Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) initiative (Von Elm et al., 2008).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Hypothesized model with the expected correlations.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Design and participants

The research design was descriptive, cross-sectional, observational and non-randomized. The sample was composed of students from the Faculty of Sport of the three campuses (Campus Ensenada, Campus Mexicali and Campus Tijuana) of the Autonomous University of Baja California (Mexico). Inclusion criteria were the following: (i) to be enrolled in the Bachelor’s degree in Physical Activity and Sport at the aforementioned campus and university; exclusion criteria: (i) failure to provide their informed consent for data use in the study; (ii) failure to duly fill out the data collection form. An a priori analysis of the necessary sampling size was conducted to provide an answer to the study objective, considering a structural equation model (SEM) composed of five latent variables and 53 observable variables. The analysis was conducted using the Free Statistics Calculator v.4.0 software (Soper, 2023) and a minimum of 1,151 participants was calculated to detect effect sizes (f2) = 0.163, with a statistical power of 0.99% and a significance level of α = 0.05. In this research 1,164 physical education students (30.0% women; 69.6% men; 0.4% other) from the three campuses of the Faculty of Sports of the Autonomous University of Baja California (19.8%, Campus Ensenada; 30.7%, Campus Tijuana; 49.6%, Campus Tijuana) participated, aged between 17 and 50 years old (M = 21.21; SD = 3.26). There were no lost values in the responses included in the study. Apart from the total sample, 29 questionnaires were discarded because they were filled incorrectly, and 14 because the respondents did not give their consent to participate in the research.

2.2. Instruments

2.2.1. Interaction Anxiety Scale

This study used the scale adapted to the Mexican context by de la Rubia et al. (2013) based on the original version by Mattick and Clarke (1998). This instrument is composed of 20 items that measure social interaction anxiety (e.g., I find it hard to socialize with my classmates. “Se me hace difícil socializar con las personas con las que estudio”). Answers were collected using a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 4 (completely). For this study, the CFA (Confirmatory Factorial Analysis) goodness-of-fit indices were acceptable: χ2/df = 5.01, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.98; TLI = 0.98; RMSEA = 0.059 (90%CI = 0.047, 0.070; pclose = 0.077), SRMR = 0.033.

2.2.2. Emotional intelligence

This study used the Mexican version by Valdivia et al. (2015) adapted from the original version by Salovey et al. (1995). The scale contains 28 items that measure emotional intelligence across three dimensions: emotional attention (8 items; e.g., I frequently think about my feelings. “A menudo pienso en mis sentimientos”), emotional clarity (8 items; e.g., I am capable of understanding my feelings. “Puedo llegar a comprender mis sentimientos”), and emotional repair (8 items; e.g., When I feel sad, I think about all the pleasures in life. “Cuando estoy triste, pienso en todos los placeres de la vida”). Answers were collected using a Likert scale ranging from 1 (completely disagree) to 5 (completely agree). For this study, the CFA goodness-of-fit indices were acceptable: χ2/gl = 4.97, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.98; TLI = 0.97; RMSEA = 0.058 (90%CI = 0.046, 0.068; pclose = 0.072), SRMR = 0.042.

2.2.3. Satisfaction with life

This study used the scale adapted into Spanish by Atienza et al. (2000) to measure satisfaction with life in general based on the original version by Diener and Emmons (1985). The scale contains 5 grouped items that measure satisfaction with life (e.g., My life circumstances are good. “Las circunstancias de mi vida son buenas”). Answers were collected using a Likert scale ranging from 1 (completely disagree) to 5 (completely agree). For this study, the CFA goodness-of-fit indices were acceptable: χ2/gl = 2.55, p = 0.054; CFI = 0.99; TLI = 0.99; RMSEA = 0.037 (90%CI = 0.000, 0.070; pclose = 0.071), SRMR = 0.011.

2.3. Procedure

First, a meeting was held with the three deputy directors and the general director of the Faculty of Sports of the three campuses of the Autonomous University of Baja California (Ensenada, Mexicali and Tijuana). The purpose of the study was explained and permission to apply the questionnaires was requested. Upon granted authorization, the participants were summoned to the institution’s computer room in March 2022. Participants were taught how to fill out the online questionnaires and informed about the importance of the research, that their participation was anonymous, and that there were no right or wrong answers; they were thus asked to be completely honest and were told that they could abandon the study at any time if they desired so. The questionnaire was completed in around 20 min and all participants gave their prior consent for their responses to be included in the study. The research protocol was approved by the Bioethics Committee of the University of Almeria (Ref: UALBIO2023/001).

