Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 29 November 2023
Sec. Addictive Behaviors

Association between problem gambling and personality traits: a longitudinal study among the general Norwegian population

  • 1Department of Psychosocial Science, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway
  • 2Optentia Research Focus Area, North-West University, Vanderbijlpark, South Africa
  • 3Norwegian Competence Center for Gambling and Gaming Research, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway
  • 4Department of Health Promotion, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Bergen, Norway
  • 5Department of Psychology, PROMENTA Research Center, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway

Objective: The present study investigates the longitudinal relationship between problematic gambling (PG) and the five factor model’s personality traits using autoregressive cross-lagged models.

Methods: The data used in the current study was collected by a national survey in 2013 (n = 10,081) and a follow-up study (n = 5,848) in 2015. PG was measured using Canadian Problem Gambling Index (CPGI) while personality was assessed using Mini-International Personality Item Pool (MINI-IPIP). Participants who completed the CPGI and all the personality items during both waves (n = 2,702) were analysed.

Results: The results show that neuroticism had positive cross-lagged associations with CPGI. In contrast, conscientiousness and agreeableness in 2013 were found to have inverse cross-lagged effect on CPGI in 2015. Finally, openness and extraversion did not have any cross-lagged associations with CPGI.

Conclusion: PG poses serious negative implications for the involved individuals as well as their associated close social circle. Hence, it is important to understand predictors of PG for prevention purposes. Personality traits are one of the influential frameworks for examining uncontrolled psychopathological behaviors like PG. The study findings offer significant theoretical as well as practical implications.

1 Introduction

Occasional gambling is often experienced as an enjoyable experience (Molde et al., 2019). However, scholars caution that the gratifying nature of gambling may lead to excessive or incontrollable gambling for some, often denoted problematic gambling (PG) (Graves, 2018). Gambling problems can be defined as maladaptive gambling behavior that disrupts personal, family and/or vocational pursuits (Hodgins and El-Guebaly, 2000). In line with the gratifying nature of gambling, the literature suggests that some individuals gamble as a strategy for mood modification and temporary escape from the negative states (Ciccarelli M. C. et al., 2016).

Even though the percentage of the general population which is deemed as problematic gambler is relatively low, it is still considered as a major health concern owing to the social, psychological and financial implications associated with such gambling (Delfabbro and King, 2020). The implications of gambling can be considered both at the individual and societal level. At the individual level, problem gamblers can suffer cognitively [e.g., cognitive distortion, poor decision making (Ciccarelli M. C. et al., 2016); foreshortened time horizon (Ciccarelli M. et al., 2016)], affectively [e.g., mood disorder (Lorains et al., 2011)], and behaviourally [e.g., substance use disorder, anti-social personality disorder (Dowd et al., 2019)]. Furthermore, problem gamblers are often found to experience various mental health issues such as emotional distress, anxiety, depression, and impulsivity among others (Ciccarelli M. C. et al., 2016). Finally, scholars also suggest that PG poses various financial challenges posed due to bankruptcy, debts and increased health-care costs (Graves, 2018). From the societal perspective, PG is associated with disturbed family life, strained relationships with family, friends and colleagues (Onyedire et al., 2019) and impairment of daily functioning (Madey, 2019). Kalischuk et al. (2006) report that a problematic gambler could affect about five to fifteen other people.

Scholars have suggested that personality traits may be influential in explaining and understanding different forms of psychopathological behaviors including PG (Strømme et al., 2021). Personality is regarded as a psychophysical structure within individuals which is dynamic in nature and affect the way humans respond to their environment (Allport, 1937). The empirical evidence of significant associations between personality traits and PG further strengthens the inference that personality can play an influential role in the development of PG (Dowd et al., 2019). One of the most influential contemporary trait theories is the five-factor model of personality. This model differentiates between five main personality dimensions: (1) Neuroticism (e.g., being nervous and anxiety prone), (2) Extraversion (e.g., being talkative and outgoing), (3) Openness to experience (e.g., being imaginative and intellectually oriented), (4) Agreeableness (e.g., being sympathetic and warm), and (5) Conscientiousness (e.g., being organized and prompt) (Wiggins, 1996). According to evolutionary perspectives the five dimensions are closely linked to solving adaptive problems, e.g., in terms of deciding who will be a burden, who will be a good cooperator and who will work industriously (Buss, 1991). Scholars have examined the association between the dimensions of the five factor model of personality and PG [(e.g., Brunborg et al., 2016; Reardon et al., 2019)].

