- 1Department of Translation, Interpreting, and Communication, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
- 2Department of Experimental Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
- 3The Accreditation Organisation of the Netherlands and Flanders (NVAO), Den Haag, Netherlands
Several studies have pointed to beneficial effects of bilingualism on executive functioning. However, observations of these beneficial effects have at times proven difficult to reproduce. Moreover, findings of studies on cognitive effects of bilingualism have been contested altogether. These contradictory outcomes leave the research field of bilingualism at unease. In the present review article, we aim to give a systematic overview of previous research on bilingual advantages in inhibition and switching in children up to the age of 12. Particular attention is paid to the experimental tasks that have been applied and the persistence of possible effects throughout critical and post-critical periods for cognitive development in children. In doing so, the review gives an insight in both the validity and robustness of possible domain-general cognitive effects of bilingualism in children. Terminological issues are also discussed.
Introduction
For quite some time now, research has been conducted on the cognitive impact of bilingualism in young children. In early work on bilingualism, the acquisition of languages other than the native one was considered a risk factor for verbal and non-verbal cognitive development. Studies reported that bilinguals performed worse relative to their monolingual peers on a variety of variables, ranging from smaller vocabulary sizes (Smith, 1949) to impaired general intelligence (Darcy, 1946). However, in a seminal paper, Peal and Lambert (1962) reported that 10-year-old bilinguals actually outperformed their monolingual peers on tests of intellectual reasoning. The result was later confirmed by Ben-Zeev (1977) for 5- to 8-year-olds, and, in fact, a bulk of research spread over the past 20 years has actually shown that bilingualism might foster cognition rather than impede it. As such, in a longitudinal study with 5-year-olds, Woumans et al. (2016) found that only bilinguals improved significantly on intelligence over a period of one year. An extensive branch of research has focused on the effect of bilingualism on executive functioning (EF), which is to be understood as covering a broad range of cognitive functions that are used to control and regulate actions and thought. Around the turning of the centuries, a consensus was reached that a bilingual's languages are always simultaneously active and interacting (Hermans et al., 1998; Costa et al., 2000; Duyck, 2005; Brysbaert and Duyck, 2010), resulting in constant cognitive conflict for the bilingual (Green, 1998; Woumans et al., 2016). It became a common research question whether this conflict, as a kind of cognitive exercise, has repercussions outside of the verbal domain. Because language control, and therefore resolution of language conflict, relies on executive functions, this has gradually led to an almost exclusive focus on advanced executive functioning as a by-product of bilingualism, moving away from the former focus on general intelligence.
Several studies have pointed to beneficial effects of bilingualism on executive functioning in children. To illustrate, Kovács and Mehler (2009) revealed through three eye-tracking studies with 7-month-old infants that they outperformed matched monolinguals on cognitive control abilities. Kang and Lust (2019) found that bilingual language proficiency in 8-year-old children was a predictor for their EF performance. Tran et al. (2019) detected a similar bilingualism effect on cognitive control processes measuring selective attention, switching, and inhibition in a longitudinal study with 3- to 4-year-olds. In a large-scale study (N = 18,200) with children aged 5 to 7, bilingualism was found to moderate the effects of socio-economic status (SES) by ameliorating the detrimental consequences of low-SES on EF (Hartanto et al., 2019).
These beneficial effects have, however, proven difficult to systematically reproduce, resulting in an ongoing profound debate on the existence and scope of the bilingual advantage. Both for EF and for intelligence, very diverging results have been reported. As such, Dick et al. (2019) found no evidence for a bilingual executive control advantage in a large sample (N = 4,524) of 9- to 10-year-olds who were tested for inhibitory control, attention, task switching, and cognitive flexibility. Similar results were obtained in a study by Jaekel et al. (2019) on bilingual Turkish immigrant children aged 5 to 15 years. Equally, no effect of bilingualism on tasks of inhibition, updating, and shifting, i.e., components of EF, were found in a study by Arizmendi et al. (2018) among 7- to 9-year-olds. A meta-analysis by Gunnerud et al. (2020) targeting children aged 18 and under gave little support for a bilingual advantage in overall EF.
Those outcomes are in line with what has been identified in bilingual advantage literature targeting adult populations. In a recent large-scale study with 11,000 participants, bilinguals showed no advantage over monolinguals in a battery of 12 EF tasks (Nichols et al., 2020). In fact, findings on bilingual cognitive advantage effects have been contested altogether, starting with the hallmark criticism study of Paap et al. (2015). Issues that have been raised in studies skeptical of bilingual advantages include the observation of and publication bias for frequent null results, insufficient sample sizes, and the use of questionable research methods. Or, it is claimed that participants would be inadequately matched on background variables and any significant differences in performance may well reflect task-specific mechanisms instead of domain-free executive functioning abilities (Paap et al., 2015; Laine and Lehtonen, 2018). The criticism was further corroborated by a meta-analysis indicating no systematic support for the benefits in cognitive control functions associated with bilingualism (Lehtonen et al., 2018). Nevertheless, the meta-analysis by De Bruin et al. (2015) showed a significant bilingual advantage effect across published studies, although the simultaneously observed publication bias for bilingual advantages received more attention and fueled the doubt about the bilingual advantage. Moreover, in his own meta-analysis, Grundy (2020) argued “that there are several reasons, often overlooked, that lead to failed replications, and that when group differences do appear on EF tasks, despite these issues, performance favors bilinguals far more often than monolinguals” (p. 177), supporting his claim with Bayesian analysis of 167 independent studies that resulted in a Bayes Factor of BF10 = 2.91 × 108, classified as “decisive” evidence.