2.4. Statistical analysis

A structural equation model (SEM) with latent variables was carried out to analyze how SIA is associated with emotional intelligence and satisfaction with life in Mexican university students. For the SEM, a two-step method following Kline (2016) was developed. In step-1, bidirectional relationships between variables were evaluated (i.e., measurement model). In step-2, the predictive effects between the variables were assessed. The SEM was controlled by the variable sex and campus of origin. The following indices were used to evaluate the models: chi square/degrees of freedom (χ2/df), CFI (Comparative Fit Index), TLI (Tucker–Lewis Index), RMSEA (Root Mean Square Error of Approximation) with a confidence interval of 90% (CI), and SRMR (Standardized Root Mean Square Residual). For the χ2/gl ratio, values <2.0 or <5.0 are, respectively, considered excellent (Tabachnick and Fidell, 2019) or acceptable (Hu and Bentler, 1999); for the CFI and TLI, values >0.95 are considered excellent, whereas the range between 0.90 and 0.95 is considered acceptable; for the RMSEA and SRME, values <0.06 are considered excellent, and <0.08, acceptable (Hu and Bentler, 1999; Marsh et al., 2004) Due to the lack of multivariate normality in the SEM (Mardia’s coefficient = 106.82; p < 0.001) the maximum likelihood (ML) method was used with the bootstrapping procedure for 5,000 re-samplings (Kline, 2016). The reliability of each scale was assessed using different parameters: McDonald’s omega (ω), composite reliability (CR), and AVE for measuring convergent validity. Reliability values >0.70 and AVE > 0.50 are deemed acceptable. For this study, even if the SWL scale yields an AVE value <0.50 (i.e., 0.47), such value is deemed acceptable according to Hair et al. (2018), as all the standardized regression weights were significant and >0.50.

3. Results

3.1. Preliminary results

Descriptive statistics and correlations between the different variables are shown in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlations among variables.

3.2. Main results

During step 1, the SEM showed excellent goodness-of-fit indices: χ2/gl = 2.40, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.98; TLI = 0.97; RMSEA = 0.035(90%CI = 0.031; 0.038; pclose = 1.00), SRMR = 0.035. During step 2, the hypothesized SEM yielded a similar and excellent fit: χ2/gl = 2.40, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.98; TLI = 0.97; RMSEA = 0.035(90%CI = 0.031; 0.038; pclose = 1.000), SRMR = 0.035. The model was controlled by the sex and campus of origin variable and reached an explained variance of 31% for SWL, 3% for emotional attention, 4% for emotional repair and 10% for emotional clarity (Figure 2). The correlations among SIA, the emotional intelligence variables (i.e., emotional attention, emotional clarity, and emotional repair) and SWL can be attested in Figure 2 and Table 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Predictive relationships of the emotional intelligence on satisfaction with life through the mediating role of the social interaction anxiety. **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05. R2 = Explained variance; CI = Confidence interval. The dashed lines represent non-significant relationships. The model was controlled by the sex and the campus of the students.

Figure 2 outlines the SEM and shows that SIA has a direct, negative and significant relationship with SWL (p < 0.001), as well as emotional clarity (p < 0.00) and emotional repair (p < 0.001). On the other hand, SIA has a direct, positive and significant relationship with emotional attention (p = 0.004). In terms of the mediating effect of the dimensions of emotional intelligence, it should be noted that emotional clarity and emotional repair play a remarkable role between SIA and SWL, as they soften the negative direct effect of SIA on SWL, given that these two dimensions (i.e., emotional clarity and emotional repair) have a positive and significant direct effect on SWL (Table 2). In addition, Figure 2 demonstrates the CI (95%) of R2, thereby confirming that these values can be considered ES measurements (Dominguez-Lara, 2017).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Estimation of significant standardized parameters and statistics of the mediation model.

4. Discussion

The purpose of this research was to analyze emotional intelligence as a mediator between SIA and SWL. The main results illustrate the important role of emotional clarity and repair as mediating variables between SIA and SWL, given that they decrease the negative effect of SIA on SWL.

A possible explanation for this is that emotional clarity is the key to regulating emotions, as the ability to clearly identify one’s emotions is the first step to successful emotional regulation and coping (Butler et al., 2006). Thus, young people who understand their emotions and recognize their own abilities to solve problems and overcome difficult situations through their own efforts will adopt a positive emotional coping style, mitigating the distress caused by SIA and improving their social performance (Li, 2020), and thus their SWL (Hodzic et al., 2016). In this vein, Guil et al. (2019) highlight the importance of the interaction between the three dimensions of emotional intelligence concerning SIA. These authors state that students who are confident in their own abilities to cope with challenging situations, do not pay much attention to their emotions, and do trust their competencies to clearly perceive and repair their emotional states will cope more efficiently with SIA (Guil et al., 2019). In terms of the relationship of SIA with the dimensions of emotional intelligence, we consider that, because of its timely execution, this study provides the global university context with an important scientific contribution, understanding that the data for the present study were collected just 2 weeks after the return to in-person classes following the COVID-19 confinement, as the fear of contagion and stress in general due to the pandemic were still visible in society (Di Crosta et al., 2020; Rodríguez-Hidalgo et al., 2020; Torales et al., 2020).