The extant research on the topic reports that the typical profile of the adult problematic gambler includes being high on neuroticism and low on conscientiousness and agreeableness (Tackett et al., 2015; Brunborg et al., 2016). Other studies suggests that problematic gamblers are found to be low in openness (Madey, 2019). Similarly, adolescents and young adults low in conscientiousness and agreeableness and high on neuroticism represent a vulnerable group, more likely to develop PG compared to their counterparts (Tackett et al., 2015; Reardon et al., 2019).

Although some associations between the five-factor model of personality and PG has been established our review of prior literature suggest three research gaps: (a) there is mixed findings regarding the possible associations due to differences in the demographic profile of respondents, culture, choice of measurement and research design. For example, scholors have suggested absence of significant association between neuroticism and PG [(e.g., Hwang et al., 2012)] while some studies suggest otherwise (Tackett et al., 2015; Pallesen et al., 2016); (b) Most prior studies have relied on small sample sizes (Tackett et al., 2015; Mackinnon et al., 2016; Reardon et al., 2019) with some exceptions (Brunborg et al., 2016). Furthermore, prior literature is predominately based on cross-sectional studies (Brunborg et al., 2016; Reardon et al., 2019) with an exception of few longitudinal studies (Mackinnon et al., 2016); (c) To the best of our knowledge, the prior literature lacks knowledge regarding the nature of reciprocal association among different personality traits and PG. The current study aims to fill the aforementioned gaps by adding new knowledge regarding the reciprocal association of personality traits and PG through a two-year long longitudinal panel study, based on a representative sample of 2,702 Norwegian gamblers. The initial hypotheses for the study are: (i) different personality traits in 2013 influence CPGI in 2015 and (ii) CPGI in 2013 influences different personality traits in 2015.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants

The present study takes into consideration all the participants who completed the CPGI and all the personality items during both waves. The age range for the analytic sample varied from 16 to 74 years with mean age of 50.66 years (SD = 13.24 years) and consists of 49.5% females (n = 1,338) and 50.5% males (n = 1,364) in 2013. Table 1 presents details of the demographic variables of the study participants.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the study participants in 2013.

2.2 Instruments

2.2.1 Gambling participation

The questionnaire contained a definition of gambling whereupon the respondents were asked if they had participated in gambling during the last 12 months (yes/no). Gambling was defined as “a game where money is bet on a specific outcome of an event or draw and where you can win cash prizes (e.g., Lotto, Tipping, scratch cards, casino games etc.).” Those who endorsed were asked to complete the Canadian Problem Gambling Index.

2.2.2 Canadian problem gambling index

The CPGI scale measured PG behavior and its probable consequences using nine items with response alternatives ranging from “never” (0) to “always” (Hodgins and El-Guebaly, 2000; Ferris and Wynne, 2001). The scale has a total of nine items were five items are examining the PG behavior while the other four items are investigating its probable consequences. The composite score ranges from 0 to 27 enabling grouping of the participants into four different severity levels of problematic gambling. The four severity levels reflect non-problem gambler (score – 0), low risk gambler (score 1–2), moderate risk gambler (score 3–7), and problem gambler (score 8–27). In the present analysis the composite score was used by adding the score of each item for each individual participant where higher score reflects more gambling problems. In 2013 the CPGI scale had a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.87 while it was 0.81 in 2015.