Taken altogether, the strongest evidence for a bilingual advantage seems to come from studies targeting very young children and aging adults (Bialystok et al., 2005; Woumans et al., 2015), suggesting that bilingualism mainly impacts the sensitive periods of cognitive development and cognitive decline. Development of the cognitive control (CC) system is one the most essential processes in childhood (Diamond, 2002), evolving rapidly, especially between the ages of three to six (Best and Miller, 2010). Beneficial effects of bilingualism are reported in children from birth up to the age of six (Martin-Rhee and Bialystok, 2008; Kovács and Mehler, 2009; Morales et al., 2013; Crivello et al., 2016), but it appears that null effects arise more frequently in children over the age of six (Martin-Rhee and Bialystok, 2008; Abdelgafar and Moawad, 2015). As such, it may be that the acquisition of a second language (L2) in addition to acquisition of the first (L1) accelerates cognitive development during the critical period, but that afterwards the monolinguals catch up again with their bilingual peers. Likewise, studies into cognitive decline and its relation to bilingualism tend to show a temporary bilingual advantage (e.g., Abutalebi et al., 2015). As such, the onset of dementia, for instance, is found to be delayed by ~4 to 5.5 years in bilinguals (Craik et al., 2010; Alladi et al., 2013; Woumans et al., 2015). This temporary nature of differences is why, in the present review focusing on children, we have differentiated between studies targeting children younger than 6 years old (critical) and studies targeting children between the ages of 6 and 12 (post-critical). In doing so, we aimed to give an overview of previous research reporting on bilingual advantage effects in young children, shedding light on the validity and robustness of possible cognitive effects of L2 acquisition. Specifically, this review considers research on inhibition and switching, two constructs which are frequently related to bilingualism as the concept of speaking two languages in itself requires inhibition of the non-spoken language and switching between languages. Particular attention is paid to the experimental tasks that have been applied and the persistence of possible effects throughout critical and post-critical periods for cognitive development.
Method
All articles considered in our analysis were retrieved from Web of Science through two systematic searches. The searches “((ALL = (bilingual advantage)) AND ALL = (children)) AND ALL = (executive control)” and “((ALL = (immersion)) AND ALL = (cognitive control)) AND ALL = (children)” resulted in 281 and 49 hits, respectively. All hits were automatically filtered on “Document type: Article” and “Web of Science categories: Psychology Experimental or Linguistics,” reducing the total number of hits to 189. During manual filtering, articles targeting age groups older than 12 were excluded and the sample was limited to articles looking into the constructs of switching and inhibition. Articles with a focus that did not entail a direct comparison between bilinguals or second language learners, and monolinguals were also removed, resulting in a final sample of 58 references.
From each article, age range, number of participants, targeted measures and tasks applied were extracted. It was indicated for each task if a significant bilingual advantage was detected. The results of this process are summarized in Table 1. In the naming of the measures and tasks, the terminology applied by the original author was kept. Articles were visually split up in the table according to the age range they were reporting on, in an attempt to visualize the persistence of any possible effects through the critical and post-critical period for cognitive development. The threshold for the transition from critical to post-critical was set at the age of 6, as the cognitive control system is especially evolving rapidly between the ages of three and six (Best and Miller, 2010).
Results
Both research into measures associated with inhibition and research into measures associated with switching was considered in the present review article. As could be expected, the overview includes mixed results for virtually every measure and every task at hand. However, the main aim of this analysis is to detect trends throughout these mixed results in both grouping them and discussing them individually. One way of grouping them is to consider them according to age range, as was done for the visual representation in Table 1.
Moreover, grouping was also done in interpreting the diverse terminology applied in pinpointing measures. We argue a great deal of different tasks and labels really come down to measuring two major constructs in executive functioning: inhibition and switching. Inhibition includes, among others, measures that have previously been called response inhibition, interference suppression, inhibitory control, and conflict resolution. Switching includes measures such as shifting, task switching and cognitive flexibility. Within the larger constructs of inhibition and switching, different tasks are individually discussed.
Inhibition
Tasks that have frequently been applied for measuring this cognitive function in our sample were Stroop-like tasks (20 times), Simon-like tasks (18 times), and Flanker-like tasks (16 times). There appear to be some differences in tasks applied for children under the age of 6 and tasks applied for children between the ages of 6 and 12. Flanker-like tasks were used more often among older children. Some tasks, such as gift delay tasks and tapping tasks, were only used with younger children, while others such as stop-signal tasks and bivalent shape tasks were only used with older children. Interestingly, several authors considered the Dimensional Change Card Sort Task (DCCS) (Zelazo, 2006) and variations thereof as a measure of inhibition (Poulin-Dubois et al., 2011; Crivello et al., 2016; Diaz and Farrar, 2018; Escobar et al., 2018; Nayak and Tarullo, 2020). In the DCCS Task, participants are asked to sort cards, switching between different rules to do so. Hence, we argue this task is rather a measure of switching and do not include it in the present section on inhibition, as the DCCS protocol (Zelazo, 2006) states that the inclusion of pre- and post-switch phases requires the formulation and use of higher-order rules for selecting which pair of rules to use on any particular trial. In other words, participants must constantly switch between rules in response to the instruction given.
In the complete set of literature, a bilingual advantage for inhibition was reported 42 out of 91 times (46%). In the subset of studies on children up to 6, a bilingual advantage was detected 25 out of 45 times (56%). In the subset with older children, the advantage was reported 17 out of 46 times (37%). More details on the tasks applied and the frequencies with which they led to bilingual advantages are to be found in Table 2.
Delay-type tasks (where response is delayed, such as gift delay) were administered 6 times, all in studies targeting children younger than 6. An effect on this task was detected only once (1/6).
Interestingly, in 4 studies questionnaire-like methods were applied to tap into inhibition, with either teachers or parents responding to the surveys (Esposito, 2020; Verhagen et al., 2020; Beaudin and Poulin-Dubois, 2022; Castillo et al., 2022) instead of the more common experimental tasks. In those questionnaires, parents or teachers are asked about a child's behavior. Questions may for example include “How often in the past two weeks did your child follow a simple instruction for a task that they were interested in (e.g., getting a nearby toy), without getting distracted” (Hendry and Holmboe, 2021). Although questionnaires might be more susceptible to biases, they might also provide a more comprehensive overview of the participants' behavior and functioning. In 3 out of 4 studies, the questionnaires pointed toward a bilingual advantage in executive functioning. It remains unclear why an advantage was not detected in Verhagen et al. (2020) while it was detected in Beaudin and Poulin-Dubois (2022), as both studies targeted similar age groups and had a comparable number of participants (95 and 81, respectively). However, Verhagen et al. (2020) applied the Early Childhood Behavior Questionnaire (ECBQ), originally designed by Putnam et al. (2006) to assess attentional focusing, inhibitory control, and attentional shifting. This use of the ECBQ was criticized by Hendry and Holmboe (2021), as they argue the questionnaire was originally developed to assess a range of temperament traits and these are not synonymous to executive functioning abilities, although some of them are closely related. In line with this criticism, Hendry and Holmboe (2021) developed the Early Executive Functions Questionnaire (EEFQ), notably the questionnaire that was used in the study by Beaudin and Poulin-Dubois (2022). Hence, it should be noted that the bilingual advantage effect was detected when using a questionnaire that was designed to target EF and was not detected in the ECBQ.