However, according to our research, emotional attention did not have a significant direct effect on SWL, nor were there significant indirect effects between SIA and SWL. In this line, Hodzic et al. (2016) also did not obtain a significant relationship between emotional attention and SWL. Although the role of emotional attention compared to emotional clarity and repair is confusing according to García-Fernández et al. (2015), on the one hand, Blasco-Belled et al. (2020) have found that emotional attention negatively and significantly predicts SWL (Blasco-Belled et al., 2020), while others found a negative but not significant prediction (Hodzic et al., 2016; Ramos-Díaz et al., 2019). Emotional attention not predicting SWL could be due to the fact that this dimension does not have as much of a potential inference in people’s behavior as emotional clarity and emotional repair do (Salovey et al., 1995). Thus, when students pay too much attention to their emotions without understanding them or having repair skills, they negatively affect their mood and psychological functioning (García-Fernández et al., 2015; Butler et al., 2018; Petrides et al., 2018; Masters et al., 2019), while also decreasing their SWL (Guerra-Bustamante et al., 2019; Martínez-Marín and Martínez, 2019; Azpiazu et al., 2022; De la Barrera et al., 2023). On the contrary, the intelligent management of emotions helps students to prevent negative feelings and increases positive ones, contributing to the increase of SWL (Sánchez-Álvarez et al., 2016) while improving academic performance with appropriate learning strategies (García-Fernández et al., 2015). Because scientific literature in this context is scarce, and given that the results obtained are in line with the international literature, we consider this study to be a scientific contribution to the Mexican university context.

It is also worth mentioning that SIA significantly, positively and directly predicted emotional attention in this research. Since past studies have negatively related SIA to emotional attention (Turk et al., 2005; Guil et al., 2019), contrary to the results obtained in this research, the relationship between SIA and emotional attention remains controversial (García-Fernández et al., 2015). However, the results obtained in the present study are in line with the findings of Boden and Berenbaum (2012), who also described a positive relationship between SIA and emotional attention. These authors claim that when one does not pay attention to the information provided by emotions, or one has a high level of emotional attention, but with a deficient emotional clarity, the predisposition toward SIA increases. A potential explanation is that people with a high level of emotional attention tend to be hypervigilant about their own emotions and signs of anxiety, becoming less flexible to explain their states of anxiety and misjudging both their severity and visibility to others, and feeling more threatened by how others understand them (Roth et al., 2001; Wells and Papageorgiou, 2001; Edelmann and Baker, 2002).

Finally, we will describe a series of limitations and strengths of the present study, as well as future research perspectives. Limitations include: (i) the timing of data collection, 2 weeks after the confinement and mobility measures due to the COVID-19 pandemic had been lifted, as this might have caused widespread emotional and psychological instability in participants, even when filling the questionnaires; (ii) the variables were evaluated neither before nor during the pandemic, so we are not able to observe the evolution of these variables after such an upsetting experience; (iii) the cross-sectional design of the study does not allow for establishing causal inferences; (iv) there was no sample randomization, so the results cannot be generalized; (v) a possible social desirability bias due to the use of self-reporting, since participants may have exaggerated their responses. On the other hand, noteworthy strengths of this research include: (i) the timing of data collection may be a strength in itself since scientific literature on this topic set just after the end of mobility restrictions is scarce; (ii) the sample size of Mexican undergraduate Physical Education students from the three campuses of the Autonomous University of Baja California (Ensenada, Tijuana and Mexicali), as well as the statistical power of the study. We consider it necessary for future studies to analyze the dimension of emotional attention by conducting a quadratic regression analysis since both excessive emotional attention and low levels of this dimension are related to SIA. We also suggest longitudinal studies a few years after the pandemic to analyze how the post-pandemic dimensions of emotional intelligence relate to occupational success.

5. Conclusion

In conclusion, it can be asserted that emotional clarity and repair had a mediating effect between SIA and SWL, as they did decrease the negative effects of SIA on SWL. In addition, SIA had a direct and positive effect on emotional attention and a negative effect on emotional clarity and repair. Furthermore, emotional clarity and repair had a direct and positive effect on SWL, although emotional attention did not predict SWL. Therefore, we believe that university institutions should train and educate students in managing SIA by further developing their emotional intelligence. It would be interesting to provide university students with strategies to control and manage the understanding of their own emotions, and to manage negative emotions resulting from SIA. Finally, it is important to highlight the importance of young people not paying too much emotional attention, as an excess of it can increase SIA levels, undermining SWL at the same time.

