Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 29 November 2023
Sec. Developmental Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Speech, Language, and Literacy Development in Individuals with Down Syndrome View all 14 articles

Stuttering in individuals with Down syndrome: a systematic review of earlier research

Updated
  • 1Department of Education, Lillehammer, Inland Norway University of Applied Sciences, Lillehammer, Norway
  • 2Department of Special Needs Education, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway

The main objective of this systematic review was to synthesize the evidence on the occurrence and characteristics of stuttering in individuals with Down syndrome and thus contribute knowledge about stuttering in this population. Group studies reporting outcome measures of stuttering were included. Studies with participants who were preselected based on their fluency status were excluded. We searched the Eric, PsychInfo, Medline, Scopus, and Web of Science Core Collection databases on 3rd January 2022 and conducted supplementary searches of the reference lists of previous reviews and the studies included in the current review, as well as relevant speech and language journals. The included studies were coded in terms of information concerning sample characteristics, measurement approaches, and stuttering-related outcomes. The appraisal tool for cross-sectional studies (AXIS) was used to evaluate study quality. We identified 14 eligible studies, with a total of 1,833 participants (mean = 131.29, standard deviation = 227.85, median = 45.5) between 3 and 58 years of age. The estimated occurrence of stuttering ranged from 2.38 to 56%, which is substantially higher than the estimated prevalence (1%) of stuttering in the general population. The results also showed that stuttering severity most often was judged to be mild-to-moderate and that individuals with Down syndrome displayed secondary behaviors when these were measured. However, little attention has been paid to investigating the potential adverse effects of stuttering for individuals with Down syndrome. We judged the quality of the evidence to be moderate-to-low. The negative evaluation was mostly due to sampling limitations that decreased the representability and generalizability of the results. Based on the high occurrence of stuttering and the potential negative effects of this condition, individuals with Down syndrome who show signs of stuttering should be referred to a speech and language pathologist for an evaluation of their need for stuttering treatment.

1 Introduction

Stuttering is a speech-fluency disorder that involves the frequent and significant interruption of typical fluency and flow of speech, which can have negative effects on emotional, behavioral, and cognitive functioning from an early age (see, e.g., Craig et al., 2009; Briley et al., 2019; Guttormsen et al., 2021). One group that is reported to have a high occurrence of stuttering is individuals with Down syndrome (see, e.g., Kent and Vorperian, 2013). Due to language disorder (see, e.g., Martin et al., 2009; Næss et al., 2011), speech-sound disorder, and inappropriate prosody, speaking rate, and voice (see, e.g., Kent and Vorperian, 2013; Jones et al., 2019; Wilson et al., 2019; Loveall et al., 2021), an individual with Down syndrome typically have pervasive communication difficulties. Because stuttering may further interrupt their communication (see, e.g., Evans, 1977; Maessen et al., 2022), the identification of stuttering in this population is important in understanding the magnitude of their communication difficulties and supporting their communicative success.

Traditionally, in typically developing individuals, stuttering has mainly been operationalized and assessed based on behavioral factors (see, e.g., Tichenor and Yaruss, 2019). Examples of these factors include the type and number of disfluencies produced: audible symptoms that cause interruptions of speech, including repetitions of sounds (c-c-c-cat), syllables (ba-ba-ba-balloon), and one-syllable words (go-go-go); the prolongation of sounds (mmmilk); and blockages or stoppages of sounds (≠balloon). These audible symptoms are often accompanied by secondary behaviors caused by tension or the struggle to speak (i.e., visual symptoms, such as facial grimaces, blinking, or head nodding in an attempt to avoid stuttering; see, e.g., Bloodstein et al., 2021). Although there seems to be an agreement that speech behaviors are identifiers of stuttering, there is disagreement concerning which behaviors are symptomatic of stuttering, leading to differing operationalizations across studies (see, e.g., Einarsdottir and Ingham, 2005). One disagreement concerns whether word repetition is considered a stuttering disfluency. For example, in their operationalization of stuttering, Druker et al. (2020) excluded word repetitions, Millard et al. (2018) included word repetitions, and Boey et al. (2007) included only one-syllable word repetitions. Additionally, there have been various practices concerning the threshold at which speech disfluency is considered to be stuttering and, therefore, requires treatment. In a systematic review of stuttering-treatment studies by Sjøstrand et al. (2021), the frequency criterion (cutoff score) at treatment intake varied from no cutoff (Lewis et al., 2008) to a cutoff of a minimum of 3% of syllables stuttered (Harris et al., 2002; Lattermann et al., 2008). The use of frequency cutoff scores in the assessment of stuttering has been debated because (a) variability in stuttering across time and situations may cause participants to be wrongly classified if judgments are based on the percentage of stuttered syllables in only one speech sample (Constantino et al., 2016; Tichenor and Yaruss, 2021), (b) participants whose stuttering frequency is at the margins of the criterion set can be wrongly classified as non-stuttering (Tumanova et al., 2014), and (c) the adverse effects are not determined based on the frequency of overt speech disruptions (Koedoot et al., 2011; Blumgart et al., 2012), as the potential adverse effects of stuttering may also be critical for individuals with mild stuttering (i.e., mild based on listener evaluation; Beilby, 2014).

An increased awareness of the potential adverse effects of stuttering has led to a heightened focus on affective and cognitive reactions in the operationalization and assessment of stuttering. Affective reactions refer to feelings and emotions (e.g., feeling embarrassed, ashamed, or anxious), while cognitive reactions refer to a person’s thoughts (e.g., anticipation) and identity (e.g., low self-confidence or self-esteem; Tichenor and Yaruss, 2019). Assessment procedures that are solely based on listener evaluations of observable behaviors can therefore be criticized for not considering the multidimensionality of stuttering. Based on a multidimensional understanding, the stuttering assessment will preferably also involve an evaluation made by the individual who stutters. As stuttering behavior can be highly variable across time and contexts (Tichenor and Yaruss, 2021), a combination of assessment approaches and outcome measures may provide a holistic picture of the condition. Additionally, for individuals with Down syndrome, who often have limited expressive language skills and short verbal expressions (see, e.g., Berglund et al., 2001; Chapman and Hesketh, 2001; Zampini and D’Odorico, 2011), it may be a challenge to record speech samples of at least 200 words, which is typically recommended for speech evaluation (Ward, 2018). Thus, using a combination of assessment strategies and outcome measures seems especially important for this clinical group.

Several narrative reviews of research on stuttering in individuals with Down syndrome exist (Zisk and Bailer, 1967; Stansfield, 1988; Van Borsel and Tetnowski, 2007; Kent and Vorperian, 2013; Bloodstein et al., 2021). These reviews refer to disagreements in the field concerning whether individuals with Down syndrome display genuine stuttering. These arguments are related to the simultaneous presence of other speech and communication disorders, as well as a lack of evidence for these individuals’ secondary behaviors and awareness of their disfluency (Van Borsel and Tetnowski, 2007; Bloodstein et al., 2021). Challenges in the previous research literature have been highlighted. Operationalizations of stuttering are either not described or imprecisely described in several research reports (Zisk and Bailer, 1967; Kent and Vorperian, 2013), and the assessment procedures used in the typical population are not necessarily appropriate for individuals with disorders of intellectual development (Stansfield, 1988). Furthermore, several gaps in the research literature have been noted, such as limited knowledge concerning the presence or absence of secondary behaviors, the level of awareness and potential adverse effects of stuttering in this population (Zisk and Bailer, 1967; Van Borsel and Tetnowski, 2007), and whether stuttering is more common in male participants than female participants, as is suggested to be the case in the typical population (Van Borsel and Tetnowski, 2007). Additionally, Kent and Vorperian (2013) show a wide range in terms of participants’ age within studies. As studies of stuttering in the typical population have found that both the occurrence (Reilly et al., 2009, 2013) and the overt and adverse symptoms of stuttering may change with age (Guitar, 2014), samples with wide age ranges may bias the results. These abovementioned reviews have not used a systematic approach (see, e.g., Higgins et al., 2022), do not cover the last decade of research in the field, and have a broad scope (e.g., focusing on speech impairment in general; Zisk and Bailer, 1967; Kent and Vorperian, 2013), and are, therefore, somewhat superficial in their review of the scientific stuttering research literature. The highlighted challenges and gaps in our knowledge about stuttering in individuals with Down syndrome call for an updated review of the literature, including a more in-depth discussion about how stuttering is operationalized and assessed in this clinical group. In the current review, we therefore summarize, assess, and synthesize the relevant existing research literature on stuttering in individuals with Down syndrome. Considering the potential negative effects of stuttering, a comprehensive overview of the relevant research has the potential to bolster the development of better strategies with which to identify those who stutter and may need treatment. The following research questions led the review process:

1. How is stuttering operationalized and measured in the included studies?

2. What is the estimated occurrence of stuttering in the included studies?

3. Does the estimated occurrence of stuttering in the included studies vary according to gender and age?

4. What characterizes stuttering in individuals with Down syndrome based on the findings of the included studies?

2 Methods

This article has been registered in Prospero in advance, and the registration ID is CRD42021273799.

To answer the research questions, we conducted a systematic literature review using explicit, accountable methods in line with standards prescribed by Gough and Thomas (2016) and Higgins et al. (2022). The study-selection process is presented using the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA; Page et al., 2021).

All statistical analyzes were conducted in SPSS statistics. We evaluated the strength of inter-rater agreement in the study-selection process, data extraction, and quality analysis by calculating Cohens’s Kappa (κ; see, e.g., Gisev et al., 2013). Confidence intervals (95%) were calculated manually using the standard normal table (z-score table).

2.1 Eligibility criteria

In the current review, we included observational studies that reported at least one individual outcome measure of stuttering in individuals with Down syndrome. These could be studies that investigated stuttering via direct assessments or reports from a third party, such as parents or speech and language pathologists (SLPs). Studies in which the author(s) stated that they investigated stuttering were included. To answer the research questions, only studies that included occurrence estimates of stuttering (% and/or number) were eligible for inclusion. Thus, studies with samples that were preselected based on fluency status were excluded. Studies that investigated the co-existence of stuttering and other developmental speech disorders were included if the stuttering data were separated from other types of data. Mixed-etiology studies were considered for inclusion if they reported separate results for the participants with Down syndrome.

2.2 Search strategy

We developed the search strategy using words related to Down syndrome and stuttering. The search was conducted on 3rd January 2022 in the following databases: PsycINFO (Ovid interface, from 1806 onward), MEDLINE (Ovid interface, from 1946 onward), Eric (Ovid interface, from 1965 onward), Scopus (from 1960 onward), and Web of Science Core Collection (from 1945 onward). See Table 1 for the search strategy used in PsychINFO (Ovid interface, from 1806 onward). We verified and supplemented the electronic database search by searching (1) previous narrative reviews of stuttering or speech disfluency in individuals with Down syndrome and/or intellectual disability (Zisk and Bailer, 1967; Stansfield, 1988; Van Borsel and Tetnowski, 2007; Kent and Vorperian, 2013), (2) the reference lists of the included articles, and (3) acknowledged speech- and language-pathology journals and Google Scholar.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Search strategy for PsychINFO.

2.3 Study selection

The study-selection process had two phases. First, we screened the headings and abstracts and retrieved full-text sources that seemed to meet our inclusion criteria, as well as full-text sources that required further inspection. Second, we assessed the eligibility of these full-text sources. If a source appearing in our search was a chapter in an anthology, we read that specific chapter. When the source appearing in our search was a complete book, we first screened the index and then read the chapter(s) that were relevant to the topic of stuttering and/or disorders of intellectual development. See Appendix A for detailed information on the screening procedures.

2.3.1 Screening of headings and abstracts

The authors individually screened all titles and abstracts yielded by the systematic search against the eligibility criteria. Inter-rater reliability was calculated based on the agreement between the review authors regarding whether to include or exclude a study, as well as the reason for exclusion. There was good agreement between the review authors, κ = 0.715 (95% CI, 0.597 to 0.833), p < 0.001. Disagreements (n = 18 of 137 sources) were resolved through discussions between the review authors, including a reexamination of the headings and abstracts. In these discussions, the review authors were equal in status. Most disagreements concerned the reason for exclusion (i.e., not whether the study should be included). See Figure 1 (flow chart) for a record of the reasons for excluding sources in the heading- and abstract-screening phase.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram (Page et al., 2021).

2.3.2 Assessment of full-text documents

A total of 33 sources were sought for retrieval based on the systematic search. Of these 33 sources, we were able to retrieve full-text manuscripts from 32. Additionally, we sought full-text manuscripts of potentially relevant sources located through supplementary searches (previous reviews, the reference lists of included articles, and free searches in relevant journals and Google Scholar). The supplementary searches revealed 25 sources, Of these 25 sources we were able to retrieve full-text manuscripts from 21. This included our own study (Hokstad et al., 2022), which was not yet published at the time of the search. See the description of the data-extraction process and quality analysis for information about how this study was treated in the review process. One study was excluded without further assessment due to beeing a duplicate.

A total of 52 sources were assessed against the eligibility criteria in the full-text assessment phase. Three sources were assessed in collaboration between the review authors for training purposes (Evans, 1977; Wilcox, 1988; Borsel and Vandermeulen, 2008). During this training, we first assessed the sources independently against the eligibility criteria before comparing and discussing our decisions. We also revised our eligibility criteria when these sources were found to be ambiguous. Our own study, Hokstad et al. (2022), was assessed by an independent third party, a trained speech and language pathologist and assistant professor at the University of Oslo. Three sources were published in languages not mastered by the review authors (Kehrer, 1973; Rabensteiner, 1975; Takagi and Ito, 2007). These sources were assessed in collaboration with third party evaluators a trained speech and language pathologist and assistant professor at the University of Oslo whose first language is German and a professor at Nagoya University whose first language is Japanese.

The remaining 45 sources were screened individually and in duplicate. Evaluations were based on the agreement between the review authors regarding whether to include or exclude a study, as well as the reason for exclusion. There was good agreement between the review authors, κ = 0.749 (95% CI, 0.580 to 0.918), p < 0.001. Disagreements (n = 7 of 45 sources) were discussed and resolved between the two review authors, who were equal in status. Four of the disagreements concerned whether a source met the inclusion criteria. The remaining three disagreements concerned the reason for exclusion. The disagreements were resolved through a reexamination of the text and, on one occasion, making contact with the main author of one study for clarification (Maessen et al., 2021). See Figure 1 (flow chart) for a record of the reasons for excluding studies in the full-text-screening phase and Appendix B for examples of the characteristics of the excluded sources on topics related to stuttering in individuals with Down syndrome.

2.4 Data extraction process

A total of 14 studies were eligible for inclusion. We extracted information related to sample characteristics, measurement approaches, and outcomes. We developed the coding scheme for the data extraction and discussed the content of each category. Then, we selected four sources (Preus, 1972; Devenny and Silverman, 1990; Stansfield, 1990; Salihovic et al., 2012) that we collaboratively assessed to refine our coding categories and training before double-coding. The training included the independent assessment of each source based on our understanding of the coding scheme. Next, we compared our results and discussed our differences. In cases in which we found our category descriptions to be ambiguous, we revised these descriptions. See Appendix C for the coding scheme for data extraction. One source published in a language not mastered by the review authors was coded in collaboration with a third party (Rabensteiner, 1975), a trained speech and language pathologist and assistant professor at the University of Oslo, whose first language is German. The study by Hokstad et al. (2022) was coded by an independent coder who is a trained speech and language pathologist and assistant professor at the University of Oslo. For the remaining eight eligible studies, the review authors extracted data independently and in duplicate. Disagreements were resolved through a reevaluation of the text and discussions between the review authors, who were equal in status. We evaluated the strength of inter-rater agreement by calculating Cohen’s kappa (κ) (see, e.g., Gisev et al., 2013) for each stuttering variable. The agreement between the review authors varied from good, κ = 0.724 (95% CI, 0.416 to 1.000), p < 0.001, to very good, κ = 1.000 (95% CI, 1.000, 1.000), p < 0.001. See Appendix D for κ values for each stuttering variable.