Flanker-like tasks
Flanker-like tasks include the standard Flanker Task (Eriksen and Eriksen, 1974) as well as the Attention Network Task (ANT) (Fan et al., 2002), which is a combination of the cued reaction time (Posner, 1980) and the Flanker Task. Flanker tasks usually involve five arrows pointing to different directions, the participant having to indicate the direction the middle arrow points toward. This type of task led to a detected bilingual advantage in half of the occasions (8/16). Interestingly, the advantage was detected 3 out of 4 times with participants younger than 6 years old, whereas it was only detected 5 out of 12 times with older children, indicating more variation in studies targeting older children.
Stroop-like tasks
Stroop-like tasks were performed 20 times throughout our sample and include tests denominated as Stroop Task (Stroop, 1935) and variations thereof, and the child version Day/Night Task (Gerstadt et al., 1994). The Stroop Task involves both congruent and incongruent trials in which participants have to say the color of a word presented (e.g., the word “blue” is displayed in green). In younger children, this type of task led to a significant effect 8 times out of 11. In older children, a bilingual advantage was only identified in 2 out of 9 instances.
Simon-like tasks
The Simon Task (Simon and Rudell, 1967) was applied 18 times in our sample, of which it led to a significant effect in 9 instances (50%). In the Simon Task, stimuli are presented both left and right on the screen, and participants are asked to respond according to the stimuli's color (e.g., left for red, right for blue). The task involves both congruent and incongruent trials. In younger children a bilingual advantage was detected 3 out of 8 times, whereas in older children it was detected 6 out of 10 times.
Go/No-go tasks
The Go/No-go Task (Donders, 1969) and its variation Bear/Dragon Task (Jones et al., 2003) were administered 11 times. In the Go/No-go Task, participants' ability to withhold a response is measured. According to the instruction given, they should press or not press a button. In the younger age group, significant effects were found in 3 out of 5 instances, which is considerably more than in the older age group (1/5).
Switching
Measures such as shifting, task switching, and cognitive flexibility were considered to relate to the construct of switching. Moreover, we identify DCCS-like tasks as measuring switching, whereas other authors have applied these for measuring inhibition (cf. supra). In fact, the DCCS Task (Zelazo, 2006) and its variations, including the Color/Shape Task (Miyake et al., 2004), are by far the most applied tasks for measuring switching in our set of studies (used 24/40 times). Other tasks include the Opposite Worlds Task (Manly et al., 2001), the Multilocation Task (Zelazo et al., 1998), and the Global/Local Task (Navon, 1977).
Across all studies, a bilingual advantage for switching was reported 18 times (18/40; 45%). In the subset of studies on children onto 6, a bilingual advantage was detected 10 out of 16 times (63%), whereas in the subset with older children the advantage was reported 8 out of 24 times (33%). More details on the tasks applied and their respective bilingual advantage detection rates are to be found in Table 2.
The Opposite Worlds Task (Manly et al., 2001) was administered twice and resulted in a bilingual advantage effect on both occasions, both in 3- to 4-year-olds and in 8- to 10-year-olds. This task requires to switch between naming systems; naming animals first by their true names (e.g., “cow”) and later by their silly name (e.g., “pig” for cow). The Multilocation Task (Zelazo et al., 1998) was likewise performed twice, on both occasions in children younger than 6. The task involves objects being hidden in different locations, children having to respond to instructions to try and find them. No bilingual advantage was detected.
DCCS-like tasks
DCCS-like tasks include the standard DCCS Task (Zelazo, 2006), the Color/Shape Task (Miyake et al., 2004), the Blue Horse/Red Cow Task (Barac and Bialystok, 2012) and the Reverse Categorization Task (Carlson et al., 2004). This type of task led to a bilingual advantage being identified in half of the occasions (12/24). The advantage was detected relatively more often in younger children (7/9) than in older children (5/15).
Discussion
Quite a lot of research has been conducted on the effect of bilingualism on inhibition and switching in children. Although findings of bilingual advantages on such cognitive control measures have dominated the research field for quite some time now, a debate is still raging on when, how, why, and even if these advantages appear. The present review set out to distinguish between bilinguals pre and post the critical age of development in order to determine whether age is a possible modulator of the effect. Our review included 58 articles on the topic and covered a total of 125 tasks.
It appears that in general, more research has been conducted on the construct of inhibition (90 tasks) than on the construct of switching (40). One possible explanation for this apparent focus of research is that all bilinguals constantly need the ability to inhibit input from the non-used language, while not all bilinguals have to switch very often (e.g., when the use of either language is restricted to different contexts). It should be noted, however, that inhibition was also defined as a broader construct than switching, including both response inhibition and interference suppression. Overall, a great deal of variation was present in the outcomes of these tasks, which strongly relates to the criticism uttered on frequent null results and the failure to reproduce bilingual effects (Paap et al., 2015). Moreover, variation in the current review was also omnipresent in terminology applied by different authors. Measures of inhibition were, among others, called “response inhibition,” “inhibitory control,” “conflict inhibition,” and “conflict resolution,” without defining resemblances and differences between any of those concepts. There appeared to be no consensus on which measures EF consists of exactly, nor what tasks can be used for measuring them, as was previously also indicated by among others Morra et al. (2018). Simon tasks (Simon and Rudell, 1967), for example, were applied to evaluate interference suppression (Baralt and Mahoney, 2020) as well as conflict resolution (Poarch and van Hell, 2012). The DCCS task was used to tap into switching (Simonis et al., 2020), inhibitory control (Escobar et al., 2018), and attentional control (Kalashnikova and Mattock, 2014). In considering the somewhat broader concepts of inhibition and switching, we tried to accommodate for these issues. As for different tasks applied to different age ranges, measures of Stroop, Simon, and Flanker were all administered among children of both critical and post-critical age, whereas parent/teacher questionnaires, gift delay tasks, and tapping tasks were employed solely among children under the age of 6 and stop-signal tasks only among older children.