6. Practical implications

The results of this research underline the importance of emotional clarity and repair in decreasing SIA and increasing SWL in Mexican university students after the pandemic and are in line with other studies conducted before the pandemic. Therefore, educational institutions should organize workshops related to the development of emotional intelligence to help young people to understand the feelings and emotions that they experience, and to remediate negative emotions that might be stressful for them (Correa-Barwick et al., 2022; Cabello-Sanz and Muñoz-Parreño, 2023; Torres-Gázquez et al., 2023). In this line, Valenti et al. (2022) consider that appropriate programs should be designed to help people to see the bright side of negative experiences, which permits a reshaping of harmful emotional outcomes by focusing on some positive aspects. In addition, it is recommended that people suffering from SIA practice activities such as Mindfulness (Butler et al., 2018), aerobic exercise (Jazaieri et al., 2012), or activities in natural environments (Chen and Huo, 2022), as they help to decrease SIA and to increase the levels of SWL.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the University of Almería (Ref: UALBIO2023/001). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

JC-N: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing. AG-G: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. RE-G: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing. RB: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This article was carried out related to a research stay of Dr. Raúl Baños at the Autonomous University Baja California (from March 21, 2021, to April 24, 2021) with Prof. Juan José Calleja-Núñez. In addition, this research stay was financed through the Rectoral Resolution of February 23, 2021 of the Polytechnic University of Madrid, the award of aid from the Development Cooperation Travel Aid Program and for the Implementation of Official Teaching Programs with Latin America of short duration for professors, research and administrative staff of the UPM, during 2021.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Amerio, A., Lugo, A., Stival, C., Fanucchi, T., Gorini, G., Pacifici, R., et al. (2021). COVID-19 lockdown impact on mental health in a large representative sample of Italian adults. J. Affect. Disord. 292, 398–404. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2021.05.117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atienza, F. L., Pons, D., Balaguer, I., and García-Merita, M. (2000). Propiedades psicométricas de la Escala de Satisfacción con la Vida en adolescentes. Psicothema 12, 314–319.

Google Scholar

Azpiazu, L., Fernández-Zabala, A., Rodríguez-Fernández, A., and Ramos-Díaz, E. (2022). Perceived emotional intelligence and subjective well-being during adolescence: the moderating effect of age and sex. Curr. Psychol. doi: 10.1007/s12144-022-04128-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baños, R., Barretos-Ruvalcaba, M., and Baena-Extremera, A. (2019). Protocol for the study of the academic, psychological and physical activity variables that influence the academic performance of Mexican and Spanish adolescents. Espiral 12, 89–99. doi: 10.25115/ecp.v12i25.2480

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baños, R., Morán-Navarro, R., Toval, Á., Del Lidón López-Iborra, M., Morales-Delgado, N., and Ferrán, J. L. (2022). Learning and evaluation of human anatomy content in sports sciences through surf videos. Espiral 15, 1–10. doi: 10.25115/ecp.v15i30.5767

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baños, R., Toval, Á., Morales-Delgado, N., and Luis Ferrán, J. (2021). Analysis of movement during climbing as a strategy for learning the anatomy of the locomotor system in sport sciences. Espiral 14, 102–112. doi: 10.25115/ecp.v14i29.4540

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berenbaum, H., Raghavan, C., Le, H. N., Vernon, L. L., and Gomez, J. J. (2003). A taxonomy of emotional disturbances. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 10, 206–226. doi: 10.1093/clipsy/bpg011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhatnagar, A., Khanna, U., and Rana, A. (2021). Digital learning ecosystem at Indian higher education system. J. Contemp. Iss. Bus. Gov. 27, 4360–4375. Available at: https://www.cibgp.com/issue-content/digital-learning-ecosystem-at-indian-higher-education-system-2399

Google Scholar

Bigman, Y. E., Mauss, I. B., Gross, J. J., and Tamir, M. (2015). Yes I can: expected success promotes actual success in emotion regulation. Cognit. Emot. 30, 1380–1387. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2015.1067188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blasco-Belled, A., Rogoza, R., Torrelles-Nadal, C., and Alsinet, C. (2020). Emotional intelligence structure and its relationship with life satisfaction and happiness: new findings from the Bifactor model. J. Happiness Stud. 21, 2031–2049. doi: 10.1007/s10902-019-00167-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boden, M. T., and Berenbaum, H. (2012). Facets of emotional clarity and suspiciousness. Pers. Individ. Differ. 53, 426–430. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.04.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, G., and Medcalf-Bell, R. (2022). Phoning it in: social anxiety, intolerance of uncertainty, and anxiety reduction motivations predict phone use in social situations. Hum. Behav. Emerg. Tech. 2022, 1–8. doi: 10.1155/2022/6153053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butler, R. M., Boden, M. T., Olino, T. M., Morrison, A. S., Goldin, P. R., Gross, J. J., et al. (2018). Emotional clarity and attention to emotions in cognitive behavioral group therapy and mindfulness-based stress reduction for social anxiety disorder. J. Anxiety Disord. 55, 31–38. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2018.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butler, E. A., Wilhelm, F. H., and Gross, J. J. (2006). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia, emotion, and emotion regulation during social interaction. Psychophysiology 43, 612–622. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00467.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabello-Sanz, S., and Muñoz-Parreño, J. A. (2023). Design, implementation, and evaluation of the “Universo Emocionante” emotional educational program through the service-learning methodology. Espiral 16, 95–105. doi: 10.25115/ecp.v16i32.8754