2.5 Quality appraisal

The appraisal tool for cross-sectional studies (AXIS) was used to assess the quality of the included studies. The tool includes items assessing sampling, justifications, clarity, and precision in descriptions of aims/objectives, methods, and results, as well as the reliability and validity of the measurement instruments (see Downes et al., 2016). For studies that included participants who did not have Down syndrome, we considered the information about the participants with Down syndrome only. In studies that investigated other areas of functioning, in addition to stuttering, we considered factors related to the measurement instruments and methodological transparency of the stuttering measures only (Q8–Q11). We made one adjustment when scoring the AXIS items; we only used the categories YES/NO (not using the “Do not know” category). Because scientific transparency is necessary for valid interpretations of the study results and the evaluation of research quality, negative evaluations were given when information in the study was lacking or insufficient for interpretation.

Again, one source (Rabensteiner, 1975) published in German, a language not mastered by the review authors, was coded in collaboration with a third party, and the authors’ own study (Hokstad et al., 2022) was coded by an independent judge. Before coding and double-coding, the authors discussed each item of appraisal and selected three sources for training purposes (Preus, 1972; Devenny and Silverman, 1990; Salihovic et al., 2012). The authors coded the remaining nine studies independently and in duplicate. Disagreements were resolved through reassessments of the articles in question and discussions between the review authors, who were equal in status. We evaluated the strength of the inter-rater agreement for the quality assessment by calculating Cohen’s kappa (see, e.g., Gisev et al., 2013). The inter-rater reliability was calculated based on the agreement between the review authors on each of the 20 AXIS items. Agreement varied from moderate, κ = 0.630 (95% CI, 0.297, 0.963), p < 0.001, to very good, κ = 1.000 (95% CI, 1.000, 1.000), p < 0.001. See Appendix E for the κ values for each AXIS item.

2.6 Data synthesis

We estimated the occurrence of stuttering in the total sample, per gender and per age group, by combining all samples included in the current review.

Occurrence = (the total number of individuals who stutter * 100)/total N.

In the data synthesis, we used the occurrence estimates reported in each individual study, independent of how stuttering was operationalized, thus combining different operationalizations of stuttering. Furthermore, we evaluated sample characteristics, measurement approaches, and stuttering outcomes by conducting a narrative synthesis of the findings consisting of statistical (frequencies, numeric summarizations, average calculations, and numeric comparisons) and narrative (content comparisons and grouping in overarching categories) analyzes. The results are presented in text and table format.

3 Results

3.1 Study selection

A total of 14 studies met the eligibility criteria for inclusion in the current review. One study was the authors’ own study (Hokstad et al., 2022), which was not yet published at the time of the systematic search. The remaining studies were identified through (1) a systematic search (n = 6), (2) a search of the reference lists of previous reviews (n = 6), and (3) the reference lists of included studies (n = 1). See Figure 1 for a flow chart depicting the selection process.

3.2 Study characteristics

The included studies were published between 1955 and 2022 and, as such, represent seven decades of research on stuttering in individuals with Down syndrome. However, most of the studies are older, with a majority (n = 10) having been published before 2000.

3.3 Operationalization of stuttering

Four studies did not contain any operationalization of stuttering (Gottsleben, 1955; Rabensteiner, 1975; Kumin, 1994; Schieve et al., 2009). In eight studies, stuttering was operationalized based on indicators related to speech behaviors alone (Rohovsky, 1965; Eggers and Van Eerdenbrugh, 2018; Hokstad et al., 2022) or in combination with secondary behaviors (Schlanger and Gottsleben, 1957; Martyn et al., 1969; Keane, 1970; Preus, 1972; Devenny and Silverman, 1990) and on affective and cognitive reactions to stuttering (Martyn et al., 1969; Keane, 1970; Preus, 1972). See Table 2 for an overview of the indicators included in the operationalization of stuttering across studies.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Operationalization of stuttering.

In addition to the presence of indicators of stuttering, four studies also reported the threshold at which (e.g., % syllables stuttered cutoff score) stuttering behaviors were considered clinically significant (Keane, 1970; Preus, 1972; Eggers and Van Eerdenbrugh, 2018; Hokstad et al., 2022). See Table 3 for an overview of the frequency cutoff scores used in these four studies. Finally, two studies (Stansfield, 1990; Salihovic et al., 2012) operationalized stuttering based on the frequency of stuttering disfluencies, the duration of stuttering blocks, and the number of physical concomitants (i.e., secondary behaviors), without specifying which types of disfluencies were considered and at what threshold disfluencies were considered stuttering. See Appendix F for a detailed overview of the operationalizations of stuttering in the included studies.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Frequency cutoff scores.

3.4 Measurement approaches

In two studies, stuttering was assessed indirectly through parental reports (Kumin, 1994; Schieve et al., 2009). As the parents simply reported whether their child stuttered or not, these studies provided limited information about stuttering besides the stuttering occurrence estimate.

In 12 studies, stuttering was identified through clinical judgment by either SLPs/SLP students and/or the researcher(s) themselves (Gottsleben, 1955; Schlanger and Gottsleben, 1957; Rohovsky, 1965; Martyn et al., 1969; Keane, 1970; Preus, 1972; Rabensteiner, 1975; Devenny and Silverman, 1990; Stansfield, 1990; Salihovic et al., 2012; Eggers and Van Eerdenbrugh, 2018; Hokstad et al., 2022). In eight of these 12 studies, stuttering was identified through speech-sample analysis (Rohovsky, 1965; Keane, 1970; Preus, 1972; Devenny and Silverman, 1990; Stansfield, 1990; Salihovic et al., 2012; Eggers and Van Eerdenbrugh, 2018; Hokstad et al., 2022), while in four studies, stuttering was identified through either written sources (Gottsleben, 1955; Schlanger and Gottsleben, 1957) or real-time observation (Martyn et al., 1969; Rabensteiner, 1975). Spontaneous speech samples were commonly elicited through planned speaking situations, such as play sessions (Eggers and Van Eerdenbrugh, 2018), conversations about pictures (Preus, 1972; Salihovic et al., 2012), and story retelling (Rohovsky, 1965; Hokstad et al., 2022). In six studies, audio data were collected (Rohovsky, 1965; Preus, 1972; Stansfield, 1990; Salihovic et al., 2012; Eggers and Van Eerdenbrugh, 2018; Hokstad et al., 2022), while video data were collected in two studies (Keane, 1970; Devenny and Silverman, 1990). The length of the speech samples and the amount of speech material collected varied greatly across studies; however, the speech samples were, with one exception (Hokstad et al., 2022), retrieved from only one speaking situation. Furthermore, while some studies used speech samples with variable lengths and amounts of speech (Preus, 1972; Hokstad et al., 2022), others based their evaluation on a set speech-sample length (Rohovsky, 1965; Keane, 1970; Stansfield, 1990) or a set number of words or syllables (Preus, 1972; Devenny and Silverman, 1990; Salihovic et al., 2012; Eggers and Van Eerdenbrugh, 2018). See Table 4 for a detailed overview of the measurement approach(es) used in each study.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Measurement approaches.