Looking at tasks of inhibition, it was noted that Stroop- and Flanker-like tasks led to a bilingual advantage relatively more often in younger children than they did in children between the ages of 6 and 12. Interestingly, the opposite was true for Simon-like tasks, where older children showed a bilingual advantage relatively more often. This seeming lack of convergent validity between different tasks is in line with the mixed findings in research on the subject (e.g., Ross and Melinger, 2017; Poarch and van Hell, 2019). However, it should be noted that in one study targeting the critical group (Martin-Rhee and Bialystok, 2008), a delay was inserted in two versions of the Simon Task (Simon and Rudell, 1967) leading to null results on both occasions. If we were to exclude those measures, the balance would already be slightly modified and lead to a bilingual advantage being detected 3 out of 6 times in the critical age group (i.e., a 50% detection rate as opposed to the 60% detection rate in the post-critical group). Lee et al. (2013) already reported that between the ages of 6 to 15, inhibition costs reduced rapidly on the Flanker Task whereas they remained present and relatively stable on the Simon Task. This might explain why there is a higher bilingual advantage rate for post-critical age groups on the Simon Task than on the Flanker Task; there is simply more room for a bilingual advantage to exist. Overall, null results on tasks of inhibition were more frequently reported than results of a bilingual advantage, in addition to the likely presence of a publication bias (cf. De Bruin et al., 2015), which already favors alternative over null results. Our findings therefore strengthen the pleas for caution and skepticism made by Paap et al. (2015).
In tasks measuring switching, we were able to document a nearly exclusive focus on DCCS-like tasks. Twenty-four out of 40 tasks were of this type, other tasks being employed three times or less. The results for most tasks were mixed. A bilingual advantage for switching was found relatively more often for the younger age group (63%) than for the older age group (33%). The difference was entirely driven by the results for DCCS-like tasks, on which children aged younger than 6 showed a bilingual advantage on 78% of the tasks, whereas children between the ages of 6 and 12 demonstrated one in 33% of the cases. It has been shown in the general literature on DCCS that, while at the age of 3 most children exhibit a pattern of inflexibility, by the age of 5 most children switch when they are instructed to do so (Zelazo, 2006). Our findings suggest that this switching ability arises earlier in bilingual children, resulting in a bilingual advantage during the critical period which tends to disappear in post-critical age groups. This could be influenced by the constant switches bilinguals make between their languages, as previous research has also indicated language switching to be a key determinant for bilingual advantages in CC processes (Verreyt et al., 2016).
The Opposite Worlds task (Manly et al., 2001) was applied only twice but showed a bilingual advantage on both occasions. Although the task is evidently connected to the DCCS Task, they are different in that the DCCS Task requires participants to respond to two visible cues whereas the Opposite Worlds task requires ignoring the visible cue in favor of the instruction. Furthermore, while most tasks were applied throughout childhood, the Multilocation Task (Zelazo et al., 1998) and questionnaires were only used among younger children, and the Global/Local Task (Navon, 1977) was only used with older children. For the latter, this can easily be explained as basic reading ability is required to complete the task. However, there is no clear indication as to why the Multilocation Task has never been applied with older children. Teacher/parent questionnaires seem to be used when researchers anticipate that improved CC cannot be observed in behavior yet, notwithstanding the experimental results that were gathered by Kovács and Mehler (2009) with participants as young as 7 months old. Nonetheless, we feel like the more comprehensive view these questionnaires offer might also prove useful in older age groups, that is, taking into account the possible susceptibility to parent/teacher bias.
In all, both on tasks measuring inhibition and switching, bilingual advantages were detected more frequently in the critical age group (inhibition: 56 vs. 37%; switching: 63 vs. 33%). Especially in tasks that were frequently applied, such as Stroop-like tasks, Flanker-like tasks, Go/No-go tasks, and DCCS-like tasks. There was a substantial difference in bilingual advantage detection rates, favoring the critical age group over the post-critical one. Furthermore, we found that across age groups the insertion of delay in a given task influenced outcomes greatly. Whenever response was delayed, bilingual advantages rarely emerged in the studies under scrutiny. In gift delay tasks measuring inhibition, five studies showed null results whereas only one study established a significant effect. The Multilocation Task (Zelazo et al., 1998) likewise led to null results on both occasions where it was used. Interestingly, this task required a 10-s delay before participants could answer the question at hand. These descriptive results deserve more attention in future research. Still, the most prominent finding of this review is that if young bilinguals show an advantage in EF over monolinguals, it tends to partially disappear as they grow older. Crucially, there is wide agreement in the monolingual literature that the age span from three to six is critical for CC development (Best and Miller, 2010; Chevalier et al., 2012; Lucenet and Blaye, 2014). Hence, there seems to be an overlap in the timeframe in which the bilingual advantage is mostly observed and the sensitive period for CC development. Our results are in line with what we hypothesized, namely that bilingualism might accelerate CC development, but that this is only a temporary effect and monolinguals manage to catch up at a later stage. As we already stated, similar findings are reported in literature on the relation between bilingualism and cognitive decline, providing more support for the overlap between periods of crucial CC evolution and periods in which bilingual advantages can be detected.
As a parting statement, it should be noted that the format of a systematic review restricts us to presenting descriptives, whereas an added meta-analysis could lead to more conclusive insights on the existence of cognitive advantages in EF for bilingual children. Moreover, several studies in the present review include not only native speakers, but also children who were exposed to another language slightly later on, both through high and low exposure. An analysis of the difference between these groups was beyond the scope of this review but might have influenced outcomes. However, the current review can prove extremely useful within the research field as it has shed light on terminological issues and frequently applied tasks in addition to providing a concise overview of research on the bilingual advantage in switching and inhibition in children. Moreover, it has differentiated between critical and post-critical age groups and, in doing so, was able to draw links between timeframes in which a bilingual advantage emerges and periods that are crucial for EF development in children.