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Catanzaro, S. J., and Mearns, J. (1999). “Mood-related expectancy, emotional experience, and coping behavior” in How expectancies shape experience. ed. I. Kirsch (Washington DC, United States: American Psychological Association), 67–91.

Google Scholar

Chartrand, H., Cox, B., El-Gabalawy, R., and Clara, I. (2011). Social anxiety disorder subtypes and their mental health correlates in a nationally representative Canadian sample. Can. J. Behav. Sci.-Rev. Can. Sci. Comport. 43, 89–98. doi: 10.1037/a0022435

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Y., and Huo, Y. (2022). Social interaction anxiety and problematic smartphone use among rural-urban adolescents in China: a moderated moderated-mediation model. Youth Soc. 55, 686–707. doi: 10.1177/0044118X221126548

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Correa-Barwick, G. S., Carrasco-Poyatos, M., and Molina-Fernández, J. D. (2022). Emotional intelligence and satisfaction with life in schoolchildren during times of pandemic. Espiral 15, 57–70.

Google Scholar

De la Barrera, U., Villanueva, L., Montoya-Castilla, I., and Prado-Gascó, V. (2023). How much emotional attention is appropriate? The influence of emotional intelligence and subjective well-being on adolescents’ stress. Curr. Psychol. 42, 5131–5143. doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-01763-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Rubia, J. M., Cadena, C. H. G., and Casas, C. J. A. (2013). Validación de la escala de ansiedad en la interacción social en estudiantes universitarios mexicanos. Pensamiento Psicol. 11, 27–42. Available at: https://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=80127000001

Google Scholar

Di Crosta, A., Palumbo, R., Marchetti, D., Ceccato, I., La Malva, P., Maiella, R., et al. (2020). Individual differences, economic stability, and fear of contagion as risk factors for PTSD symptoms in the COVID-19 emergency. Front. Psychol. 11:567367. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.567367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E., and Emmons, R. A. (1985). The independence of positive and negative affect. J. Pers. Assess. 99, 91–95.

Google Scholar

Dixon-Gordon, K., Dovidio, J., Gilbert, K., Hilt, L., Marroquin, B., Mclaughlin, K., et al. (2014). Impaired emotional clarity and psychopathology: a transdiagnostic deficit with symptom-specific pathways through emotion regulation. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 33, 319–342. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2014.33.4.319

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dominguez-Lara, S. (2017). Magnitud del efecto en análisis de regresión. Interacciones 3, 3–5. doi: 10.24016/2017.v3n1.46

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edelmann, R. J., and Baker, S. R. (2002). Self-reported and actual physiological responses in social phobia. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 41, 1–14. doi: 10.1348/014466502163732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ercengiz, M., Yildiz, B., Savci, M., and Griffiths, M. D. (2020). Differentiation of self, emotion management skills, and nomophobia among smartphone users: the mediating and moderating roles of intolerance of uncertainty. Soc. Sci. J., 1–15. doi: 10.1080/03623319.2020.1833148

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erliksson, O. J., Lindner, P., and Mörtberg, E. (2020). Measuring associations between social anxiety and use of different types of social media using the Swedish social anxiety scale for social media users: a psychometric evaluation and cross-sectional study. Scand. J. Psychol. 61, 819–826. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12673

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Extremera, N., and Fernández, P. (2005). Inteligencia emocional percibida y diferencias individuales en el meta-conocimiento de los estados emocionales: Una revisión de los estudios con el TMMS. Ansiedad Estres 11, 101–122. Available at: https://www.ansiedadyestres.es/ref/2005/11-101-122

Google Scholar

Faraci, P., Bottaro, R., Valenti, G. D., and Craparo, G. (2022). Psychological well-being during the second wave of COVID-19 pandemic: the mediation role of generalized anxiety. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. 15, 695–709. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S354083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García-Fernández, J. M., Inglés, C. J., Suriá, R., Martín, N. L. S., Gonzálvez-Maciá, C., Aparisi, D., et al. (2015). Profiles of emotional intelligence and learning strategies in a sample of Chilean students. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 30, 437–455. doi: 10.1007/s10212-015-0254-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gohm, C. L., and Clore, G. L. (2002). Four latent traits of emotional experience and their involvement in well-being, coping, and attributional style. Cognit. Emot. 16, 495–518. doi: 10.1080/02699930143000374