2.2.3 Mini-international personality item pool

The MINI-IPIP scale was used for assessing the personality of the respondents (Donnnellan et al., 2006). The scale consists of 20 items measuring the traits of the five-factor model of personality namely: extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism, and openness. Each personality trait was measured using four items, with response alternatives ranging from “very wrong” (Molde et al., 2019) to “very correct” (Delfabbro and King, 2020). The Cronbach’s alpha values in wave 1 were as follows: 0.78 for extraversion, 0.69 for agreeableness, 0.65 for conscientiousness, 0.66 for neuroticism and 0.68 for openness. For wave 2, the corresponding values were 0.77, 0.70, 0.64, 0.66 and, 0.68, respectively.

2.3 Procedure

The longitudinal study was conducted among the general adult Norwegian population on the behalf of the Norwegian Gaming Authority, over a period of two years. The study was approved by Regional Committee for Medical and Health Related Ethics in Western Norway (REK-Vest, project no. 2013/120). The first wave survey was conducted in 2013 where 24,000 people were invited to participate. In total 10,081 valid responses were received, generating a response rate of 42% after removal of those who could not be reached due to wrong addresses. The follow-up wave was conducted in 2015 where the participants of the 2013 study were invited. Of the 10,081 who responded in the 2013 wave 9,741 respondents were reachable, of which 5,809 responded, amounting to a response rate of 59.6%. In both waves, a maximum of two reminders were sent to those who did not respond. Furthermore, all respondents were informed about their participation in a lottery where they had the chance to win the gift voucher of NOK 500 upon answering. The Norwegian Gambling Authority and the Regional Committee for Medical Research and Health Research Ethics in Western Norway had no objections with regards to the usage of lottery as a mode for increasing the response rate. Furthermore, the procedure did not reflect gambling in itself since it did not involve staking money or any other materials of value. Moreover, prior literature reports that incentives could increase response rates together with representativeness of the sample (Olsen et al., 2012). The participants reporting problems through the survey were not contacted actively by the researchers. However, contact information of the researchers was provided to the participants (phone and emails) and the researchers could upon being contacted refer to the ones needing help with relevant treatment facilities.

2.4 Statistical analysis

As mentioned before, respondents who completed both CPGI and all personality traits in both waves were considered for the analysis. Furthermore, the participants with no missing value on any composite personality scores were included. Thus, resulting in the effective sample size of 2,702 respondents.

Analyses were conducted in R (version 4.1.1) using the lavaan package (0.6–9) (R Core Team, 2021). An autoregressive cross lagged model with observed indicators was tested to measure the cross-lagged associations among PG and different personality traits (see Figure 1). Autoregressive cross-lagged models examine the longitudinal associations between two constructs over a period of time while controlling for the stability of each construct over time. Moreover, the possibility for the estimation of reciprocal effects on change between two variables over time, together with maintaining of the temporal order offers an advantage compared to other longitudinal modelling approaches (Selig and Little, 2012). For both the CPGI and the five personality traits composite scores were calculated for each wave, respectively. The composite score for all the items on each specific personality trait and CPGI was calculated for first and second time point. The analysis used robust maximum likelihood estimation with Satora-Bentler scaled chi-squared test involving robust standard errors. As a 2-wave autoregressive model are just-identified (contains no degrees of freedom) traditional model fit SEM statistics are not available. Hence, the analysis was conducted by running four different models with criterion based fit indices Akaike’s information criteria (AIC), Bayesian information criteria (BIC) and Expected cross-validation index (ECVI) in addition to traditional model fit indices of Model Chi-Square2), Comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker Lewis index (TLI), Root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), Standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) and 95% confidence intervals (95% CI) were available. The null-model (M0) represents a model with no path effects. The stability model (M1) represents a model where only the auto-regressive paths are investigated. The cross-lagged model (M2) represents a model where only the cross-lagged effects are investigated. Finally, the just-identified model (M3) represents a model where both cross-lagged and autoregressive paths are investigated. The idea behind just-identified model to evaluate the proposed paths rather than how well the data fits the model. However, to compare the just-identified model to the other models (M0, M1, and M2) criterion based fit indices are used where lower values for AIC, BIC and ECVI indicated a better and more pragmatic fitted model.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Research Model.