Data availability statement
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.
Author contributions
NP and EW conceptualized the study. NP conducted the search on Web of Science, drew up the table, and drafted the first manuscript. Both WD and EW have presented valuable feedback counting on their profound expertise in the subject at hand. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Funding
The work of NP is supported by FWO, Research Foundation—Flanders; grant number G004922N.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Abdelgafar, G. M., and Moawad, R. A. (2015). Executive function differences between bilingual Arabic-English and monolingual Arabic children [Article]. J. Psycholinguistic Res. 44, 651–667. doi: 10.1007/s10936-014-9309-3
Abutalebi, J., Guidi, L., Borsa, V., Canini, M., Della Rosa, P. A., Parris, B. A., et al. (2015). Bilingualism provides a neural reserve for aging populations. Neuropsychologia 69, 201–210. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.01.040
Alladi, S., Bak, T. H., Duggirala, V., Surampudi, B., Shailaja, M., Shukla, A. K., et al. (2013). Bilingualism delays age at onset of dementia, independent of education and immigration status. Neurology 81, 1938–1944. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000436620.33155.a4
Antoniou, K., Grohmann, K. K., Kambanaros, M., and Katsos, N. (2016). The effect of childhood bilectalism and multilingualism on executive control [Article]. Cognition 149, 18–30. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.12.002
Arizmendi, G. D., Alt, M., Gray, S., Hogan, T. P., Green, S., and Cowan, N. (2018). Do bilingual children have an executive function advantage? results from inhibition, shifting, and updating tasks [Article]. Lang. Speech Hearing Serv. Schools 49, 356–378. doi: 10.1044/2018_LSHSS-17-0107
Barac, R., and Bialystok, E. (2012). Bilingual effects on cognitive and linguistic development: role of language, cultural background, and education. Child Develop. 83, 413–422. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01707.x
Baralt, M., and Mahoney, A. D. (2020). Bilingualism and the executive function advantage in preterm-born children [Article]. Cogn. Develop. 11, 100931. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2020.100931
Beaudin, K., and Poulin-Dubois, D. (2022). Testing the bilingual cognitive advantage in toddlers using the early executive functions questionnaire [Article]. Languages 7, 122. doi: 10.3390/languages7020122
Ben-Zeev, S. (1977). The influence of bilingualism on cognitive strategy and cognitive development. Child Develop. 17, 1009–1018. doi: 10.2307/1128353
Best, J. R., and Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Develop. 81, 1641–1660. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x
Bialystok, E., and Barac, R. (2012). Emerging bilingualism: Dissociating advantages for metalinguistic awareness and executive control [Article]. Cognition 122, 67–73. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.08.003
Bialystok, E., Barac, R., Blaye, A., and Poulin-Dubois, D. (2010). Word mapping and executive functioning in young monolingual and bilingual children [Article]. J. Cogn. Develop. 11, 485–508. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2010.516420
Bialystok, E., Martin, M. M., and Viswanathan, M. (2005). Bilingualism across the lifespan: the rise and fall of inhibitory control. Int. J. Bilingualism 9, 103–119. doi: 10.1177/13670069050090010701
Bialystok, E., and Viswanathan, M. (2009). Components of executive control with advantages for bilingual children in two cultures [Article]. Cognition 112, 494–500. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.014
Brysbaert, M., and Duyck, W. (2010). Is it time to leave behind the Revised Hierarchical Model of bilingual language processing after fifteen years of service? Bilingualism: Lang. Cogn. 13, 359–371. doi: 10.1017/S1366728909990344
Cape, R., Vega-Mendoza, M., Bak, T. H., and Sorace, A. (2021). Cognitive effects of Gaelic medium education on primary school children in Scotland [Article]. Int. J. Biling. Educ. Bilingual. 24, 1065–1084. doi: 10.1080/13670050.2018.1543648
Carlson, S. M., Mandell, D. J., and Williams, L. (2004). Executive function and theory of mind: stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3. Develop. Psychol. 40, 1105–1122. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1105
Castillo, A., Khislavsky, A., Altman, M., and Gilger, J. W. (2022). Executive function developmental trajectories kindergarten to first grade: monolingual, bilingual and English language learners [Article]. Int. J. Biling. Educ. Bilingual. 25, 1101–1119. doi: 10.1080/13670050.2020.1742649
Chevalier, N., Sheffield, T. D., Nelson, J. M., Clark, C. A. C., Wiebe, S. A., and Espy, K. A. (2012). Underpinnings of the costs of flexibility in preschool children: the roles of inhibition and working memory. Develop. Neuropsychol. 37, 99–118. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2011.632458
Cho, I., Park, J., Song, H. J., and Morton, J. B. (2021). Disentangling language status and country-of-origin explanations of the bilingual advantage in preschoolers [Article]. J. Experiment. Child Psychol. 212, 105235. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105235
Costa, A., Caramazza, A., and Sebastian-Galles, N. (2000). The cognate facilitation effect: implications for models of lexical access. J. Experiment. Psychol. Learn. Memory Cogn. 26, 1283. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.26.5.1283
Cottini, M., Pieroni, L., Spataro, P., Devescovi, A., Longobardi, E., and Rossi-Arnaud, C. (2015). Feature binding and the processing of global-local shapes in bilingual and monolingual children [Article]. Memory Cogn. 43, 441–452. doi: 10.3758/s13421-014-0467-1
Craik, F. I. M., Bialystok, E., and Freedman, M. (2010). Delaying the onset of Alzheimer disease. Bilingual. Cogn. Reser. 75, 1726–1729. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181fc2a1c
Crespo, K., Gross, M., and Kaushanskaya, M. (2019). The effects of dual language exposure on executive function in Spanish-English bilingual children with different language abilities [Article]. J. Experiment. Child Psychol. 188, 104663. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104663
Crivello, C., Kuzyk, O., Rodrigues, M., Friend, M., Zesiger, P., and Poulin-Dubois, D. (2016). The effects of bilingual growth on toddlers' executive function [Article]. J. Experiment. Child Psychol. 141, 121–132. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.08.004
Czapka, S., and Festman, J. (2021). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test reveals a monitoring advantage but not a switching advantage in multilingual children [Article]. J. Experiment. Child Psychol. 204, 105038. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2020.105038
