- 1School of Early-Childhood Education, Nanjing Xiaozhuang University, Nanjing, Jiangsu, China
- 2Business School, Sun Yat-sen University, Shenzhen, Guangdong, China
- 3Research Institute of Social Development, Southwestern University of Finance and Economics, Chengdu, Sichuan, China
A growing body of research contributes to our knowledge about unethical behavior. However, very little is known about how group-based competition shape members’ unethical behavior. Building on social learning theory, we conducted three studies to reveal how group-based competition may affect individual’s unethical behavior for their team. Study 1 and 2 are laboratory experiments in which participants were randomly assigned into groups of three members and engaged in group-based competition (or engaged in individual-based competition in an individual context) with monetary incentives. Different from individual-based competition where mean number of unethical behaviors for the self in the losing condition was larger than that in the winning condition, in group-based competition mean number of unethical behaviors in favor of group between the winning and the losing condition was not significantly different. Both studies also showed that there are less unethical behaviors in the group-based competition than in the individual-based competition. Study 2 further revealed that collective efficacy negatively associated with mean number of unethical behaviors in group-based competition. Study 3 was a field study with employees from bank subsidiaries working as teams, and results from their self-reported data confirm the relationship between collective efficacy and unethical behaviors observed in Study 2. Together, these results suggest that collective efficacy has the effect of curbing unethical behavior in group-based competition, thus contributing to the understanding of group-based experience on unethical behaviors.
Introduction
Groups are widely utilized to organize people toward collective goals (Kozlowski and Ilgen, 2006). In the course of goal pursuit, there are usually multiple groups striving for incompatible goals (e.g., companies compete for market share, football teams compete for championship). One’s progress in attaining desired outcomes decreases others’ probability of goal attainment, so group members in this context will be motivated to gain or maintain advantages (Deutsch, 1949; Pruitt, 1998; De Dreu, 2010; Garcia et al., 2013).
Competition not only can promote competitiveness, leading to improved group performance (Pike et al., 2018), but also can provoke unethical behavior (Goette et al., 2012; Bennett et al., 2013; Kilduff et al., 2016). Although group-based competition are pervasive in human society, empirical research examining unethical behavior in this context is still limited. The present research hereby explores whether unethical behavior in group-based competition differs from that in individual-based competition, and tries to reveal how social bonding in groups affects individuals’ unethical behavior.
Competition
Competition is interaction processes in which individuals vie for goals or resources, with one party’s goal pursuit impeding another party’s goal striving (Deutsch, 1949).
Engaging in competition may entail increased physiological arousal (Neave and Wolfson, 2003; To et al., 2018), result in risk-taking behaviors (To et al., 2018) and heightened motivation to outperform their opponents (Yip et al., 2018).
When individuals are outperformed by opponents, such experience may hurt their identity (Aquino and Douglas, 2003; Leavitt and Sluss, 2015) and give rise to discouragement and lowered self-satisfaction (Bandura, 1991). According to social learning perspective, individuals in this situation would apply their moral standards more leniently (Bandura, 1999; Sharma et al., 2014) and tend to morally disengage from self-regulation (Bandura, 1991, 1999). Thus the situation of being disadvantageous would inflame unethical impulse, triggering unethical behaviors aiming at gaining advantage (Pierce et al., 2013). Based on the above reasoning, we could expect an enhanced value competition losers would place on retaining advantages, leading to an increase in unethical behavior, as compared to winners.
Group-based competition and unethical behavior
Individuals have the innate need to affiliate with social groups (Baumeister and Leary, 1995), and to identify with their groups (Tajfel, 1982; Ashforth and Mael, 1989). In group-based competition contexts, because individuals are concerned about their relative group outcomes, they are apt to participate in unethical behaviors toward competitors to maintain group advantage (Halevy et al., 2010; Goette et al., 2012). Unethical behaviors targeting at improving group interests rather than the self are coined pro-group unethical behaviors (Thau et al., 2015) and unethical pro-organizational behavior (Umphress and Bingham, 2011). Such unethical behaviors have been consistently observed in recent studies (Molinsky and Margolis, 2005; Gino and Pierce, 2009; Umphress et al., 2010; Umphress and Bingham, 2011; Shalvi and De Dreu, 2014; Thau et al., 2015; Chen et al., 2016; Fehr et al., 2019; Jiang et al., 2022). It is suggested that individuals conduct pro-group unethical behaviors with the purpose of being included by their groups (Thau et al., 2015) or maintaining exchange relationship with their groups (Umphress et al., 2010; Umphress and Bingham, 2011).
In many work groups, task jointness and coordination produce a sense of shared responsibility which fosters affective ties to groups (Thye et al., 2019), promote ingroup cohesiveness (Deutsch, 1949; Stein, 1976; Tajfel and Turner, 1979) and cooperation (Erev et al., 1993; Bornstein et al., 2002). Individual members embedded in such cohesive relational network act as valuable resources to each other, as their interactions consist of such features as mutual aid, reciprocal norms, and interpersonal trust that enable individuals to act together effectively to pursue collective goals (Putnam, 1993). According to Bandura’s social cognitive theory (Bandura, 2000), the interdependence of individual member functioning and members’ shared beliefs in their collective power to produce effects provide a basis for the development of collective efficacy belief—belief in the collective power to produce desired outcomes (Gibson, 1999; Bandura, 2000; Kozlowski and Ilgen, 2006). This belief has positive impact on psychological wellbeing. For instance, it associates with resilience to impediments and setbacks (Bandura, 2000), serves as buffer of stressor-strain relations (Jex and Bliese, 1999; Tucker et al., 2013; Esnard and Roques, 2014), and reduces anxiety (Salanova et al., 2003). Thus collective efficacy belief developed in group-based competition is helpful in buffering stress produced in individuals’ interaction with rivals, leading to a more benign appraisal of stressful events in competitive interaction (e.g., group-based disadvantage), as suggested by previous literature (Kawachi and Berkman, 2001).