3 Results

The descriptive statistics and correlations among the study constructs over the time of two years are reported in Table 2. Overall, Table 2 show that there is no association between extraversion and openness and CPGI. The association between agreeableness, neuroticism, and consciousness ranges from marginal to small. Furthermore, there is a strong within correlation (WC) between all personally traits in 2013 and 2015, and a medium association between CPGI in 2013 and 2015. Table 3 presents four models tested as a part of the analysis containing details of their model fit and path estimates. Among the four tested models, cross-lagged associations were only tested in cross-lagged models and just-identified models. In general, the null model and cross-lagged models were found to have poor model fit for all the personality traits, mainly due to high AIC, BIC and ECVI values compared to the stability and just-identified model for all the personality traits. In contrast, the stability model and just-identified model were found to have good model fit for the different personality factors. However, the just-identified model was preferred over the stability model despite being a better model in the case of openness and extraversion, because the stability model only tested autoregressive paths.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistics and correlations between personality traits and CPGI in 2013 and 2015.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. A cross-lagged path model of personality and problem gambling.

For all personality traits, the final analysis (M3) showed significant trait to trait and CPGI to CPGI relationships (Path A and Path D) (see Table 3). For extraversion and openness, the cross-lagged association between trait to CPGI and CPGI to trait was not significant (Path B and Path C, respectively). For agreeableness and conscientiousness, there was a significant negative association between the trait in 2013 and CPGI score in 2015 (Paths B), b = −0.084, p < 0.01 and b = −0.060, p < 0.05, respectively, showing that greater agreeableness and conscientiousness in 2013 is associated with lower CPGI score in 2015. For neuroticism, there was a significant positive cross-lagged association between neuroticism in 2013 and CPGI in 2015 (Path B), b = 0.052, p < 0.05 and CPGI in 2013 and neuroticism in 2015 (Path C), b = 0.054, p < 0.01, suggesting that greater neuroticism is associated with higher CPGI score in 2015, and that higher CPGI score in 2013 is associated with greater neuroticism in 2015.

4 Discussion

The study findings report that neuroticism had significant positive cross-lagged effects with PG as neuroticism in 2013 was found to be positively correlated with CPGI in 2015, while CPGI in 2013 is found to influence neuroticism in 2015. An individual with higher levels of neuroticism is usually found to have higher likelihood of experiencing stress and anxiety and is emotionally vulnerable. Such individuals might engage in gambling as way for relieving their stress, which is in line with the escape hypothesis (Rogier et al., 2019). In line with the escape hypothesis, gambling is used as a strategy to escape negative or uncomfortable emotional states (Ciccarelli M. C. et al., 2016; Rogier et al., 2019). For example, gambling may be sought in order to escape from unpleasant feelings and states such as boredom and anxiety (Wulfert et al., 2005). Accordingly, gambling has been found to provide gratifying and enjoying experiences that could assist in temporary stress and anxiety management (Konietzny et al., 2007). The current finding regarding neuroticism and CPGI is in line with previous findings both on PG and other types of addictions (Whiting et al., 2019). Moreover, the prior literature has reported that PG creates stress and anxiety among the gamblers (Ste-Marie et al., 2006). Since individuals high on neuroticism have lower levels of self-control and are more prone to react strongly to stressful life events their PG condition may over time increase their neurotic tendencies, due to the negative consequences of PG (Paterson et al., 2020). Hence, the existence of a bidirectional cross-lagged influence between CPGI and neuroticism seems reasonable.