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gollwitzer, M., Platzer, C., Zwarg, C., and Göritz, A. S. (2021). Public acceptance of Covid-19 lockdown scenarios. Int. J. Psychol. 56, 551–565. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12721

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guerra-Bustamante, J., León-Del-Barco, B., Yuste-Tosina, R., López-Ramos, V. M., and Mendo-Lázaro, S. (2019). Emotional intelligence and psychological well-being in adolescents. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 16:1720. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16101720

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guil, R., Gómez-Molinero, R., Merchán-Clavellino, A., and Gil-Olarte, P. (2021). Lights and shadows of trait emotional intelligence: its mediating role in the relationship between negative affect and state anxiety in university students. Front. Psychol. 11:615010. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.615010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guil, R., Gómez-Molinero, R., Merchan-Clavellino, A., Gil-Olarte, P., and Zayas, A. (2019). Facing anxiety, growing up. Trait emotional intelligence as a mediator of the relationship between self-esteem and university anxiety. Front. Psychol. 10:567. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00567

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hahn, D. (2020). The psychological well-being of student entrepreneurs: a social identity perspective. Int. Entrep. Manag. J. 16, 467–499. doi: 10.1007/s11365-019-00607-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hair, J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., and Anderson, R. E. (2018). Multivariate data analysis. 8th Edn. London, United Kingdom: Pearson.

Google Scholar

Hodzic, S., Botella, P. R., and Costa, H. (2016). Are emotionally intelligent students more resilient to stress? Behav. Psychol. 24, 253–272. Available at: https://www.behavioralpsycho.com/wp-content/uploads/2018/10/04.Hodzic_24-2oa-1.pdf

Google Scholar

Hu, L. T., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jazaieri, H., Goldin, P. R., Werner, K., Ziv, M., and Gross, J. J. (2012). A randomized trial of MBSR versus aerobic exercise for social anxiety disorder. J. Clin. Psychol. 68, 715–731. doi: 10.1002/jclp.21863

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kashdan, T. B., and Breen, W. E. (2008). Social anxiety and positive emotions: a prospective examination of a self-regulatory model with tendencies to suppress or express emotions as a moderating variable. Behav. Therapy 39, 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2007.02.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kashdan, T. B., and Steger, M. F. (2006). Expanding the topography of social anxiety an experience-sampling assessment of positive emotions, positive events, and emotion suppression. Psychol. Sci. 17, 120–128. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01674.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, R. C. (2003). The impairments caused by social phobia in the general population: implications for intervention. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. Supplement, 108(417), 19–27. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.108.s417.2.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klemanski, D. H., Curtiss, J., McLaughlin, K. A., and Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2017). Emotion regulation and the transdiagnostic role of repetitive negative thinking in adolescents with social anxiety and depression. Cogn. Ther. Res. 41, 206–219. doi: 10.1007/s10608-016-9817-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kline, R. B. (2016). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York City, United States: The Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Kuo, T. M., Tsai, C. C., and Wang, J. C. (2021). Linking web-based learning self-efficacy and learning engagement in MOOCs: the role of online academic hardiness. Internet High. Educ. 51:100819. doi: 10.1016/j.iheduc.2021.100819

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kwon, S. J., Kim, Y., and Kwak, Y. (2018). Difficulties faced by university students with self-reported symptoms of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a qualitative study. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry Ment. Health 12:12. doi: 10.1186/s13034-018-0218-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, D. (2020). Influence of the youth’s psychological capital on social anxiety during the COVID-19 pandemic outbreak: the mediating role of coping style. Iran. J. Public Health 49, 2060–2068. doi: 10.18502/ijph.v49i11.4721

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Makadi, E., and Koszycki, D. (2020). Exploring connections between self-compassion, mindfulness, and social anxiety. Mindfulness 11, 480–492. doi: 10.1007/s12671-019-01270-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., Hau, K. T., and Wen, Z. (2004). In search of golden rules: comment on hypothesis-testing approaches to setting cutoff values for fit indexes and dangers in overgeneralizing Hu and Bentler’s (1999) findings. Struct. Equ. Model. 11, 320–341. doi: 10.1207/s15328007sem1103_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martínez-Marín, M. D., and Martínez, C. (2019). Subjective well-being and gender-typed attributes in adolescents: the relevance of emotional intelligence. Aust. J. Psychol. 71, 296–304. doi: 10.1111/ajpy.12247