On the other hand, individuals like to think of themselves as moral, so conducts that are in line with their moral standards build their sense of self-worth, whereas behaving in ways that violate their moral standards are detrimental to their self-worth and may cause self-condemnation (Bandura, 1999). Since individuals who engage in joint tasks can count on other group members’ expertise and efforts, collective efficacy belief would have the effect of mitigating individuals’ impulse to behave in ways that will violate their moral standards in group-based competitions. Indeed, recent work suggests that individuals sometimes are aversive to unethical behavior when it advances their own interests because they want to maintain their positive view of self-concept (Mazar et al., 2008; Mead et al., 2009; Shalvi et al., 2011a,b).
Therefore, group-based competitions would have differential impact on behaviors, as compared to individual-based competitions where outcomes are solely determined by personal performance. We could expect a mitigated motivation to behave unethically in group-based competitions, compared with that in individual-based competitions, and a negative association between collective efficacy belief and unethical behavior in group-based competitions.
Hypothesis 1: There are more unethical behaviors in the losing condition than in the winning condition in individual-based competitions.
Hypothesis 2: There are less unethical behaviors in the group-based competitions than in the individual-based competition.
Hypothesis 3: There is a negative association between collective efficacy belief and unethical behavior in group-based competitions.
Overview of the present research
To test the hypotheses, the present study focused on unethical behavior after winning or losing a competition. Study 1 and 2 are laboratory experiments in which participants were assigned into groups of three members and engaged in group-based competitions (or individual-based competitions in an individual context) with monetary incentives. Participants were instructed to engage in two consecutive competitions with the same competitor, namely repeated team-based competition (RTB) or repeated individual-based competition (RIB). In Study 1, winning or losing outcome was determined by participants’ true performances. The authors compared unethical behaviors between the winning and the losing conditions and between the RTB and the RIB. In Study 2, participants were randomly assigned to a winning or a losing condition. The purposes of Study 2 were to replicate findings in Study 1, and test the relationship between collective efficacy and pro-group unethical behavior. Study 3 was a survey study with employees from bank subsidiaries working as teams, to garner empirical evidence for ecological validity of our conclusions.
Study 1
Methods
Participants
A total of 132 native Chinese-speaking undergraduate students from a university in southwest China (44% male; Mage = 20.77, SDage = 1.58; NRTB = 72, NRIB = 60) participated in this experiment. The experiment employed a 2 (competition mode: individual-based competition vs. team-based competition) × 2 (competition outcome: win vs. loss) between-subjects design. Gender ratio was balanced in each cell. In the absence of a suitable estimate of expected effect size, the sample size was derived from a previous study using a similar protocol (Schurr and Ritov, 2016). Three participants in RIB were dropped out from data analysis because of equal performance in a dyad in the first competition (N = 2) or skepticism about the competition outcome (N = 1).
Experimental procedure
The experiment was conducted in a computer lab. Upon arrival, participants read and signed the informed consent, and were ushered to sit in front of their computer screens. Before the onset of the experiment, an experimenter introduced all the experimental tasks, and then participants proceeded to complete each task under the guidance of the experimenter.
Participants were instructed to engage in two consecutive competitions with the same competitor for bonus (see Figure 1). In RTB, participants were informed that they had been randomly assigned into teams of three and each team had been randomly paired with another team to engage in two consecutive competitions. In RIB, participants were informed that they had been randomly paired with another individual to engage in two consecutive competitions. They first engaged in a cognitive task (the 1st competition) in which they experienced winning or losing, and then competed with the same competitor in a problem-solving task (the 2nd competition which was also used to track their unethical behavior). They participated in exchange for 20 RMB (2.74 USD) plus 15 RMB (2.05 USD) bonus for winning each competition. To minimize social influences, participants were anonymous to one another. Finally, participants indicated whether they were skeptical about the outcomes they received in the two competitive tasks.
Cognitive task
This was the first competition task used to elicit winning or losing experience. There were 10 practice trials and 50 formal trials (see Figure 2). In each trial, participants identified the number of identical characters between two random character strings shown on their screen (e.g., “df%&*” and “d9ij#%”) and responded by pressing a number key within 10 s (press “2” for the above sample). The number of characters in each string ranged from 4 to 11, and the number of identical characters between each pair of strings ranged from 0 to 9. There was no feedback after each trial; participants were shown on their screen their total correct responses once completing all the trials.
Figure 2. The cognitive task. *Appears in the middle of the screen for 500 ms between the blank screen and the response screen to calibrate visual fixation on the screen.
The cognitive task was completed in private and after all participants completed this task, two experimenters kept record of their scores. Participants were informed that winning and losing outcomes were determined by their actual performance and those whose total number of correct responses exceeded their competitor were winners. Information regarding their own score, team total score (only in RTB), competitor’s total score, competition outcome (i.e., win/loss), and reward (i.e., 0/15RMB) was filled on the first page of questionnaires distributed to them according to their seat numbers.