The personality trait of conscientiousness and agreeableness in 2013 was found to exert inverse cross-lagged influence on CPGI in 2015. The findings stand supported by prior literature showing that problematic gamblers have lower scores on conscientiousness (Brunborg et al., 2016; Whiting et al., 2019) and agreeableness (Tackett et al., 2015; Brunborg et al., 2016). The present findings indicate conscientiousness and agreeableness act as protective traits against developing PG. However, it should be noted that other factors (e.g., gender, age, education, attachment, and self-regulation) could mediate or moderate the association of conscientiousness and agreeableness with PG. It could be assumed that people with high scores on agreeableness avoid excessive gambling, as this typically causes interpersonal conflicts, which such individuals are motivated to avoid. People high on conscientiousness are characterized by high planning ability and self-control, which assumingly will put them in less risk of excessive gambling (Andreassen et al., 2013). In contrast, CPGI in 2013 had no cross-lagged influence on conscientiousness and agreeableness in 2015. Overall, the findings suggest that over time personality seems to exert stronger influence on CPGI than vice versa. This seems conceivable as personality is assumed to be relatively stable (Roberts et al., 2006) whereas CPGI seems more changeable (Nelson et al., 2009). These assumptions were also supported by the current data, showing stronger temporal stability for the personality traits (path A) than for PG (path D).

Extraversion and openness were found to share no cross-lagged correlation with CPGI. The absence of association of extraversion with PG is consistent with the prior literature (Brunborg et al., 2016). This reflects that extraversion as a personality trait seems not to influence PG even in the case of a longitudinal research setting. Similarly, openness did not influence CPGI over time. This finding contradicts the extant literature stating problem gamblers to be low on openness (Brunborg et al., 2016; Madey, 2019). In contrast, lack of association of CPGI in 2013 with both extraversion and openness in 2015 could be attributed to the conception that personality is relatively stable (Roberts et al., 2006).

It should be noted that personality traits associated with development of gambling problems are associated with other mental health problems. For example, several mental health and psychiatric conditions, such as depression and bipolar disorder are linked to the development of gambling problems (Kessler et al., 2008). Consequently, the relationship between personality and later gambling problems is possibly confounded by mental health problems or other psychiatric conditions.

4.1 Study implications

The present study offers different theoretical as well as practical implications. As pointed earlier, the majority of the extant literature provides information on the association between personality and CPGI at one point of time. However, the need for longitudinal investigations has also been emphasised in prior literature (Pallesen et al., 2016). As such the present study offers new knowledge on the temporal association of personality traits and PG. Practically, the findings could help practitioners and therapists in devising targeted prevention efforts for assisting individuals suffering from gambling disorder by focusing specifically on their personality traits. The study results suggest that neurotic individuals have higher tendency to develop PG. Similarly, problematic gamblers have tendency to further increase the level of stress and anxiety usually suffered by the neurotic individuals over the time. Hence, practitioners should focus on devising strategies for managing stress and anxiety of the problematic gamblers that would in turn aid in reducing PG. Consistent with prior literature, the individuals likely to develop PG have different personality characteristics compared to the general population (Tackett et al., 2015; Brunborg et al., 2016; Madey, 2019; Reardon et al., 2019). Overall, the study informs clinical psychologists that personality of an individual seems to have an influential role in determining their tendency to develop PG over time rather than the other way around. Hence, it is important clinical psychologists should consider performing personality screening and identifying individuals that are higher on neuroticism and low on conscientiousness and agreeableness. For individuals identified with a personality associated with higher risk of PG, clinicians may consider psychoeducative steps to inform about how an individual’s personality puts them at risk for problematic gambling behaviour in the future. Personality may also be relevant for the treatment process. Studies have for example shown that low scores on extraversion is associated with increased risk of drop-out from group treatment (MacNair and Corazzini, 1994) and that low scores on conscientiousness is associated with treatment non-compliance (Scherphof et al., 2014).