3.5 Sample characteristics

A total of 1,833 (M = 131.29, SD = 227.85, median = 45.5) individuals with Down syndrome participated in the included studies, with sample sizes ranging from 26 to 897 participants. Kumin (1994) represented an extreme value (± 3 standard deviations from the mean) with 897 participants. The average sample size with this extreme outlier excluded was 72 participants (SD = 60.46, min = 26, max = 200). Participants of all ages were represented across the included studies. The studies that reported participant age had wide age ranges (Gottsleben, 1955; Rohovsky, 1965; Keane, 1970; Preus, 1972; Rabensteiner, 1975; Devenny and Silverman, 1990; Schieve et al., 2009; Salihovic et al., 2012; Eggers and Van Eerdenbrugh, 2018; Hokstad et al., 2022), with an average age gap of 21 years between the youngest and oldest participants. Hokstad et al. (2022) had the narrowest age spread, at 2 years, while Gottsleben (1955) had the widest age spread, at 43 years. Eight studies reported the gender distribution of the participants (Gottsleben, 1955; Rohovsky, 1965; Keane, 1970; Preus, 1972; Devenny and Silverman, 1990; Schieve et al., 2009; Eggers and Van Eerdenbrugh, 2018; Hokstad et al., 2022), and a total of 310 male and 278 female participants took part in these studies. Seven studies reported including only individuals who used speech (Gottsleben, 1955; Rohovsky, 1965; Preus, 1972; Devenny and Silverman, 1990; Kumin, 1994; Eggers and Van Eerdenbrugh, 2018; Hokstad et al., 2022). However, the level of speech proficiency is often not specified. See Table 5 for a detailed overview of the sample characteristics.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Sample characteristics.

3.6 The occurrence of stuttering in individuals with Down syndrome

The reported occurrence of stuttering varied between 2.38 and 56.00% across the included studies. Combining all samples (total number of individuals who stutter * 100/total N) resulted in an occurrence estimate of 19.80%. Occurrence by gender or the information necessary to calculate occurrence by gender was reported in five studies (Gottsleben, 1955; Rohovsky, 1965; Keane, 1970; Devenny and Silverman, 1990; Eggers and Van Eerdenbrugh, 2018). The results suggest a gender factor of 2:1; 27.11% (45 of 166) of the male participants and 13.64% (21 of 154) of the female participants in these studies were determined to stutter. Information about the age of the stuttering participants was reported in six studies, and their ages ranged from 5 to 58 years of age (Gottsleben, 1955; Rohovsky, 1965; Keane, 1970; Stansfield, 1990; Salihovic et al., 2012; Eggers and Van Eerdenbrugh, 2018). When categorizing the included studies based on age groups (preschool age, school age, adulthood, and mixed), we found seven studies reported on mixed-age samples: from preschool age through adulthood (Kumin, 1994), preschool age through school age (Schieve et al., 2009; Eggers and Van Eerdenbrugh, 2018), and school age through adulthood (Gottsleben, 1955; Keane, 1970; Preus, 1972; Stansfield, 1990). Three of these studies reported separate findings based on age or age intervals, but the participants were not equally distributed across age groups (Stansfield, 1990; Kumin, 1994; Eggers and Van Eerdenbrugh, 2018). Three studies reported on only school-aged participants (Rohovsky, 1965; Salihovic et al., 2012; Hokstad et al., 2022), while one study reported on only adults (Devenny and Silverman, 1990). The remaining three studies could not be categorized due to the lack of information on participant age (Schlanger and Gottsleben, 1957; Martyn et al., 1969; Rabensteiner, 1975). See Table 6 for detailed information on the occurrence of stuttering across studies. See Table 7 for an overview of the occurrence of stuttering across age groups.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Occurence of stuttering.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Occurrence by age group.

3.7 Stuttering outcomes

Stuttering frequency in individuals who stuttered was reported in four studies (Keane, 1970; Salihovic et al., 2012; Eggers and Van Eerdenbrugh, 2018; Hokstad et al., 2022). The approaches to calculating stuttering frequency varied, and these studies were, therefore, not directly comparable. For example, while Eggers and Van Eerdenbrugh (2018) reported an average of 5.1% stuttering-like disfluencies per 100 syllables, Keane (1970) reported an average of 11.35% stuttered words. The distribution of disfluency types in individuals who stutter was only reported in the dissertation by Keane (1970). Based on her reporting of stuttering types, 78% of the disfluencies in individuals with Down syndrome who stuttered (n = 20) were prolongations, which occurred in 19 of 20 participants, and 22% were part-word repetitions, which occurred in 17 of 20 participants.

Stuttering severity was reported in six studies. In half of these studies (Stansfield, 1990; Salihovic et al., 2012; Eggers and Van Eerdenbrugh, 2018), judgments were based on the total score on the Stuttering Severity Instrument (Riley, 1980, 1994), while in the other half (Rohovsky, 1965; Keane, 1970; Hokstad et al., 2022), judgments were based on placement on a severity scale after a perceptual evaluation. According to the findings of these studies, most of the participants displayed mild-to-moderate stuttering, and severe stuttering was rare.

Secondary behaviors were reported in six studies (Rohovsky, 1965; Keane, 1970; Preus, 1972; Stansfield, 1990; Salihovic et al., 2012; Eggers and Van Eerdenbrugh, 2018). Four of these studies reported the number of stuttering participants displaying secondary behaviors (Rohovsky, 1965; Keane, 1970; Stansfield, 1990; Eggers and Van Eerdenbrugh, 2018), while for the remaining two studies, this information was not reported (Salihovic et al., 2012) or was unclear (Preus, 1972). Across the studies that did report the occurrence of secondary behaviors, these behaviors were observed in 66.13% of the stuttering participants.

Outcomes related to the potential adverse effects of stuttering were reported in three studies (Martyn et al., 1969; Keane, 1970; Preus, 1972), all of which concluded that there was no evidence of affective or cognitive reactions in their participants. However, it must be noted that this is our interpretation based on the descriptive information that exists in these research reports. Martyn et al. (1969) stated that stuttering, in their participants, did not appear to be associated with anticipation or avoidance, while Preus (1972) stated that, even though there were signs of avoidance and postponement in some participants, none of them seemed to be aware of or embarrassed by their stuttering. Finally, Keane (1970) placed all stuttering individuals in one of Bloodstein’s four developmental phases of stuttering (Bloodstein, 1960) and reported that none of the participants had reached phase four, advanced stuttering, which includes the anticipation of stuttering, word substitutions, and avoidance of speaking, as well as evidence of fear and embarrassment. None of the more recent studies included information on affective or cognitive reactions to stuttering. See Table 8 for the stuttering outcomes and information regarding the instruments these outcomes are based on.

TABLE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Stuttering outcomes.

3.8 Quality of the included studies

3.8.1 Inter-rater reliability

Inter-rater reliability was given for some or all stuttering measures reported in three of the included studies (Keane, 1970; Eggers and Van Eerdenbrugh, 2018; Hokstad et al., 2022). See Table 9 for an overview of inter-rater reliability measures. Based on the low number of stuttering outcome measures tested for consistency across studies, test validity is an area of great insecurity in existing research on stuttering in individuals with Down syndrome. Additionally, research on typically developing individuals has raised concerns about the inter-rater reliability of both the Stuttering Severity Instrument (SSI; Davidow, 2021) and disfluency-type measures (Cordes, 2000; Einarsdottir and Ingham, 2005), both of which have been used across studies in the current review. It is therefore important, especially with this population, which has profound speech and language difficulties (see, e.g., Martin et al., 2009; Næss et al., 2011; Jones et al., 2019; Wilson et al., 2019; Loveall et al., 2021), that speech evaluations are performed by more than one rater.