Problem-solving task
To track unethical behavior, the authors created a Chinese version of anagram task. Inspired from standard anagram task in which participants create words from different series of seven letters under time pressure (Gino and Margolis, 2011; Thau et al., 2015; Welsh et al., 2015), participants in the present study were asked to find as many four-character idioms as possible based on 200 Chinese characters spread in a 10 × 20 table within 60 s. They then wrote on an answer sheet the number of idioms they found, giving them the opportunity to over-report their performance. Unbeknownst to them, the problem was unsolvable except for two obvious idioms intentionally aligned in the center of the table (i.e., “根深蒂固,” “坐井观天”). All participants should be familiar with the two idioms. Participants could win 15RMB bonus if the number of idioms they reported exceeded that of their rival, so they had the motivation to over-report. Following Thau et al. (2015), the number of idioms over-reported constituted our measure of unethical behavior.
Results
Figure 3 displays the mean number of idioms over-reported by condition. The analysis yielded a significant main effect of competition outcome [Mloss = 1.95, SDloss = 3.54 vs. Mwin = 0.97, SDwin = 1.61; F(1, 125) = 5.09, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.04]. Further analyses showed that the number of idioms over-reported in the losing group was significantly larger than that in the winning condition in RIB [Mloss = 3.14, SDloss = 4.69 vs. Mwin = 1.29, SDwin = 1.96; F(1, 55) = 3.74, p = 0.058, ηp2 = 0.06], thus supporting Hypothesis 1. This difference was not statistically significant in RTB [Mloss = 1.00, SDloss = 1.79 vs. Mwin = 0.72, SDwin = 1.26; F(1, 70) = 0.581, p = 0.448]. Results also provide support for Hypothesis 2 by showing a main effect of competition mode—the number of idioms over-reported in RIB was larger than that in RTB [MRIB = 2.23, SDRIB = 3.70 vs. MRTB = 0.86, SDRTB = 1.54; F(1, 125) = 8.18, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.06]. There was no interaction effect.
Figure 3. Study 1: mean number of idioms overstated by condition. Error bars represent standard errors.
Findings in Study 1 suggest that group or team has an effect of mitigating the demonstration of unethical behavior. We reasoned that unethical behavior in team-based competitions could be constrained by collective efficacy. However, Study 1 did not provide direct evidence for this point, thus Study 2 was designed to replicate findings in Study 1 and to shed light on the relationship between collective efficacy and unethical behavior in team-based competition.
Study 2
Methods
Participants
A total of 120 students (45% male; Mage = 21.23, SDage = 1.76; NRIB = 60, NRTB = 60) participated in this study. Experimental protocol was similar to the one used in Experiment 1. Nine participants in RIB were dropped out from data analysis because of equal performance in a pair of students in the cognitive task (N = 2), or skepticism about the competition results (N = 2), or computer glitch (N = 5).
Experimental procedure
Experimental procedure was identical to Study 1 with the following exceptions: (1) Winning and losing in the cognitive task were determined by chance, allowing to control individual difference variables (e.g., cognitive ability) that could affect their unethical behaviors. (2) After the practice of the cognitive task, participants in RTB completed items measuring their collective efficacy and group identification. According to the minimal group paradigm (Tajfel, 1970), merely being categorized into an experimental group is sufficient to induce favoritism to the ingroup and discrimination against an out-group. These measures can reveal how collective efficacy and group identification link to unethical behavior after winning or losing a competition. In addition, the authors also measured affectivity as a control variable.
Measure
In RTB, competition outcomes are determined by collective efforts, and each member has no direct control about team outcome they desire. Under such circumstance, individuals turn to collective agency—relying on peers’ expertise and efforts in group tasks (Bandura, 2000). Therefore, collective efficacy may play an important role in shaping their behaviors. To reveal the relationship between collective efficacy and unethical behavior, the authors included a four-item scale adopted from Jex and Bliese (1999), α = 0.85 (e.g., “I have real confidence in my team’s ability to perform its mission”). Responses were given on a 9-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 9 (strongly agree). Previous literature suggests that group identification associates with unethical behavior in favor of in-groups (Umphress et al., 2010; Umphress and Bingham, 2011), so the present study assessed participants’ team identification with a four-item scale modeled from Doosje et al. (1995), α = 0.86 (e.g., “I am glad to be a member of our team”). Responses were given on a nine-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 9 (very much). An affectivity scale (Watson et al., 1988) was used to measure participants’ feelings after the first competition (Cronbach’s α:0.84 for Positive Affect subscale, 0.88 for Negative Affect subscale). Participants indicated to what extent they had experienced the feelings depicted at the present moment (e.g., “hostile,” “excited”) on a five-point scale ranging from 1 (very slightly or not at all) to 5 (extremely).
Results
Figure 4 displays the mean number of four-character idioms over-reported by condition. Results showed a main effect of competition mode [MRIB = 3.35, SDRIB = 4.61 vs. MRTB = 1.75, SDRTB = 2.10; F(1, 107) = 6.28, p < 0.02, ηp2 = 0.06], and a main effect of competition outcome [Mloss = 3.11, SDloss = 3.93 vs. Mwin = 1.88, SDwin = 3.06; F(1, 107) = 4.26, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.04]. There was no interaction effect. Furthermore, the number of idioms over-reported in losing condition was larger than that in the winning condition, and the difference was significant in RIB [Mloss = 4.60, SDloss = 5.00 vs. Mwin = 2.15, SDwin = 3.93; F(1, 51) = 3.79, p = 0.057, ηp2 = 0.07], but not in RTB [Mloss = 1.87, SDloss = 2.13 vs. Mloss = 1.63, SDloss = 2.09; F(1, 60) = 0.183, p = 0.67]. These results were consistent with Study 1, suggesting that our findings were robust.
Figure 4. Study 2: mean number of idioms over-reported by condition. Error bars represent standard errors.