Czapka, S., Wotschack, C., Klassert, A., and Festman, J. (2020). A path to the bilingual advantage: pairwise matching of individuals [Article]. Bilingual. Lang. Cogn. 23, 344–354. doi: 10.1017/S1366728919000166
Darcy, N. T. (1946). The effect of bilingualism upon the measurement of the intelligence of children of preschool age. J. Educ. Psychol. 37, 21. doi: 10.1037/h0056165
de Abreu, P., Cruz-Santos, A., and Puglisi, M. L. (2014). Specific language impairment in language-minority children from low-income families [Article]. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 49, 736–747. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12107
De Bruin, A., Treccani, B., and Della Sala, S. (2015). Cognitive advantage in bilingualism: an example of publication bias? Psychologic. Sci. 26, 99–107. doi: 10.1177/0956797614557866
Diamond, A. (2002). Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry. Principl. Front. Lobe Funct. 466, 503. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195134971.003.0029
Diaz, V., and Farrar, M. J. (2018). Do bilingual and monolingual preschoolers acquire false belief understanding similarly? the role of executive functioning and language [article]. First Lang. 38, 382–398. doi: 10.1177/0142723717752741
Dick, A. S., Garcia, N. L., Pruden, S. M., Thompson, W. K., Hawes, S. W., Sutherland, M. T., et al. (2019). No evidence for a bilingual executive function advantage in the ABCD study. Nat. Human Behav. 3, 692–701. doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0609-3
Donders, F. C. (1969). On the speed of mental processes. Acta psychologica 30, 412–431. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(69)90065-1
Dunabeitia, J. A., Hernandez, J. A., Anton, E., Macizo, P., Estevez, A., Fuentes, L. J., et al. (2014). The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited. myth or reality? [article]. Experiment. Psychol. 61, 234–251. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000243
Duyck, W. (2005). Translation and associative priming with cross-lingual pseudohomophones: evidence for nonselective phonological activation in bilinguals. J. Experiment. Psychol. Learn. Memory Cogn. 31, 1340. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.31.6.1340
Ebert, K. D., Rak, D., Slawny, C. M., and Fogg, L. (2019). Attention in bilingual children with developmental language disorder [article]. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 62, 979–992. doi: 10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-18-0221
Eriksen, B. A., and Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Percept. Psychophy. 16, 143–149. doi: 10.3758/BF03203267
Escobar, G. P., Kalashnikova, M., and Escudero, P. (2018). Vocabulary matters! the relationship between verbal fluency and measures of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children [article]. J. Experiment. Child Psychol. 170, 177–189. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.01.012
Esposito, A. G. (2020). Executive functions in two-way dual-language education: a mechanism for academic performance [Article]. Bilingual Res. J. 43, 417–432. doi: 10.1080/15235882.2021.1874570
Esposito, A. G., Baker-Ward, L., and Mueller, S. T. (2013). Interference suppression vs. response inhibition: An explanation for the absence of a bilingual advantage in preschoolers' Stroop task performance [Article]. Cogn. Develop. 28, 354–363. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2013.09.002
Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., and Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 340–347. doi: 10.1162/089892902317361886
Filippi, R., Ceccolini, A., and Bright, P. (2022). Trajectories of verbal fluency and executive functions in multilingual and monolingual children and adults: a cross-sectional study [Article]. Q. J. Experiment. Psychol. 75, 130–147. doi: 10.1177/17470218211026792
Foy, J. G., and Mann, V. A. (2014). Bilingual children show advantages in nonverbal auditory executive function task [Article]. Int. J. Bilingualism 18, 717–729. doi: 10.1177/1367006912472263
Gerstadt, C. L., Hong, Y. J., and Diamond, A. (1994). The relationship between cognition and action: performance of children 312–7 years old on a stroop- like day-night test. Cognition 53, 129–153. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90068-X
Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Lang. Cogn. 1, 67–81. doi: 10.1017/S1366728998000133
Grote, K. S., Scott, R. M., and Gilger, J. (2021). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning: Evidence from a low-income sample [Article]. First Language 41, 677–700. doi: 10.1177/01427237211024220
Grundy, J. G. (2020). The effects of bilingualism on executive functions: an updated quantitative analysis. J. Cult. Cogn. Sci. 4, 177–199. doi: 10.1007/s41809-020-00062-5
Gunnerud, H. L., Ten Braak, D., Reikerås, E. K. L., Donolato, E., and Melby-Lervåg, M. (2020). Is bilingualism related to a cognitive advantage in children? a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychologic. Bull. 146, 1059. doi: 10.1037/bul0000301
Hartanto, A., Toh, W. X., and Yang, H. (2019). Bilingualism narrows socioeconomic disparities in executive functions and self-regulatory behaviors during early childhood: evidence from the early childhood longitudinal study. Child Develop. 90, 1215–1235. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13032
Hendry, A., and Holmboe, K. (2021). Development and validation of the early executive functions questionnaire: a carer-administered measure of executive functions suitable for 9-to 30-month-olds. Infancy 26, 932–961. doi: 10.1111/infa.12431
Hermans, D., Bongaerts, T., De Bot, K., and Schreuder, R. (1998). Producing words in a foreign language: can speakers prevent interference from their first language? Bilingual. Lang. Cogn. 1, 213–229. doi: 10.1017/S1366728998000364
Jaekel, N., Jaekel, J., Willard, J., and Leyendecker, B. (2019). No evidence for effects of Turkish immigrant children‘s bilingualism on executive functions. PloS one 14, e0209981. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0209981
Johann, V. E., Enke, S., Gunzenhauser, C., Konen, T., Saalbach, H., and Karbach, J. (2022). Executive functions in mono- and bilingual children: factor structure and relations with fluid intelligence [Article]. J. Experiment. Child Psychol. 224, 105515. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105515
Jones, L. B., Rothbart, M. K., and Posner, M. I. (2003). Development of executive attention in preschool children. Develop. Sci. 6, 498–504. doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00307
Kalashnikova, M., and Mattock, K. (2014). Maturation of executive functioning skills in early sequential bilingualism [Article]. Int. J. Bilingual Educ. Bilingual. 17, 111–123. doi: 10.1080/13670050.2012.746284