TABLE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Table 9. Inter-rater reliability.

3.8.2 Study quality appraisal

The studies in the current review met between 6 and 19 of 20 potential criteria items of the AXIS tool (M = 11.07, SD = 3.99). Thus, the results of the quality appraisal indicate that the quality of the included studies ranges from low to high, with most studies being of moderate-to-low quality. Many studies were found to be lacking in areas related to sampling procedures. First, most studies had small sample sizes, and all studies lacked sample size justifications (AXIS item 3). To provide an example regarding the number of participants necessary for an accurate estimation of stuttering frequency in a population, given an estimated population of 3,725 individuals with Down syndrome in Norway (De Graaf et al., 2021), the minimum sample size for determining the frequency of stuttering in the Norwegian population is 349 participants (95% confidence level, anticipated frequency unknown; Dean et al., 2013). Additionally, as the incidence of stuttering is known to be influenced by the age group sampled (Reilly et al., 2009, 2013), the appropriate sample size is likely to be even higher. Second, convenience sampling was common across studies, and only two studies reported systematic recruitment procedures (Schieve et al., 2009; Hokstad et al., 2022). Thus, for most of the included studies, it was unlikely that samples closely represented the population of individuals with Down syndrome they were drawn from (AXIS items 5 and 6). Small samples recruited through convenience sampling are not well suited to providing estimates of occurrences, as random sampling and adequate sample sizes are necessary for precise prevalence and incidence estimates (Munn et al., 2014). Inaccuracies in the occurrence estimates and related outcomes must therefore be assumed. A related area of concern is the treatment and reporting of non-responders (AXIS items 7, 13, and 14), which is unreported or unclear in several studies. Given the large variation in speech and language proficiency in this population (Karmiloff-Smith et al., 2016), it is, for example, likely that some participants across studies did not provide sufficient speech and/or intelligible speech for an evaluation of stuttering to be conducted. However, in many of the included studies, it is generally not clear whether and how many participants were lost due to restricted speech and language. See Table 10 for an overview of the quality assessment of the included studies for each AXIS item.

TABLE 10
www.frontiersin.org

Table 10. Quality appraisal.

4 Discussion

The current review has five main findings: (1) there was no common approach to identifying stuttering in individuals with Down syndrome, but there was a one-sided focus on observational aspects; (2) the occurrence estimates were generally high but varied across studies; (3) the occurrence estimates were higher in school-aged and adult groups than in the preschool-aged group; (4) the occurrence estimates were higher in male than in female participants; and (5) stuttering was mild-to-moderate, and secondary behaviors were found when measured.

To identify stuttering, the included studies used various assessment approaches, which were initially developed for the typically developing population, mainly focusing on the identification of speech disfluencies, both with and without frequency cutoff scores. However, no studies included self-reports of experiences related to stuttering. One reason for the heavy focus on stuttering behavior may be the high frequency of older studies included in this review. The multidimensional view of stuttering may represent a more recent understanding of the disorder, one in line with the changes in the diagnostic criteria for stuttering in the latest revision of the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11, World Health Organization, 2022), to include the significant effects on functioning (e.g., social communication and personal and family life) in addition to observable behaviors. Another reason for the heavy focus on stuttering behavior may be low expectations regarding these individuals’ capability to evaluate and report their own reactions due to reduced language skills (Martin et al., 2009; Næss et al., 2011) and reduced non-verbal mental ability (Næss et al., 2021). However, reactions to stuttering are found in typically developing children from a very low chronological age (Boey et al., 2009), which may correspond to the lower developmental age in individuals with Down syndrome. Furthermore, the existence of affective and cognitive reactions to stuttering has been described in both children (Bray, 2017) and adults (Jackson et al., 2014) with Down syndrome. Based on these indications, individuals with Down syndrome’s own evaluations can be considered in the identification of stuttering.

Although the results of this review showed that listener evaluation in the form of clinical judgment was a common approach, the factors considered to be indicative of stuttering varied across studies, especially those related to types of repetition, as did what threshold disfluencies were considered clinically significant. However, research on typically developing individuals faces the same challenges, which means that comparisons are restricted between both the studies in this review and research projects on stuttering in general (Einarsdottir and Ingham, 2005). The lack of agreement in the field about the indicators of stuttering, as well as at what threshold (cut-off) disfluency is considered stuttering may influence who is considered to need treatment. It may also fuel the discussion about whether the disfluencies seen in individuals with Down syndrome represent genuine stuttering.

In addition to these general challenges within the field of stuttering related to assessment, there are some specific challenges related to the identification of stuttering behaviors in individuals with Down syndrome. As language development is significantly delayed in this population, there are likely differences in the amount of speech material available for listener evaluation across age groups; preschool-aged children with Down syndrome who have begun to speak will, for example, often produce short utterances (Berglund et al., 2001; Zampini and D’Odorico, 2011). This means that the amount of speech material elicited in one speaking situation may be very limited for some participants, as is the case in, e.g., Eggers and Van Eerdenbrugh (2018). As previously mentioned, concerns have been raised regarding the poor reliability of stuttering measures based on the identification of speech disfluencies in speech samples (Cordes, 2000; Einarsdottir and Ingham, 2005; Davidow, 2021), suggesting that the identification of stuttered disfluencies in the typical population can be challenging. Limited speech, in combination with atypical speech features in individuals with Down syndrome, may pose an added challenge in this regard. The reviewed studies that did include inter-rater reliability analysis did, however, report good or very good reliability. The same results were found in Maessen et al. (2023), who included a preselected sample of individuals with Down syndrome who stuttered. Whether the good reliability scores in these studies are related to, for example, speech characteristics, including the frequency of stuttering types, or to the use of summary agreement scores across all disfluency types (i.e., each disagreement has less influence when the number of stuttering disfluencies is large) is unknown.

The abovementioned concerns and limitations related to stuttering assessment, in addition to the quality of the included studies, constitute the frame within which we can interpret and understand the findings of the current review. Nevertheless, studies consistently report high occurrences of stuttering in individuals with Down syndrome when interpreted against the estimated 1% prevalence for the typical adult population (see, e.g., Månsson, 2000) and the 5–11% cumulative incidence of stuttering in typically developing children (Reilly et al., 2009, 2013). The fact that a high percentage of the individuals in the included studies displayed core stuttering behaviors to a degree which they were judged to stutter is in line with previous research, showing that individuals with Down syndrome are vulnerable to speech, language, and communication difficulties (see, e.g., Martin et al., 2009; Næss et al., 2011; Jones et al., 2019; Wilson et al., 2019; Loveall et al., 2021), including stuttering (Kent and Vorperian, 2013). Recent research has also shown that coexisting speech and/or language disorders are common in individuals who stutter and do not have Down syndrome (Wolk and LaSalle, 2023), as are comorbidities between stuttering and other neurodevelopmental disorders (e.g., disorders of intellectual development; Briley and Ellis, 2018). It is therefore likely that the high rate of occurrence is related to an increased vulnerability to stuttering associated with the biomedical condition of Down syndrome and is not a characteristic of Down syndrome.

The occurrence estimates were high (approximately 18–25%) across school-aged and adult participants, while in preschool-aged participants, the occurrence estimate was low relative to the cumulative incidence in typically developing children. These results indicate an opposite pattern to that commonly observed in typically developing individuals, in which the occurrence usually is higher in the preschool years and decreases with age (Bloodstein et al., 2021). Even though the results of the present study may indicate higher occurrences of stuttering in older individuals with Down syndrome, the results do not necessarily mean that the occurrence of stuttering in this population increases with age. As there are no studies following the same participants across time, the results represent the occurrence of stuttering in different age groups and not the developmental pattern of stuttering. The occurrence of stuttering is expected to vary with the sampled age group (Samson, 2022). However, several aspects related to the design of the included studies make it difficult or even impossible to discuss differences in occurrence estimates across age groups. The combination of non-probability sampling techniques, small sample sizes, and age-spread samples do, for example, mean that there are uneven numbers of participants across age groups. Thus, occurrence estimates may suffer from an overrepresentation of age groups where stuttering is more or less common, or findings simply being coincidental as single participants may have a large influence on the results.