In RTB, the number of idioms over-stated between the winning and the losing condition was not significantly different, so the authors lump together the data to test the relationship between collective efficacy and unethical behavior. A regression analysis predicting the numbers of idioms overstated as a function of collective efficacy, controlling for competition outcome, positive affect, negative affect, and team identification, revealed a significantly negative effect of collective efficacy, b = −0.81, p < 0.001, supporting Hypothesis 3. This finding contributes to literature by emphasizing the role of collective efficacy in inhibiting unethical behavior in team-based competitions. Moreover, results also showed a significantly positive effect of team identification, b = 0.50, p < 0.05, suggesting that the more individuals are identified with their team the more likely they are willing to help their team through unethical means. Thus this finding seems to be in concordance with past research on unethical pro-organizational behaviors (Umphress et al., 2010).
Study 3
Methods
Participants
We selected work groups in bank subsidiaries because they fit the definition of teams as ongoing groups imbedded in an organizational system whose members exhibit interdependencies with respect to workflow, goals, and outcomes (Kozlowski and Ilgen, 2006). The relations between bank subsidiaries also fit the notion of competition or rivalry characterized as repeated competition for market share (Kilduff et al., 2010). The data for the study were collected by means of self-administered questionnaires delivered in person to employees who were assured that their responses would be anonymous. Of the 160 questionnaires delivered, 159 usable ones were returned (45.3% male, Mage = 34.68). All had a university degree.
Measures
Respondents’ unethical behaviors were assessed with six items drawn from the unethical pro-organizational behavior scale (Umphress et al., 2010), α = 0.91 (e.g., “If it would help my organization, I would misrepresent the truth to make my organization look good”). Team identification (α = 0.91) and collective efficacy (α = 0.89) were same as the ones used in Study 2. Following prior literature (Kish-Gephart et al., 2010; Umphress et al., 2010; Thau et al., 2015), we also included following control variables: self-control, job satisfaction, and impression management. Self-control scale was assessed via scale by Tangney et al. (2004), α = 0.78 (e.g., “I am able to work effectively toward long-term goals”). Job satisfaction was assessed with four items drawn from a job satisfaction scale (Brooke et al., 1988), α = 0.84 (e.g., “I feel fairly satisfied with my present job”). Impression management subscale was from the Balanced Inventory of Desirable Responding (Paulhus, 1984), α = 0.86 (e.g., “Once in a while I laugh at a dirty joke”). In addition, we measured respondents’ competitiveness, perceived organizational support, and perceived market performance, as these variables could also affect their unethical behaviors in favor of their team. Competitiveness was measured by five items from the competitive orientation scale (Chen et al., 2011), α = 0.71 (e.g., “I feel somewhat disappointed when others perform better than me”). Perceived organizational support was assessed with six items (Eisenberger et al., 2001), α = 0.88 (e.g., “Our bank really cares about my wellbeing”). All responses to these scales were given on a seven-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). We also controlled for organizational performance by using three items (marketing/profitability/market share) from a perceived market performance scale (Delaney and Huselid, 1996) which concerned respondents’ perceptions of their organization’s performance relative to their competitors. Responses to this scale were given on a four-point scale ranging from 1 (worse) to 4 (much better), α = 80.
Results
Regression analysis was used to test Hypotheses 3. Results were shown in Table 1. When controlling for job satisfaction, competitiveness, perceived organizational support, impression management, perceived market performance, as well as demographic variables (gender and age), results revealed a significantly negative effect of collective efficacy, b = −0.39, p < 0.05. This finding supports Hypothesis 3 that there is a negative association between collective efficacy and unethical behavior in group-based competition. This finding is consistent with Study 2, and provides further insight for the role of collective efficacy belief in constraining unethical behavior in team-based competition.
General discussion
Past decades have witnessed a growing body of research contributing to our knowledge about the antecedents and consequences of unethical behavior (Treviño, 1986; Ford and Richardson, 1994; O’Fallon and Butterfield, 2005; Tenbrunsel and Smith-Crowe, 2008; Bazerman and Gino, 2012; Treviño et al., 2014). However, empirical research examining unethical behavior in group-based competitions is limited. The present research provides insights concerning how and why group-based competition experiences shape members’ unethical behavior. We showed that there are less unethical behaviors in the group-based competition than in the individual-based competition, and that collective efficacy has the effect of curbing unethical behavior in group-based competition.
Our studies make the following theoretical contributions to literature: Research suggests that goal settings can stimulate unethical behavior (Ordóñez et al., 2009), particularly when people fail in attaining their goals (Schweitzer et al., 2004). Our studies confirmed this goal-induced unethical behaviors in individual-based competition, and more importantly, we shed further light on psychological constraints (i.e., collective efficacy belief) on unethical behavior in group-based competitions. Our research also contributes to competition and rivalry literature (Kilduff et al., 2010). Our behavioral experiments consist of two consecutive competitions, each with a winning and a losing outcome. This repeated competitive relationship is consistent to the defining feature of rivalry (Kilduff et al., 2010). Existing literature suggests that rivalries boost unethical behaviors (Kilduff et al., 2010). Our findings suggest that loss in a group-based competition did not result in more unethical behaviors as compared to the winning condition. Thus we provide new knowledge about how group-based rivalry influences individuals’ unethical behaviors.