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masrek, M. N., Ahmed, W., Jalil, A., and Baharuddin, M. F. (2022). Mobile game addiction and social interaction anxiety of malaysian youth. Environ. Behav. Proc. J. 7, 3–8. doi: 10.21834/ebpj.v7isi10.4094

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masters, M. R., Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., and Farrell, L. J. (2019). Transactional associations between adolescents’ emotion dysregulation and symptoms of social anxiety and depression: a longitudinal study. J. Early Adolesc. 39, 1085–1109. doi: 10.1177/0272431618806053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattick, R. P., and Clarke, J. C. (1998). Development and validation of measures of social phobia scrutiny fear and social interaction anxiety. Behav. Res. Ther. 36, 455–470. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(97)10031-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Medda, E., Toccaceli, V., Gigantesco, A., Picardi, A., Fagnani, C., and Stazi, M. A. (2022). The COVID-19 pandemic in Italy: depressive symptoms immediately before and after the first lockdown. J. Affect. Disord. 298, 202–208. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2021.10.129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nguyen, B. T. N., Nguyen, T. T., and Le, U. T. T. (2022). Nomophobia and stress among Vietnamese high school students in COVID-19 pandemic: a mediation model of loneliness. J. Biochem. Technol. 13, 34–40. doi: 10.51847/zn5yxlp0nk

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Toole, M. S., Hougaard, E., and Mennin, D. S. (2013). Social anxiety and emotion knowledge: a meta-analysis. J. Anxiety Disord. 27, 98–108. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.09.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Obschonka, M., Gewirtz, A. H., and Zhu, L. (2021). Psychological implications of the COVID-19 pandemic around the world: introduction to the special issue. Int. J. Psychol. 56, 493–497. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12793

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Özdin, S., and Bayrak Özdin, Ş. (2020). Levels and predictors of anxiety, depression and health anxiety during COVID-19 pandemic in Turkish society: the importance of gender. Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry 66, 504–511. doi: 10.1177/0020764020927051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parhiala, P., Torppa, M., Vasalampi, K., Eklund, K., Poikkeus, A. M., and Aro, T. (2018). Profiles of school motivation and emotional well-being among adolescents: associations with math and reading performance. Learn. Individ. Differ. 61, 196–204. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2017.12.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrides, K. V., Sanchez-Ruiz, M. J., Siegling, A. B., Saklofske, D. H., and Mavroveli, S. (2018). “Emotional intelligence as personality: measurement and role of trait emotional intelligence in educational contexts” in Emotional intelligence in education. eds. K. V. Keefer, J. D. A. Parker, and D. H. Saklofske (New York City, United States: Springer), 49–81, New York City, United States.

Google Scholar

Pietrabissa, G., and Simpson, S. G. (2020). Psychological consequences of social isolation during COVID-19 outbreak. Front. Psychol. 11:2201. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Racine, S., Miller, A., Mehak, A., and Trolio, V. (2022). Examining risk and protective factors for psychological health during the COVID-19 pandemic. Anxiety Stress Coping 35, 124–140. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2021.1958789

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramos-Díaz, E., Rodríguez-Fernández, A., Axpe, I., and Ferrara, M. (2019). Perceived emotional intelligence and life satisfaction among adolescent students: the mediating role of resilience. J. Happiness Stud. 20, 2489–2506. doi: 10.1007/s10902-018-0058-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez-Hidalgo, A. J., Pantaleón, Y., Dios, I., and Falla, D. (2020). Fear of COVID-19, stress, and anxiety in university undergraduate students: a predictive model for depression. Front. Psychol. 11:591797. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.591797

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roth, D., Antony, M. M., and Swinson, R. P. (2001). Interpretations for anxiety symptoms in social phobia. Behav. Res. Therapy 39, 129–138. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00159-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rozen, N., and Aderka, I. M. (2023). Emotions in social anxiety disorder: a review. J. Anxiety Disord. 95:102696. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2023.102696

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salguero, J. M., Palomera, R., and Fernández-Berrocal, P. (2012). Perceived emotional intelligence as predictor of psychological adjustment in adolescents: a 1-year prospective study. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 27, 21–34. doi: 10.1007/s10212-011-0063-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salovey, P., Mayer, J. D., Goldman, S. L., Turvey, C., and Palfai, T. P. (1995). “Emotional attention, clarity, and repair: exploring emotional intelligence using the trait meta-modo scale” in Emotion, disclosure, and health. ed. J. W. Pennebaker (Washington DC, United States: American Psychological Association), 125–154.

Google Scholar

Sánchez-Álvarez, N., Extremera, N., and Fernández-Berrocal, P. (2016). The relation between emotional intelligence and subjective well-being: a meta-analytic investigation. J. Posit. Psychol. 11, 276–285. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2015.1058968

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soper, D. S. (2023). A-priori Sample Size Calculator for Structural Equation Models [Software]. Available at: https://www.danielsoper.com/statcalc/ (Accessed June 15, 2023).