A minority of the studies in this review provided information on the occurrence of stuttering by gender. The synthesis of the results of these studies suggests that stuttering was twice as common in male participants as in female participants. However, the gender balance seems to be similar across studies (based on total n). This result is in line with findings from studies of the typical adult population, in which more male participants are found to stutter than female participants (gender ratio of between 2:1 and 4:1; Craig et al., 2002). In typically developing young children, the gender distribution is more balanced (Samson, 2022), but more male participants than female participants still stutter (gender ratio of 1.6:1; Sjøstrand, 2022). Whether this asynchrony between genders occurs because of skewed birth figures for boys and girls, because male participants are more vulnerable to stuttering or is related to the indicators of stuttering, is unknown.

Conclusions regarding the characteristics of stuttering are restricted by the specificity of the information provided in the included studies. For example, although studies have consistently reported a high occurrence of stuttering, the current review cannot provide much information regarding the distribution of disfluency types, as the dissertation by Keane (1970) is the only study that provides information about the disfluency types identified in participants who stutter. However, the existence of repetitions, prolongations, and blocks, which are usually included in the evaluation of stuttering in typically developing individuals (Bloodstein et al., 2021), were common identifiers of stuttering across studies. Thus, the results indicate that individuals with Down syndrome display the same speech behaviors as typically developing individuals who stutter.

Furthermore, the focus on the potential adverse effects of stuttering is very limited in the reviewed studies. Even though the participants in the current review exhibited stuttering severity in the mild-to-moderate range, this does not mean that the potential effects of stuttering are not extensive, as studies demonstrated no significant relationship between stuttering frequency and negative feelings about communication (Erickson and Block, 2013).

4.1 Implications for practice and research

As stuttering is common, can cause negative reactions (Jackson et al., 2014; Bray, 2017), and have negative consequences for communication in individuals with Down syndrome (Evans, 1977; Maessen et al., 2022), practitioners must refer those with disfluent speech to speech and language therapists for assessment and, eventually, treatment. Thus, validated assessment procedures and research-based treatments developed especially for this population should be trialed in future research. In addition, information that is relevant to teachers, parents, and healthcare professionals should be developed to inform them about the high occurrences of stuttering in individuals with Down syndrome and when referral for the evaluation of stuttering is appropriate.

4.2 Limitations

We want to highlight four limitations related to the occurrence estimates of the present study. Because the measures and sample characteristics differ across the studies, it is not straightforward to conclude the occurrence of stuttering. The findings should be interpreted as estimates, as they are likely influenced by (1) sample size, (2) how stuttering is operationalized and assessed, (3) the language proficiency of the participants, and (4) at what age stuttering is measured.

Furthermore, because this review is based on only concurrent data regarding stuttering, we do not know how the occurrence rate by age relates to the tractability of stuttering in this population. For example, it is unknown whether those who stutter at young ages continue to stutter later in life. To answer this question, longitudinal studies are needed. Also, as this review only included studies that investigated the occurrence of stuttering, studies with samples preselected based on fluency status have not been included. This implies that there may be more available research investigating the characteristics of stuttering in individuals with Down syndrome that has not been included in this study. Finally, it should be mentioned that some of the confidence intervals in our inter-rater reliability analysis are wide, indicating a limitation in the precision of these estimates. This uncertainty in some of the estimated effect sizes may reflect the low number of studies in this review (Hazra, 2017), as each disagreement has a large influence on the effect size and its confidence level.

5 Conclusion

The results of this systematic review show a high rate of stuttering occurrence in individuals with Down syndrome, independent of assessors, when interpreted against results derived from studies on occurrence estimates in typically developing individuals. This applies to both male and female participants, but the relative proportion of male participants among stutterers is higher. Furthermore, the occurrence in the school-aged and adult participant groups is especially high. While the operationalization of stuttering varied across the studies, the identification of repetitions, prolongations, and blocks was typically included. Stuttering was commonly judged to be mild-to-moderate, and secondary behavior was found when measured.

Author contributions

SH and K-AN: conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis, resources, investigation, data curation, writing–original draft preparation, and writing–review and editing. SH: visualization and project administration: K-AN and SH: funding acquisition. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Simon Sundström, Assistant Professor at the University of Oslo, for the coding and quality appraisal of the article published by the review authors; Melanie Kirmess, Assistant Professor at the University of Oslo, for her assistance with translation from German to Norwegian; and Shoji Ishida, Professor at Nagoya University, for his assistance with translation from Japanese to English. Finally, the authors thank the Research Council of Norway (Grant No. 260567 and 238030) and Inland Norway University of Applied Sciences for providing funding.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1176743/full#supplementary-material

References

Note: *Included in review.

Google Scholar

Beilby, J. (2014). Psychosocial impact of living with a stuttering disorder: knowing is not enough. Semin. Speech Lang. 35, 132–143. doi: 10.1055/s-0034-1371756

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berglund, E., Eriksson, M., and Johansson, I. (2001). Parental reports of spoken language skills in children with Down syndrome. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 44, 179–191. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2001/016)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bloodstein, O. (1960). The development of stuttering: II. Developmental phases. J. Speech Hear. Disord. 25, 366–376. doi: 10.1044/jshd.2504.366

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bloodstein, O., Ratner, N. B., and Brundage, S. B. (2021). A handbook on stuttering. San Diego: Plural Publishing.

Google Scholar

Blumgart, E., Tran, Y., Yaruss, J. S., and Craig, A. (2012). Australian normative data for the overall assessment of the Speaker’s experience of stuttering. J. Fluen. Disord. 37, 83–90. doi: 10.1016/j.jfludis.2011.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boey, R. A., Van de Heyning, P. H., Wuyts, F. L., Heylen, L., Stoop, R., and De Bodt, M. S. (2009). Awareness and reactions of young stuttering children aged 2–7 years old towards their speech disfluency. J. Commun. Disord. 42, 334–346. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2009.03.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boey, R. A., Wuyts, F. L., Van de Heyning, P. H., De Bodt, M. S., and Heylen, L. (2007). Characteristics of stuttering-like disfluencies in Dutch-speaking children. J. Fluen. Disord. 32, 310–329. doi: 10.1016/j.jfludis.2007.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borsel, V., and Vandermeulen, A. (2008). Cluttering in Down syndrome. Folia Phoniatr. Logop. 60, 312–317. doi: 10.1159/000170081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bray, M. (2017). Exploring fluency in Down syndrome. Havant: J and R Press Limited.