In two behavioral experiments, we observed that losers in the individual-based competition were more intended to behave unethically as compared to winners. Few study has aimed at revealing how competition loss (vs. win) might affect unethical behavior. Our study is consistent with a related stream of research showing that individual-level financial deprivation (Kern and Chugh, 2009; Reinders Folmer and De Cremer, 2012; Sharma et al., 2014) and perceived payment inequity (Greenberg, 1990, 2002; Gino and Pierce, 2009, 2010a,2010b; Houser et al., 2012; John et al., 2014) lead to maleficent acts. Both competition loss and relative deprivation are forms of goal failure that may lead to heightened motive to restore advantages among those who are falling behind (Locke and Latham, 1990). These individuals tend to place greater significance on competition outcomes, leading to eager styles of goal pursuit (Kilduff, 2014; Converse and Reinhard, 2016; Kilduff and Galinsky, 2017) and unethical acts aiming at outperforming their opponents (Kilduff et al., 2016; Kim and Guinote, 2021). Thus our finding is consistent with goal-induced unethical behavior observed in prior studies (Schweitzer et al., 2004; Ordóñez et al., 2009; Welsh and Ordóñez, 2014).
In Study 1 and 2, different from the individual-based competition, we found that loss (vs. win) in the group-based competition did not lead to more unethical behaviors in a subsequent group-based competition. However, it is unclear whether experiencing a group-based competition would affect unethical behavior in favor of personal benefits in a subsequent individual-based competition. Therefore, we conducted a follow-up experiment in which all participants engaged in a team-based competition, followed by an individual-based competition in which each participant competed with an individual from the competing team. Experimental protocol was identical to the one used in Study 2 with the following exception: The first competition (cognitive task) was a team-based competition and the second one was an individual-based competition in which each participant competed with an individual from the competing team. We speculated that losers in the first competitive task (the team-based competition) are more unethical than winners in the consecutive individual-based competition. A total of 64 students (47% male; Mage = 19.69, SDage = 1.42) were randomly assigned to a losing (N = 32) or a winning (N = 32) condition. The experiment was a between-subjects design. The number of idioms participants over-reported in the losing condition (Mloss = 2.13, SD = 2.550) was larger than those in the winning condition [Mwin = 0.84, SD = 1.273, F(1, 62) = 6.469, p = 0.013, ηp2 = 0.094]. This result confirmed our speculation and suggest that group-based competition experience did not alter unethical behavior for the personal interests in the individual-based competition.
Competitions and rivalries are ubiquitous in organizational life, but it is far from clear how group-based competition processes influence employees’ unethical behaviors. Some literature points to the relation between perceived loafing and lesser effort in groups (Schnake, 1991; Mulvey and Klein, 1998; Heuzé and Brunel, 2003), while other literature suggests that engaging in group-based competitions or rivalries may promote members’ intrinsic motivation in group tasks (Tauer and Harackiewicz, 2004), motivating their cooperation with partners (Erev et al., 1993; Bornstein et al., 2002; De Cremer and van Dijk, 2002), and improving group performance (Mulvey and Ribbens, 1999; Kilduff et al., 2010; Pike et al., 2018). Our research suggests that collective efficacy belief developed in group-based competition would increase members’ confidence in achieving good performance, and such collective efficacy belief would have the effect of inhibiting unethical behaviors. Thus managers could use techniques (e.g., team-based incentives) to promote employees’ collective efficacy.
The present study has some limitations: Our research did not answer how intensity of competition affects unethical behaviors. Cartwright and Menezes (2014) interestingly showed in their experiment that competition intensity at medium level is more likely to produce unethical behavior. It is far from clear the mechanism underlying their finding, thus future research could devote to exploring how and why different levels of competitions intensity would influence unethical behaviors, especially in group-based competitions. Another limitation relates to the fact that our research did not consider whether degree of similarity between current competitive interactions and past ones would affect unethical behavior. Rivalry relational schemata develop from past competitive experience, and high degree of similarity between current competitive interactions and past ones will facilitate the activation of rivalry relational schema, and will evoke unethical behaviors to gain advantages (Kilduff, 2014; Converse and Reinhard, 2016). Future research can manipulate degree of similarity to see how it influences unethical behaviors.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
The studies involving humans were approved by the Nanjing Xiaozhuang University Institutional Review Board. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author contributions
YZ: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft. LZ: Methodology, Data curation, Writing – review and editing. YH: Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – review and editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Aquino, K., and Douglas, S. (2003). Identity threat and antisocial behavior in organizations: The moderating effects of individual differences, aggressive modeling, and hierarchical status. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Processes 90, 195–208. doi: 10.1016/S0749-5978(02)00517-4
Ashforth, B. E., and Mael, F. (1989). Social identity theory and the organization. Acad. Manage. Rev. 14, 20–39. doi: 10.5465/amr.1989.4278999
Bandura, A. (1991). Social cognitive theory of self-regulation. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 50, 248–287. doi: 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90022-L
Bandura, A. (1999). Moral disengagement in the perpetration of inhumanities. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 3, 193–209. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0303_3
Bandura, A. (2000). Exercise of human agency through collective efficacy. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 9, 75–78. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00064
Baumeister, R. F., and Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychol. Bull. 117, 497–529. doi: 10.1521/jscp.1990.9.2.165
Bazerman, M. H., and Gino, F. (2012). Behavioral ethics: Toward a deeper understanding of moral judgment and dishonesty. Annu. Rev. Law Soc. Sci. 8, 85–104. doi: 10.1146/annurev-lawsocsci-102811-173815
Bennett, V. M., Pierce, L., Snyder, J. A., and Toffel, M. W. (2013). Customer-driven misconduct: How competition corrupts business practices. Manage. Sci. 59, 1725–1742. doi: 10.1287/mnsc.1120.1680
Bornstein, G., Gneezy, U., and Nagel, R. (2002). The effect of intergroup competition on group coordination: An experimental study. Games Econ. Behav. 41, 1–25. doi: 10.1016/S0899-8256(02)00012-X
Brooke, P. P., Russell, D. W., and Price, J. L. (1988). Discriminant validation of measures of job satisfaction, job involvement, and organizational commitment. J. Appl. Psychol. 73, 139–145. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.73.2.139
Cartwright, E., and Menezes, M. L. C. (2014). Cheating to win: Dishonesty and the intensity of competition. Econ. Lett. 122, 55–58. doi: 10.1016/j.econlet.2013.10.016
Chen, M., Chen, C. C., and Sheldon, O. J. (2016). Relaxing moral reasoning to win: How organizational identification relates to unethical pro-organizational behavior. J. Appl. Psychol. 101, 1082–1096. doi: 10.1037/apl0000111
Chen, X., Xie, X., and Chang, S. (2011). Cooperative and competitive orientation among Chinese people: Scale development and validation. Manage. Organ. Rev. 7, 353–379. doi: 10.1111/j.1740-8784.2011.00215.x
Converse, B. A., and Reinhard, D. A. (2016). On rivalry and goal pursuit: Shared competitive history, legacy concerns, and strategy selection. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 110, 191–213. doi: 10.1037/pspa0000038
De Cremer, D., and van Dijk, E. (2002). Reactions to group success and failure as a function of identification level: A test of the goal-transformation hypothesis in social dilemmas. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 38, 435–442. doi: 10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00009-4
De Dreu, C. K. W. (2010). “Social conflict: The emergence and consequences of struggle and negotiation,” in Handbook of social psychology, eds S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, and G. Lindzey (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc).