Kang, C., and Lust, B. (2019). Code-switching does not predict Executive Function performance in proficient bilingual children: Bilingualism does. Bilingual. Lang. Cogn. 22, 366–382. doi: 10.1017/S1366728918000299
Kapa, L. L., and Colombo, J. (2013). Attentional control in early and later bilingual children [Article]. Cogn. Develop. 28, 233–246. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2013.01.011
Karimi, M. N., and Rad, Z. H. (2021). Bilingual advantage hypothesis: testing the fit among L1-dominant child and adolescent bilinguals [Article]. J. Neurolinguist. 60, 101017. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2021.101017
Kaushanskaya, M., Gross, M., and Buac, M. (2014). Effects of classroom bilingualism on task-shifting, verbal memory, and word learning in children [Article]. Develop. Sci. 17, 564–583. doi: 10.1111/desc.12142
Kovács, Á. M., and Mehler, J. (2009). Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proceed. Nat. Acad. Sci. 106, 6556–6560. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811323106
Laine, M., and Lehtonen, M. (2018). Cognitive consequences of bilingualism: where to go from here? Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 33, 1205–1212. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2018.1462498
Lee, K., Bull, R., and Ho, R. M. (2013). Developmental changes in executive functioning. Child Develop. 84, 1933–1953. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12096
Lehtonen, M., Soveri, A., Laine, A., Jarvenpaa, J., de Bruin, A., and Antfolk, J. (2018). Is bilingualism associated with enhanced executive functioning in adults? a meta-analytic review. Psychologic. Bull. 144, 394–425. doi: 10.1037/bul0000142
Lucenet, J., and Blaye, A. (2014). Age-related changes in the temporal dynamics of executive control: a study in 5- and 6-year-old children [Original Research]. Front. Psychol. 5, 831. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00831
Manly, T., Anderson, V., Nimmo-Smith, I., Turner, A., Watson, P., and Robertson, I. H. (2001). The differential assessment of children's attention: the test of everyday attention for children (TEA-Ch), normative sample and ADHD performance. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatr. Allied Discipl. 42, 1065–1081. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00806
Martin-Rhee, M. M., and Bialystok, E. (2008). The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children [Article]. Bilingual. Lang. Cogn. 11, 81–93. doi: 10.1017/S1366728907003227
Miyake, A., Emerson, M. J., Padilla, F., and Ahn, J.-C. (2004). Inner speech as a retrieval aid for task goals: the effects of cue type and articulatory suppression in the random task cuing paradigm. Acta Psychologica 115, 123–142. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2003.12.004
Morales, J., Calvo, A., and Bialystok, E. (2013). Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children [Article]. J. Experiment. Child Psychol. 114, 187–202. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.002
Morra, S., Panesi, S., Traverso, L., and Usai, M. C. (2018). Which tasks measure what? Reflections on executive function development and a commentary on Podjarny, Kamawar, and Andrews (2017). J. Experiment. Child Psychol. 167, 246–258. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.11.004
Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cogn. Psychol. 9, 353–383. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(77)90012-3
Nayak, S., and Tarullo, A. R. (2020). Error-related negativity (ERN) and 'hot' executive function in bilingual and monolingual preschoolers [Article]. Bilingual. Lang. Cogn. 23, 897–908. doi: 10.1017/S1366728919000725
Neveu, A., Crespo, K., Weismer, S. E., and Kaushanskaya, M. (2021). Does long-term dual-language immersion affect children's executive functioning? [Article]. J. Experiment. Child Psychol. 208, 105127. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105127
Nguyen, T. K., and Astington, J. W. (2014). Reassessing the bilingual advantage in theory of mind and its cognitive underpinnings [Article]. Bilingual. Lang. Cogn. 17, 396–409. doi: 10.1017/S1366728913000394
Nichols, E. S., Wild, C. J., Stojanoski, B., Battista, M. E., and Owen, A. M. (2020). Bilingualism affords no general cognitive advantages: a population study of executive function in 11,000 people. Psychologic. Sci. 31, 548–567. doi: 10.1177/0956797620903113
Nicolay, A. C., and Poncelet, M. (2013). Cognitive advantage in children enrolled in a second-language immersion elementary school program for three years [Article]. Bilingual. Lang. Cogn. 16, 597–607. doi: 10.1017/S1366728912000375
Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., and Sawi, O. (2015). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex 69, 265–278. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.04.014
Papastergiou, A., Pappas, V., and Sanoudaki, E. (2022a). The executive function of bilingual and monolingual children: a technical efficiency approach [Article]. Behav. Res. Methods 54, 1319–1345. doi: 10.3758/s13428-021-01658-7
Papastergiou, A., Sanoudaki, E., Tamburelli, M., and Chondrogianni, V. (2022b). A study on the executive functioning skills of Greek-English bilingual children—a nearest neighbour approach [Article; Early Access]. Bilingual. Lang. Cogn. 17, 335. doi: 10.1017/S1366728922000335
Park, J., Miller, C. A., Sanjeevan, T., van Hell, J. G., Weiss, D. J., and Mainela-Arnold, E. (2019). Bilingualism and attention in typically developing children and children with developmental language disorder [Article]. J. Speech Lang. Hearing Res. 62, 4105–4118. doi: 10.1044/2019_JSLHR-L-18-0341
Park, S., Dotan, P. L., and Esposito, A. G. (2022). Do executive functions gained through two-way dual-Language education translate into math achievement? [Article; Early Access]. Int. J. Biling. Educ. Bilingual. 15, 6973. doi: 10.1080/13670050.2022.2116973
Peal, E., and Lambert, W. E. (1962). The relation of bilingualism to intelligence. Psychologic. Monograph. Gen. Appl. 76, 1. doi: 10.1037/h0093840
Poarch, G. J., and van Hell, J. G. (2012). Executive functions and inhibitory control in multilingual children: evidence from second-language learners, bilinguals, and trilinguals [Article]. J. Experiment. Child Psychol. 113, 535–551. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.06.013
Poarch, G. J., and van Hell, J. G. (2019). “Does performance on executive function tasks correlate?,” in Bilingualism, Executive Function, and Beyond: Questions and Insights, eds I. A. Sekerina, L. Spradlin, and V. Valian (London: John Benjamins Publishing Company). pp. 223–234. Available online at: http://ebookcentral.proquest.com/lib/unigent-ebooks/detail.action?docID=5786688 (accessed May 9, 2023).