Google Scholar

Briley, P. M., and Ellis, C. (2018). The coexistence of disabling conditions in children who stutter: evidence from the national health interview survey. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 61, 2895–2905. doi: 10.1044/2018_JSLHR-S-17-0378

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Briley, P. M., O’Brien, K., and Ellis, C. (2019). Behavioral, emotional, and social well-being in children who stutter: evidence from the National Health Interview Survey. J. Dev. Phys. 31, 39–53. doi: 10.1007/s10882-018-9625-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chapman, R., and Hesketh, L. (2001). Language, cognition, and short-term memory in individuals with Down syndrome. Down Syndr. Res. Pract. 7, 1–7. doi: 10.3104/reviews.108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Constantino, C. D., Leslie, P., Quesal, R. W., and Yaruss, J. S. (2016). A preliminary investigation of daily variability of stuttering in adults. J. Commun. Disord. 60, 39–50. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2016.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cordes, A. K. (2000). Individual and consensus judgments of disfluency types in the speech of persons who stutter. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 43, 951–964. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4304.951

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, A., Blumgart, E., and Tran, Y. (2009). The impact of stuttering on the quality of life in adults who stutter. J. Fluen. Disord. 34, 61–71. doi: 10.1016/j.jfludis.2009.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, A., Hancock, K., Tran, Y., Craig, M., and Peters, K. (2002). Epidemiology of stuttering in the community across the entire lifespan. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 45, 1097–1105. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2002/088)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davidow, J. H. (2021). Reliability and similarity of the stuttering severity instrument-and a global severity rating scale. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 24, 20–27. doi: 10.1080/2050571X.2020.1730545

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Graaf, G., Buckley, F., and Skotko, B. G. (2021). Estimation of the number of individuals with Down syndrome in Europe. Eur. J. Hum. Genet. 29, 402–410. doi: 10.1038/s41431-020-00748-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dean, A. G., Sullivan, K. M., and Soe, M. M. (2013). OpenEpi: Open Source Epidemiologic Statistics for Public Health, Version. Available at: https://www.openepi.com/SampleSize/SSPropor.htm (Accessed August 15, 2023).

Google Scholar

*Devenny, D. A., and Silverman, W. P. (1990). Speech dysfluency and manual specialization in Down’s syndrome. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res., 34: 253–260. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.1990.tb01536.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Downes, M. J., Brennan, M. L., Williams, H. C., and Dean, R. S. (2016). Development of a critical appraisal tool to assess the quality of cross-sectional studies (AXIS). BMJ Open 6:e011458. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2016-011458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Druker, K., Mazzucchelli, T., Hennessey, N., and Beilby, J. (2020). An evaluation of an integrated stuttering and parent-administered self-regulation program for early developmental stuttering disorders. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 63, 2894–2912. doi: 10.1044/2020_JSLHR-19-00310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Eggers, K., and Van Eerdenbrugh, S. (2018). Speech disfluencies in children with Down syndrome. J. Commun. Disord. 71, 72–84. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2017.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Einarsdottir, J., and Ingham, R. J. (2005). Have disfluency-type measures contributed to the understanding and treatment of developmental stuttering? Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 14, 260–273. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2005/026)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erickson, S., and Block, S. (2013). The social and communication impact of stuttering on adolescents and their families. J. Fluen. Disord. 38, 311–324. doi: 10.1016/j.jfludis.2013.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, D. (1977). The development of language abilities in mongols: a correlational study. J. Ment. Defic. Res. 21, 103–117. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.1977.tb00030.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gisev, N., Bell, J. S., and Chen, T. F. (2013). Interrater agreement and interrater reliability: key concepts, approaches, and applications. Res. Soc. Adm. Pharm. 9, 330–338. doi: 10.1016/j.sapharm.2012.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Gottsleben, R. H. (1955). The incidence of stuttering in a group of mongoloids. Train. Sch. Bull., 51, 209–218.

Google Scholar

Gough, D., and Thomas, J. (2016). Systematic reviews of research in education: aims, myths and multiple methods. Rev. Educ. Res. 4, 84–102. doi: 10.1002/rev3.3068

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guitar, B. (2014). Stuttering: an integrated approach to its nature and treatment. 4th Edn. Philadelphia, Pa: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.

Google Scholar

Guttormsen, L. S., Yaruss, J. S., and Næss, K. A. B. (2021). Parents’ perceptions of the overall impact of stuttering on young children. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 30, 2130–2142. doi: 10.1044/2021_AJSLP-20-00113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, V., Onslow, M., Packman, A., Harrison, E., and Menzies, R. (2002). An experimental investigation of the impact of the Lidcombe program on early stuttering. J. Fluen. Disord. 27, 203–214. doi: 10.1016/S0094-730X(02)00127-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hazra, A. (2017). Using the confidence interval confidently. J. Thorac. Dis. 9, 4124–4129. doi: 10.21037/jtd.2017.09.14

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, J. P. T., Thomas, J., Chandler, J., Cumpston, M., Li, T., Page, M. J., et al. (2022). Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions version 6.3 (updated February 2022). Cochrane. Available at: www.training.cochrane.org/handbook.

Google Scholar

*Hokstad, S., Næss, K. A. B., Yaruss, J. S., Hoff, K., Melle, A. H., and Lervåg, A. O. (2022). Stuttering behavior in a national age cohort of Norwegian first-graders with Down syndrome. J. Speech Lang. Hear., 65: 4133–4150. doi: 10.1044/2022_JSLHR-21-00605

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, C., Cavenagh, P., and Clibbens, J. (2014). Communication and self-esteem in adults with Down syndrome. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 49, 275–287. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12060

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, H. N., Crisp, K. D., Kuchibhatla, M., Mahler, L., Risoli, T. Jr., Jones, C. W., et al. (2019). Auditory-perceptual speech features in children with Down syndrome. Am. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 124, 324–338. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-124.4.324

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karmiloff-Smith, A., Al-Janabi, T., D’Souza, H., Groet, J., Massand, E., Mok, K., et al. (2016). The importance of understanding individual differences in Down syndrome. F1000Research 5:389. doi: 10.12688/f1000research.7506.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Keane, V. A. (1970). An investigation of disfluent speech behavior in Down’s syndrome unpublished dissertation. Eugene, Oregon: University of Oregon.

Google Scholar

Kehrer, H. E. (1973). Behaviour therapy in child and adolescent psychiatry. Acta Paedopsychiatr. 40, 58–65.

Google Scholar

Kent, R., and Vorperian, H. K. (2013). Speech impairment in Down syndrome: a review. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 56, 178–210. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2012/12-0148)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koedoot, C., Versteegh, M., and Yaruss, J. S. (2011). Psychometric evaluation of the Dutch translation of the overall assessment of the Speaker’s experience of stuttering for adults (OASES-AD). J. Fluen. Disord. 36, 222–230. doi: 10.1016/j.jfludis.2011.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Kumin, L. (1994). Intelligibility of speech in children with Down syndrome in natural settings: parents’ perspective. Percept. Mot. Skills, 78, 307–313. doi: 10.2466/pms.1994.78.1.307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lattermann, C., Euler, H. A., and Neumann, K. (2008). A randomized control trial to investigate the impact of the Lidcombe program on early stuttering in German-speaking preschoolers. J. Fluen. Disord. 33, 52–65. doi: 10.1016/j.jfludis.2007.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, C., Packman, A., Onslow, M., Simpson, J. M., and Jones, M. (2008). A phase ii trial of telehealth delivery of the lidcombe program of early stuttering intervention. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 17, 139–149. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2008/014)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loveall, S. J., Hawthorne, K., and Gaines, M. (2021). A meta-analysis of prosody in autism, Williams syndrome, and Down syndrome. J. Commun. Disord. 89:106055. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2020.106055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maessen, B., Rombouts, E., Maes, B., and Zink, I. (2022). Influence of gestures on the intelligibility and comprehensibility of utterances with stuttering events in individuals with Down syndrome. J. Commun. Disord. 95:106178. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2021.106178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maessen, B., Zink, I., Maes, B., and Rombouts, E. (2021). An experiment on measuring awareness of stuttering in individuals with Down syndrome. J. Fluen. Disord. 68:105849. doi: 10.1016/j.jfludis.2021.105849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maessen, B., Zink, I., Maes, B., and Rombouts, E. (2023). The effect of manual movements on stuttering in individuals with Down syndrome. J. Fluen. Disord. 75:105958. doi: 10.1016/j.jfludis.2023.105958