4.2 Limitations and strengths

The current study suffers from some limitations that should be noted. First, the study is based on self-reported data that could possibly introduce social desirability bias. Second, the study findings cannot be generalized with reservations, due to the national and research context. The study offers insights on gamblers from the general population using specific scales for measuring gambling problems As pointed by Brunborg et al. (2016), PG behavior could vary based on the chosen assessment strategy, geographical location, and cultural background of the respondents. The low ECVI value for the research model indicates that the obtained results have good predictability. Still, it would be useful to verify the findings of the present research in other settings. For example, future studies on this topic should use different scales for measuring PG and should include samples from other geographical locations. The generalization of the results might be influenced by attrition and selection. Still, the response rate is good as 59.6% of the original sample in 2013 also responded in 2015 (Guo et al., 2016). The findings also show that attrition is less likely to be problematic when examining relationships as compared to examining univariate distributions (Rindfuss et al., 2015). Furthermore, the current study reports the results across the full range of scores on CPGI, ranging from non-problem as well as problem gamblers (varying intensities of risk). As previously reported, the majority of the respondents of the present study were non-problem gamblers (86.12% in 2013 and 86.08% in 2015). This might influence the retrieved cross-lagged associations among different personality traits and PG. It should be noted that analysis has been conducted using robust model as an attempt to overcome such issues.

5 Conclusion

The present study adds to the extant knowledge on the nature of association between PG and personality. Through the lens of longitudinal research, the study sheds light on the temporal association among CPGI and personality traits. The proposed study hypotheses are partially supported. The results show that some personality traits (agreeableness, conscientiousness, and neuroticism) influence CPGI over time while only neuroticism has influence on CPGI over time. The study offers insightful information that could add depth in assisting individuals suffering from PG with focus on their personality.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Author contributions

SP, RM, EE, and TL designed the study and collected the data. PK drafted the first version of the manuscript and did the revision that was approved by all the authors. TL conducted the analysis. SP, RM, EE, TL, and RC critically revised the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality: a psychological interpretation. New York: Holt.

Google Scholar

Andreassen, C. S., Griffiths, M. D., Gjertsen, S. R., Krossbakken, E., Kvam, S., and Pallesen, S. (2013). The relationships between behavioral addictions and the five-dfactor model of personality. J. Behav. Addict. 2, 90–99. doi: 10.1556/jba.2.2013.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brunborg, G. S., Hanss, D., Mentzoni, R. A., Mold, H., and Pallesen, S. (2016). Problem gambling and the five-factor model of personality: a large population-based study. Addiction 111, 1428–1435. doi: 10.1111/add.13388

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buss, D. M. (1991). Evolutionary personality psychology. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 42, 459–491. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.42.020191.002331

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ciccarelli, M. C., Griffiths, M. D., Nigro, G., and Cosenza, M. (2016). Decision making, cognitive distortions and emotional distress: a comparison between pathological gamblers and healthy controls. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 54, 204–210. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2016.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ciccarelli, M., Malinconico, R., Griffiths, M. D., Nigro, G., and Cosenza, M. (2016). Reward preferences of pathological gamblers under conditions of uncertainty: an experimental study. J. Gambl. Stud. 32, 1175–1189. doi: 10.1007/s10899-016-9593-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delfabbro, P., and King, D. L. (2020). Don’t say the ‘P’ word: problem gambling is more than harm. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 18, 835–843. doi: 10.1007/s11469-020-00274-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donnnellan, M. B., Oswald, F. L., Baird, B. M., and Lucas, R. E. (2006). The Mini-IPIP scales: tiny yet-effective measures of big five factors of personality. Psychol. Assess. 18, 192–203. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.18.2.192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dowd, D. A., Keough, M. T., Jakobson, L. S., Bolton, J. M., and Edgerton, J. D. (2019). A latent class analysis of young adult gamblers from the Manitoba longitudinal survey of young adults. Int. Gambl. Stud. 19, 148–166. doi: 10.1080/14459795.2018.1520909

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferris, J., and Wynne, H. The Canadian problem gambling index: final report. Ottawa, ON:Canadian Center on Substance Abuse (2001).