Delaney, J. T., and Huselid, M. A. (1996). The impact of human resource management practices on perceptions of organizational performance. Acad. Manage. J. 39, 949–969. doi: 10.2307/256718
Deutsch, M. (1949). A theory of co-operation and competition. Hum. Relat. 2, 129–152. doi: 10.1177/001872674900200204
Doosje, B., Ellemers, N., and Spears, R. (1995). Perceived intragroup variability as a function of group status and identification. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 31, 410–436. doi: 10.1006/jesp.1995.1018
Eisenberger, R., Armeli, S., Rexwinkel, B., Lynch, P. D., and Rhoades, L. (2001). Reciprocation of perceived organizational support. J. Appl. Psychol. 86, 42–51. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.86.1.42
Erev, I., Bornstein, G., and Galili, R. (1993). Constructive intergroup competition as a solution to the free rider problem: A field experiment. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 29, 463–478. doi: 10.1006/jesp.1993.1021
Esnard, C., and Roques, M. (2014). Collective efficacy: A resource in stressful occupational contexts. Rev. Eur. Psychol. Appl. 64, 203–211. doi: 10.1016/j.erap.2014.05.003
Fehr, R., Welsh, D., Yam, K. C., Baer, M., Wei, W., and Vaulont, M. (2019). The role of moral decoupling in the causes and consequences of unethical pro-organizational behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 153, 27–40. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2019.05.007
Ford, R., and Richardson, W. (1994). Ethical decision making: A review of the empirical literature. J. Bus. Ethics 13, 205–221. doi: 10.1007/BF02074820
Garcia, S. M., Tor, A., and Schiff, T. M. (2013). The psychology of competition: A social comparison perspective. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 8, 634–650. doi: 10.1177/1745691613504114
Gibson, C. B. (1999). Do they do what they believe they can? Group efficacy and group effectiveness across tasks and cultures. Acad. Manage. J. 42, 138–152. doi: 10.5465/257089
Gino, F., and Margolis, J. D. (2011). Bringing ethics into focus: How regulatory focus and risk preferences influence (Un)ethical behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 115, 145–156. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2011.01.006
Gino, F., and Pierce, L. (2009). Dishonesty in the name of equity. Psychol. Sci. 20, 1153–1160. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02421.x
Gino, F., and Pierce, L. (2010a). Lying to level the playing field: Why people may dishonestly help or hurt others to create equity. J. Bus. Ethics 95, 89–103. doi: 10.1007/s10551-011-0792-2
Gino, F., and Pierce, L. (2010b). Robin hood under the hood: Wealth-based discrimination in illicit customer help. Organ. Sci. 21, 1176–1194. doi: 10.1287/orsc.1090.0498
Goette, L., Huffman, D., Meier, S., and Sutter, M. (2012). Competition between organizational groups: Its impact on altruistic and antisocial motivations. Manage. Sci. 58, 948–960. doi: 10.1287/mnsc.1110.1466
Greenberg, J. (1990). Employee theft as a reaction to underpayment inequity: The hidden cost of pay cuts. J. Appl. Psychol. 75, 561–568. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.75.5.561
Greenberg, J. (2002). Who stole the money, and when? Individual and situational determinants of employee theft. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 89, 985–1003. doi: 10.1016/S0749-5978(02)00039-0
Halevy, N., Chou, E. Y., Cohen, T. R., and Bornstein, G. (2010). Relative deprivation and intergroup competition. Group Process. Intergr. Relat. 13, 685–700. doi: 10.1177/1368430210371639
Heuzé, J. P., and Brunel, P. C. (2003). Social loafing in a competitive context. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 1, 246–263. doi: 10.1080/1612197X.2003.9671717
Houser, D., Vetter, S., and Winter, J. (2012). Fairness and cheating. Eur. Econ. Rev. 56, 1645–1655. doi: 10.1016/j.euroecorev.2012.08.001
Jex, S. M., and Bliese, P. D. (1999). Efficacy beliefs as a moderator of the impact of work-related stressors: A multilevel study. J. Appl. Psychol. 84, 349–361. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.84.3.349
Jiang, W., Liang, B., and Wang, L. (2022). The double-edged sword effect of unethical pro-organizational behavior: The relationship between unethical pro-organizational behavior, organizational citizenship behavior, and work effort. J. Bus. Ethics 183, 1159–1172. doi: 10.1007/s10551-021-05034-5