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Q. J. Experiment. Psychol. 32, 3–25. doi: 10.1080/00335558008248231
Poulin-Dubois, D., Blaye, A., Coutya, J., and Bialystok, E. (2011). The effects of bilingualism on toddlers' executive functioning [Article]. J. Experiment. Child Psychol. 108, 567–579. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.10.009
Poulin-Dubois, D., Neumann, C., Masoud, S., and Gazith, A. (2022). Effect of bilingualism on infants' cognitive flexibility [Article]. Bilingual. Lang. Cogn. 25, 484–497. doi: 10.1017/S1366728921000912
Puric, D., Vuksanovic, J., and Chondrogianni, V. (2017). Cognitive advantages of immersion education after 1 year: effects of amount of exposure [Article]. J. Experiment. Child Psychol. 159, 296–309. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.02.011
Putnam, S. P., Gartstein, M. A., and Rothbart, M. K. (2006). Measurement of fine-grained aspects of toddler temperament: the early childhood behavior questionnaire. Infant Behav. Develop. 29, 386–401. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.01.004
Ross, J., and Melinger, A. (2017). Bilingual advantage, bidialectal advantage or neither? comparing performance across three tests of executive function in middle childhood [Article]. Develop. Sci. 20, e12405. doi: 10.1111/desc.12405
Santillan, J., and Khurana, A. (2018). Developmental associations between bilingual experience and inhibitory control trajectories in Head Start children [Article]. Develop. Sci. 21, e12624. doi: 10.1111/desc.12624
Simon, J. R., and Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory SR compatibility: the effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. J. Appl. Psychol. 51, 300. doi: 10.1037/h0020586
Simonis, M., Van der Linden, L., Galand, B., Hiligsmann, P., and Szmalec, A. (2020). Executive control performance and foreign-language proficiency associated with immersion education in French-speaking Belgium [Article]. Bilingual. Lang. Cogn. 23, 355–370. doi: 10.1017/S136672891900021X
Smith, M. E. (1949). Measurement of vocabularies of young bilingual children in both of the languages used. Pedagogic. Semin. J. Genetic Psychol. 74, 305–310. doi: 10.1080/08856559.1949.10533499
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Experiment. Psychol. 18, 643. doi: 10.1037/h0054651
Tran, C. D., Arredondo, M. M., and Yoshida, H. (2019). Early executive function: The influence of culture and bilingualism [Article]. Bilingual. Lang. Cogn. 22, 714–732. doi: 10.1017/S1366728918000160
Verhagen, J., de Bree, E., and Unsworth, S. (2020). Effects of bilingual language use and language proficiency on 24-month-olds' cognitive control [Article]. J. Cogn. Develop. 21, 46–71. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2019.1673752
Verhagen, J., Mulder, H., and Leseman, P. P. M. (2017). Effects of home language environment on inhibitory control in bilingual three-year-old children [Article]. Bilingual. Lang. Cogn. 20, 114–127. doi: 10.1017/S1366728915000590
Verreyt, N., Woumans, E., Vandelanotte, D., Szmalec, A., and Duyck, W. (2016). The influence of language-switching experience on the bilingual executive control advantage. Bilingual. Lang. Cogn. 19, 181–190. doi: 10.1017/S1366728914000352
White, L. J., and Greenfield, D. B. (2017). Executive functioning in Spanish- and English-speaking head start preschoolers [Article]. Develop. Sci. 20, e12502. doi: 10.1111/desc.12502
Wimmer, M. C., and Marx, C. (2014). Inhibitory processes in visual perception: a bilingual advantage [Article]. J. Experiment. Child Psychol. 126, 412–419. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.03.004
Woumans, E., Santens, P., Sieben, A., Versijpt, J., Stevens, M., and Duyck, W. (2015). Bilingualism delays clinical manifestation of Alzheimer's disease. Bilingual. Lang. Cogn. 18, 568–574. doi: 10.1017/S136672891400087X
Woumans, E., Surmont, J., Struys, E., and Duyck, W. (2016). The longitudinal effect of bilingual immersion schooling on cognitive control and intelligence [Article]. Lang. Learn. 66, 76–91. doi: 10.1111/lang.12171
Yang, S., and Yang, H. (2016). Bilingual effects on deployment of the attention system in linguistically and culturally homogeneous children and adults [Article]. J. Experiment. Child Psychol. 146, 121–136. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.01.011
Yang, S. J., Yang, H. J., and Lust, B. (2011). Early childhood bilingualism leads to advances in executive attention: Dissociating culture and language [Article]. Bilingual. Lang. Cogn. 14, 412–422. doi: 10.1017/S1366728910000611
Zelazo, P. D. (2006). The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children. Nat. Protocol. 1, 297–301. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.46
Zelazo, P. D., Reznick, J. S., and Spinazzola, J. (1998). Representational flexibility and response control in a multistep multilocation search task. Develop. Psychol. 34, 203. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.34.2.203
Keywords: bilingual advantage, executive functioning, cognitive development, bilingualism, response inhibition, interference suppression, switching
Citation: Planckaert N, Duyck W and Woumans E (2023) Is there a cognitive advantage in inhibition and switching for bilingual children? A systematic review. Front. Psychol. 14:1191816. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1191816
Received: 22 March 2023; Accepted: 09 May 2023;
Published: 16 June 2023.
Edited by:
Roberto Filippi, University College London, United KingdomReviewed by:
Greg Poarch, University of Groningen, NetherlandsKathryn Mason, University College London, United Kingdom
Copyright © 2023 Planckaert, Duyck and Woumans. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Niels Planckaert, niels.planckaert@ugent.be