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Månsson, H. (2000). Childhood stuttering: incidence and development. J. Fluen. Disord. 25, 47–57. doi: 10.1016/S0094-730X(99)00023-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, G. E., Klusek, J., Estigarribia, B., and Roberts, J. E. (2009). Language characteristics of individuals with Down syndrome. Top. Lang. Disord. 29, 112–132. doi: 10.1097/tld.0b013e3181a71fe1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Martyn, M. M., Sheehan, J., and Slutz, K. (1969). Incidence of stuttering and other speech disorders among the retarded. Am. J. Ment. Defic., 74: 206–211.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Millard, S. K., Zebrowski, P., and Kelman, E. (2018). Palin parent–child interaction therapy: the bigger picture. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 27, 1211–1223. doi: 10.1044/2018_AJSLP-ODC11-17-0199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Munn, Z., Moola, S., Riitano, D., and Lisy, K. (2014). The development of a critical appraisal tool for use in systematic reviews addressing questions of prevalence. Int. J. Health Policy Manag. 3, 123–128. doi: 10.15171/ijhpm.2014.71

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Næss, K. A. B., Lyster, S. A. H., Hulme, C., and Melby-Lervåg, M. (2011). Language and verbal short-term memory skills in children with Down syndrome: a meta-analytic review. Res. Dev. Disabil. 32, 2225–2234. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2011.05.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Næss, K. A. B., Ostad, J., and Nygaard, E. (2021). Differences and similarities in predictors of expressive vocabulary development between children with Down syndrome and young typically developing children. Brain Sci. 11:312. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11030312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Onslow, M., Webber, M., Harrison, E., Arnott, S., Bridgman, K., Carey, B., et al. (2020). The Lidcombe program treatment guide. Lidcombe Program Trainers Consortium. Available at: https://www.uts.edu.au/sites/default/files/2020-07/Lidcombe%20Program%20Treatment%20Guide%202020%201.0_1.pdf

Google Scholar

Page, M. J., McKenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., et al. (2021). The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 372:n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Preus, A. (1972). Stuttering in Down’s syndrome. Scand. J. Educ. Res., 16: 89–104. doi: 10.1080/0031383720160106

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Rabensteiner, B. (1975). Social behavior, musicality, and visual perception in mongoloid children (author’s transl). Padiatr. Padol., 4: 59–69.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Reilly, S., Onslow, M., Packman, A., Cini, E., Conway, L., Ukoumunne, O. C., et al. (2013). Natural history of stuttering to 4 years of age: a prospective community-based study. Pediatrics 132, 460–467. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-3067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reilly, S., Onslow, M., Packman, A., Wake, M., Bavin, E. L., Prior, M., et al. (2009). Predicting stuttering onset by the age of 3 years: a prospective, community cohort study. Pediatrics 123, 270–277. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-3219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riley, G. D. (1980) Stuttering severity instrument. CC Publications, Oregon.

Google Scholar

Riley, G. D. (1994). Stuttering severity instrument for children and adults. 3rd Edn. Austin, TX. Pro-ed Inc.

Google Scholar

*Rohovsky, K. A. (1965). A study of stuttering in institutional and non-institutional mongoloids [Unpublished master’s thesis]. Columbus: Ohio State University.

Google Scholar

Samson, I. (2022). Stuttering in young females and males [dissertation]. [Stockholm, Sweden]: Karolinska Institutet.

Google Scholar

*Salihovic, N., Hasanbasic, S., and Begic, L. (2012). Incidence of stuttering in school-age children with Down syndrome. J. Spec. Educ. Rehabil., 13:31. doi: 10.2478/v10215-011-0017-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Schieve, L. A., Boulet, S. L., Boyle, C., Rasmussen, S. A., and Schendel, D. (2009). Health of children 3 to 17 years of age with Down syndrome in the 1997-2005 National Health Interview Survey. Pediatrics, 123: e253–e260. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-1440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Schlanger, B. B., and Gottsleben, R. H. (1957). Analysis of speech defects among the institutionalized mentally retarded. J. Speech Hear. Disord., 22: 98–103. doi: 10.1044/jshd.2201.98

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sjøstrand, Å. (2022). Gender variance among kindergarten children who stutter In Norwegian: Kjønnsfordeling blant barnehagebarn som stammer. Christmas calendar Department of special needs education. Video. ISP Julekalender 20, December. Available at: https://www.uv.uio.no/isp/bilder/2022/julekalender-dag-20.mp4?vrtx=view-as-webpage

Google Scholar

Sjøstrand, Å., Kefalianos, E., Hofslundsengen, H., Guttormsen, L. S., Kirmess, M., Lervåg, A., et al. (2021). Non-pharmacological interventions for stuttering in children six years and younger. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD013489.pub2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stansfield, J. (1988). Stuttering and cluttering in the mentally handicapped population: a review of the literature. Br. J. Ment. Subnorm. 34, 54–61. doi: 10.1179/bjms.1988.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Stansfield, J. (1990). Prevalence of stuttering and cluttering in adults with mental handicaps. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res., 34: 287–307. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.1990.tb01541.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takagi, J., and Ito, T. (2007). Disfluent utterances of Japanese youth with Down’s syndrome. Jpn. J. Spec. Educ. 45, 117–125. doi: 10.6033/tokkyou.45.117

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tichenor, S. E., and Yaruss, J. S. (2019). Stuttering as defined by adults who stutter. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 62, 4356–4369. doi: 10.1044/2019_JSLHR-19-00137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tichenor, S. E., and Yaruss, J. S. (2021). Variability of stuttering: behavior and impact. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 30, 75–88. doi: 10.1044/2020_AJSLP-20-00112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tumanova, V., Conture, E. G., Lambert, E. W., and Walden, T. A. (2014). Speech disfluencies of preschool-age children who do and do not stutter. J. Commun. Disord. 49, 25–41. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2014.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Borsel, J., and Tetnowski, J. A. (2007). Fluency disorders in genetic syndromes. J. Fluen. Disord. 32, 279–296. doi: 10.1016/j.jfludis.2007.07.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward, D. (2018). Stuttering and cluttering: Frameworks for understanding and treatment. Abingdon: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Wilcox, A. (1988). An investigation into non-fluency in Down’s syndrome. Br. J. Disord. Commun. 23, 153–170. doi: 10.3109/13682828809019884

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, E. M., Abbeduto, L., Camarata, S. M., and Shriberg, L. D. (2019). Speech and motor speech disorders and intelligibility in adolescents with Down syndrome. Clin. Linguist. Phon. 33, 790–814. doi: 10.1080/02699206.2019.1595736

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolk, L., and LaSalle, L. (2023). T-PALS framework to assess children who stutter with coexisting disorders: a tutorial. J. Fluen. Disord. 76:105974. doi: 10.1016/j.jfludis.2023.105974

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization. (2022). ICD-11: International classification of diseases (11th revision). Available at: https://icd.who.int/ (Accessed August 18, 2023).

Google Scholar

Zampini, L., and D’Odorico, L. (2011). Lexical and syntactic development in Italian children with Down’s syndrome. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 46, 386–396. doi: 10.3109/13682822.2010.508764

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zisk, P. K., and Bailer, I. (1967). Speech and language problems in mongolism: a review of the literature. J. Speech Hear. Disord. 32, 228–241. doi: 10.1044/jshd.3203.228

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Down syndrome, stuttering, speech, disfluency, stuttering assessment, systematic review

Citation: Hokstad S and Næss K-AB (2023) Stuttering in individuals with Down syndrome: a systematic review of earlier research. Front. Psychol. 14:1176743. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1176743

Received: 28 February 2023; Accepted: 09 October 2023;
Published: 29 November 2023.

Edited by:

Susan Loveall, University of Nebraska-Lincoln, United States

Reviewed by:

Ellen Rombouts, KU Leuven, Belgium
Harrison Jones, Duke University, United States

Copyright © 2023 Hokstad and Næss. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Silje Hokstad, c2lsamUuaG9rc3RhZEBpbm4ubm8=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.