John, L. K., Loewenstein, G., and Rick, S. I. (2014). Cheating more for less: Upward social comparisons motivate the poorly compensated to cheat. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 123, 101–109. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2013.08.002
Kawachi, I., and Berkman, L. F. (2001). Social ties and mental health. J. Urban Health 78, 458–467. doi: 10.1093/jurban/78.3.458
Kern, M. C., and Chugh, D. (2009). Bounded ethicality: The perils of loss framing. Psychol. Sci. 20, 378–384. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02296.x
Kilduff, G. J. (2014). Driven to win: Rivalry, motivation, and performance. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 5, 944–952. doi: 10.1177/1948550614539770
Kilduff, G. J., and Galinsky, A. D. (2017). The spark that ignites: Mere exposure to rivals increases machiavellianism and unethical behavior. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 69, 156–162. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2016.10.007
Kilduff, G. J., Elfenbein, H. A., and Staw, B. M. (2010). The psychology of rivalry: A relationally dependent analysis of competition. Acad. Manage. J. 53, 943–969. doi: 10.5465/amj.2010.54533171
Kilduff, G. J., Galinsky, A. D., Gallo, E., and Reade, J. J. (2016). Whatever it takes to win: Rivalry increases unethical behavior. Acad. Manage. J. 59, 1508–1534. doi: 10.5465/amj.2014.0545
Kim, K. H., and Guinote, A. (2021). Cheating to win or not to lose: Power and situational framing affect unethical behavior. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 52, 137–144. doi: 10.1111/jasp.12852
Kish-Gephart, J. J., Harrison, D. A., and Treviño, L. K. (2010). Bad apples, bad cases, and bad barrels: Meta-analytic evidence about sources of unethical decisions at work. J. Appl. Psychol. 95, 1–31. doi: 10.1037/a0017103
Kozlowski, S. W. J., and Ilgen, D. R. (2006). Enhancing the effectiveness of work groups and teams. Psychol. Sci. Public Int. 7, 77–124. doi: 10.1111/j.1529-1006.2006.00030.x
Leavitt, K., and Sluss, D. (2015). Lying for who we are: An identity-based model of workplace dishonesty. Acad. Manage. Rev. 40, 587–610. doi: 10.5465/amr.2013.0167
Locke, E. A., and Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Mazar, N., Amir, O., and Ariely, D. (2008). The dishonesty of honest people: A theory of self-concept maintenance. J. Market. Res. 45, 633–644. doi: 10.1509/jmkr.45.6.633
Mead, N. L., Baumeister, R. F., Gino, F., Schweitzer, M. E., and Ariely, D. (2009). Too tired to tell the truth: Self-control resource depletion and dishonesty. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 45, 594–597. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.02.004
Molinsky, A., and Margolis, J. (2005). Necessary evils and interpersonal sensitivity in organizations. Acad. Manage. Rev. 30, 245–268. doi: 10.5465/amr.2005.16387884
Mulvey, P. W., and Klein, H. J. (1998). The impact of perceived loafing and collective efficacy on group goal processes and group performance. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 74, 62–87. doi: 10.1006/obhd.1998.2753
Mulvey, P. W., and Ribbens, B. A. (1999). The effects of intergroup competition and assigned group goals on group efficacy and group effectiveness. Small Group Res. 30, 651–677. doi: 10.1177/104649649903000601
Neave, N., and Wolfson, S. (2003). Testosterone, territoriality, and the ‘home advantage’. Physiol. Behav. 78, 269–275. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(02)00969-1
O’Fallon, M., and Butterfield, K. (2005). A review of the empirical ethical decision-making literature: 1996–2003. J. Bus. Ethics 59, 375–413. doi: 10.1007/s10551-005-2929-7
Ordóñez, L. D., Schweitzer, M. E., Galinsky, A. D., and Bazerman, M. H. (2009). Goals gone wild: The systematic side effects of overprescribing goal setting. Acad. Manage. Perspect. 23, 6–16. doi: 10.5465/amp.2009.37007999
Paulhus, D. L. (1984). Two-component models of socially desirable responding. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 46, 598–609. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.46.3.598
Pierce, J. R., Kilduff, G. J., Galinsky, A. D., and Sivanathan, N. (2013). From glue to gasoline: How competition turns perspective takers unethical. Psychol. Sci. 24, 1986–1994. doi: 10.1177/0956797613482144
Pike, B. E., Kilduff, G. J., and Galinsky, A. D. (2018). The long shadow of rivalry: Rivalry motivates performance today and tomorrow. Psychol. Sci. 29, 804–813. doi: 10.1177/0956797617744796
Pruitt, D. G. (1998). “Social conflict,” in Handbook of social psychology, 4th Edn, Vol. 2, eds S. T. F. D. Gilbert and G. Lindzey (New York, NY: McGraw-Hill), 470–503.