Google Scholar

Graves, C. (2018) Problem gambling: The impact of personality on gambling. [Doctoral Dissertation]. Canada: Faculty of Graduate Studies of the University of Manitoba.

Google Scholar

Guo, Y., Kopec, J. A., Cibere, J., Li, L. C., and Goldsmith, C. H. (2016). Population survey features and response rates: a randomized experiment. Am. J. Public Health 106, 1422–1426. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2016.303198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodgins, D. C., and El-Guebaly, N. (2000). Natural and treatment-assisted recovery from gambling problems: a comparison of resolved and active gamblers. Addiction 95, 777–789. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2000.95577713.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hwang, J. Y., Shin, Y.-C., Lim, S.-W., Park, H. Y., Shin, N. Y., Jang, J. H., et al. (2012). Multidimensional comparison of personality characteristics of the big five model, impulsiveness, and affect in pathological gambling and obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Gambl. Stud. 28, 351–362. doi: 10.1007/s10899-011-9269-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalischuk, R. G., Nowatzki, N., Cardwell, K., Klein, K., and Solowoniuk, J. (2006). Problem gambling and its impact on families: a literature review. Int. Gambl. Stud. 6, 31–60. doi: 10.1080/14459790600644176

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, R. C., Hwang, I., LaBrie, R., Petukhova, M., Sampson, N. A., Winters, K. C., et al. (2008). DSM-IV pathological gambling in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Psychol. Med. 38, 1351–1360. doi: 10.1017/s0033291708002900

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Konietzny, J., Caruana, A., and Cassar, M. L. (2007). Fun and fair, and I don't care: the role of enjoyment, fairness and subjective norms on online gambling intentions. J. Retail. Consum. 44, 91–99. doi: 10.1016/j.jretconser.2018.06.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorains, F. K., Cowlishaw, S., and Thomas, S. A. (2011). Prevalence of comorbid disorders in problem and pathological gambling: systematic review and meta-analysis of population surveys. Addiction 106, 490–498. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03300.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mackinnon, S. P., Lambe, L., and Stewart, S. H. (2016). Relations of five-factor personality domains to gambling motives in emerging adult gamblers: a longitudinal study. J. Gambl. Issues 34, 179–200. doi: 10.4309/jgi.2016.34.10

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacNair, R. R., and Corazzini, J. G. (1994). Client factors influencing group therapy dropout. Psychotherapy (Chic.) 31, 352–362. doi: 10.1037/h0090226

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madey, R. (2019) Problem gambling, personality traits, and culture. [Psychology Undergraduate Honours Thesis]. Canada:University of Regina.

Google Scholar

Molde, H., Holmøy, B., Merkesdal, A. G., Torsheim, T., Mentzoni, R. A., Hanns, D., et al. (2019). Are video games a gateway to gambling? A longitudinal study based on a representative norwegian sample. J. Gambl. Stud. 35, 545–557. doi: 10.1007/s10899-018-9781-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, S. E., Gebauer, L., LaBrie, R. A., and Shaffer, H. J. (2009). Gambling problem symptom patterns and stability across individual and timeframe. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 23, 523–533. doi: 10.1037/a0016053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olsen, F., Abelsen, B., and Olsen, J. A. (2012). Improving response rate and quality of survey data with a scratch lottery ticket incentive. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 12:52. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-12-52

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Onyedire, N. G., Chukwuorji, J. C., Orjiakor, T. C., Onu, D. U., Aneke, C. I., and Ifeagwazi, C. M. (2019). Associations of dark triad traits and problem gambling: moderating role of age among university students. Curr. Psychol. 40, 2083–2094. doi: 10.1007/s12144-018-0093-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pallesen, S., Hanss, D., Molde, H., and Griffiths, M. R. A. (2016). A longitudinal study of factors explaining attitude change towards gambling among adolescents. J. Behav. Addict. 5, 59–67. doi: 10.1556/2006.5.2016.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paterson, M., Taylor, M., and Gray, M. (2020). Trajectories of social and economic outcomes and problem gambling risk in Australia. Soc. Indic. Res. 148, 297–321. doi: 10.1007/s11205-019-02194-w