Google Scholar

Sturgill, R., Martinasek, M., Schmidt, T., and Goyal, R. (2021). A novel artificial intelligence-powered emotional intelligence and mindfulness app (Ajivar) for the college student population during the COVID-19 pandemic: quantitative questionnaire study. JMIR Form. Res. 5:e25372. doi: 10.2196/25372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tabachnick, B. G., and Fidell, L. S. (2019). Using multivariate statistics. 7th Edn. London, United Kingdom: Pearson.

Google Scholar

Thompson, R. J., Boden, M. T., and Gotlib, I. H. (2017). Emotional variability and clarity in depression and social anxiety. Cognit. Emot. 31, 98–108. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2015.1084908

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torales, J., O’Higgins, M., Castaldelli-Maia, J. M., and Ventriglio, A. (2020). The outbreak of COVID-19 coronavirus and its impact on global mental health. Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry 66, 317–320. doi: 10.1177/0020764020915212

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torres-Gázquez, S. D. C., López-García, G. D., and Granero-Gallegos, A. (2023). Emotional intelligence and resilience in secondary school physical education students during the COVID-19 pandemic. Espiral 16, 51–63. doi: 10.25115/ecp.v16i32.9098

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turk, C. L., Heimberg, R. G., Luterek, J. A., Mennin, D. S., and Fresco, D. M. (2005). Emotion dysregulation in generalized anxiety disorder: a comparison with social anxiety disorder. Cogn. Ther. Res. 29, 89–106. doi: 10.1007/s10608-005-1651-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valdivia, J. A., Rubio, J. C. A., and French, B. F. (2015). Examination of the Spanish trait meta-mood scale–24 factor structure in a Mexican setting. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 33, 473–482. doi: 10.1177/0734282914552052

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valenti, G. D., Bottaro, R., and Faraci, P. (2022). Effects of difficulty in handling emotions and social interactions on nomophobia: Examining the mediating role of feelings of loneliness. Int. J. Ment. Heal. Addict., 1–15. doi: 10.1007/s11469-022-00888-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Von Elm, E., Altman, D. G., Egger, M., Pocock, S. J., Gøtzsche, P. C., and Vandenbroucke, J. P. (2008). The strengthening the reporting of observational studies in epidemiology (STROBE) statement: guidelines for reporting observational studies. Bull. World Health Organ. 85, 867–872. doi: 10.2471/BLT.07.045120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weeden, K. A., and Cornwell, B. (2020). The small-world network of college classes: implications for epidemic spread on a university campus. Sociol. Sci. 7, 222–241. doi: 10.15195/V7.A9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wells, A., and Papageorgiou, C. (2001). Social phobic interoception: effects of bodily information on anxiety, beliefs and self-processing. Behav. Res. Ther. 39, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(99)00146-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Werner, K. H., Goldin, P. R., Ball, T. M., Heimberg, R. G., and Gross, J. J. (2011). Assessing emotion regulation in social anxiety disorder: the emotion regulation interview. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 33, 346–354. doi: 10.1007/s10862-011-9225-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wittchen, H. U., Fuetsch, M., Sonntag, H., Müller, N., and Liebowitz, M. (2000). Disability and quality of life in pure and comorbid social phobia. Findings from a controlled study. Eur. Psychiatry 15, 46–58. doi: 10.1016/s0924-9338(00)00211-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiong, J., Lipsitz, O., Nasri, F., Lui, L. M. W., Gill, H., Phan, L., et al. (2020). Impact of COVID-19 pandemic on mental health in the general population: a systematic review. J. Affect. Disord. 277, 55–64. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zwilling, M. (2022). The impact of nomophobia, stress, and loneliness on smartphone addiction among young adults during and after the COVID-19 pandemic: an Israeli case analysis. Sustainability 14:3229. doi: 10.3390/su14063229

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: social anxiety, emotional repair, emotional clarity, emotional attention, university

Citation: Calleja-Núñez JJ, Granero-Gallegos A, Espinoza-Gutiérrez R and Baños R (2023) Mediating effect of social interaction anxiety between emotional intelligence and life satisfaction in physical education students: post-COVID-19 study. Front. Psychol. 14:1284664. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1284664

Received: 28 August 2023; Accepted: 15 September 2023;
Published: 02 October 2023.

Edited by:

Manuel Gómez-López, University of Murcia, Spain

Reviewed by:

David Manzano Sánchez, University of Extremadura, Spain
Jesús Romero, Santo Tomás University, Colombia

Copyright © 2023 Calleja-Núñez, Granero-Gallegos, Espinoza-Gutiérrez and Baños. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Antonio Granero-Gallegos, YWdyYW5lcm9AdWFsLmVz

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.