Putnam, R. D. (1993). Making democracy work: Civic traditions in modern Italy. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Reinders Folmer, C. P., and De Cremer, D. (2012). Bad for me or bad for us? Interpersonal Orientations and the impact of losses on unethical behavior. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 38, 760–771. doi: 10.1177/0146167211436252
Salanova, M., Llorens, S., Cifre, E., Martínez, I. M., and Schaufeli, W. B. (2003). Perceived collective efficacy, subjective well-being and task performance among electronic work groups: An experimental study. Small Group Res. 34, 43–73. doi: 10.1177/1046496402239577
Schnake, M. E. (1991). Equity in effort: The “sucker effect” in co-acting groups. J. Manage. 17, 41–55. doi: 10.1177/014920639101700104
Schurr, A., and Ritov, I. (2016). Winning a competition predicts dishonest behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 113, 1754–1759. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1515102113
Schweitzer, M. E., Ordóñez, L., and Douma, B. (2004). Goal setting as a motivator of unethical behavior. Acad. Manage. J. 47, 422–432. doi: 10.2307/20159591
Shalvi, S., and De Dreu, C. K. W. (2014). Oxytocin promotes group-serving dishonesty. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 5503–5507. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1400724111
Shalvi, S., Dana, J., Handgraaf, M. J. J., and De Dreu, C. K. W. (2011a). Justified ethicality: Observing desired counterfactuals modifies ethical perceptions and behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 115, 181–190. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2011.02.001
Shalvi, S., Handgraaf, M. J. J., and De Dreu, C. K. W. (2011b). Ethical manoeuvring: Why people avoid both major and minor lies. Br. J. Manage. 22, S16–S27. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8551.2010.00709.x
Sharma, E., Mazar, N., Alter, A. L., and Ariely, D. (2014). Financial deprivation selectively shifts moral standards and compromises moral decisions. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 123, 90–100. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2013.09.001
Stein, A. A. (1976). Conflict and cohesion: A review of the literature. J. Conflict Resolut. 20, 143–172. doi: 10.1177/002200277602000106
Tajfel, H. (1982). Social psychology of intergroup relations. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 33, 1–39. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.33.020182.000245
Tajfel, H., and Turner, J. C. (1979). “An integrative theory of intergroup conflict,” in The social psychology of intergroup relations, eds W. G. Austin and S. Worchel (Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole), 33–47.
Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., and Boone, A. L. (2004). High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. J. Pers. 72, 271–324. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x
Tauer, J. M., and Harackiewicz, J. M. (2004). The effects of cooperation and competition on intrinsic motivation and performance. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 86, 849–861. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.86.6.849
Tenbrunsel, A. E., and Smith-Crowe, K. (2008). 13Ethical decision making: Where we’ve been and where we’re going. Acad. Manage. Ann. 2, 545–607. doi: 10.1080/19416520802211677
Thau, S., Derfler-Rozin, R., Pitesa, M., Mitchell, M. S., and Pillutla, M. M. (2015). Unethical for the sake of the group: Risk of social exclusion and pro-group unethical behavior. J. Appl. Psychol. 100, 98–113. doi: 10.1037/a0036708
Thye, S., Lawler, E. J., and Yoon, J. (2019). The formation of group ties in open interaction groups. Soc. Psychol. Q. 82, 98–113. doi: 10.1177/0190272518813562
To, C., Kilduff, G. J., Ordoñez, L., and Schweitzer, M. E. (2018). Going for it on fourth down: Rivalry increases risk taking, physiological arousal, and promotion focus. Acad. Manage. J. 61, 1281–1306. doi: 10.5465/amj.2016.0850
Treviño, L. K. (1986). Ethical decision making in organizations: A person-situation interactionist model. Acad. Manage. Rev. 11, 601–617. doi: 10.5465/amr.1986.4306235
Treviño, L. K., den Nieuwenboer, N. A., and Kish-Gephart, J. J. (2014). (Un)Ethical Behavior in Organizations. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 65, 635–660. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143745
Tucker, M. K., Jimmieson, N. L., and Oei, T. P. (2013). The relevance of shared experiences: A multi-level study of collective efficacy as a moderator of job control in the stressor-strain relationship. Work Stress 27, 1–21. doi: 10.1080/02678373.2013.772356
Umphress, E. E., and Bingham, J. B. (2011). When employees do bad things for good reasons: Examining unethical pro-organizational behaviors. Organ. Sci. 22, 621–640. doi: 10.1287/orsc.1100.0559
Umphress, E. E., Bingham, J. B., and Mitchell, M. S. (2010). Unethical behavior in the name of the company: The moderating effect of organizational identification and positive reciprocity beliefs on unethical pro-organizational behavior. J. Appl. Psychol. 95, 769–780. doi: 10.1037/a0019214
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
Welsh, D. T., and Ordóñez, L. D. (2014). The dark side of consecutive high performance goals: Linking goal setting, depletion, and unethical behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 123, 79–89. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2013.07.006
Welsh, D. T., Ordóñez, L. D., Snyder, D. G., and Christian, M. S. (2015). The slippery slope: How small ethical transgressions pave the way for larger future transgressions. J. Appl. Psychol. 100, 114–127. doi: 10.1037/a0036950
Keywords: intergroup competition, lying behavior, collective efficacy beliefs, immoral behavior, group identification
Citation: Zhu Y, Zheng L and Hu Y (2023) Psychological constraint on unethical behavior in team-based competition. Front. Psychol. 14:1274414. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1274414
Received: 08 August 2023; Accepted: 27 October 2023;
Published: 14 November 2023.
Edited by:
Tian Xie, Wuhan University, ChinaReviewed by:
Catarina Morais, University of Porto, PortugalZeynep Aytug, California State Polytechnic University, Pomona, United States
Copyright © 2023 Zhu, Zheng and Hu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Lijing Zheng, zhenglj26@mail.sysu.edu.cn; Yu Hu, huyu@swufe.edu.cn