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2021). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria. Available at: https://www.R-project.org/

Google Scholar

Reardon, K. W., Wang, M., Neighbors, C., and Tackett, J. L. (2019). The personality context of adolescent gambling: better expained by the big five or sensation-seeking? J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 41, 69–80. doi: 10.1007/s10862-018-9690-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rindfuss, R. R., Choe, M. K., Tsuya, N. O., Bumpass, L. L., and Tamaki, E. (2015). Do low survey response rates bias results? Evidence from Japan. Demogr. Res. 32, 797–828. doi: 10.4054/DemRes.2015.32.26

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, B. W., Walton, K. E., and Viechtbaurer, W. (2006). Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychol. Bull. 132, 1–25. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogier, G., Picci, G., and Velotti, P. (2019). Struggling with happiness: a pathway leading depression to gambling disorder. J. Gambl. Stud. 35, 293–305. doi: 10.1007/s10899-018-09825-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scherphof, C. S., van den Eijnden, R. J., Lugtig, P., Engels, R. C., and Vollebergh, W. A. (2014). Adolescents' use of nicotine replacement therapy for smoking cessation: predictors of compliance trajectories. Psychopharmacology 231, 1743–1752. doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3511-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Selig, J. P., and Little, T. D. (2012). “Autoregressive and cross-lagged panel analysis for longitudinal data” in Handbook of developmental research methods. eds. B. Laursen, T. D. Little, and N. A. Card (New York: The Guilford Press), 265–278.

Google Scholar

Ste-Marie, C., Gupta, R., and Derevensky, J. L. (2006). Anxiety and social stress related to adolescent gambling behavior and substance use. J. Child Adolesc. Subst. Abuse 15, 55–74. doi: 10.1300/J029v15n04_03

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strømme, R., Børstad, K. H., Rø, A. E., Erevik, E. K., Sagoe, D., Chegeni, R., et al. (2021). The relationship between gambling probems and the five-factor model of personality: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front. Psych. 12:PMC8545825. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.740235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tackett, J. L., Rodriguez, K. L. M., Rinker, D. V., and Neighbors, C. (2015). A personality-based latent class analysis of emerging adult gamblers. J. Gambl. Stud. 31, 1337–1351. doi: 10.1007/s10899-014-9500-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whiting, S. W., Hoff, R. A., Balodis, I. M., and Potenza, M. N. (2019). An exploratory study of relationships among five-factor personality measures and forms of gambling in adults with and without probable pathological gambling. J. Gambl. Stud. 35, 915–928. doi: 10.1007/s10899-018-9809-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiggins, J. S. (1996). The five-factor model of personality: theoretical perspectives. New York: The Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Wulfert, E., Roland, B. D., Hartley, J., Wang, N., and Franco, C. (2005). Heart rate arousal and excitement in gambling: winners versus losers. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 19, 311–316. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.19.3.311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: problem gambling, personality traits, cross-lagged, longitudinal study, quantitative study

Citation: Kaur P, Leino T, Chegeni R, Erevik EK, Mentzoni RA and Pallesen S (2023) Association between problem gambling and personality traits: a longitudinal study among the general Norwegian population. Front. Psychol. 14:1241365. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1241365

Received: 16 June 2023; Accepted: 03 October 2023;
Published: 29 November 2023.

Edited by:

Richard J. Tunney, Aston University, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Francisco González-Sala, University of Valencia, Spain
Eteri Machavariani, Yale University, United States

Copyright © 2023 Kaur, Leino, Chegeni, Erevik, Mentzoni and Pallesen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Puneet Kaur, UHVuZWV0LmthdXJAdWliLm5v

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.